Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 September 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology

Impact of Life Experiences and Use of Web 2.0 Tools in Adults and Older Adults

  • 1Personality, Evaluation and Psychological Treatment, Department of Psychology, Sociology and Philosophy, Universidad de León, León, Spain
  • 2Developmental and Educational Psychology, Department of Psychology, Sociology and Philosophy, Universidad de León, León, Spain
  • 3Educational Psychology, Department of Humanities, Universidad de la Costa, Barranquilla, Colombia

The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between favorable and stressful life experiences and perceived quality of life, practices that promote quality of life, psychological profiles, and the daily use of web 2.0 tools in adults and older adults. An online questionnaire was designed, which was administered to 1,095 Spanish adults and older adults, and conducted descriptive and multivariate analyses using the general linear model. Our results showed that favorable and stressful life experiences alike were associated with differential patterns in psychological profiles, perceived quality of life and daily activities and practices that affect quality of life, including the use of web 2.0 tools. Favorable life experiences mainly affected psychological profiles and the use of web 2.0 tools, whereas stressful life experiences affected the other factors analyzed. Statistically significant differences were not found according to age and gender. These findings have important implications for promoting successful psychological and social interventions.

Introduction

As population aging becomes one of the main challenges for societies worldwide, there is a growing interest in addressing the determinants that have an impact on the well-being and quality of life of the elderly, among them, the life experiences. People's life experiences largely determine who they become. Their psychological profiles, personalities, lifestyles, moods, routines, interests, and even identities are partially shaped by the events that have happened in their lives, including the drastic changes that society has witnessed in recent years driven by the irruption of new technologies (Bai et al., 2014; El Haj and Antoine, 2017). Without a doubt, this change has largely been defined by the establishment of the internet and web 2.0 tools as an integral part of our lives and social participation, tools that allow to create and share information, collaborate, promote social contacts, and full integration into society. These technologies have been changing over the years. The main contribution of web 2.0 is the user's participation in the environment, creating and sharing information, unlike what happened in the web 1.0. Taking this into account, web 2.0 could be defined as an environment that favors the communication, collaboration, social relations, exchange, creation, collection, and transformation of content. Web 2.0 technologies include a wide range of tools, including blogs, social networks, wikis, podcasts, among others (Açikgül and Serdar, 2019). This has marked a before and after in the way in which we handle information, interact with each other and with the world around us and carry out our daily activities (Díaz-Prieto and García-Sánchez, 2016). Although data are available on the impact of life experiences on psychological profiles, practices and routines, and more generally on life satisfaction, well-being and quality of life in adults and older adults (Thomsen et al., 2016) little is known to date about the relationship between life experiences and the use of web 2.0 tools.

Studies on life experiences have traditionally employed techniques such as reminiscence therapy, a review of life or significant life events, reminiscence writing and life stories, with varying results. While some studies have highlighted the benefits, others have called these into question and have identified potential difficulties. Those that have found benefits include studies suggesting that these interventions exert a positive effect on depressive symptoms, anxiety, loneliness, socialization, memory, and self-esteem, and more generally on physical and mental health, life satisfaction, well-being, and quality of life, in both the healthy population and the population affected by a range of psychological, cognitive, affective-emotional, behavioral, or social problems (Hyams and Scogin, 2015; Latorre et al., 2015; Lopes et al., 2016; El Haj and Antoine, 2017; Wren, 2017). However, other studies have indicated the maladaptive effect of reminiscence, whereby experiences may be emphasized in an unhealthy manner or prompt rumination, self-blame and pessimism, and memories of these may undermine happiness, satisfaction, and well-being or trigger a series of negative emotions (Henkel et al., 2016; Stikkelbroek et al., 2016). Moreover, most studies have focused on analyzing stressful life events, their negative consequences and the coping strategies employed to get over them, especially in the older population (Latorre et al., 2015; Randall et al., 2015; Lasgaard et al., 2016). These studies have identified a series of events that negatively affect mental health, well-being and quality of life, including illness, grief, loneliness, social problems, changes of residence and work or financial problems, among many others (Donoghue et al., 2016; Chukwuorji et al., 2017; Tveit-Sekse et al., 2019). For example, theoretical approaches to solitude have demonstrated that important life events such as the death of a loved one or divorce prompt changes in interpersonal relations, triggering or perpetuating feelings of loneliness (Lasgaard et al., 2016). Other studies have focused on analyzing the psychopathological effect of stressful life events, associating them with high rates of anxiety and depression (Eisenbarth et al., 2019) and changes in personality traits (Bleidorn et al., 2018). Some studies have found that the negative life events accumulated throughout the life cycle have long-term effects on well-being. Thus, negative socio-economic circumstances, emotional abuse and neglect during childhood and negative socio-economic circumstances, sexual abuse, emotional abuse and neglect, relational stress, and problem behavior during late adulthood, are associated with higher rates of depression in old age (Kraaij and Wilde, 2001). Life experiences thus have a proven impact on psychosocial and emotional profiles and perceived quality of life, although this evidence is mainly based on negative rather than favorable life experiences and their consequences. It therefore seems pertinent to analyse the extent to which favorable life experiences are related to psychological profiles, given their plausible impact on the well-being and quality of life of adults and older adults.

