Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 25 February 2020
Sec. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurolaw: The Call for Adjusting Theory Based on Scientific Results View all 13 articles

Neuroscience in Youth Criminal Law: Reconsidering the Measure of Punishment in Latin America

  • Department of Law and Political Science, National University of La Matanza, San Justo, Argentina

Introduction

Due to the new discoveries and advances made in technology in the field of neuroscience in the last few decades, it has been possible to get a better understanding of the development of the human brain. This has had a significant impact on youth criminal law, especially in relation to the behavior of adolescents and their capacity to control impulsive reactions.

In this article, we will discuss the repercussions of this improved understanding on the amount of penalty for convicted adolescents in Latin America.

It is important to mention that the minimum age of criminal responsibility on each country of this region is quite different (mostly between 12 and 16 years old). Despite this and other divergences, we think it is possible to make an approach from the point of view of the Inter-American Human Rights System.

Measure of Punishment: Comparative Disproportion

It could be argued that the majority of actions or omissions which constitute a crime in a certain country usually also constitute a crime in most countries around the world. However, the measure of the punishment that could be imposed as result of that same crime does not follow this generalization. In this respect, for example, there are several countries that do not impose capital punishment or life imprisonment.

In this context, and according to Comparative Law, we find large disparities between the penalties applied in different countries by the youth criminal law, the body of law that regulates crimes committed by a person under the age of majority. Latin American countries are a great example of this situation: while Brazil has a maximum penalty of 3 years of imprisonment for any crime committed by an adolescent between 12 and 18-year-old (Law 8069 [Estatuto da Criança e do Adolescente], s. 121), other countries, like Bahamas [Penal Code, s. 263 (3)] allow capital punishment. More examples are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. This figure shows the maximum penalty in some Latin American countries that may be imposed on adolescents (as per references listed below). Countries that allow life imprisonment or capital punishment have been excluded for not been able to be shown. Antigua and Barbuda: Child Justice Act (No. 23 of 2005), c. X, s. 69(2); Bolivia: Law 548 [Código Niña, Niño y Adolescente], s. 268; Brazil: Law 8069 [Estatuto da Criança e do Adolescente], s. 121; Chile: Law 20084 [Sistema de Responsabilidad de los Adolescentes por Infracciones a la Ley Penal], s. 18; Colombia: Law 1098/2006 [Código de la Infancia y la Adolescencia], s. 187; Costa Rica: Law 7576 [Ley de Justicia Penal Juvenil], s. 131; Dominican Republic: Law 136-03 [Código para la protección de los derechos de los Niños, Niñas y Adolescentes], s. 340; Ecuador: Law 100 [Código de la Niñez y Adolescencia], s. 358(3); El Salvador: Law 869 [Ley del Menor Infractor], s. 15-17; Guatemala: Law 27/2003 [Ley de Protección integral de la niñez y adolescencia], s. 252(b); Honduras: Law 73/96 [Código de la Niñez y la Adolescencia], s. 205; Nicaragua: Law 287/1998 [Código de la Niñez y la Adolescencia], s. 206; Panamá: Law 40/1999 [Régimen Especial de Responsabilidad Penal para la Adolescencia], s. 141; Paraguay: Law 1680/2001 [Código de la Niñez y la Adolescencia], s. 207; Perú: Law 27337 [Código de los Niños y Adolescentes], s. 235; Uruguay: Law 17823 [Código de la Niñez y la Adolescencia], s. 91; Venezuela: Law 5859 [Ley Orgánica para la Protección del Niño, Niña y Adolescente], s. 628. Canada (Youth Criminal Justice Act [S.C. 2002, c. 1, s. 64(1)] and Grenada [Juvenile Justice Act, Act No. 24 of 2012, s. 4(2)] may impose life imprisonment. Through the reports called “Concluding observations” made by the United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child (available on https://www.ohchr.org/SP/Countries/LacRegion/Pages/LacRegionIndex.aspx) we were able to establish that the death penalty could be imposed in Bahamas and Saint Lucia; and life imprisonment could be imposed in Barbados, Belize, Cuba, Dominica, Guyana, Haiti, Jamaica, Mexico, Saint Kitts and Nevis, Saint Vincent and the Grenadines, Suriname and Trinidad and Tobago.

In order to analyze this correctly, we propose to classify the different legislative methods into three groups. First, there are legal systems that allow the transfer of young offenders to a criminal court (also known as “trial as an adult”). Second, there are those that allow the juvenile court to impose an adult sentence. Third, there are those that only allow juvenile sentences for young offenders, which are considerably less severe than adult sentences.

The first method is common in countries that have adopted the legal system known as Common Law (pure or mixed). The decision to transfer a young offender may contemplate several factors, but the most important ones are the severity of the offense and the age of the offender. This decision may be made by a judge (judicial waiver), a prosecutor (prosecutorial discretion), or by the law itself (statutory exclusion).

