Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 24 July 2020
Sec. Psychology of Language

Meta-Analysis Reveals a Bilingual Advantage That Is Dependent on Task and Age

  • Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia

Debate continues on whether a bilingual advantage exists with respect to executive functioning. This report synthesized the results of 170 studies to test whether the bilingual advantage is dependent on the task used to assess executive functioning and the age of the participants. The results of the meta-analyses indicated that the bilingual advantage was both task- and age-specific. Bilinguals were significantly faster than monolinguals (Hedges' g values ranged from 0.23 to 0.34), and significantly more accurate than monolinguals (Hedges' g values ranged between 0.18 and 0.49) on four out of seven tasks. Also, an effect of age was found whereby the bilingual advantage was larger for studies comprising samples aged 50-years and over (Hedges' g = 0.49), compared to those undertaken with participants aged between 18 and 29 years (Hedges' g = 0.12). The extent to which the bilingual advantage might be due to publication bias was assessed using multiple methods. These were Egger's Test of Asymmetry, Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill, Classic Fail-Safe N, and PET-PEESE. Publication bias was only found when using Egger's Test of Asymmetry and PET-PEESE method, but not when using the other methods. This review indicates that if bilingualism does enhance executive functioning, the effects are modulated by task and age. This may arise because using multiple languages has a highly specific effect on executive functioning which is only observable in older, relative to younger, adults. The finding that publication bias was not uniformly detected across the different methods raises questions about the impact that unpublished (or undetected) studies have on meta-analyses of this literature.

Introduction

An issue of ongoing debate concerns whether bilingualism enhances cognitive functions (Bialystok and Martin, 2004; Bialystok, 2011; Paap and Greenberg, 2013; Paap and Sawi, 2014; Pelham and Abrams, 2014), particularly, executive functioning (Bialystok, 1999, 2011; Bialystok et al., 2005; Poulin-Dubois et al., 2011; Marzecová et al., 2013a,b; Pelham and Abrams, 2014; Antón et al., 2016). This has been examined in over one-hundred studies (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Paap, 2019) and has been the subject of several narrative (Hilchey and Klein, 2011; Hilchey et al., 2015; Paap et al., 2015) and quantitative literature reviews (Adesope et al., 2010; Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019). In this report, meta-analysis was used to examine three outstanding issues. The first was whether the task used to assess executive functioning might influence the extent to which the bilingual advantage is observed. The second was whether the magnitude of the bilingual advantage changes across the lifespan. The third was whether the bilingual advantage might be attributed to publication bias (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019).

It has been proposed that learning and using multiple languages enhances executive functioning (Bialystok, 1999, 2010, 2011; Bialystok and Martin, 2004; Carlson and Meltzoff, 2008; Bialystok and Viswanathan, 2009; Bialystok et al., 2010; Prior and MacWhinney, 2010). According to one view, in order to use multiple languages, one needs to inhibit the non-target language and then select the target language (Marian and Spivey, 2003; Marian et al., 2003; Green and Abutalebi, 2013). This constant switching and selection of different languages has been proposed to enhance executive functioning, even for non-linguistic tasks (e.g., Green, 1998; Bialystok, 1999, 2010, 2011).

Past Reviews Examining the “Bilingual Advantage” for Executive Functioning

A large number of studies have been published examining whether a bilingual advantage exists for performance on executive functioning tasks (e.g., Hernandez et al., 2010; Antón et al., 2014; von Bastian et al., 2016). Furthermore, this literature has since been the subject of several reviews (Adesope et al., 2010; Hilchey and Klein, 2011; Hilchey et al., 2015; Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019). Interestingly, these reviews have not reached the same conclusion.

Hilchey and Klein's (2011) systematic review initially found evidence for a bilingual advantage. This review summarized the results of 13 studies comprising adult and pediatric samples on three executive functioning tasks. These were the Simon, Flanker, and Attentional Network Tasks. A synthesis of individual study results revealed bilinguals had faster reaction times on these tasks. However, across the studies, the bilinguals did not consistently show superior performance on the components of these tasks that assess executive functioning. Thus, these findings suggest that bilingualism may improve general processing speeds rather than executive functioning.

In another review, evidence was found that bilingualism is associated with superior executive functioning. Adesope et al. (2010) used meta-analysis to examine whether there were differences between bilinguals and monolinguals with respect to meta-linguistic awareness, meta-cognitive awareness, working memory, abstract and symbolic reasoning, attentional control and problem solving. A total of 63 studies were included in this review. Relevant to the current report were analyses examining attentional control, as this construct aligns closely with executive functioning. Results of the meta-analysis revealed a bilingual advantage with respect to metalinguistic awareness (Hedges' g = 0.33, p < 0.01), working memory (Hedges' g = 0.48, p < 0.01), and abstract and symbolic reasoning (Hedges' g = 0.57, p < 0.01). However, the largest difference favoring bilinguals, was observed on the meta-analyses examining attentional control (Hedges' g = 0.96, p < 0.01). This pattern of results suggests a bilingual advantage that is most pronounced in the area of executive functioning.

