Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 21 January 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology

Effects of Two Teaching Strategies on Preschoolers’ Oral Language Skills: Repeated Read-Aloud With Question and Answer Teaching Embedded and Repeated Read-Aloud With Executive Function Activities Embedded

Updated
  • Department of Early Childhood Education, National Taitung University, Taitung, Taiwan

The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of two teaching strategies on preschoolers’ oral language skills: repeated read-aloud with question and answer teaching embedded, and repeated read-aloud with executive function (EF) activities embedded. A quasi-experimental pretest–posttest design was employed. Children ranging in age from 4 years 6 months to 6 years and 4 months participated in the study (n = 53). They were recruited from preschools in Taitung, Taiwan, randomly assigned to the three study groups. 36 children were divided and assigned to the two experimental groups (question and answer teaching group and EF group), while the remaining 17, to the control group. The participating preservice teachers attended 32 h of training which included the theory, practice, and outcome evaluation measure for the teaching strategies implemented. The intervention spanned 2 months. Oral language tests (curriculum-based receptive vocabulary, inferential comprehension, and oral comprehension) were administered before and after the intervention. The findings revealed that both experimental groups positively impacted participants’ receptive vocabulary and oral comprehension when compared with the control group, although the performances between the two experimental groups did not differ significantly. For inferential comprehension, there was no statistically significant difference across the three groups. Implications of the study findings are discussed and potential topics for future research proposed.

Introduction

Reading books aloud to children by parents or teachers is one of the most recommended practices to help children learn languages (Snow et al., 1998; Whitehurst et al., 1999; Neuman et al., 2000). Research has shown that read-aloud can help increase children’s vocabulary (Beck et al., 2002; Biemiller and Boote, 2006; Pollard-Durodola et al., 2011) and oral comprehension skills (Cunningham and Stanovich, 1997; Morrow and Gambrell, 2002). A meta-analysis by the National Early Literacy Panel (2009) reported that reading aloud to children by teachers or parents can raise children’s verbal language abilities and knowledge acquired from print materials.

Reading picture books aloud provides children with a richer context to gain understanding of a word or text. However, during the initial round of read-aloud, children’s “encounter” with the word and text remains largely incidental, characterized by a partial and limited impression formed in children’s cognition of the pronunciation, semantics, and use of the new word and sentences strung together by these words. Only through repeated read-aloud can the process of encoding be reinforced and the process of acquiring new vocabulary and textual information become complete. These new words and textual information will be stored in children’s memory, and can be retrieved when children come across the same or similar contexts in the future (Sénéchal, 1997). Some scholars believe that a minimum of three rounds of read-aloud will allow children to internalize the meanings of the text or vocabulary (Hoffman, 1976).

Biemiller and Boote (2006) reviewed and analyzed 13 studies from 1989 to 2002, and found that repeated read-aloud combined with explaining the vocabulary explicitly resulted in an average increase of 26% in the learning of word meanings among primary graders. Similar observations were also made among students who listened to a text, received explanations of the target vocabulary in the context of the text, and made greater strides in learning the vocabulary (Nagy et al., 1987).

Beyond the learning of vocabulary, question and answer (Q&A) teaching where teachers ask questions and students answer is often implemented to deepen students’ text comprehension (Hansen and Pearson, 1983; van den Broek et al., 2001; McGee and Johnson, 2003; Richards and Anderson, 2003). Asking questions has long been used as a tool to not only promote comprehension but also assess students’ knowledge and stimulate critical thinking (Raphael and McKinney, 1983). Q&A both facilitates students’ comprehension of the content pertaining to the questions directly and enhances students’ comprehension and memory of the text as a whole (van den Broek et al., 2001). Teachers often engage listeners with different kinds of questioning, by targeting different levels of comprehension as espoused by Kintsch (1998) and formulating the questions accordingly.

Meanwhile, executive functions (EFs) have been shown to play an essential role in effective early academic instruction. Oral comprehension is a complex process which activates such cognitive functions as encoding, discriminating, flexibility (in switching between verbal, meaningful codes), and inference (Cain et al., 2004). EFs also help an individual to stay on task. EFs consist of a multitude of components, such as working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility (Miyake et al., 2000). Some scholars believe that EFs are closely related to preschoolers’ vocabulary acquisition (Wolfe and Bell, 2004; Moriguchi et al., 2008) and can serve as a predictor of school children’s oral comprehension ability in the classroom (Hungerford et al., 2012). The dramatic growth in EFs between the ages of three and 5 years (Center on the Developing Child, 2012) has now been widely recognized. Scholars have also found that EF activities facilitated the early development of literacy among kindergarteners (Foy and Mann, 2013).