The second method is mostly used in countries that have adopted the civil law system. Like the prior one, the severity of the offense and the age of the offender are the main factors used to make the decision. Despite their differences, both systems enable the sentencing of a young offender as an adult.

The third one, however, prohibits that kind of penalty, which also means it prohibits capital punishment and life imprisonment. It is not possible to make any other assumption about this matter since the maximum amount of penalty is quite diverse in every jurisdiction.

Additionally, there are international laws that prohibit capital punishment or life imprisonment for young offenders, like article 37 of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child, and human-rights courts that do not allow for adolescents to be sentenced with the same punishment that may be imposed on an adult, like the leading case “Mendoza et al. v. Argentina, Preliminary Objections, Merits, and Reparations, Judgment” of the Inter-American Court of Human Rights (ser. C, No. 260, May 14, 2013).

Overview of Neurolaw Regarding Adolescents

Having said all that, it should be affirmed that several neuroscientific studies have proved that adolescents do not have the same cognitive capacity as an adult. In particular, it has been suggested that the frontal lobe, whose functions involve controlling and judging impulse and risk, projecting future consequences resulting from current actions (Fuster, 2001; Martinez Selva et al., 2006), continues its development well into young adulthood (Gogtay et al., 2004; Giedd, 2008).

Thus, disadvantageous decision making and risky behavior shown by adolescents are considered to be related to the slower developing prefrontal cortex (Smith et al., 2012), which has been linked to prominent differences in cognitive capacity (Cauffman and Steinberg, 2000; Galvan et al., 2006; Eshel et al., 2007). Further investigations have been made, some of them related to drug abuse or peer influence, which support this matter (Blakemore, 2012; Spear, 2013; Brizio et al., 2015; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2016)1.

The impact of those studies was meaningful for the judiciary system of the United States since they were used by its Supreme Court to sentence the leading cases Roper v. Simmons (543 U.S. 551), Graham v. Florida (560 U.S. 48), and Miller v. Alabama (567 U.S. 460). In addition, there is an ongoing debate about their legal implications (Steinberg, 2009; Delmage, 2013).

Discussion

The Supreme Court of the United States stated that “a sentence lacking any legitimate penological justification is by its nature disproportionate to the offense” (Graham v. Florida, 560 U.S. 48, p. 20). However, it is necessary to analyze if penological justifications designed for adults are applicable to juveniles.

This implies a change in basic assumptions. Penological justifications have been created and built on suppositions tied up with notions of agency, freedom, and free will. Whenever a sentence requires a person acting purposely, the lack of intent means there is absence of blameworthiness as well as absence of any justification for condemning. Therefore, if it is proved that adolescents do not have the same capacity as an adult to observe the law, it does not only impact on the personal culpability but also the assumptions of the penological justification itself.

In this regard, equality and non-discrimination before the law should not only be considered as giving the same legal treatment to all human beings in general, but also to give different treatment to those who are not equals. Consequently, applying similar punishment to juvenile offenders and adult offenders for the same crime should be judged incompatible with legal principles and also with the current state of the science.

In modern criminal law there is no debate that any sentence must take into consideration the moral responsibility of the perpetrator. However, this same principle wrongly causes controversy when the outcome of its application consists of a reduction in culpability, and therefore in the size of the imposed penalty.

For all these reasons, we consider that any law or jurisprudence that makes the transfer of a juvenile offender to an adult court possible, or allows an adult sentence to be imposed on them, ought to be reconsidered. As it was mentioned before, there are many countries whose legislation provides considerable differences between juvenile and adults offenders, and the Inter-American Court of Human Rights has represented an important step in this direction (Llamas, 2019).

Nevertheless, the legal impact of the neuroscientific findings and technologies is an open debate (Muñoz Ortega, 2013, 2018). Nowadays, we are observing an exponential increase of publications about adolescents and their behavior related to alcohol, drugs, stress, and peer influence, among other topics. Some of them even suggest that the age of 18 is not a scientifically correct watershed between adolescent and adult criminal responsibility (Mercurio, 2012; Mercurio et al., 2019).

The topic is crucial when considering some countries with high levels of poverty and malnutrition in childhood, which may affect the development of the human brain and its cognitive abilities (Mercurio, 2016), as well as the known effects of deprivation (Llamas and Marinaro, 2017)2.

As a final reflection, we want to mention that some very old Spanish laws, which were in force long before the independence of Latin-American countries (López de Guevara, 1843), did not allow adolescents to be sentenced as adults. In a way, it seems that new discoveries might prove scientifically what was presumed righteous long ago.

Author Contributions

NL and JM wrote the manuscript with equal contributions.

Funding

This work was supported by the National University of La Matanza, Argentina.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank José Manuel Muñoz Ortega for comments and useful discussions regarding earlier versions of this manuscript. We also thank Carolina Valeria De Valois for the translation work.

Footnotes

1. ^There are other aspects of the development of prefrontal cortex which might play a major role in terms of behavioral outcomes, such as hormonal influences onto the brain (Blakemore et al., 2010).