Adesope et al. (2010) also examined whether the magnitude of the bilingual advantage was influenced by publication bias. Publication bias describes the occurrence whereby studies with positive or statistically significant findings are more likely to be published than those with non-significant findings (Easterbrook et al., 1991). In meta-analysis, this can lead to an overestimation of the “true” or “population” effect size, since only studies with significant findings are identified and included in the analysis. This issue has been raised as a concern in the bilingual advantage literature (de Bruin et al., 2015b). Adesope et al. examined this issue by assessing the number of un-retrieved or unidentified studies required to overturn their significant results. This was achieved by computing the Classic Fail-Safe N and Orwin's Fail-Safe N and then, comparing these values to the 5k + 10 guideline, where k denotes the number of effect sizes or studies included in the meta-analysis (Rosenthal, 1979). Using this approach, both Fail-Safe N values exceeded the 5k + 10 critical value. It was therefore suggested that publication bias was unlikely to be influencing the meta-analysis findings.

A systematic review by van den Noort et al. (2019) also reported a bilingual advantage in executive functioning. This review examined the results of 46 studies comparing bilingual and monolingual children and adults, on tasks assessing executive functioning. To synthesize the results from this literature, the authors summed the number of studies that reported a significant bilingual advantage. Using this approach, 54.3% of the studies reported that a bilingual advantage was present. It was also noted that 56.4% of studies reporting a significant bilingual advantage comprised adult samples, whereas 42.8% comprised pediatric samples. Differences in brain structure between adults and children were proposed to explain this trend in the literature. The suggestion was that in children, the regions of the brain that support executive functioning are still developing. As such, the effect of bilingualism on executive functioning may also be more variable during childhood compared to adults. Publication bias was not assessed in this review.

Reviews have also emerged questioning the magnitude and existence of the bilingual advantage. Donnelly et al. (2019) undertook a meta-analysis summarizing the results of 80 studies that compared executive functioning in bilingual and monolingual individuals. Initially a significant bilingual advantage was found, however, the observed effect size was small (Hedges' g = 0.11, p = 0.007). Publication bias was first assessed using Egger's Test of Asymmetry (Egger et al., 1997). Using this approach, publication bias was not found to be present. However, when the effect size was corrected for publication bias using the PET-PEESE model (Stanley and Doucouliagos, 2014), the bilingual advantage was no longer found to be significant.

Donnelly et al. (2019) also undertook a series of moderator analyses to examine potential variables influencing study level effect sizes. One moderator analysis examined whether average effect sizes differed between studies undertaken with children (participants aged 13-years or younger), younger adults (participants aged between of 15–40 years), and older adults (participants aged 60-years or older). However, no effect of age on effect sizes was found. Another moderator analysis examined whether effect sizes differed between the tasks used to examine executive functioning in the included studies (i.e., Flanker, Simon, Stroop, and Attentional Network Tasks). Again, for this analysis, task type was not found to moderate effect sizes.

Similar findings were reported by Lehtonen et al. (2018) who undertook a meta-analysis summarizing the results of 152 published and unpublished studies comparing bilingual and monolingual adults on executive functioning tasks. Initially the meta-analysis revealed a significant bilingual advantage, but the effect size was small in magnitude (Hedges' g = 0.06, p < 0.05). However, after correcting for publication bias using the PET-PEESE model (Stanley and Doucouliagos, 2014), the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals was no longer significant. (Hedges' g = −0.08, p = 0.099). Further analyses revealed that task and age were not significant moderators of study level effect sizes. The key finding from this review is that the bilingual advantage may be an artifact of publication bias.

Paap (2019) also presented evidence suggesting the bilingual advantage may be attributed to publication bias. This review summarized data from 109 studies. The included studies measured executive functioning using either the Stroop, Simon, Flanker, or Attentional Network Tasks. In one analysis examining differences in executive functioning, a bilingual advantage was initially found (Hedges' g = 0.11, p < 0.001). However, after correcting for publication bias using the PET-PEESE model, the average effect size approached zero and the difference between the two language groups was no longer significant (Hedges' g = −0.02, p = 0.708).

The Current Meta-Analysis

Results from multiple meta-analyses are yet to determine the effect of bilingualism on executive functioning. In this review, 170 published and unpublished studies were identified to examine three outstanding issues. The first concerns whether the type of task used to measure executive functioning moderates the bilingual advantage effect. This may be expected given that the types of task used in this literature to measure this aspect of cognitive functioning are not highly correlated. For example, the correlation between the Simon and Flanker tasks has been found to be low (r = 0.14) and non-significant (Keye et al., 2009; Paap and Greenberg, 2013). Additionally, the correlations between the Flanker, Simon and Stroop tasks have been found to be less than 0.2 and non-significant (Stins et al., 2005). Based on these correlations it is possible to perform well on one task but not the other.

It is still unclear whether the bilingual advantage is task specific. This issue is important to address since in three previous meta-analyses, effect sizes from different tasks were combined in order to compute an overall average (Adesope et al., 2010; Hilchey and Klein, 2011; Hilchey et al., 2015). One potential problem with this approach is that it assumes different executive functioning tasks assess the same cognitive and/or neural processes. However, this may not be the case.

The second issue addressed in the current report concerns whether the age of the participants is moderating effect sizes. In one review (van den Noort et al., 2019), the bilingual advantage was found to be more commonly reported in adults compared to children. However, this result has not always been found (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019) and in two reviews (Adesope et al., 2010; Hilchey and Klein, 2011), this variable was not examined as a moderator. There is still inconsistency regarding the effects of age on the magnitude of the bilingual advantage.