Executive functions can be enhanced through cognitive curricula (Barnett et al., 2008; Goldin et al., 2014). Research has also shown that domain-specific EF training targeting a particular EF component can yield the most change in learning behaviors (Wass, 2015). Blair and Razza (2007) studied a group of 3- to 5-year old children from low-income households to examine the role of self-regulation in their emerging academic abilities. They found that inhibitory control training benefited children’s learning of vocabulary. They went on to suggest that curricula designed to improve self-regulation as well as early literacy abilities might be more effective in enhancing children’s learning. This finding has been echoed in other studies (Segers et al., 2016). Empirical evidence has also been found that behavioral control training can significantly improve children’s performance in reasoning-related tests (Liu et al., 2015). Hence, one of the goals of the current study was to examine whether EF training could lead to improvement in more advanced language skills, such as oral comprehension and inferential comprehension, in addition to youngsters’ vocabulary acquisition.

There has been some empirical evidence with regard to the effects of Q&A teaching for language learning among school age children and preschoolers (Wang, 2012; Lee and Chien, 2019). On the other hand, despite the sound reasoning presented in the above literature review of the potential correlation between enhanced EFs and language learning, there remains little empirical evidence to confirm this association. Furthermore, any positive effect of robust EFs on language learning will be indirect, that is, through the mediation of enhanced cognitive functions. Hence, such benefits may take longer to manifest. Meanwhile, embedded instruction has been shown to benefit children’s learning and lead to greater academic achievement (McClelland et al., 2007; Raver et al., 2011). Therefore, our research hypotheses were formulated as follows:

(a) Within the study period, both the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded and repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded more positively impact the curriculum-based vocabulary measure among children than repeated read-aloud alone; the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded also more positively impacts the vocabulary measure than the repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded;

(b) Within the study period, both the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded and repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded more positively impact the inferential comprehension measure among children than repeated read-aloud alone; the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded also more positively impacts the inferential comprehension measure than the repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded; and

(c) Within the study period, both the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded and repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded more positively impact the oral comprehension measure among children than repeated read-aloud alone; the repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded also more positively impacts the oral comprehension measure than the repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded.

Methods

Research Participants

Student Participants

Fifty-three participants were recruited from two preschools in Taitung County (Taiwan) for this study, aged from 4 years 6 months to 6 years and 4 months. Using a random number generator, participants were assigned to the Q&A group (Experimental Group I) (n = 18), the EF group (Experimental Group II) (n = 18), or the control group (n = 17). The average age was 65.58 months (SD = 8.06). Informed consent was obtained from all parents. Exclusion criteria included hearing impairment, other types of special needs, or serious behavioral problems. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) (M = 110.51, SD = 13.52) scores of student participants were within the normal range. As EFs and language comprehension could vary with age (Fuster, 2002; Berk, 2006), pre-intervention analysis was conducted and no statistically significant age differences were found across the study groups [F(2,52) = 0.12, p = 0.89]. Participants’ demographic information is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Background information to group participants.

Instructors

A total of 10 instructors participated who were all senior-level preservice teachers at the university with which the author of this manuscript is affiliated. Instructors completed 32 h of training which covered three aspects—basics for pedagogical knowledge and pedagogical content knowledge, practice teaching and outcome measure—of the teaching strategies to be implemented: repeated read-aloud, repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded, and repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded (Table 2). All instructors attended the basics and outcome measure sessions, because they would be the test administrators of the study groups to which they were not assigned. Hence, they needed a general understanding of the different teaching strategies as well as the testing procedure and its relationship to the curriculum goal.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Instructor training program.

Instructors stayed with their respective groups through the completion of the study. Implementation fidelity was ensured prior to and during the interventions. Instructors were supervised in a group setting every 2 weeks. Fidelity monitoring was also carried out through sporadic in-class observations and one-on-one supervision. The researcher provided additional intensive one-on-one guidance to instructors who did not have prior experience in administering related curricula. All teaching sessions were videotaped. The treatment fidelity among instructors was as high as 84%, which met the requirement for the program.

Research Design

This quasi-experiment compared preschoolers’ performance in oral language and comprised three program groups: the Q&A group (Experimental Group I), the EF group (Experimental Group II), and control group.

Experimental group I: “repeated read-aloud,” “teaching vocabulary,” and “Q&A teaching”;

Experimental Group II: “repeated read-aloud,” “teaching vocabulary,” and “EF activities”;

Control Group: “repeated read-aloud” only.

