Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 May 2020
Sec. Movement Science

Development and Validity of the Fear of Water Assessment Questionnaire

  • Faculty of Sports, University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia

Fear of water is the strongest predictor for no or low swimming competencies. Some individuals will never learn to swim due to their complete avoidance of water, whereas others might have difficulty with learning due to the fact that they cannot sufficiently relax their body to facilitate floating or swimming. Therefore, it is important to identify these people and to establish effective teaching strategies that can best help this specific population. Recognizing this, there is a clear need for an assessment tool which can help swim teachers and coaches identify people with a fear of water. The study aimed to first develop and then validate a fear of water assessment questionnaire (FWAQ). 2074 male and female people participated in the creation of a 40-item questionnaire. The exploratory factor showed that a 3 factor solution including 20 items was most sensible – such a solution accounted for 31.69% of explained variance and the Cronbach’s alpha α was 0.831, which makes for a reliable enough solution. A subsequent discriminant function analysis correctly classified 98.2% of participants. We concluded that the findings from this study support that the FWAQ is a valid scale that effectively identify people with fear of water.

Introduction

Well-developed swimming skills are not only essential for drowning prevention, but also contribute to the development and maintenance of overall fitness (Brenner et al., 2009; Beggs et al., 2013). Learning how to swim is not only a physical, but also a cultural achievement (Majumder and Choudhury, 2014). Therefore, instructional swimming programs for both beginners and advanced swimmers form part of physical education curriculums at different levels of education in many European countries (Jurgec et al., 2016). Gilchrist et al. (2000) found that, despite many efforts to improve swimming knowledge, many people still don’t know how to swim. We still have little data about the swimming abilities of children and adolescents, and the available data are inadequate and unable to be compared between different countries due to varying methodological approaches. By using an interview and examination survey, data illustrated that 14.5% of 5- to 17-year-olds in Germany were unable to swim (Kuntz et al., 2016). Moreover, in the United Kingdom, approximately half of children aged 7–11 years of age were found to be incapable of swimming 25 m (Amateur Swimming Association, 2013). Nearly 64% of African American children, 45% of Hispanic children, and 40% of Caucasian children have little or no swimming ability (The United States Association Swimming Foundation, 2017). Data from the Ministry of Education, Science and Sport of the Republic of Slovenia illustrate that 7% of people 12 years of age cannot swim 50 m, thus they are classified as non-swimmers (Grujić, 2018).

Theoretical Background

There are a variety of reasons why many children and adolescents cannot swim (Pharr et al., 2018). In children and adults who avoid swimming lessons there exists barriers which makes them avoid swimming. Such barriers may include accessibility to the pool, cultural issues of not wanting to learn to swim, racial and ethnic factors such as hair care, discomfort of being seen in swimsuits, parents whose fear of water could discourage their children from learning to swim, injuries that happened to family and friends, drowning, illness, and/or negative experiences (Lachocki, 2012). However, fear of drowning is a very common factor (Berukoff and Hill, 2010; Pharr et al., 2018). Indeed, it is the strongest predictor of no or low swimming ability (i.e., even stronger than family finances or access to swimming facilities) (Ziara, 2005; Irwin et al., 2010). The fear of drowning could originate within the common overall fear of water (Whiting and Stembridge, 1965; Shank, 1987). Fear of water or aquaphobia is considered to be a “specific phobia,” which means “a marked and persistent fear that is cued by circumscribed clearly discernible objects or situations” (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000, pp. 219), The prevalence rate of aquaphobia in the general population is between 2 and 3% (Stinson et al., 2007), and it is more common among children than adults (Menzies and Harris, 1997). Specific phobias, such as a fear of water, usually originate from childhood and are frequently intensified through adulthood (Becker et al., 2007). The origins of a fear of water during childhood has been examined. The most common belief is that it is usually linked to a previous bad experience (Whiting and Stembridge, 1965; Shank, 1987). These could be bad swimming lessons, an accidental fall into deep water, near drowning experiences, etc (Shank, 1987). In contrast, it has also been suggested that the origins of the fear of water can best be explained by non-associative processes (Menzies and Clarke, 1993; Graham and Gaffan, 1997). This means that it mainly reflects a biological fear that manifests often without aversive experiences (Poulton et al., 1998). A broad range of situations may elicit the fear of water, such us being or swimming in water that is dark or opaque (i.e., without clear vision of what is in the water), submerging one’s head below the water, being near fountains, and traveling on a boat (Milosevic and McCabe, 2015). Rarely, even bathing may provoke a fear response to water. Considering this, the fear of water can disrupt a number of activities which are carried out in water or near it (Milosevic and McCabe, 2015). Some individuals never learn to swim due to their complete avoidance of water, whereas others might have difficulty with learning due to the fact that they cannot sufficiently relax their body to facilitate floating or swimming (Milosevic and McCabe, 2015). Therefore, it is reasonable to conclude that the fear of water is a considerable factor, which may put children or adolescents at a high risk of drowning (Irwin et al., 2015). These individuals are the most likely to panic if they find themselves in a dangerous situation, which is a key determinant of fatal or nonfatal drowning events (Grenfell, 2004).