A third issue concerns whether the bilingual advantage is an artifact of publication bias. In three meta-analyses (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019), evidence was presented suggesting this was the case. However, not all meta-analyses have found evidence to support this claim (Adesope et al., 2010). It is interesting to note that the meta-analyses which found evidence of publication bias used the PET-PEESE model. Meta-analyses that did not find evidence of publication bias used the Fail-Safe N, Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill method or Egger's Test of Asymmetry.

Quantifying and correcting the impact of publication bias in meta-analysis is problematic. This is because there is no universally accepted method that achieves this goal (Peters et al., 2007; Idris and Ruzni, 2012; Gervais, 2015). One noted problem with correcting for publication bias using PET-PEESE is that this approach may overestimate the effect of publication bias (Gervais, 2015). The opposite appears to be the case for other methods such as the Fail-Safe N, Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill or Egger's Test of Asymmetry (Peters et al., 2007). A stronger case for the argument that the bilingual advantage solely reflects publication bias could be made if this type of bias was present using different assessment methods.

Objectives of the Current Report

This report examined whether a bilingual advantage exists with respect to executive functioning. Data from 170 studies comparing monolingual and bilingual individuals on commonly used tests of executive functioning were summarized using meta-analysis. The primary objectives of this report were to examine whether (i) the type of task used to assess executive functioning influenced the effect size measuring differences between bilinguals and monolinguals, (ii) whether the age of participants influenced effect sizes and, (iii) whether detecting publication bias depends on the type of method used in this literature.

Methods

Study Design

This report included both published and unpublished studies. Published studies were identified by searching the following electronic databases: Psychological Information Database (PsycINFO), Ageline, Medical Literature Analysis and Retrieval System Online (MEDLINE), Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), Education Resources Information Center (ERIC), and Excerpta Medical Database (EMBASE). The search strategy used to identify relevant studies involved searching titles and abstracts for records containing synonyms for “bilingualism” and “executive functioning.” The complete search syntax used to search each database is available for download via the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/ykp5w/?view_only=1d90f8c86146466e8dcfb6c4d23a334f).

The first search was executed in January, 2017. An additional search was undertaken in March, 2018 and then again in December, 2019 to ensure that any records published since the initial search were also included in the meta-analysis. Unpublished studies were found by searching the reference lists from three recent reviews (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019). This method was chosen to identify unpublished literature as there is no consensus about which databases used to search for gray literature. In this review we also wanted to ensure that the analyses of unpublished literature would include the same studies used in past meta-analysis, which was achieved using this method.

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included in this review if they met the following criteria. First, participants in each study needed to be healthy adults or children. Studies undertaken with clinical samples (e.g., ADHD or ASD) were excluded. In cases where a study presented data from both clinical and healthy samples (e.g., Mor et al., 2015), only data from healthy participants were extracted. Second, the study needed to compare a group comprising bilingual participants to a group comprising monolingual participants on an executive functioning task. Studies presenting data from only bidialectal (Antoniou et al., 2016; Wu et al., 2016; Scaltritti et al., 2017) or trilingual groups were excluded. Where a study included both bilingual and trilingual/bidialectal samples, only data from bilingual participants were extracted. This criterion aimed to find studies with fairly homogenous participants with respect to language functioning.

Third, the study needed to have measured executive functioning using either the Stroop Task, Simon Task, Attentional Network Task, Flanker Task, Trail Making Test, Task-Switching Paradigms, and/or Card Sort Tasks. These tasks were identified through a process using studies from the first search. The tasks used in each of these studies were noted. After examination, the authors decided that any task used in less than seven publications was to be excluded. The goal here was to reduce variability between study findings due to methodological differences. Also, the tasks included in this meta-analysis have been found to be valid measures of executive functioning (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974; MacLeod, 1991; Rubinstein et al., 2001; Zelazo, 2006; Hommel, 2011; Salthouse, 2011). In this meta-analysis, average effect sizes (described below) were computed separately for each of these tasks. Studies that used non-standard versions of the aforementioned tasks (Coderre et al., 2013; Calvo and Bialystok, 2014) were excluded. Details on task exclusion criteria and an overview of each task are available for download via the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/ykp5w/?view_only=1d90f8c86146466e8dcfb6c4d23a334f).

Study Selection

The first search (completed in January, 2017) led to the identification of 81 studies that met the aforementioned criteria. The second search (completed in March, 2018) led to the identification of an additional 16 studies, and the third search (completed in December, 2019), an additional 14 studies. Finally, 59 unpublished studies were identified from the reference lists of three recent relevant reviews (Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019; Paap, 2019). A total of 170 studies were included in the meta-analyses. Figures 13 present PRISMA flow charts detailing the screening process of articles obtained from the first, second and third round of searches respectively.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart detailing the first search of the literature undertaken in January, 2017.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flowchart detailing the second search of the literature undertaken in March, 2018.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. PRISMA flowchart detailing the third search of the literature undertaken in December, 2019.