Interventions

The interventions spanned eight weeks, with four sessions per week. A total of 32 sessions were completed and each session lasted approximately 30 min. Each picture book was read to participants aloud in three rounds during the study. Respective interventions were carried out in the three study groups as follows.

First Round

Experimental groups I and II

The instructor explained and discussed new vocabulary by using picture cards as well as synonyms and antonyms to help students recognize the vocabulary. The picture book was then read aloud to students, followed by students playing the morpheme game.

Control group

The instructor read the picture book aloud, without additional interventions.

Second Round

Experimental group I

The instructor read the picture book aloud to participants, and then asked participants to answer a number of questions (for examples: what, who, what, etc.) which were formulated based on the text.

Experimental group II

The instructor read the picture book aloud. The instructor then asked participants to pay attention and memorize the meanings of the vocabulary discussed during the first stage, before engaging participants in a word game where participants were instructed to act out the correct response. For example, participants would clap hands when the instructor said the word “white” and stomp their feet when the instructor said the word “black.” This procedure was repeated three to five times until students mastered the one-to-one relationship between the body movement and the corresponding vocabulary. Afterward, participants responded with the body movement that was opposite what they learned earlier in this stage. For example, when the instructor said the word “black,” students would clap hands; when the instructor said the word “white,” participants would stomp their feet. Once participants mastered the rules, they played the game for six times using the synonyms and antonyms derived from the vocabulary in the picture book.

Control group

The instructor read the picture book aloud, without additional interventions.

Third Round

Experimental group I

The instructor read the picture book aloud to student participants, followed by inferential inquiries. The instructor guided the participants to distil hidden meanings in the text by asking questions like “why” and “how.” The instructor also encouraged students to identify causal relationships in the text by utilizing clues provided in the text as well as students’ existing background knowledge.

Experimental group II

The instructor read the picture book aloud to the participants. The instructor then mixed types of statements—active/passive and simple/complex—and presented children with four kinds of sentences: active plausible, active implausible, passive plausible, and passive implausible (Ye and Zhou, 2008). Children were asked to present their judgment of the veracity of the sentences by raising the signs of “correct” or “false.” This exercise also allowed the instructor to gauge participants’ grasp of the storyline in the picture book as a whole. There was a total of five such exercises.

Control group

The instructor read the picture book aloud, without additional interventions.

Instruments

A battery of tests were used to examine student participants’ oral language abilities and EF abilities. All tests have been standardized and validated. PPVT-R and The Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders test were administered during the two weeks prior to the start of interventions (“pretest”), due to the scheduling. The remaining tests were administered during both the pretest and post-test which also lasted two weeks immediately after the completion of interventions, due to the scheduling.

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R)

The screening instrument used to recruit participants was a Mandarin Chinese version of the PPVT (Dunn and Dunn, 1981), which was standardized and revised by Lu and Liu (1994). The test is suitable for measuring the receptive vocabulary comprehension of children aged 3 to 12, by having them point to one of the four picture cards which best represents the word spoken to them. The reliability of the PPVT-R as reported in the manual is 0.88. The test took children in this study 15 to 22 min to complete.

Executive Function Measure

The Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders (HTKS) task was conceptualized by Ponitz et al. (2008) as a measure of EFs, including inhibitory control (a child must inhibit the dominant response of imitating the examiner), working memory (a child must remember the rules of the task), and attention focusing (a child must pay attention to the directions being presented by the examiner). First, one of the participating instructors (a senior in college who was knowledgeable about the HTKS task) engaged with children in groups (of 17–18 children each) to practice the HTKS:

Practice 1: The instructor directed children to touch their head (or their toes), but instead of following the command, the children were supposed to do the opposite and touch their toes. This round was repeated three times.

Practice 2: Once participants mastered the head/toes part of the task, the knees and shoulders commands were presented to them. This round was repeated three times.

Practice 3: Once children mastered the knee/shoulder part of the task, all commands were combined. Once again, children were supposed to do the opposite of what the instructor said. This round was repeated four times.

After the practice sessions, 3–5 participating instructors (different from the instructor who led the practice sessions) administered the test by conducting it with each child one on one at a quiet corner in the classroom. The HTKS task was carried out 10 items during the test session and scored as follows: two points for responding correctly the first time; one point for responding incorrectly the first time, but enacting rectification right away; and zero point for responding incorrectly, without rectification. The internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α) was 0.95.