Just as life experiences influence quality of life, it has also been found that various empirical evidence-based practices contribute to its optimization. Among others, physical exercise, mental activity, and self health care have proven effective in enhancing quality of life and promoting active aging (Foster and Walker, 2015; Hongthong et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2015; Marcus-Varwijk et al., 2016). Some studies have found how certain activities and practices promote the quality of life of adults and seniors: have good social relationships, help and support; living in a nice home and neighborhood, safety, use of community facilities and services; participate in leisure activities; social participation; have a positive psychological perspective and acceptance of circumstances that can not be changed; good health and mobility; and have enough money to satisfy basic needs, enjoy life and preserve independence and control of life (Gabriel and Bowling, 2004). However, the data on how life experiences determine such practices are scant.

Studies have also been conducted on the relationship between life experiences and pathological, but not day-to-day, internet use. An abundance of research exists on the predictors, patterns, and benefits of use of this medium in the adult population (Zheng et al., 2015; Díaz-Prieto and García-Sánchez, 2016; Marston et al., 2016); however, few studies have analyzed the impact of life experiences on use of the internet and web 2.0 tools, and those which have done so, have analyzed stressful life events and their impact on problematic internet use, since this medium often serves as a coping, refuge or escape mechanism, being used as a strategy to achieve mental disconnection or seek information and support (Li et al., 2010; Chan, 2015; van Ingen et al., 2016; van Ingen and Matzat, 2018; Xiao et al., 2019). According to these studies, internet addiction is a response to stressful life events that generate psychological stress in individuals who use this medium as a way of coping and regulating negative emotions in the absence of other types of positive coping strategies (Salovaara et al., 2010; Li et al., 2016). Other studies have used the internet as a tool to implement interventions based on a review of life, finding positive effects on depression, well-being, self-esteem, and obsessive reminiscence (Preschl et al., 2012). Nevertheless, further research is required to analyse the relationship between favorable as well as stressful life experiences and the use and benefits of web 2.0 tools.

Consequently, the research question that guided the present study was whether a relationship existed between favorable and stressful life experiences and perceived quality of life, practices that promote quality of life, psychological profiles and the daily use of web 2.0 tools in adults and older adults. Given that aging involves a wide range of life experiences, it would be expected to find differential patterns in quality of life practices, perceived quality of life, psychological profiles, and daily use of web 2.0 tools according to subjects' favorable and stressful life experiences, age, and gender.

Methods

Participants

An online assessment instrument was administered to 1,095 Spanish adults and older adults, 439 of whom were men and 656 women (Table 1). The sample was recruited through various institutions, organizations, centers, public and private universities, and university programmes for older adults throughout Spain, from September 2016 to February 2017. The inclusion criteria were: (i) people aged over 18 years old; (ii) informed consent to participate; (iii) basic digital competence; and (iv) sufficient autonomy to answer assessment instruments themselves.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Distribution of participants by age and sex (n = 1,095).

Instrument and Variables

The instrument Practices in Adults and Older Adults (Spanish acronym: PRAMA) was designed using the Google Forms tool, and administered it online. This instrument consists of six scales which measure the following specific variables:

1. PRAMA-DD: this includes a series of items related to sociodemographic data: sex, age, marital status, place of origin, place of residence, educational level, employment status, occupation, economic level, indicator of independent living, and degree of independence.