372000 people die from drowning every year, which makes drowning a serious public health threat and one of the ten leading causes of death in children under 5 years of age and young males between 15 and 19 years (World Health Organization [WHO], 2014). Learning to swim and education on water safety are two of the most important strategies for preventing drowning (Leavy et al., 2019), some authors especially emphasize the protective value of swimming ability against drowning (Brenner et al., 2009). Data from the United States show that 74% of drowning victims didn’t know how to swim (Cody et al., 2004), similarly over one third of drowning victims between 5 and 14 years of age in Canada had no or poor swimming abilities (Canadian Red Cross, 2003). There are also interesting findings regarding good swimming abilities - better swimming ability has been found to be related to more water-related risk behaviors (Ma et al., 2010), which was translated into higher drowning risk by overconfidence in the water (Smith, 1995).

Collectively, it is important that people with a fear of water learn how to swim. Moreover, it is important to identify them and to establish effective teaching strategies that can best help this specific population (Stillwell, 2011). Recognizing this, there is a clear need for an assessment tool which will help swimming teachers and coaches identify those with a fear of water. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to develop and to validate the fear of water assessment questionnaire (FWAQ).

Three studies were conducted to test our adapted FWAQ. In Study 1, we composed the items and looked for the factor structure of the FWAQ, allowing important items to be retained and subsequently interpreted. In Study 2, we assessed the reliability of the FWAQ. Finally, in Study 3, we examined the discriminant function of the FWQA. All studies were approved by an ethical committee and were conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Study 1 Item Generation and Exploratory Factor Analysis

Methods

Item Generation

To develop the items for FWAQ, an interdisciplinary team of researchers from the sport sciences was used, which included three experts from different fields: A sport psychologist, a swim pedagogue, and a swimming coach experienced in teaching swimming were assembled to accomplish this project. The following is a survey designed with a list of 40 items (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Item generation.

Participants

Two thousand seventy four participants [1002 males and 1072 females, ages ranging from 13 to 76 years (average age was 24.5 ± 11.7 years)] participated in the study. They were extensively informed regarding the aims of the study before providing their written consent. Participants were from the cities Mitrovica, Peja, Gjakova, Prizren, Ferizaj, Gjilan, Pristina, and Podujevo in the Republic of Kosovo. These cities were selected due to their diverse populations.

Procedure

The interviewers stood in the main street of each city and selected every third person who came by. They tried to sample similar numbers of men and women. We asked the participants to fill out the FWAQ. They rated each item according to their degree of agreement or disagreement by using a five-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = not sure, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree).

Data Analysis

We used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with principal components analysis (PCA) with a Varimax rotation. Data was analyzed with the SPSS 20.0 package (SPSS Inc., Chicago, United States).

Results

Sample size turned out to be large enough for a factor analysis (KMO = 0.912), this decision was supported by Bartlett’s test of sphericity’s significance (χ2 = 12304.337; df = 780; p = 0.000).

Item communalities ranged from 0.308 to 0.651 (M = 0.496), we decided on a 3-factor solution based on the eigenvalues using the Kaiser criterion, even though the percentage of explained variance is rather low – this structure accounted for 31.69% of the total variance. Eigenvalues ranged from 18.9 to 4.73.