Effect Size Calculations and Data Extraction Procedures

Data was extracted from each study so that Hedges' g (Hedges and Olkin, 1985) could be computed. Hedges' g measures differences between groups in standard deviation units, adjusted for sample size. For each study, data were extracted in order to compute effect sizes for the Stroop Task, Simon Task, Attentional Network Task, Flanker Task, Trail Making Test, Task-Switching Paradigms, and Card Sort Tasks. It was common for studies to quantify performance using accuracy, reaction time, and/or a measure of an interference effect. Effect sizes were computed separately for each of these variables.

For each task, data were extracted so that separate effect sizes could be obtained for incongruent and congruent trials, as well as for an interference effect. Congruent trials place the least demands on executive functioning processes. Performance on these trials has been interpreted to reflect general processing speed (Bialystok et al., 2005; Hilchey and Klein, 2011). Incongruent trials place greater demands on executive functioning processes, as one is required to inhibit a response in favor of another (Miyake et al., 2000; Nigg, 2000). For example, on the Stroop task, participants are asked to state the color of the font of a presented word. Congruent trials are those where the word and the color in which the word is presented, are the same; the word “GREEN” presented in green ink. Conversely, incongruent trials involve a mismatch between the word, and its color. For example, the word “GREEN” presented in red ink. In this literature, an additional measure used to examine executive functioning processes involves computing the difference in response times between congruent and incongruent trials. This variable measures the increased cognitive demands required to resolve incongruent, relative to congruent, trials. On this variable, smaller values indicate that task is placing fewer demands on executive functioning processes. This variable is referred to as the interference effect in this report.

Since the focus of this report was to examine the effects of bilingualism on executive functioning, effect sizes from incongruent trials and interference scores were primarily of interest. However, for completeness, data were also extracted to compute effect sizes summarizing performance on congruent trials as well. For congruent trials, incongruent trials, and the interference effect, positive Hedges' g values indicate superior performance by the bilingual group. Effect sizes for these variables were computed separately for accuracy and reaction time data.

Each study effect size was also coded based on the age of the sample from the study. Effect sizes extracted from studies where the mean age of the sample was less than 18-years, were coded as representing a “Child” sample. We did not examine child and adolescent samples separately. This was because only one study was identified in our search, that was undertaken with an adolescent sample. All other pediatric samples were undertaken with participants who were under the age of 13-years. Data extracted from experiments where the mean sample age was between 18 and 29 were coded as “Young Adult.” “Adult” samples comprised effect sizes from experiments where the mean age of the sample was between 30 and 49 years of age. Finally, effect sizes extracted from studies where participants were 50-years of age or older were coded as “Older Adults.” Study level effect sizes used in the meta-analyses are available for download via the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/ykp5w/?view_only=1d90f8c86146466e8dcfb6c4d23a334f).

Meta-Analytic Procedures

Averaging Effect Sizes

Meta-analyses were undertaken to examine whether (i) the difference between bilingual and monolingual groups varied between executive functioning tasks, and, (ii) if the average effect size between bilingual and monolingual groups varied in magnitude by age group.

To examine the effect of task on the bilingual advantage, the average effect size was computed separately for each of the studied tasks. That is, meta-analyses were conducted separately for the Stroop Task, Simon Task, Attentional Network Task, Flanker Task, Trail Making Test, Task-Switching Paradigms, and Card Sort Tasks. For all meta-analyses, study level effect sizes were averaged using a random effects model (Hedges and Olkin, 1985). Effect sizes were averaged separately for incongruent and congruent trials and for accuracy, reaction time, and the interference effect variable.

Two approaches were used to examine whether the bilingual advantage differed between tasks. First, for each task and variable (i.e., congruent trials, incongruent trials, interference effect), we examined if the average effect size was significant. For these and all analyses, alpha was set at 0.05. Second, mixed-effects subgroup analyses (Borenstein et al., 2011) tested whether effect sizes for each variable was significantly different between the tasks. A similar approach was used to examine whether effect sizes varied across the different age bands.

Publication Bias

Multiple methods were used to detect publication bias in this report. Specifically, publication bias was assessed using Egger's Test of Asymmetry, Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill method, Classic Fail-Safe N, and PET-PEESE. The rationale for using a wide range of bias tests was to see whether findings of publication bias were specific to one test, or present across multiple tests. This was important to determine since no consensus has been reached regarding the most accurate method to detect publication bias (Peters et al., 2007; Idris and Ruzni, 2012; Gervais, 2015). Additionally, we empirically quantified the effect of publication bias by computing the average effect size for all variables separately, for published and unpublished studies. Mixed-effects analyses were undertaken to compare whether the average effect sizes from published and unpublished studies were significantly different. This approach served as a benchmark from which to compare the results from the aforementioned methods for assessing publication bias which are based on a theoretical distribution of effect sizes.

Egger's Test of Asymmetry

Egger's Test of Asymmetry assesses for publication bias using the properties of funnel plots. Funnel plots show the association between study level effect sizes and standard error (Egger et al., 1997). When publication bias is absent, effect sizes are symmetrically distributed around the average of the effect sizes. When publication bias is present, the funnel plot has an asymmetrical shape. This asymmetry is formally tested using Egger's Test of Asymmetry. When this test returns a significant result, publication bias may be present.

Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill

Duval and Tweedie's (2000) Trim and Fill method uses the properties of the funnel plot to correct for publication bias. In this method, missing effect sizes that give rise to an asymmetrical funnel plot are imputed. In the current report, missing studies were assumed to have negative effect size values. That is, missing effect sizes were assumed to show that monolinguals performed better than bilinguals on a given variable. An imputed average effect size can then be compared with the observed average effect size. A limitation of this approach is that it often underestimates the extent of bias and can be too lenient in the correction (Idris and Ruzni, 2012).