2. PRAMA-PQL: this includes 15 items that measure perceived quality of life in 15 areas of life, namely: physical health, mood, memory, family, friends, intimate relationships, place of residence, ability to meet basic needs, ability to perform household tasks, ability to perform tasks outside the home, leisure and entertainment, money, occupation, personal satisfaction, and life in general. The questionnaire asked about how the participants valued their life in different areas (For example: How do you rate your mood?)

3. PRAMA-PRA: this assesses the frequency of performing empirical evidence-based practices that promote quality of life, including: physical exercise, mental activity, self-care activities, meetings and contact with relatives and friends, intimate relationships, training activities, leisure and social activities, tourist activities, and volunteering. For example, How often do you practice physical exercise?

4. PRAMA-LE: this includes two subscales that measure favorable (LE-FAV) and stressful (LE-STR) life experiences. The subscales share a number of items in common that assess the following aspects: area (Example: In relation to what area was that event), stage (Example: At what stage of your life did that event happen?), description of the most important life event from a small narrative, emotions (Example: What did you feel?), affect in the short and medium term (Example: What effects did this event have in the short and medium term?), and present influence (Example: Does that event influence your present life). In addition, LE-STR includes a question that evaluates the coping strategies employed, namely: acceptance, denial, active, planning, self-distraction, emotional support, instrumental support, emotional discharge, resignation, self-criticism, positive reformulation, humor, and religion.

5. PRAMA-psychological: this is based on the INMA-psychological scale (Díaz-Prieto and García-Sánchez, 2016), which has shown satisfactory validity and theoretical and construct reliability (Cronbach's α = 0.826). It assesses several psychosocial and emotional indicators, namely: emotional intelligence (Example: Indicate how often you recognize your feelings), achievement motivation (Example: Indicate how often you persist until you achieve your goals), social dimension (Example: Please, indicate how much you agree with the following statements- I have a broad social circle), and self-efficacy in active aging (Example: To what extent do you feel able to be autonomous and independent to manage your money?).

6. PRAMA-Internet: this is adapted from the INMA-Internet scale, and includes a series of items that assess the use of web 2.0 tools (Example: Indicate which one or which of the following computer or internet tools you use or have used: communication tools, social networks, email, image and sound tools, Apps, browsers and search engines, cloud tools, functional tools, educational tools, tools for selecting, classifying and sharing information, and office automation tools) and the perceived benefits (Example: Since I use…I carry out activities for myself that I did not do before).

Taken together, the instrument showed acceptable psychometric properties, with satisfactory content, theoretical and construct validity, as well as reliability, obtaining a total Cronbach's alpha of 0.720. By individual scale, a Cronbach's alpha was obtained of 0.748 for the PRAMA-PQL scale, 0.819 for PRAMA-PRA, 0.641 for PRAMA-LE, 0.769 for PRAMA-psychological, and 0.742 PRAMA-Internet. The average variance extracted (AVE) are above 0.50 in the different scales. The composite reliability (CR) goes from 0.80. Proven factor analysis models, the determinants are <0.001, as well as Barlett's sphericity tests. The KMO contrasts give above 0.85–0.99.

Design and Procedure

The six scales comprising the instrument were designed following a review of various national and international descriptive and intervention studies and the instruments used in relation to empirical evidence-based quality of life practices, perceived quality of life, life experiences, psychosocial and emotional variables and internet use. Having designed the instrument and selected the type of sample, a pilot study was conducted with two groups of participants in a university programme for older adults in León (Spain) and another group of people attending a training course on technology tools, in order to determine the time required to complete the questionnaire, detect problems related to item interpretation and identify other problems that might arise during questionnaire completion. Subsequently, potential participants were contacted in person, by telephone, fax and the internet, to inform them about the study objectives and request their participation. Prior to completing the questionnaire independently at the time and place of their choice, participants gave their informed consent in accordance with the ethical and professional conduct rules applicable to all scientific research. The maximum time required to complete the questionnaire was 30–35 min, although there were differences depending on the participants' level of digital competence. Once the questionnaires were completed, the results were extracted in Excel format and codified before conducting the pertinent statistical analyses.

