Impact Factor 2.129 | CiteScore 2.40
More on impact ›

Opinion ARTICLE Provisionally accepted The full-text will be published soon. Notify me

Front. Psychol. | doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01982

Why Most Psychologists Should Assess and Report Personality

  • 1Independent researcher, Spain

1 Introduction
The person-situation debate is probably one of the most significant debates in the history of psychology since the late 1960s. Most of the empirical issues that ignited the debate have been resolved (e.g., Fleeson, 2004; Mischel & Shoda, 1998; Lucas & Donnellan, 2009). Many researchers recognize that both the characteristics of persons and situations have important effects on behavior: (a) personality traits are useful for predicting cognitions, emotions, and behaviors across many situations and (b) characteristics of a specific situation are useful in making successful predictions of a given individual (Sherman, Rauthmann, Brown, Serfass, & Jones, 2015).
The evidence for the importance of personality is overwhelming. A number of meta-analyses have found robust statistically significant relations between personality characteristics and a wide variety of variables. For example, conscientiousness shows consistent relations with job proficiency and training proficiency (Barrick & Mount, 1991); neuroticism and extraversion influence job satisfaction (Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002); males are found to be more assertive and have slightly higher self-esteem than females, while females are higher than males in tendermindedness, extraversion, anxiety, and trust (Feingold, 1994); repressive-defensiveness, trust, emotional stability, locus of control-chance, desire for control, hardiness, positive affectivity, private collective self-esteem, and tension are closely associated with subjective well-being (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998); creative people are more open to new experiences, less conventional and less conscientious, more self-accepting, self-confident, hostile, impulsive, driven, ambitious, and dominant (Feist, 1998); academic performance is found to correlate with agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness (Poropat, 2009); extraversion is consistently related to three dimensions of transformational leadership: idealized influence-inspirational motivation (charisma), intellectual stimulation, and individualized consideration (Bono & Judge, 2004); individuals increase in measures of social dominance, conscientiousness, and emotional stability, especially in young adulthood (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006); neuroticism predicts problematic strategies like wishful thinking, withdrawal, and emotion-focused coping but, like extraversion, also predicts support seeking (Connor-Smith, & Flachsbart, 2007); individuals with anxiety, depressive, and/or substance abuse disorders tend to be high on neuroticism and low on conscientiousness (Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010); the dark triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) is more prevalent among men than women and is generally associated with various types of negative psychosocial outcomes such as aggression/delinquency, interpersonal difficulties, sex-related issues, and antisocial tactics (Muris, Merckelbach, Otgaar, & Meijer, 2017); extraversion, neuroticism and conscientiousness are correlated with physical activity (Rhodes & Smith, 2006); individuals scoring high on openness to experience tend to value novelty (self-direction and stimulation values) and particularly novel ideas and broadmindedness, whereas individuals who score low on openness to experience tend to value tradition, conformity, and security values (Parks-Leduc, Feldman, & Bardi, 2015); neuroticism and consciousness are the strongest correlates of trait emotional intelligence and ability emotional intelligence (van der Linden et al., 2017); the average effect of genetic contributions to individual differences in personality is 40%, while 60% is due to environmental influences (Vukasović & Bratko, 2015).
Researchers interested in exploring this field further, may want to read Theodore Millon’s evolutionary theory of personality and psychopathology (Millon, 2011) and the new edition of the Handbook of personality: Theory and Research (Robins & John, in press).
2 Personality is missing
The American Psychological Association has recently published new reporting standards for quantitative and qualitative research recommending researchers to report participant characteristics that might influence the data collected, such as major demographic characteristics (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity, socioeconomic status) and important topic-specific information (Appelbaum et al., 2018; Levitt et al., 2018), but failed to mention personality explicitly. One might assume that this omission is perhaps a sign that personality type may not be a pertinent criterium to consider, but this assumption is not supported by the accumulated evidence in the field.