Classic Fail-Safe N

The Classic Fail-Safe N estimates the number of hypothetical un-retrieved studies required to return a significant average effect size to non-significance. When the Fail-Safe N is low, publication bias is possible. This is because the observed result in a meta-analysis could be overturned by a small number of unpublished or un-retrieved studies that were missed during the search process. When the Fail-Safe N value is high, publication bias is unlikely. Rosenthal (1979) suggested that when the Fail-Safe N value is greater than 5k + 10, where k is the number of studies (or effect sizes) included in the meta-analysis, publication bias is unlikely. This approach was used in the current report.

PET-PEESE

The PET-PEESE method (Stanley and Doucouliagos, 2014) is a relatively more recent approach for assessing publication bias. This method uses weighted least squares regression to predict individual study effect sizes from their standard error. In this model, the intercept is considered to represent the average effect size after controlling for potential publication bias. One criticism of the PET-PEESE model is that it overestimates the effect of publication bias on the average effect size (Gervais, 2015).

Results

Results from the meta-analyses are presented in three sections. The first section presents analyses examining whether the bilingual advantage is more likely to be observed on some executive functioning tasks over others. The second section presents analyses examining whether effect sizes vary significantly between child, young adult, adult and older adult samples. The third section presents results from the assessment of publication bias.

Is the Bilingual Advantage Task Dependent?

Results from the meta-analyses examining differences between bilingual and monolingual participants for the studied executive functioning tasks are presented in Figure 4. This figure shows the averaged effect sizes reported by task.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Average effect sizes reported by Task, Trial Type, and for accuracy and reaction time data. No interference effect/cost effect sizes were available for the Card Sort Tasks and Trail Making Test. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.

Results from the meta-analyses summarizing performance from the different tasks on the incongruent trials are considered first. With respect to accuracy data, averaged effect sizes ranged from −0.282 to 0.485. Effect sizes for four out of the seven meta-analyses were significant. These were from the Attentional Network Task, Stroop Task, Task-Switching Paradigms, and Card Sort Tasks. For these tasks, the effect sizes ranged from small to medium and were positive. This indicates that bilinguals were more accurate than monolinguals on incongruent trials for these tasks. Average effect sizes for the other tasks were non-significant.

Mixed-effects subgroup analysis revealed that the effect sizes for accuracy on incongruent trials varied significantly by task (p < 0.001). This result indicates that the type of task used to assess executive functioning influenced the magnitude of the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals. To explore this result further, mixed-effects subgroup analyses were conducted to examine differences in pairs of effect sizes. Results from these comparisons are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. p-values from subgroup analyses comparing pairs of accuracy effect sizes from incongruent trials.

Table 1 shows that the average effect size for Card Sort Tasks was significantly larger than the effect sizes for the Attentional Network Task, Simon Task and Trail Making Test. Effect sizes from the Stroop Task, Simon Task, Attentional Network Task, and Task-Switching paradigms were significantly larger than the effect size for the Trail Making Test. Results from all other comparisons were not statistically significant.

Effect sizes from reaction times on incongruent trials ranged from 0.039 to 0.336. On four tasks, the Attentional Network Task, Simon Task, Flanker Task, and the Trail Making Test, the average effect size was positive and significant, indicating a bilingual advantage. On these tasks, the average finding in the literature is that bilinguals respond faster than monolinguals on incongruent trials. All other analyses were not significant.

Mixed-effects subgroup analysis comparing differences in reaction time effect sizes from incongruent trials was not significant (p = 0.183). This result indicates that the task used did not influence the magnitude of the difference between the groups on this variable.

Analyses examining the influence of executive functioning tasks on effect sizes from congruent trials are now considered. For accuracy data, effect sizes ranged from −0.170 to 0.178. On the Attentional Network Task, the average effect size was positive and significant, although small in magnitude. This result indicates that for congruent trials on this task, bilinguals are more accurate than monolinguals. There were no other significant results from this set of analyses.

Mixed-effects subgroup analyses comparing differences in these task-level average effect sizes were not significant (p = 0.727). Thus, for accuracy data obtained from congruent trials, the type of task used was not influencing effect sizes.

Effect sizes for reaction time data collected from congruent trials ranged from −0.198 to 0.278. The average effect sizes from the Trail Making Test, Attentional Network Task, and Simon Task were all positive and significant, though small. This result indicates that for these tasks on congruent trials, the average finding is that bilinguals have faster reaction times compared to monolinguals. All other comparisons were non-significant.

Mixed-effects subgroup analyses indicated that task influenced the magnitude of the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals (p = 0.014) with respect to reaction time on congruent trials. Results from pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 2. The average effect sizes from the Simon Task, Attentional Network Task, and Trail Making Test were significantly larger than the effect size from the Stroop Task. Additionally, effect sizes from the Flanker, Simon, Attentional Network, and Trail Making tasks were significantly larger than the effect size for Task-Switching Paradigms.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. p-values from subgroup analyses comparing pairs of reaction time effect sizes from congruent trials.