Statistical Analysis

First, descriptive analyses were conducted (frequencies and percentages, means, and standard deviations). Normal distribution of the variables was confirmed by calculating skewness and kurtosis. Multivariate analyses were performed based on general linear models (GLM), using the IBM statistical software package SPSS Statistics 24.0. And for the calculation of Macdonald's omega/composite reliability, and average variance extracted indexes, Excel spreadsheet programs were carried out through from the pattern matrices of the factorial analysis of the instruments.

Results

Statistically significant results for both of the grouping variables considered, with large effect sizes were found: (i) favorable life experiences [λWilks = 0.061; F (1, 536, 6, 926) = 1,886; p ≤ 0.001; η2 = 0.295] and (ii) stressful life experiences [λWilks = 0.005; F (3, 281, 14, 119) = 1,576; p ≤ 0.001; η2 = 0.267]. Statistically significant differences according to age and gender were not found.

Similarly, the test for between-subject effects yielded statistically significant relationships, as will be described in four subsections that follow.

Differential Patterns of Perceived Quality of Life and Psychological Profiles According to Favorable Life Experiences

Subjects' favorable life experiences were associated with differential patterns in their perceived quality of life and psychological profiles (Table 2). For example, people who highlighted a workplace-related event scored higher on occupation-related perceived quality of life (e.g., perceived quality of life-occupation, Mjob = 3.05 vs. Mfinances = 2.57, p = 0.01). Similarly, favorable finance-related events seemed to be related to greater achievement motivation (e.g., total MOTIVATION, Mfinance = 11.71 vs. Meducation = 10.01, p = 0.02).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Differential patterns in perceived quality of life and psychological profiles according to favorable life experiences.

Differential Patterns of Practices That Promote Quality of Life and Use of Web 2.0 Tools According to Favorable Life Experiences

Although, variability was found according to the type of tool, generally speaking, life experiences related to physical health, important life changes (e.g., birth of a child), finances and education appeared to be associated with greater use of web 2.0 tools (Table 3). Thus, for example, frequency of browser use seemed to be higher among subjects whose most favorable life events were related to physical health, major life changes, education and social life (e.g., USE of browsers, Mlifechange = 4.89 vs. Mjob = 2.51, p = 0.01). With regard to the benefits of the use of web 2.0 tools, statistically significant results were obtained in relation to personal satisfaction. In particular, those who reported positive events related to finance, law, mental health, leisure and tourism, sports and spirituality, among others, seemed to experience higher levels of personal satisfaction derived from the use of these tools.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Differential patterns in quality of life practices, use of web 2.0 tools, and perceived benefits according to favorable life experiences.

Differential Patterns of Perceived Quality of Life and Psychological Profiles According to Stressful Life Experiences

As with favorable life experiences, stressful events were also associated with differential patterns in perceived quality of life and psychological profiles. Thus, for example, addiction was associated with lower levels of perceived quality of life, whereas the highest levels were associated with the category of other experiences, which included experiences of prison, war or armed conflict, physical and psychological abuse, life transitions (retirement, working life), and abortion (e.g., total perceived quality of life, Maddiction = 39.71 vs. Mother = 48.59, p = 0.01) (Table 4). Similarly, differential patterns depending on the type of stressful life experience in factors constituting the subjects' psychological profiles were found, including: emotional expression, size of social network, feelings of loneliness, and self-efficacy in relation to management of finances.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Differential patterns in perceived quality of life and psychological profiles according to stressful life experiences.

Differential Patterns of Practices That Promote Quality of Life and Use of Web 2.0 Tools According to Stressful Life Experiences

Differential patterns in practices that promote quality of life and the use of web 2.0 tools were also observed (Table 5). In general, the death of a loved one was associated with greater subsequent practice of activities that promote quality of life. However, there were exceptions. For example, the death of a loved one was also related to greater reliance on consumption of substances harmful to health (e.g., consumption of harmful substances, Mdeathofalovedone = 1.77 vs. Mmentalillness = 2.8, p = 0.01). Regarding the use of web 2.0 tools, stressful life experiences were again associated with differential patterns. For example, subjects who reported educational problems tended to make more use of social networks (e.g., USE of social networks, Meducation = 4.72 vs. Maddiction = 3.01, p = 0.01). Similarly, differences in relation to the perceived benefits arising from the use of web 2.0 tools were found. Thus, subjects who had suffered the death of a loved one perceived greater physical and mental health benefits (e.g., health BENEFITS, Mdeathofalovedone = 3.19 vs. Mother = 2.19, p = 0.01).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Differential patterns in quality of life practices, use of web 2.0 tools, and perceived benefits according to stressful life experiences.