Oral Language Measures

Curriculum-based picture vocabulary test

This instrument was created by the research team to assess participants’ vocabulary comprehension. This computer-assisted test was designed using the Java programing language and administered individually. The questions (50) were modeled after those in the PPVT, and participants score by clicking on the correct picture after listening to the test questions. Five questions were created for each picture book. Each question contains three choices and counts for one point. The total score is indicative of participants’ performance in vocabulary comprehension. This test took participating children 6.5 to 12 min to complete in this study. A Shapiro–Wilk test showed W(53) = 0.98, p = 0.44 (greater than 0.05), indicating the normality of data. The points on the Q–Q plot graph closely follow the normal distribution line (Gravetter and Wallnau, 2014).

Oral comprehension test for young children

This instrument (Chien et al., 2017) was designed to test oral comprehension among children aged 4–6 years. It has been standardized for the Mandarin Chinese-speaking children in Taiwan (n = 352). Children first listened to the instructions and answered practice questions. Once they became familiar with the process and made no errors, the official test began.

The test includes three short texts and five dialogic texts with equal difficulty. The texts feature three themes: food, kindergarten activity, and holiday. The short text and dialogic text each contain 144–170 words, and were read to participants using an audio recorder. For each text read, two to four questions were posed to participants who then located answers in the text (by either identifying a specific word/phrase or stringing together clues scattered in the text). For each question, there are three answers for children to choose from. Children pointed to the answer of their choice on the answer sheet and the test administrator recorded the answer. The test took participants 20 to 30 min to complete in this study.

The discrimination indices range from 0.27 to 0.51, and the difficulty indices, from 0.29 to 0.82. The internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α) is 0.81 and the test–retest reliability coefficient is 0.68. Both are acceptable reliability measurements. The test items also fit the Rasch model. A Shapiro–Wilk test showed W(53) = 0.97, p = 0.23 (greater than 0.05), indicating the normality of data. The points on the Q–Q plot graph also closely follow the normal distribution line.

Inferential comprehension test for young children

This instrument was developed based on the inferential comprehension test for young children created by Chien et al. (2017) for kindergarteners aged 4 to 6, by also taking into consideration the recommendations of Florit et al. (2011) as well as Silva and Cain (2015). It has been standardized for the Mandarin Chinese-speaking children in Taiwan (n = 234). Participants first listened to directions and went through a practice session. Once no more errors were made, the formal test began.

The test comprises short sentences which are read to participants using an audio-recorder. For example, “Xiaoming took a glass of milk and accidentally kicked the chair,” “What happened, please?,” “Mom brought a rag,” and “What does Mom do with a rag?” Test questions were posed to participants after each sentence. There are 20 questions for the pretest, and 22 questions for the post-test. To answer the questions, participants needed to identify explicit clues in the sentences (text-based comprehension) and/or retrieve relevant information from the existing pool of their background knowledge (knowledge-based comprehension). The test administrator then recorded participants’ answers. After the test, the administrator scored the answer sheet using a two-point scale (0 or 1). The sum of the scores indicates participants’ comprehension performance. The higher the sum, the stronger the inferential comprehension. The test took participants 25 to 33 min to complete in this study.

The discrimination indices range from 0.30 to 0.56 and the internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α) is 0.82. Pearson’s coefficient of skewness of 0.18 and the coefficient of kurtosis of 0.05 indicate a normal univariate distribution. A Shapiro–Wilk test showed W(53) = 0.97, p = 0.15 (greater than 0.05), indicating the normality of data. The points on the Q–Q plot graph also closely follow the normal distribution line. There are two equivalent sets of test form which could be used for testing before and after the intervention.

Materials

The materials used for read-aloud were mostly picture books containing scientific information or content close to children’s daily life. Initially, 28 titles were recommended by three senior kindergarten teachers. Ten of them were later selected by two experts in the fields of early childhood language intervention and natural science, respectively (Lee and Chien, 2019). Seven of the 10 picture books were read aloud in the following order: (1) “Rosie’s Walk” (Hutchins, 2009), (2) “The Soles of Your Feet” (Yagyu, 1987a), (3) “Your Hand and Fingers” (Yagyu, 1987a, b), (4) “The Story of Bones” (Horiuchi, 2013), (5) “Miki’s First Errand” (Hayashi, 2010), (6) “Where is the Rhinoceros Beetle?” (Matsuoka, 2011), (7) “The Story of Fart”” (Cho, 1984) (Appendix). The remaining three picture books used were compiled by researchers of this study based on the information extracted from encyclopedias or professional natural science websites. The titles of these three books were “Burp,” “Grow Hair,” and “Microwave,” and read aloud to children in this order. The content was verified by two experts in applied science and healthcare, respectively, to ensure the accuracy of texts and illustrations.