Finally, analyses examining the influence of task type on effect sizes for interference effects are presented. The effect sizes for these meta-analyses ranged from 0.081 to 0.693. For three tasks, the Attentional Network Task, Stroop Task, and Task-Switching Paradigms, the average effect sizes were significant and positive. This suggests that on average, on these tasks, bilinguals display a smaller interference effect relative to monolinguals. The effect sizes for the Flanker and Simon tasks were small and non-significant. Mixed-effect subgroup analyses indicated that task influenced the magnitude of the difference between the groups (p = 0.022). Results from pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 3. The average effect sizes for the Attentional Network Task and Task-Switching Paradigms were significantly larger than the effect sizes for the Stroop, Simon, and Flanker tasks.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. p-values from subgroup analyses comparing pairs of effect sizes from the interference effect.

The bilingual advantage across separate tasks: An interim Summary. As noted earlier, positive effect sizes from incongruent trials and the interference effect indicate a bilingual advantage for executive functioning. The findings reported above indicate superior executive functioning in bilinguals that is consistently observed on the Attentional Network Task. A bilingual advantage was not consistently found on the other examined tasks. Subgroup analyses revealed that task type moderated effect sizes. Thus, the bilingual advantage does not appear to be present on all tasks.

Is the Bilingual Advantage Dependent on Age?

Figure 5 shows the results from the meta-analyses examining differences between bilinguals and monolinguals on congruent trials, incongruent trials, and the interference effect. Results are reported separately for each age group.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Average effect sizes reported by Age Group, Trial Type, and for accuracy and reaction time data. No interference effect, effect sizes were available for the Card Sort Tasks and Trail Making Test. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.

Meta-analyses conducted for accuracy on incongruent trials will be discussed first. On this variable the average effect size ranged from 0.103 to 0.314. The small effect sizes for adult, young adult and child samples were significant and positive. This suggests that for these age groups, bilinguals are more accurate than monolinguals on incongruent trials across the included tasks. Mixed-effect subgroup analysis revealed no significant difference in the averaged effect sizes between each age group (p = 0.529).

The effect sizes from reaction times for the different age bands on incongruent trials ranged from 0.120 to 0.491. The average effect sizes were significant for child, adult, and older adult samples. This suggests that on average, bilinguals in these age groups are faster than monolinguals on incongruent trials. Mixed-effect subgroup analysis revealed significant differences in the age-level effect sizes (p = 0.043). Results from pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 4. These analyses indicated that the average effect size from young adults was significantly smaller than the effect size from older adult samples.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. p-values from subgroup analyses comparing pairs of reaction time effect sizes from incongruent trials, by age.

The meta-analyses conducted for accuracy on congruent trials revealed effect sizes ranging from −0.341 to 0.247. A significant, small, effect size was observed for child samples. This indicates that the average finding for this group is that bilingual children are more accurate than monolingual children on congruent trials. Effect sizes for other age groups were small and non-significant. Mixed-effect subgroup analysis did not indicate significant differences in the effect sizes between the age groups (p = 0.293).

Average effect sizes for reaction time on congruent trials ranged from 0.187 to 0.309. Significant effect sizes were observed for adult and child samples, indicating that on average, bilinguals in these age groups were faster than monolinguals on congruent trials. There were no significant differences between bilinguals and monolinguals in the other two age samples. Mixed-effects subgroup analysis revealed no significant differences between the age group effect sizes (p = 0.268).

Last, the effect sizes for the interference effect ranged from 0.121 to 0.441. Positive, significant effect sizes were observed for the older adult, adult, and young adult samples suggesting that bilinguals in this age group have smaller interference effects than their monolingual peers. The effect size for child samples was comparatively smaller and non-significant. There were no significant differences between the age group level effect sizes (p = 0.293).

The bilingual advantage across age groups: An interim Summary. The main result to emerge from the above meta-analyses is that a significant bilingual advantage on variables assessing executive functioning was more likely to be observed in studies undertaken with older participants. In one analysis, age was moderating effect sizes. Typically, effect sizes from older adult samples were significantly larger than those from younger age groups.

Evaluation of Publication Bias

Publication bias was separately examined for effect sizes computed from incongruent and congruent trials, both accuracy and reaction time, as well as for the interference effect. For each of these variables, effect sizes were averaged using a random effects model. These weighted average effect sizes are presented in Table 5 under “Observed Effect Size.” For these effect sizes, significant positive values were observed for four out of five variables, indicating a bilingual advantage.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results from Analyses of Publication Bias.

Also presented in Table 5 are the average effect sizes for each variable computed separately for published and unpublished studies. These effect sizes were averaged using a random effects model. Generally, effect sizes for published studies were larger compared to unpublished studies, but the magnitude of the difference varied between the variables. For example, the effect size from published studies computed using accuracy data from incongruent trials was 0.063 points larger than the corresponding effect size for unpublished studies. The effect size for the interference effect was 0.242 points larger for published studies compared to unpublished studies. Mixed-effects analysis was used to formally test whether there were significant differences between the effect sizes from published and unpublished studies. There was no significant difference between published and unpublished average effect sizes for accuracy on incongruent (p = 0.566) or congruent trials (p = 0.839). However, effect sizes from published studies were found to be significantly larger for reaction times on both incongruent (p = 0.013) and congruent trials (p = 0.007), as well as for the interference effect (p = 0.004). These analyses indicate that based on the studies included in this review, published studies had a larger effect size than unpublished studies for three out of the five variables.