Relationships Between Factors

We examined both the pattern matrix and the structure matrix – Table 2 shows factor loadings on each item, whilst correlations between factors are shown in the structure matrix.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor loading.

Interpreting and Naming the Factors

Following the recommendations of Tabachnick and Fidell (2013), we kept the items with a factor loading of more than 0.32 and the meaning of each factor was based mostly on the strongest loading items. Thus we kept 20 items and, according to the recommendations of Henson and Roberts (2006), we conducted a second EFA on the remaining 20 items. The 3-factor structure was confirmed, accounting for 40% of the total explained variance.

Study 2 Reliability of the FWAQ

Methods

Participants

We used the same participants and data as in Study 1 in order to test the internal consistency of FWAQ.

Data Analysis

We calculated Cronbach’s alpha coefficients, data was analyzed with the SPSS 20.0 package (SPSS Inc., Chicago, United States).

Results

A Cronbach’s alpha of 0.831 was determined, which makes for a very good internal consistency of FWAQ. Internal consistency for Factor 1 was 0.805, Factor 2 was 0.798, and 0.71 for Factor 3.

Study 3 Examining the Discriminant Function of the FWAQ

We wanted to test if the FWQA could effectively discriminate between “Fear of water” and “No fear of water” participants.

Methods

Participants

One hundred and ten participants (53 males, 57 females), ages ranging from 10 to 12 years of age (average age was 10.5 ± 0.5 years), participated in the study.

Procedure

Ethical approval was granted from the authors and the study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki for ethical principles. Participants and their parents were extensively informed regarding the aims of the study before written consent of the parents was provided. We asked the participants to fill out the FWAQ. They rated each item according to their degree of agreement or disagreement by using five-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = not sure, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree).

When the data was collected, a physical education teacher with experience of teaching swimming was asked to be an evaluator. They asked participants to rate their responses on a five-point Likert scale based on imagining themselves in water-area scenarios. Those ranked “Completely disagree” or “Completely agree” on the subjective rating scale were classified as “No fear of water” (n = 58), those who scored 4 or 5 on the scale were classified as “With fear of water” (n = 52).

Data Analysis

To test for differences between groups we calculated MANOVA and used the level of significance at p < 0.05 (Field, 2018) and then a discriminant function analysis to check for prediction of group membership. We used the SPSS 20.0 package (SPSS Inc., Chicago, United States).

Results

A Mahalanobis distance of 43.10 was calculated, which is below the critical value of 45.31, suggesting multivariate normality (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013). Correlations between the variables ranged from 0.189 to 0.553 and we found a difference in FWAQ scores between the groups with fear of water and no fear of water, [F(20.89) = 31.21, p < 0.005, Wilks Lambda = 0.125, partial eta squared = 0.875]. Descriptive statistics of final FWAQ scores of fear of water and no fear of water groups, effect sizes, and significance levels of differences between groups are presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Discriminant function.

An initial examination of the variables means show that those in the groups with a fear of water scored better (i.e., higher on the first factors, and second factor while lower on the third factor), suggesting that those people rated as more likely to have a fear of water. Twenty of the variables showed statistically significant level of 0.001. Bonferroni adjustment testing resulted in similar significance, alpha 0.001.

We found a significant discriminant function of the FWAQ (Wilks Lambda = 0.125, χ2 = 203.975, p < 0.001), with a canonical correlation of 0.936. The FWAQ was able to correctly predict 98.18% (51 out of 52) of the “with fear of water” group members and 98.3% (57 out of 58) of the “no fear of water” group members; in total 98.2% of the 108 participants could be correctly classified. The standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients show that factors one and two (active coping) and factor three (adverse response to fear of water) are important for group differentiation as can be seen from Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree plot of the factor analysis.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to develop and to validate the FWAQ. The final FWAQ consisted of 20 items (Appendix Table A1) and includes three factors, which account for 31.69% of the total variance. Discriminant function analysis shows that the FWAQ can correctly classify participants as having or not having fear of water, which suggests that the FWAQ could be a new tool designed to assess the fear of water. Factor analysis revealed that the scale has three meaningful factors. We found strong reliability for the FWAQ and we believe it to be a robust predictor of the fear of water.