The text length ranges from 383 to 418 words, and the sentence count, from 31 to 37. There are on average 18.9 difficult words in the text, equivalent to the reading level of second- to third-graders (in general, children’s listening comprehension level is higher than their level of reading comprehension). The vocabulary chosen from the books for further discussion were deemed important to understanding the story in the book (Kuhn and Stahl, 1998).

Data Analysis

Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted using the experimental groups (2) and control group as independent variables, pretest scores as covariates, and scores of the outcome measures as dependent variables.

Results

The descriptive statistics for all measures and study groups before and after the interventions are tabulated in Table 3. The pretest scores were used as covariates in the data analysis.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean scores (SDs) at pre-test, post-test, and adjusted means at post-tests for orallanguage skills.

There were no significant differences in all pretest measures across the study groups: curriculum-based picture vocabulary [F(2,47) = 3.20, p > 0.05], oral comprehension [F(2,47) = 1.06, p > 0.05], and inferential comprehension [F(2,47) = 0.233, p > 0.05]. The homogeneity of regression slopes assumption was also tested and met. The interactions between the covariate and the groups for each measure (dependent variable) were as follows: F(2,47) = 0.76, p = 0.47, η2 = 0.26 (curriculum-based picture vocabulary), F(2,47) = 1.08, p = 0.20, η2 = 0.08 (oral comprehension), and F(2,47) = 0.62, p = 0.54, η2 = 0.03 (inferential comprehension). Where pre- and post-test scores differed significantly, the effect sizes fell between small and medium ranging from 0.128 to 0.212.

Impact of the Interventions on Receptive Vocabulary

The results of ANCOVA revealed a significant main effect [F(2,46) = 3.41, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.129, observed power = 0.72]. Post hoc analysis further revealed that post-test scores of both the Q&A group (34.83) and the EF group (30.47) were significantly higher than the score of the control group (28.94). However, there was no significant difference between the two experimental groups.

Impact of the Interventions on Inferential Comprehension

The difference across the three study groups did not reach statistical significance [F(2,46) = 0.33, p = 0.72], indicating no significant contributory effect of either intervention. Hence, no further analysis was conducted.

Impact of the Interventions on Oral Comprehension

Analysis results showed a main effect [F(2,46) = 6.17, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.212, observed power = 0.89], indicating significant contributory effects of different teaching strategies on children’s oral comprehension. Based on post hoc analysis, the post-test scores in the Q&A group (21.83) and the EF group (22.17) were significantly higher than the score in the control group (15.06). However, there was no significant difference between the two experimental groups.

Discussion and Implications

The purpose of this study was to examine the effects on various oral language abilities of two interventions: repeated read-aloud with Q&A embedded, and repeated read-aloud with EF activities embedded. The study found that the experimental interventions improved participants’ curriculum-based receptive vocabulary and oral comprehension.

The higher post-test scores in receptive vocabulary in both the Q&A and EF groups (employing both read-aloud and vocabulary teaching) than the score in the control group (employing only read-aloud) confirmed the findings from earlier studies that instructors need to, in addition to reading aloud, explain the vocabulary and its contextual meaning in the text in order to effectively promote children’s vocabulary acquisition (Hargrave and Sénéchal, 2000; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001; Penno et al., 2002; Biemiller and Boote, 2006; Lee and Chien, 2019). This study is evidence that picture cards, morpheme games can be an effective strategy to explain vocabulary in the preschool age group. Instructors in both experimental groups in this study used picture cards, synonyms, antonyms, and morpheme games during the first stage of intervention to illustrate in greater depth the meanings of the vocabulary. These meanings were then reinforced through the WH questions in the Experimental Group I (Q&A) and EF play in the Experimental Group II during the second stage of the intervention, respectively.

The oral comprehension scores among participants in the two experimental groups improved significantly after the interventions, which also echoes the findings from earlier research (Morrow and Gambrell, 2002).

However, the scores in inferential comprehension in the three study groups did not differ significantly. Inferential comprehension requires readers or listeners to draw from their existing background knowledge which pertains to the topics in the text (Carr and Thompson, 1996). Comprehending science-related texts (as in some of the materials used in this study) relies even more on making inferences from existing background knowledge. Instructors shall also be instrumental in guiding children to generate knowledge-based or text-based inferences by encouraging children to connect information presented in non-adjacent parts in the text (McNamara and Magliano, 2009; Bos et al., 2016). In the current study, the requirement to adhere to the teaching protocol might have hindered instructors from engaging in more robust exchange with children to assist children in tapping into their existing knowledge pool and making inferences. The lack of statistically significant difference across the study groups in children’s inferential comprehension performance could also be linked to the absence of predictive discussion prior to the read-aloud (Hansen and Pearson, 1983). When combined with Q&A after the read-aloud, predictive discussion has been shown to help participants more effectively connect their background knowledge with the text read to them, so a cohesive and meaningful mental representation of the text can be created to deepen the comprehension.