Publication bias for each variable was assessed using the Classic Fail-Safe N, Egger's Test of Asymmetry, Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill method, and PET-PEESE. Results from each of these methods are presented in Table 5. The Fail-Safe N value indicated publication bias was unlikely. Rosenthal (1979) suggested that if the Fail-Safe N value was greater than 5k + 10, then it is unlikely that a significant meta-analysis result was due to publication bias. For all variables, the Fail-Safe N values were greater than the critical values estimated by the formula. However, Eggers Test of Asymmetry, indicated that publication bias was likely for four out of five of the variables.

The tests that both detect publication bias and estimate a corrected effect size, returned contradictory results. The imputed average effect size using Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill method indicated publication bias was unlikely. For all variables, the imputed average effect size was equal to the observed value. It should be noted this result was obtained by assuming missing studies all reported negative effect sizes (indicating a monolingual advantage). Conversely, results from PET-PEESE indicated publication bias was likely. This estimate for all variables was negative and significant, indicating a monolingual advantage. The results of the PET-PEESE analysis indicate the bilingual advantage is an artifact of publication bias.

Discussion

This report used meta-analysis to examine the extent task and age influenced the bilingual advantage with respect to executive functioning. To our knowledge, this is the largest meta-analysis undertaken on this topic to date, in terms of the number of studies reviewed (k = 170). Three main findings emerged from this review. First, there is evidence to suggest that bilinguals may outperform monolinguals on some, but not all, executive functioning tasks. Second, a bilingual advantage in executive functioning was more likely to be present for studies undertaken with adult and older participants (i.e., aged over 30-years). Third, publication bias may be leading to an overestimation of effect sizes in meta-analyses examining the bilingual advantage, however, the extent of this overestimation is unclear.

The Bilingual Advantage Appears to Be Task-Dependent

The results of the meta-analyses indicate that the bilingual advantage is moderated by the type of task used to assess executive functioning. Also, in this report, a bilingual advantage was consistently observed on the Attentional Network Task. This effect was found across all studied variables (i.e., congruent trials, incongruent trials, interference effect). For other tasks, a bilingual advantage was not consistently found on all variables. Based on this set of results, if bilingualism improves executive functioning, it appears to be most pronounced on the processes assessed by the Attentional Network Task.

It is interesting to note that while the bilingual advantage was present on the Attentional Network Task, the effect was generally absent for the Flanker Task. This result was surprising given the similarity between both tasks. Both require participants to respond to the direction of a center arrow and ignore two flanking arrows on either side. Also, both comprise congruent trials, where the flanking and center arrows are pointing in the same direction, and incongruent trials, where the flanking arrows are pointing in a different direction to the center arrow.

However, there is a methodological difference relating to the presentation of visual stimuli on the Attentional Network and Flanker Tasks which may provide new insights into the effects of bilingualism on cognitive functioning. On the Flanker Task, stimuli are typically presented in the center of the screen, following a fixation cross. However, on the Attentional Network Task, stimuli are presented either above or below the previous location of the central fixation cross. This particular feature of the Attentional Network Task is included as a measure of attention, as participants must orient their attention to the stimuli before responding (Fan et al., 2002). In this report, the meta-analyses indicated a bilingual advantage across all variables of the Attentional Network Task but not the Flanker Task. Thus, there may be a bilingual advantage which is specific to attention. That is, bilinguals are able to orient their attention to the stimuli faster than monolinguals. This may explain why the results of the meta-analyses indicated that, on average, studies reported that bilinguals outperformed monolinguals on congruent trials as well as incongruent trials and interference effects on the Attentional Network Task.

There are at least two explanations to account for our finding that the cognitive benefits of bilingualism vary across executive functioning tasks. One explanation is that bilingualism influences a specific neural network or set of cognitive processes that is not activated (or required) in all executive functioning tasks. Consequently, differences between bilingual and monolingual groups are only present on tasks that activate the neural networks or cognitive processes that are enhanced by bilingualism. For example, one mixed-effects subgroup analysis showed effect sizes observed for the Simon Task were significantly smaller than those from the Stroop Task. There is evidence to suggest that even though both tasks are valid measures of executive functioning, they elicit a different pattern of neural activation. Peterson et al. (2002) found that similar brain regions were involved in the Stroop and Simon tasks, however there was variation in the level of brain activation between the tasks. For example, both tasks led to increased activation in the inferior frontal, parietal, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices and the anterior cingulate and supplementary motor area. This reflects that both tap into parts of the brain that support executive functioning (e.g., Curtis and D'Esposito, 2003; Kondo et al., 2004; Obeso et al., 2013). However, the Simon Task led to greater levels of activation in the superior and inferior regions of the parietal lobe. These regions of the brain support the allocation of attention (Behrmann et al., 2004). Thus, it could be that bilingualism has an effect that is region- or network-specific.