Discussion

The study objective was achieved, namely to answer the initial research question. Both favorable and stressful life experiences of adults and older adults were associated differential patterns in intimate and personal aspects such as psychological profiles and perceived quality of life, and in daily activities and practices that influence quality of life, such as the use of web 2.0 tools, which yielded cognitive, affective, social, emotional, physical, and behavioral benefits, among others (Chen and Schulz, 2016; Díaz-Prieto and García-Sánchez, 2016; Khosravi and Ghapanchi, 2016). In spite of this, differences according to age and gender were not found.

One of the main contributions of this study is the analysis of the role of favorable life experiences. To date, most studies have focused on stressful life events and their consequences (Kendler and Gardner, 2016; Mayo et al., 2017; Pan et al., 2017). However, our study demonstrates that favorable life experiences also play an important role, mainly in the construction of an individual's psychological profile and use of web 2.0 tools, but also in perceived quality of life and engagement in practices that promote said quality. Favorable life experiences were associated with a higher number of differences in the subjects' psychological profiles and use of web 2.0 tools, whereas stressful life experiences were associated with greater differential patterns in perceived quality of life and quality of life practices. It is the combined experiences of an adult or older adult that determine his or her identity, personality and lifestyle and affect his or her physical and mental health status and well-being (Blonski et al., 2016).

Traditionally, past adverse or stressful life events have been associated with a life marked by negative events (Lim and DeSteno, 2016). In line with more recent models and theories such as adaptation to development and resilience (Cho et al., 2015), our study demonstrates that stressful life experiences sometimes act as powerful catalysts, spurring individuals to overcome these events and triggering a process of personal growth that exerts a positive effect psychologically and with regard to daily practices and activities.

In relation to perceived quality of life, life experiences were associated with differences in terms of interpersonal relationships, finances, occupation, satisfaction, assessment of life in general, and the global quality of life score, with differential patterns according to whether the experience was favorable or stressful and the specific type of experience, although no clear trend was observed. Several studies have previously reported the impact of life experiences on quality of life and well-being (Pocnet et al., 2017), but few have described the specific areas influenced.

As regards psychological profiles, favorable and stressful life experiences alike shaped the emotional, social, and self-efficacy dimensions, but only favorable experiences were associated with differences in motivation. Whereas, previous studies have reported the impact of mainly stressful life experiences on psychological profiles (Lasgaard et al., 2016), our study shows that favorable experiences also contribute to their construction.

In terms of the frequency of engaging in empirical evidence-based practices that promote quality of life, favorable, and stressful life experiences alike were associated with differential patterns in physical and mental health, self-determination, social, material, and functional domains and emotional well-being. All this contributes to enhancing the quality of life, as evidenced by various studies (Gómez et al., 2015).

Lastly, with regard to the use of web 2.0 tools and the perceived benefits of this, life experiences, in this case mainly favorable ones, were associated with differential patterns of use for almost all of the tools studied. Nevertheless, unfavorable life experiences were associated with greater differences in the perceived benefits in relation to physical and mental health and personal satisfaction. This may be because better standards of living probably favor the use of these media. In fact, several previous studies have confirmed the relationship between a higher socioeconomic level and greater use of the internet and web 2.0 tools in various age groups (Lai and Kwan, 2017). Our results in relation to the benefits indicate that these tools possess significant potential as a coping resource, in agreement with previous studies (Li et al., 2016).

Nonetheless, despite these differential patterns, the differences between them were very small and no clear trends were observed. This may be because emotional associations may exert a greater influence regardless of the type of experience. It might also be explained in light of theories such as resilience, according to which, people tend to focus on positive events and to deploy a series of strategies for coping with stressful events in order to promote positive adaptation (Randall et al., 2015).