Interestingly, no significant difference in the vocabulary learning and oral comprehension was found between the two experimental groups. As any potential benefit to language learning from EF activities will be indirect through the mediation of important cognitive functions (as presented in the introduction section of this report), such benefit may take longer to manifest than the time frame selected for the current study. Another plausible contributor to this lack of difference might lie in the small sample size used for this study. Hence, follow-on research can track a larger group of participants over a longer time horizon to further confirm or challenge our research hypotheses.

We believe that the present study contributes to the existing body of literature by providing empirical evidence that teaching strategies of repeated read-aloud with Q&A teaching or EF activities embedded can improve preschoolers’ oral language abilities (in particular, vocabulary and oral comprehension). This finding once again highlights the complexity in instructor-student interactions encompassing linguistic and cognitive processes (non-linguistic), and both are required for children to successfully acquire oral language skills. The teaching strategies implemented in the Q&A group embody the linguistic component—that is, utilizing linguistic resources such as pictures, synonyms, and antonyms to facilitate and deepen children’s learning during the early literacy phase. On the other hand, the evidence emerging from the current study also supports the benefits delivered by enhanced EF faculties in acquiring oral language skills. This mirrors findings from earlier research that EF activities focusing on a particular component of EF (for example, inhibitory control, as in this study) can be incorporated into teaching to enhance children’s learning of vocabulary. Children’s inhibitory control can contribute positively to their mastery of the more complicated learning, for example, a curriculum that features the learning of combinations of simple/complicated and active/passive sentences (as seen in the Experimental Group II in this study). Therefore, the preliminary findings of the current study could potentially further expand teachers’ toolbox by arming them with more teaching ammunitions: the strategies of reading-aloud and Q&A which are more commonly utilized in today’s classrooms (albeit not in combination), and the more innovative approach by incorporating EF games into the teaching of oral language skills.

Meanwhile, there are a number of limitations in the present study. First, the effect sizes ranged from small to medium where significant differences in pre- and post-test scores were found, likely due to the small sample size. Thus, the preliminary findings in the current research will need to be confirmed in a larger study. Second, for the measure of oral language comprehension, a more open-ended approach based on free recall can be considered in follow-on studies, in addition to or instead of using pre-formulated tests as in this study, to gain a fuller picture of children’s curriculum-based text comprehension and oral comprehension. Third, future studies could recruit in-service teachers who would likely be better prepared to manage young children’s responses and understand their intent so as to enhance the classroom interactions and deepen the teaching effects. Fourth, to confirm the lasting effects of interventions, a follow-up after a longer interval could be initiated with study participants. In the current study, the post-test measures were conducted immediately after the completion of the interventions.

Conclusion

Teaching strategies of repeated read-aloud embedded with Q&A or EF activities can benefit young children’s receptive vocabulary learning and oral comprehension.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the National Chen Kung University Human Research Ethics Committee, by project title “The effects of repetition, read-aloud, and executive function exercises in read-aloud on receptive vocabularies and listening comprehension for young children” (Approval No: NCKU HREC-F-105-032-2). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

HC conceived and designed the study, organized the data, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST-105-2410-H-143-009).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author is grateful to the project staff and children from the participating preschools for their help and cooperation.

References

Barnett, W. S., Jung, K., Yarosz, D. J., Thomas, J., Hornbeck, A., Stechuk, R., et al. (2008). Educational effects of the tools of the mind curriculum: a randomized trial. Early Child. Res. Q. 23, 299–313. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-10-61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., and Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction. New York: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Berk, L. E. (2006). Child Development. Boston: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

Biemiller, A., and Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 44–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.44

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bos, L. T., De Koning, B. B., Wassenburg, S. I., and van der Schoot, M. (2016). Training inference making skills using a situation model approach improves reading comprehension. Front. Psychol. 7:116 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain, K. E., Bryant, P. E., and Oakhill, J. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 31–42. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, S. C., and Thompson, B. (1996). The effects of prior knowledge and schema activation strategies on the inferential reading comprehension of children with and without learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Q. 19, 48–61. doi: 10.2307/1511053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Center on the Developing Child, (2012). Executive Function (Inbrief). Available at: www.developingchild.harvard.edu (accessed March 28, 2016).