The most likely reason for this omission is that the authors focused exclusively on generating ideas to increase methodological transparency and basically didn’t consider new ideas in other areas. There is a growing sense of crisis in psychological science (e.g., Maxwell, Lau, & Howard, 2015; Tackett et al., 2017). Investigators have concerns over questionable research practices that result in grossly inflated false positive error rates (e.g., John, Loewenstein, & Prelec, 2012), reproducibility of reported analyses (e.g., Shrout & Rodgers, 2018), and replication of empirical findings (e.g., Rahal & Open Science Collaboration, 2015). Recommendations designed to speed up knowledge construction in psychology have focused on methodological and procedural steps, such as preregistering hypotheses, addressing measurement error, setting aside data for confirmation, justifying restriction on the samples, or collaborating to replicate, among many others (for a large number of these recommendations, see Shrout & Rodgers , 2018). The APA papers have addressed many of these suggestions.
I’d argue that knowledge construction is not just a matter of transparency and methodological excellence. It’s also a matter of integrating disparate findings into a coherent set of propositions with higher explanatory power. Personality is one of the missing links that may allow such integration. By neglecting the importance of personality, we are not learning as much as we might.
Empirical researchers are encouraged to assess and report personality data in their papers. Documenting this type of information may allow for more nuanced interpretations of results. Meta-analyses will be able to find relevant personality effects not encountered in the original papers, and our understanding of human behavior will be remarkably enhanced by integrating disparate results from different fields using personality as a common denominator. Unlike behavior genetics, personality measurement is inexpensive, and some instruments only require a few minutes of administration time.
Studies with enough statistical power may contemplate incorporating personality either as a moderator variable or as a covariate, depending on the research question. Personality is normally considered a latent variable although whether we consider a variable latent or not depends on the definition used (Bollen, 2002). Regardless of statistical power, investigators shouldn’t avoid including a proper description (average and SD) of the personality of participants.
3 Discussion
Researchers tend to describe situational variables reasonably well in their studies. Most psychology focus on static situations (i.e., situational variables that do not or cannot change). In these cases, situations are normally well described by simply referring to the experimentally manipulated stimuli. For more complex situations such as dynamic situations (e.g., a person in a party), researchers have recently developed the Situational Eight DIAMONDS Model (e.g., Rauthmann & Sherman, 2016).
The same cannot be said of personality. Why aren’t psychologists regularly including measures of personality in most of their studies? The majority are simply following the current conventions of their field. Some investigators may be unaware of the predictive power of personality, while others may not see the relevance of measuring the construct when it’s unrelated with the problem under study.
Researchers interested in assessing and reporting personality may be unsure about how to measure it properly due to the number of tools available. The two most widely used and empirically validated instruments are the NEO Inventories and the Big Five Inventory (Costa, McCrae, & Löckenhoff, 2019). Based on John, Naumann, & Soto (2008), the following indications are recommended, depending on participant time constraints: When time is not at a problem, participants are well educated and test-savvy, and the research question targets personality specifically, then the full 240-item NEO-PI-3, a revision of the revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McRae, 2010) would be most useful. Otherwise, the 60-item Big Five Inventory 2 (BFI-2; Soto & John, 2017a) is a good alternative, taking only about 6 minutes of administration time. The suggested inventories have good psychometric properties and may facilitate the integration of results across multiple studies. There are also two abbreviated versions of the BFI-2 (Soto & John, 2017b): the 30-item BFI-2-S and the 15-item BFI-2-XS. Other commonly used measures include the 300-item International Personality Item Pool-NEO (Goldberg et al., 2006), and the 100-item Revised HEXACO Personality Inventory (HEXACO-PI-R; Ashton & Lee 2016). The HEXACO-PI-R has six rather than five factors; it adds an Honesty/Humility factor that the five-factor model includes within its broader Agreeableness factor.
Most psychologists recognize that human beings are complex psychological entities, composed of multiple dimensions. To advance more efficiently towards effective knowledge accumulation, a psychology of the 21st century needs to bring the person back. Assessing and reporting personality is a big step in that direction.

Keywords: Personality Assessment, reporting standards, methodology, research participants, knowledge accumulation

Received: 18 Jul 2019; Accepted: 13 Aug 2019.

Copyright: © 2019 Pinon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Dr. Adelson Pinon, Independent researcher, Santiago de Compostela, Spain, adelsonpr3@gmail.com