This study presents a series of limitations that must be taken into consideration. First, use of the tool Google Forms entailed problems related to privacy, the impossibility of establishing a password, technical problems that made it necessary to eliminate responses and the impossibility of saving responses when answering the questionnaire, which could lead to loss of all information entered in the event of a connection or application failure. In general, the researchers were able to overcome these difficulties, making the necessary adjustments during the design process and questionnaire administration alike, and thus they did not affect the results to any great extent. However, many of these difficulties could be avoided by administering the questionnaire via another type of tool such as SurveyMonkey. It is also necessary to note the possible existence of sample bias. For example, since voluntary sampling was used, factors such as participant motivation to complete the questionnaire, the availability of technological resources or the need for basic digital competence may have influenced the final sample obtained. Different results to ours might be obtained in populations with other sociodemographic, economic, and educational characteristics, and it is therefore difficult to generalize our findings. In addition, people with problems of autonomy were not included, and this would be an interesting future avenue to explore using another type of instrument through individualized questionnaires. Furthermore, although this study included a representative sample of adults and older adults from various parts of Spain, it may nevertheless be necessary to conduct a comparative study of the different regions in Spain which would complement the results obtained here.

This study underscores the impact of favorable and stressful life experiences on perceived quality of life, psychological profiles, and practices that promote quality of life, including the use of web 2.0 tools. Differential patterns in relation to life experiences were found, indicating that besides considering sociodemographic factors, as has been the custom to date, interventions aimed at improving quality of life should also take into account life experiences and the other factors analyzed here. In this way, this study could have important implications at a theoretical, practical, political, psychological, educational, and social level. On the one hand, because the findings obtained contribute to enrich knowledge in relation to the study of life experiences in adulthood and in old age. On the other hand, because this could contribute to the implementation of more successful psychological and social interventions within the framework of Educational and Social Gerontology. Finally, because this study opens the doors to education in life experiences, education for coping with life problems, addressing its implications in the mentioned psychoeducational constructs, as well as for the promotion of development at different ages and throughout the life cycle. This could contribute to the implementation of more successful psychological and social interventions and attract the attention of social policies.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was reviewed, approved, and carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the scientific and research Ethical Committee of Universidad de León, with written informed consent from participants through the App used, before all subjects were enrolled in our study.

Author Contributions

CD-P and J-NG-S designed the study and wrote the article. CD-P reviewed the antecedents and implemented the study. J-NG-S performed the data analysis. AC-G reviewed and contributed instrumentally in the study as a whole and its implementation. All authors made relevant contributions in the final result.

Funding

During this study, we received from the company BIOGES Starters S.A., which is associated with the Universidad de León. CD-P received funds through a Ph.D. Fellowship (FPU-MECD REF. 12/04517). J-NG-S also received a sabbatical research stay funded by the Universidad de la Costa CUC (Barranquilla, Colombia). AC-G received a Ph.D. Fellowship by JCyL [EDU/1083/2013], and co-financed by the European Social Fund, within the ERDF 2014–2020.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank participants for completing the questionnaire.

References

Açikgül, E., and Serdar, M. (2019). Effects of instruction supported by web 2.0 tools on prospective teachers' biotechnology literacy. Comput. Educ. 135, 61–74. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, X., Ho, D. W. H., Fung, K., Tang, L., He, M., Young, K. W., et al. (2014). Effectiveness of a life story work program on older adults with intellectual disabilities. Clin. Interv. Aging 9, 1865–1872. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S56617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J., and Lucas, R. E. (2018). Life events and personality trait change. J. Pers. 86, 83–96. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blonski, S. C., Conradi, H. J., Oldehinkel, A. J., Bos, E. H., and de Jonge, P. (2016). Associations between negative and positive life events and the course of depression: a detailed repeated-assessments study. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 204, 175–180. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M. (2015). Multimodal connectedness and quality of life: examining the influences of technology adoption and interpersonal communication on well-being across the life span. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 20, 3–18. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. R., and Schulz, P. J. (2016). The effect of information communication technology interventions on reducing social isolation in the elderly: a systematic review. J. Med. Internet Res. 8:e18. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, J., Martin, P., and Poon, L. W. (2015). Successful aging and subjective well-being among oldest-old adults. Gerontologist 55, 132–143. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chukwuorji, J. C., Nwoke, M. B., and Ebere, M. O. (2017). Stressful life events, family support and successful ageing in the Biafran War generation. Aging Ment. Health 21, 95–103. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1083946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Prieto, C., and García-Sánchez, J. N. (2016). Psychological profiles of older adult Web 2.0 tool users. Comput. Human Behav. 64, 673–681. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donoghue, H. M., Traviss-Turner, G. D., House, A. O., Lewis, H., and Gilbody, S. (2016). Life adversity in depressed and non-depressed older adults: a cross-sectional comparison of the brief LTE-Q questionnaire and life events and difficulties interview as part of the CASPER study. J. Affect. Disord. 193, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenbarth, H., Godinez, D., du Pont, A., Corley, R. P., Stallings, M. C., and Rhee, S. H. (2019). The influence of stressful life events, psychopathy, and their interaction on internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 272, 438–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.145