Google Scholar

Chien, H. Y., Lien, C. S., and Chang, S. Y. (2017). Effects of story prompting and working memory ability on young children’s story comprehension. J. Res. Educ. Sci. 62, 181–207Google Scholar

Cho, S. (1984). The Story of Fart (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Eco Publishing Co Ltd.

Google Scholar

Cunningham, A. E., and Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Dev. Psychol. 33, 934–945. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, D. K. and P. O. Tabors (eds.) (2001). Beginning Literacy With Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Google Scholar

Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Google Scholar

Florit, E., Roch, M., and Levorato, C. M. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: the role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Process. 48, 119–138. doi: 10.1080/0163853x.2010.494244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foy, J. G., and Mann, V. A. (2013). Executive function and early reading skills. Read. Writ. 26, 453–472. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9376-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2002). “Physiology of executive functions: the perception-action cycle,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, eds D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 96–108. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin, A. P., Hermida, M. J., Shalom, D. E., Costa, M. E., Lopez-Rosenfeld, M., Segretin, M. S., et al. (2014). Far transfer to language and math of a short software-based gaming intervention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 6443–6448. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320217111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gravetter, F., and Wallnau, L. (2014). Essentials of Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Google Scholar

Hansen, J., and Pearson, P. D. (1983). An instructional study: improving the inferential comprehension of good and poor fourth-grade readers. J. Educ. Psychol. 75, 821–829. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.75.6.821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hargrave, A. C., and Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: the benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Child. Res. Q. 15, 75–90. doi: 10.1016/s0885-2006(99)00038-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayashi, A. (2010). Miki’s First Errand (M. Z. Lin, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

Google Scholar

Hoffman, D. H. (1976). Ten days with inga and In the Night Kitchen: an episode in language development. Commun. Educ. 25, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/03634527609384592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horiuchi, S. (2013). The Story of Bones (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

Google Scholar

Hungerford, S., Douglas, P., and Selvarajah, L. (2012). Executive functions as predictors of classroom listening skills. Paper Presented at the Meeting of the American Speech Language Hearing Association, Atlanta.

Google Scholar

Hutchins, P. (2009). Rosie’s Walk (Hsinex International Corporation, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Hsinex International Corporation.

Google Scholar

Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: a Paradigm for Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kuhn, M. R., and Stahl, S. A. (1998). Teaching children to learn word meanings from context: a synthesis and some question. J. Lit. Res. 30, 119–138. doi: 10.1080/10862969809547983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, L. H., and Chien, H. Y. (2019). Ti wen shi chong fu lang du dui you er zai yu wen li jie biao xian de xiao guo yan jiu [The effects of questioning integrated into repeated reading aloud program on oral comprehension performance of preschoolers]. Ke Cheng Yu Jiao Xue Ji Kan 22, 215–240. doi: 10.6384/CIQ.201907_22(3).0009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q., Zhu, X., Ziegler, A., and Shi, J. (2015). The effects of inhibitory control training for preschoolers on reasoning ability and neural activity. Sci. Rep. 5:14200. doi: 10.1038/srep14200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., and Liu, M. X. (1994). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Taipei: Psychological Publishing.

Google Scholar

Matsuoka, T. (2011). Where is the Rhinoceros Beetle? (Y. W. Huang, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Children’s Publications Co Ltd.

Google Scholar

McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., and Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Dev. Psychol. 43, 947–959. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGee, A., and Johnson, H. (2003). The effect of inference training on skilled and less skilled comprehenders. Educ. Psychol. 23, 49–59. doi: 10.1080/01443410303220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNamara, D. S., and Magliano, J. (2009). “Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 51, The Psychology of Learning and Motivation ed. B. H. Ross (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier Academic Press), 297–384. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51009-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., and Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: a latent variables analysis. Cognit. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y., Okanda, M., and Itakura, S. (2008). Young children’s yes bias: how does it relate to verbal ability, inhibitory control, and theory of mind? First Lang. 28, 431–442. doi: 10.1177/0142723708092413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrow, L. M., and Gambrell, L. B. (2002). “Literature-based instruction in the early years,” in Handbook Of Early Literacy Research eds S. B. Neuman and D. K. Dickinson. (New York, NY: Guilford).

Google Scholar

Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R. C., and Herman, P. A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. Ame. Educ. Res. J. 24, 237–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Early Literacy Panel, (2009). Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Jessup, MD: National Institute for Literacy.

Google Scholar

Neuman, S., Copple, C., and Bredekamp, S. (2000). Learning to Read and Write: Developmentally Appropriate Practices for Young Children. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children New York.