The factor analyses emphasized that the FWAQ has three replicable factors: Water environment contact, Natural force of water, and Motion control in water. This suggests that the fear of water assessment questionnaire interpretations are multifaceted. The first factor, Water environment contact, highlighted the importance of the first steps of learning how to swim, such as exercises of submerging the face and opening the eyes under water. Beside learning and training these two skills, the usage of goggles or mask could be another way to reduce stress and anxiety when in water. Masks or goggles enable unobstructed vision and therefore greater ease in submerging the face (Kapus et al., 2018). Submerging the face might have some positive biomechanical effects in beginner swimmers (e.g., increased buoyancy), which allows them to learn more easily. These aids may help to increase a beginner’s confidence, allowing them to break contact with the bottom of the pool floor or the side of the pool. Moreover, they may help to place swimmers in the proper horizontal body position, thereby simplifying the complex coordination of arms, legs, and breathing (Parker et al., 1999). This could give beginner swimmers additional motivation to try more challenging swimming exercises. However, we should emphasize it may also increase their dependency on those items, which in turn could hinder the acquisition of the two skills and may in fact increase overall fear of the water when a mask or goggles are not available. Therefore, the suggested usage of a mask or goggles should be viewed solely as an aid to decrease initial stress and anxiety from submerging the face and opening the eyes under the water. Beginners should be able to open their eyes under water so that they can better orientate themselves when swimming, in the case of accidently falling into the water without a mask or goggles.

The second factor, Nature force of water, gathered the items concerning fear of conditions, which appears in open water. This factor could be expected due to the fact that most drownings occur in open water (Weiss et al., 2010; Tyler et al., 2017). In an open-water situation, water competence can be hindered by cold air, low water temperatures, rough (e.g., waves, surf) water, and clothing (Moran, 2015) – this same effect was found also by Kjendlie et al. (2013), who found an 8% decrease of water competency in a 200-m swim and a 24% decrease in floating performance in 11 year old children. It was also found that life guards swim between 30 to 57% slower when exposed to rough waters (Tipton et al., 2008).

According to this, Stallman et al. (2017) suggested:

1. Water competencies taught have to relate to open as well as closed water environments.

2. Open water competencies can be introduced from an early age through simple tasks, such as water splashing and simulating waves and currents.

3. Pool water safety programs should also simulate rough water.

The third factor, Motion control in water, highlighted the importance of the acquisition of two swimming competencies, such us safe entry and gliding (Stallman et al., 2017). The degree of risk upon water entry depends on the task, environment, and the individual (Langendorfer, 2010). Accidental falls into water require the person to hold their breath, reorient themselves, return to the surface, obtain a floating position, rest, and/or start moving in a certain direction. We can safely state that teaching a safe entrance into water (both head and feet first) should be a part of every aquatics and water safety program (Stallman et al., 2017). Due to the meaning of items, which were gathered with this factor (Table 1), the negative correlations between factors were expected. People who were recognized as having a fear of water ranked the items from the first and the second factor with high scores and items from the third factor with low scores.

We suggest swim teachers and coaches consider the use of the FWAQ as means of identification for people with a fear of water; it could also help them find out what frightens an individual or group. This is especially important for personalizing instruction and adapting the learning method when teaching people how to swim (Stillwell, 2011).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Faculty of sport, University of Ljubljana. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

TK was in charge of psychological aspects. FM developed the idea and carried out the measurements. SM assisted in statistical processing. JK helped with interpretational issues.

Funding

This research was supported with grants to Aziz Misimi, Xhezide Misimi, and Laurata Uka. Prishila Qelemeni, BSN, RN. and Denis Qelemeni, B.A. which served as English language editors of the first reviewer edition. The project was supported by Public research agency of the Republic of Slovenia grant (P5-0147).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Arif Misimi, Halim Zeqiri, and Arsim Hajra for realizing surveys. An acknowledgment of Dr. James R. Churilla from the University of North Florida, who served as an English language editor for this manuscript would be appreciated.