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

El Haj, M., and Antoine, P. (2017). Describe yourself to improve your autobiographical memory: a study in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 88, 165–162. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, L., and Walker, A. (2015). Active and successful aging: a European policy perspective. Gerontologist 55, 83–90. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, Z., and Bowling, A. (2004). Quality of life from the perspectives of older people. Ageing Soc. 24, 675–691. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X03001582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez, L. E., Verdugo, M. A., and Arias, B. (2015). Validity and reliability of the INICO-FEAPS scale: an assessment of quality of life for people with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 36, 600–610. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henkel, L. A., Kris, A., Birney, S., and Krauss, K. (2016). The functions and value of reminiscence for older adults in long-term residential care facilities. Memory 25, 425–435. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1182554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hongthong, D., Somrongthong, R., and Ward, P. (2015). Factors influencing the quality of life (Qol) among Thai older people in a rural area of Thailand. Iran. J. Public Health. 44, 479–485.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hyams, A., and Scogin, F. (2015). “Reminiscence/life review therapy,” in The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology, eds R. L. Cautin and S. O. Lilienfeld (New York, NY: Wiley & Sons), 1–3. doi: 10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., and Gardner, C. O. (2016). Depressive vulnerability, stressful life events and episode onset of major depression: a longitudinal model. Psychol. Med. 46, 1865–1874. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khosravi, P., and Ghapanchi, A. H. (2016). Investigating the effectiveness of technologies applied to assist seniors: a systematic literature review. Int. J. Med. Inform. 85, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2015.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Woo, E., and Uysal, M. (2015). Tourism experience and quality of life among elderly tourists. Tour. Manag. 46, 465–476. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2014.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraaij, V., and Wilde, E. J. (2001). Negative life events and depressive symptoms in the elderly: a life span perspective. Aging Mental Health 5, 84–91. doi: 10.1080/13607860020020681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, F. T. T., and Kwan, J. L. Y. (2017). Socioeconomic influence on adolescent problematic Internet use through school-related psychosocial factors and pattern of Internet use. Comput. Human Behav. 68, 121–136. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasgaard, M., Armour, C., Holm, R., and Goossens, L. (2016). Major life events as predictors of loneliness in adolescence. J. Child Fam. Stud. 25, 631–637. doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0243-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latorre, J. M., Serrano, J. P., Ricarte, J., Bonete, B., Ros, L., and Sitges, E. (2015). Life review based on remembering specific positive events in active aging. J. Aging Health 27, 140–157. doi: 10.1177/0898264314541699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Zhang, W., Li, X., Zhen, S., and Wanga, Y. (2010). Stressful life events and problematic Internet use by adolescent females and males: a mediated moderation model. Comput. Human Behav. 26, 1199–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Zou, Y., Wang, J., and Yang, X. (2016). Role of stressful life events, avoidant coping styles, and neuroticism in online game addiction among college students: a moderated mediation model. Front. Psychol. 7:1794. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, D., and DeSteno, D. (2016). Suffering and compassion: the links among adverse life experiences, empathy, compassion, and prosocial behavior. Emotion 16, 175–182. doi: 10.1037/emo0000144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, T. S., Lopes, R. M., and Ribeiro, O. M. (2016). A quasi-experimental study of a reminiscence program focused on autobiographical memory in institutionalized older adults with cognitive impairment. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 66, 183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2016.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus-Varwijk, A. E., Koopmans, M., Visscher, T. L. S., Seidell, J. C., Slaets, J. P. J., and Smits, C. H. M. (2016). Optimizing tailored health promotion for older adults: understanding their perspectives on healthy living. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2:2333721415625293. doi: 10.1177/2333721415625293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marston, H. R., Kroll, M., Fink, D., de Rosario, H., and Gschwind, Y. J. (2016). Technology use, adoption and behavior in older adults: results from the iStoppFalls Project. Educ. Gerontol. 