Google Scholar

Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A. G., and Moore, D. W. (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: do they overcome the matthew effect? J. Educ. Psychol. 94, 23–33. doi: 10.1037//0022-0663.94.1.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollard-Durodola, S., Gonzalez, J., Simmons, D., Kwok, O., Taylor, A., Davis, M., and Simmons, L. (2011). The effects of an intensive shared book-reading intervention for preschool children at risk for vocabulary delay. Except. Children 77, 161–183. doi: 10.1177/001440291107700202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Jewkes, A. M., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., and Morrison, F. J. (2008). Touch your toes! developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood. Early Child. Res. Q. 23, 141–158. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raphael, T. E., and McKinney, J. (1983). An examination of fifth and eighth-grade children’s question-answering behavior: an instructional study in metacognition. J. Read. Behav. 15, 67–86. doi: 10.1080/10862968309547490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C. P., Zhai, F., Bub, K, and Pressler, E. (2011). CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ pre-academic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Dev. 82, 362–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. C., and Anderson, N. A. (2003). How do you know? a strategy to help emergent readers make inferences. Read. Teach. 57, 290–293.

Google Scholar

Segers, E., Damhuis, C. M. P., Van de Sande, E., and Verhoeven, L. (2016). Role of executive functioning and home environment in early literacy development. Learn. Individ. Differ. 49, 251–259. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sénéchal, M. (1997). The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers’ acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary. J. Child Lang. 24, 123–138. doi: 10.1017/s0305000996003005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, M., and Cain, K. (2015). The relations between lower and higher level comprehension skills and their role in prediction of early reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 321–331. doi: 10.1037/a0037769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

C. E. Snow, S. Burns, and P. Griffin (eds.) (1998). Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.

Google Scholar

van den Broek, P., Tzeng, Y., Risden, K., and Trabasso, T. (2001). Inferential questioning: effects on comprehension of narrative texts as a function of grade and timing. J. Educ. Psychol. 93:521. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C. C. (2012). Review of literature on character and vocabulary instruction studies in Taiwan. Bull. Educ. Psychol. 44, 253–272.

Google Scholar

Wass, S. V. (2015). Applying cognitive training to target executive functions during early development, Child Neuropsychol. 21, 150–166. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.882888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehurst, G. J., Zevenbergen, A. A., Crone, D. A., Schultz, M. D., Velting, O. N., and Fischel, J. E. (1999). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention from head start through second grade. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 261–272. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, C. D., and Bell, M. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Dev. Psychobiol. 44, 68–83. doi: 10.1002/dev.10152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yagyu, G. (1987a). The Soles of Your Feet (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

Google Scholar

Yagyu, G. (1987b). Your Hand and Fingers (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.). Taipei City. Taiwan: Eco Publishing Co Ltd.

Google Scholar

Ye, Z., and Zhou, X. (2008). Involvement of cognitive control in sentence comprehension: evidence from ERPs. Brain Res. 1203, 103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Picture Books Used in the Study

(1) Hutchins, Pat (2009). Rosie’s Walk (Hsinex International Corporation, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Hsinex International Corporation.

(2) Matsuoka, Tatsyhide (2011). Where is the Rhinoceros Beetle? (Y. W. Huang, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Children’s Publications, Co., Ltd.

(3) Horiuchi, Seiichi (2013). The Story of Bones (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

(4) Hayashi, Akiko (2010). Miki’s First Errand (M. Z. Lin, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

(5) Yagyu, Genichiro (1987). The Soles of Your Feet (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Han Sheng Publisher.

(6) Cho, Shinta (1984). The Story of Fart (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Eco Publishing, Co., Ltd.

(7) Yagyu, Genichiro (1987). Your Hand and Fingers (Han Sheng Publisher, Trans.).

Taipei City, Taiwan: Eco Publishing, Co., Ltd.

Keywords: read-aloud, question and answer, executive function, oral comprehension, receptive vocabulary

Citation: Chien HY (2020) Effects of Two Teaching Strategies on Preschoolers’ Oral Language Skills: Repeated Read-Aloud With Question and Answer Teaching Embedded and Repeated Read-Aloud With Executive Function Activities Embedded. Front. Psychol. 10:2932. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02932

Received: 25 May 2019; Accepted: 11 December 2019;
Published: 21 January 2020.

Edited by:

Teresa Limpo, University of Porto, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Livia Taverna, Free University of Bozen-Bolzano, Italy
Naymé Salas, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Chien. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hsin Ying Chien, linyu8888@nttu.edu.tw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.