References

Amateur Swimming Association (2013). Learning the Lessons: The Future of School Swimming. Leicester: University of Leicestershire.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–5). Available online at: https://www.psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm

Google Scholar

Becker, E. S., Rinck, M., Türke, V., Kause, P., Goodwin, R., Neumer, S., et al. (2007). Epidemiology of specific phobia subtypes: findings from the Dresden Mental Health Study. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beggs, S., Foong, Y. C., Le, H. C., Noor, D., Wood-Baker, R., and Walters, J. A. (2013). Swimming training for asthma in children and adolescents aged 18 years and under. Paediatr. Respirat. Rev. 14, 96–97. doi: 10.1016/j.prrv.2013.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berukoff, K. D., and Hill, G. M. (2010). A study of factors that influence the swimming performance of hispanic high school students. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 4, 409–421. doi: 10.25035/ijare.04.04.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenner, R. A., Taneja, G. S., Haynie, D. L., Trumble, A. C., Qian, C., Klinger, R. M., et al. (2009). Association between swimming lessons and drowning in childhood: a case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 163, 203–210. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cody, B., Quraishi, A., Dastur, M., and Mickalide, D. (2004). A National Study of Childhood Drowning and Related Attitudes and Behaviors. Washington, DC: The National SAFE KIDS Campaign.

Google Scholar

Field, A. (2018). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Ltd.

Google Scholar

Gilchrist, J., Sacks, J. J., and Branche, C. M. (2000). Self-reported swimming ability in US adults, 1994. Public Health Reports, 115, 110–111.

Google Scholar

Graham, J., and Gaffan, E. A. (1997). Fear of water in children and adults: Etiology and familial effects. Behav. Res. Therapy 35, 91–108. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00086-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grenfell, R. (2004). Drowning management and prevention. Austral. Fam. Phys. 32, 990–993.

Google Scholar

Grujić, S. (2018). Pregled športa v Republiki Sloveniji V Obdobju Od Leta 2013 do 2018 Ljubljana: Zavod za šport Republike Slovenije Planica.

Google Scholar

Henson, R. K., and Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66, 393–416.

Google Scholar

Irwin, C., Irwin, R., Martin, N., and Ross, S. (2010). Constraints Impacting Minority Swimming Participation PHASE II. Memphis: The University of Memphis.

Google Scholar

Irwin, C., Pharr, J., and Irwin, R. (2015). Understanding factors that influence fear of drowning in children and adolescents. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 9, 136–148.

Google Scholar

Jurgec, N., Kapus, J., and Majerič, M. (2016). Učenje plavanja v nekaterih evropskih državah (Swimming teaching in some European countries). Šport 64, 42–46.

Google Scholar

Kapus, J., Moravec, T., and Lomax, M. (2018). Effects of head position on the duration of breaststroke swimming in preschool swimming beginners. Res. Gate 24, 17–27.

Google Scholar

Kjendlie, P.-L., Pedersen, T. E., Thoresen, T., Setlo, T., Moran, K., and Stallman, R. K. (2013). Can you swim in waves? Children’s swimming, floating, and entry skills in calm and simulated unsteady water conditions. Int. J. Aqua. Res. Edu. 7, 301–313. doi: 10.25035/ijare.07.04.04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntz, B., Frank, L., Manz, K., Rommel, A., and Lampert, T. (2016). Social determinants of swimming ability among children and adolescents in Germany. Results of KiGGS Wave 1. Deutsche Z. Sportmed. 2016, 137–143. doi: 10.5960/dzsm.2016.238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachocki, T. M. (2012). More Swimmers Will Result in a Healthier Society, Fewer Drownings and Reduced Healthcare Costs. Washington, DC: WCP.

Google Scholar

Langendorfer, S. J. (2010). Considering safe water entry. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 4:2. doi: 10.25035/ijare.04.04.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavy, J. E., Jancey, J., Hall, C., Nimmo, L., and Crawford, G. (2019). A content analysis of news media coverage of drowning events in Western Australia over two summers, 2014-2016. Health Promot. J. Austral. 31, 192–198. doi: 10.1002/hpja.267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, W. J., Nie, S. P., Xu, H. F., Xu, Y. J., Song, X. L., Guo, Q. Z., et al. (2010). An analysis of risk factors of non-fatal drowning among children in rural areas of guangdong province, China: a case-control study. BMC Public Health 10:156. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majumder, P. P., and Choudhury, S. R. (2014). Reasons for success in competitive swimming among various human races. IOSR J. Sports Phys. Educ. 1, 24–30. doi: 10.9790/6737-0132430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzies, R. G., and Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of childhood water phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 31, 499–501. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90131-d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzies, R. G., and Harris, L. M. (1997). Mode of onset in evolutionary-relevant and evolutionary-neutral phobias: evidence from a clinical sample. Depress. Anxiety. 5, 134–136. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6394(1997)5:3<134::aid-da5>3.0.co;2-e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milosevic, I., and McCabe, E. R. (2015). Phobias: The Psychology of Irrational Fear: The Psychology of Irrational Fear. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Google Scholar