42, 371–387. doi: 10.80/03601277.2015.1125178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayo, D., Corey, S., Kelly, L. H., Yohannes, S., Youngquist, A. L., Stuart, B. K., et al. (2017). The role of trauma and stressful life events among individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: a review. Front. Psychiatry 8:55. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, L. A., Goldstein, T. R., Rooks, B. T., Hickey, M., Fan, J. Y., Merranko, J., et al. (2017). The relationship between stressful life events and Axis I diagnoses among adolescent offspring of probands with bipolar and non-bipolar psychiatric disorders and healthy controls: the Pittsburgh Bipolar Offspring Study (BIOS). J. Clin. Psychiatry 78, 234–243. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m09815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pocnet, C., Dupuis, M., Congard, A., and Jopp, D. (2017). Personality and its links to quality of life: mediating effects of emotion regulation and self-efficacy beliefs. Motiv. Emot. 41, 196–208. doi: 10.1007/s11031-017-9603-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preschl, B., Maercker, A., Wagner, B., Forstmeier, S., Baños, R. M., Alcañiz, M., et al. (2012). Life-review therapy with computer supplements for depression in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. Aging Ment. Health 16, 964–974. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.702726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, W., Baldwin, C., McKenzie-Mohr, S., McKimd, E., and Furlong, D. (2015). Narrative and resilience: a comparative analysis of how older adults story their lives. J. Aging Stud. 34, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2015.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovaara, A., Lehmuskallio, A., Hedman, L., Valkonen, P., and Näsänen, J. (2010). Information technologies and transitions in the lives of 55-65-year-olds: the case of colliding life interest. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 68, 803–821. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2010.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stikkelbroek, Y., Bodden, D. H. M., Kleinjan, M., Reijnders, M., and van Baar, A. L. (2016). Adolescent depression and negative life events, the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation. PLoS ONE 11:e0161062. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomsen, D. K., Steiner, K. L, and Pillemer, D.B. (2016). Life story chapters: past and future, you and me. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 5, 143–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tveit-Sekse, R. J., Dunberger, G., Olesen, M. L., Østerbye, M., and Seibæk, L. (2019). Lived experiences and quality of life after gynaecological cancer—An integrative review. J. Clin. Nurs. 28, 1393–1421. doi: 10.1111/jocn.14721.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ingen, E., and Matzat, U. (2018). Inequality in mobilizing online help after a negative life event: the role of education, digital skills, and capital-enhancing Internet use. Inform. Commun. Soc. 21, 481–498. doi: 10.1080/1369118X.2017.1293708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ingen, E., Utz, S., and Toepoel, V. (2016). Online coping after negative life events: measurement, prevalence, and relation with Internet activities and well-being. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 34, 511–529. doi: 10.1177/0894439315600322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wren, R. (2017). Effect of life review on quality of life for older adults living in nursing homes. Phys. Occup. Ther. Geriatr. 34, 186–204. doi: 10.1080/02703181.2016.1268236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, J., Li, D., Jia, J., Wang, Y., Sun, W., and Li, D. (2019). The role of stressful life events and the Big Five personality traits in adolescent trajectories of problematic Internet use. Psychol. Addict. Behav. doi: 10.1037/adb0000466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, R., Spears, J., Luptak, M., and Wilby, F. (2015). Understanding older adults' perceptions of Internet use: an exploratory factor analysis. Educ. Gerontol. 41, 504–518. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2014.1003495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: differential patterns, life experiences, quality of life, practices, psychological profiles, web 2.0 tools

Citation: Díaz-Prieto C, García-Sánchez J-N and Canedo-García A (2019) Impact of Life Experiences and Use of Web 2.0 Tools in Adults and Older Adults. Front. Psychol. 10:2158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02158

Received: 30 April 2019; Accepted: 06 September 2019;
Published: 27 September 2019.

Edited by:

Claudio Longobardi, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Andrzej Klimczuk, Independent Researcher, Białystok, Poland
Juan Francisco Romero Pérez, University of Málaga, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Díaz-Prieto, García-Sánchez and Canedo-García. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús-Nicasio García-Sánchez, jn.garcia@unileon.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.