2. ^It is important to remark that there are cultural and ethical non-scientific aspects that were at the basis of the exclusion of the most severe punishments for young people in Europe and in the United States.

References

Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage 61, 397–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J., Burnett, S., and Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapp. 31, 926–933. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brizio, A., Gabbatore, I., Tirassa, M., and Bosco, F. M. (2015). “No more a child, not yet an adult:” studying social cognition in adolescence. Front. Psychol. 6:1011. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cauffman, E., and Steinberg, L. (2000). (Im) maturity of judgment in adolescence: why adolescents may be less culpable than adults. Behav. Sci. Law 18, 741–760. doi: 10.1002/bsl.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delmage, E. (2013). The minimum aof criminal responsibility: a medico-legal perspective. Youth Justice 13, 102–110. doi: 10.1177/1473225413492053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eshel, N., Nelson, E. E., Blair, R. J., Pine, D. S., and Ernst, M. (2007). Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia 45, 1270–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex—an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30, 319–333. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00285-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., et al. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 26:6885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. J. Adolesc. Health 42, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llamas, N. E. (2019). La pena en el derecho penal juvenil [The penalty in youth criminal law] (Tesis doctoral). Universidad Nacional de La Matanza, Escuela de Posgrado, San Justo, Argentina.

Llamas, N. E., and Marinaro, J. Á. (2017). La hipótesis Cajal-Hebb: Vinculación entre las deprivaciones de los jóvenes delincuentes y las neurociencias [The Cajal-Hebb hypothesis: link between the deprivations of young offenders and neurosciences]. Rev. Derecho Penal Criminología (La Ley) VII, 203–209.

Google Scholar

López de Guevara, G. (ed.). (1843). Las Siete Partidas del Sabio Rey Don Alfonso IX [The Seven Divisions of Law of the Wise King Don Alfonso IX]. Available online at: http://www.cervantesvirtual.com/obra/las-siete-partidas-del-sabio-rey-don-alfonso-el-ix-sic

Google Scholar

Martinez Selva, J. M., Sanchez Navarro, J. P., Bechara, A., and Román Lapuente, F. (2006). Mecanismos cerebrales de la toma de decisiones [Brain mechanisms involved in decision-making]. Rev. Neurol. 42, 411–418. doi: 10.33588/rn.4207.2006161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercurio, E. N. (2012). Cerebro y Adolescencia: Implicancias Jurídico-Penales [Brain and Adolescence: Legal-Criminal Implications], 1st Edn. Buenos Aires: Ad-Hoc.

Mercurio, E. N. (2016). Pobreza y discapacidad intelectual en el sistema penal: los invisibles [Poverty and intellectual disability in the criminal system: the invisible]. VERTEX Rev. Argentina Psiquiatría XXVII, 197–207.

Google Scholar

Mercurio, E. N., García López, E., and Morales Quintero, L. A. (2019). Psicopatología forense y neurociencias: aportaciones al sistema de justicia para adolescentes [Forensic phsychopathology and neuroscience: contributions to the justice system for adolescents]. Bol. Mexicano Derecho Comparado 1, 931–971.

Muñoz Ortega, J. M. (2013). Neurofilosofía y libre albedrío [Neurophilosophy and free will]. Daimon Rev. Int. Filosofia 59, 57–70.

Muñoz Ortega, J. M. (2018). Mental causation and neuroscience: the semantic pruning model. Theoria Int. J. Theory Hist. Found. Sci. 33, 379–399. doi: 10.1387/theoria.17312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. G., Xiao, L., and Bechara, A. (2012). Decision making in children and adolescents: impaired iowa gambling task performance in early adolescence. Dev. Psychol. 48, 1180–1187. doi: 10.1037/a0026342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2013). Adolescent neurodevelopment. J. Adolesc. Health 52, S7–S13. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2009). Should the science of adolescent brain development inform public policy? Am. Psychol. 64, 739–750. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.64.8.739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peters, S., Braams, B. R., and Crone, E. A. (2016). What motivates adolescents? neural responses to rewards and their influence on adolescents' risk taking, learning, and cognitive control. Adolesc. Brain 70, 135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurolaw, youth criminal law, juvenile crime and delinquency, juvenile criminal behavior, juvenile criminal law, capital punishment, life imprisonment, life imprisonment without parole

Citation: Llamas NE and Marinaro JÁ (2020) Neuroscience in Youth Criminal Law: Reconsidering the Measure of Punishment in Latin America. Front. Psychol. 11:302. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00302

Received: 28 August 2019; Accepted: 07 February 2020;
Published: 25 February 2020.

Edited by:

Elena Rusconi, University of Trento, Italy

Reviewed by:

Amedeo Santosuosso, University of Pavia, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Llamas and Marinaro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicolás Ezequiel Llamas, nicolasllamas@hotmail.com; José Ángel Marinaro, joseangelmarinaro@yahoo.com.ar

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.