Moran, K. (2015). Can you swim in clothes? Reflections on the perception and reality of the effect of clothing on water competency. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 9, 116–135. doi: 10.25035/ijare.09.02.04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, H. E., Blanksby, B. A., and Quek, K. L. (1999). Learning to swim using buoyancy aides. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 11, 377–392. doi: 10.1123/pes.11.4.377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pharr, J., Irwin, C., Layne, T., and Irwin, R. (2018). Predictors of swimming ability among children and adolescents in the United States. Sports 6:17. doi: 10.3390/sports6010017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulton, R., Davies, S., Menzies, R. G., Langley, J. D., and Silva, P. A. (1998). Evidence for a non-associative model of the acquisition of a fear of heights. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 537–544. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10037-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shank, C. B. (1987). A child’s fear of the water environment. Childrens Environ. Q. 4, 33–37.

Google Scholar

Smith, G. S. (1995). Drowning prevention in children: the need for new strategies. Injury Prevent. 1, 216–217.

Google Scholar

Stallman, R. K., Moran, K., Quan, L., and Langendorfer, S. (2017). From swimming skill to water competence: towards a more inclusive drowning prevention future. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 10:3. doi: 10.25035/ijare.10.02.03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stillwell, B. (2011). The subjective experiences of those afraid in water. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 5:7. doi: 10.25035/ijare.05.01.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Patricia Chou, S., Smith, S., Goldstein, R. B., June Ruan, W., et al. (2007). The epidemiology of DSM-IV specific phobia in the USA: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Psychol. Med. 37, 1047–1059. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics (sixth). London: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

The United States Association Swimming Foundation (2017). USA Swimming Foundation Announces 5-10 Percent Increase in Swimming Ability Among U.S. Children. Availble online at: https://www.usaswimmingfoundation.org/utility/landing-pages/news/2017/05/25/usa-swimming-foundation-announces-5-10-percent-increase-in-swimming-ability-among-u.s.-children

Google Scholar

Tipton, M., Reilly, T., Rees, A., Spray, G., and Golden, F. (2008). Swimming performance in surf: the influence of experience. Int. J. Sports Med. 29, 895–898. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1038510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, M. D., Richards, D. B., Reske-Nielsen, C., Saghafi, O., Morse, E. A., Carey, R., et al. (2017). The epidemiology of drowning in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review. BMC Public Health 17:413. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4239-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, J., Gardner, H. G., Baum, C. R., Dowd, M. D., Durbin, D. R., Ebel, B. E., et al. (2010). Policy statement—Prevention of drowning. Pediatrics 126, 178–185. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiting, H. T., and Stembridge, D. E. (1965). Personality and the persistent nonswimmer. Res. Q. 36, 348–356.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2014). Drowning. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/drowning

Google Scholar

Ziara, W. (2005). Relationships between progress in acquisition of swimming skills and anxiety level in ten-year-old childre. Hum. Mov. 6, 93–97.

Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. Fear of Water Assessment Questionnaire (FWAQ).

Keywords: swimming assessment, coaching, drowning, testing, swimming

Citation: Misimi F, Kajtna T, Misimi S and Kapus J (2020) Development and Validity of the Fear of Water Assessment Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 11:969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00969

Received: 22 March 2020; Accepted: 20 April 2020;
Published: 29 May 2020.

Edited by:

Miguel-Angel Gomez-Ruano, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Spain

Reviewed by:

Beat Knechtle, University Hospital Zurich, Switzerland
António José Figueiredo, University of Coimbra, Portugal

Copyright © 2020 Misimi, Kajtna, Misimi and Kapus. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tanja Kajtna, tanja.kajtna@fsp.uni-lj.si; Jernej Kapus, jernej.kapus@fsp.uni-lj.si

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.