Secondly, the psychometric properties of executive functioning tasks might also explain the results of this meta-analysis. Differences in task reliability could lead to the same pattern of results observed in this report. This is because poor reliability attenuates effect sizes (Baugh, 2002; Paap, 2019). As the measurement error of a task increases (i.e., the reliability decreases), the effect size decreases. Differences in study level effect sizes may, in part, reflect differences in task reliability. For example, the Stroop Task may have had higher reliability than the Simon Task in the studies comparing bilingual and monolingual participants. In meta-analysis it is possible to correct average effect sizes for the impact of poor reliability (Baugh, 2002; Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). However, this correction requires knowing the reliability associated with the task being used in a study. In the studies reviewed for our meta-analyses, reliability of the executive functioning tasks was not reported. If reliabilities are reported in future research, the impact of measurement error on effect sizes can be estimated.

The Bilingual Advantage Is Dependent on Age

A potentially novel finding in this report was that the bilingual advantage was consistently observed in samples aged between 30 and 49 years. For this age group, a bilingual advantage was observed on four variables (accuracy and reaction time on incongruent trials, reaction times on congruent trials and interference effect). Although the magnitude of the advantage was not significantly larger than in other age groups, this trend hints at a bilingual advantage that may be more consistent in studies undertaken with participants aged between 30 and 49 years. A second novel finding with respect to age was that the magnitude of the bilingual advantage for response times on incongruent trials was significantly larger in samples aged 50-years and above, compared to samples aged between 18 and 29 years of age. This suggests that the bilingual advantage may be larger in studies undertaken with older samples, compared to young adults.

Our finding that the age of participants influences effect sizes is not consistent with previous meta-analyses, in which age was not found to be a significant moderator of the bilingual advantage (e.g., Lehtonen et al., 2018; Donnelly et al., 2019). One explanation for this discrepancy between our review and others is the method used to correct for publication bias. Both Lehtonen et al. and Donnelly et al. used the PET-PEESE method to correct effect sizes before interpreting results. The PET-PEESE approach has been criticized for overestimating the effect of publication bias (Gervais, 2015). This issue is discussed in the next section (Publication Bias and the Bilingual Advantage) in detail. Using this method may have potentially masked age-related bilingual advantage differences.

If the bilingual advantage is greater in older samples, why might this be so? It has been documented that executive functioning often declines with age (Ardila and Rosselli, 1989; Parkin and Lawrence, 1994; Lustig et al., 2001; Bowles and Salthouse, 2003). One suggestion is that bilingualism reduces the effects of cognitive decline. That is, bilingualism may help protect against the decline of executive functions (Lee Salvatierra and Rosselli, 2011; Schroeder and Marian, 2012). One idea is that using multiple languages places constant demands on executive functioning which in turn, reduces the impact of age-related brain changes on this aspect of cognitive functioning (Fiszer, 2008). In older age groups, bilinguals may evidence superior executive functioning skills compared to monolinguals because this skill is relatively more preserved. In younger age groups, the bilingual advantage in executive functioning may be small or absent. This is because in younger age groups, executive functioning processes are already at their peak level of performance. Ceiling effects in executive functioning in younger age groups may lead to non-significant differences between bilinguals and monolinguals.

Publication Bias and the Bilingual Advantage

A final issue addressed in this report is the extent to which the bilingual advantage can be considered an artifact of publication bias. The potential effect of publication bias on reviews examining the bilingual advantage was demonstrated by de Bruin et al. (2015a). In this study, authors examined conference abstracts from 1999 to 2012 and found that those reporting a bilingual advantage were more likely to be published. Meta-analyses have also suggested that the bilingual advantage may be an artifact of publication bias. Both Donnelly et al. (2019) and Paap (2019), initially reported a small bilingual advantage which became non-significant after using the PET-PEESE method to correct for publication bias.

The analyses of publication bias presented in this report questions whether the bilingual advantage can be completely attributed to publication bias. In this review, both the PET-PEESE method and Egger's Test of Asymmetry suggest that publication bias is present. A key issue emerging from our analyses is that it is unclear how much this publication bias is inflating the average effect sizes in meta-analyses examining the bilingual advantage. After adjusting the average effect size for publication bias using Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill method, a reliable bilingual advantage was still found. However, using the PET-PEESE approach the bilingual advantage was no longer present. An outstanding question to be addressed from our analyses is which approach most accurately corrects the observed effect size. This is an issue that will need to be addressed in future research evaluating meta-analysis methods.

However, in the context of the bilingual advantage, we tentatively suggest that the PET-PEESE method might be overestimating the effect of publication bias. In this report we computed average effect sizes separately for published and unpublished studies (See Table 5). It is noted that in all cases, the effect sizes for published studies were larger compared to unpublished. Across the studied variables, the difference in average effect sizes between published and unpublished studies ranged between 0.024 and 0.307. However, using the PET-PEESE method, the observed average effect size was reduced by between 0.110 and 0.681. This reduction is substantially larger than the difference we observed between published and unpublished studies. Therefore, we somewhat align our findings with the suggestion by Donnelly et al. (2019) that the magnitude of the advantage has been “overstated.” We do suggest, however, that it is not entirely clear whether publication bias completely explains the tendency for bilinguals to perform better on some cognitive tasks than monolinguals. Thus, research investigating how learning and using multiple languages affects cognitive functioning outside the linguistic domain is still needed.

Limitations

In this report we were not able to examine other moderating variables that may also influence effect sizes. For instance, there are varying dimensions of bilingualism that are potential moderators of the bilingual advantage. These may include differences in language proficiency, age of acquisition and the context of language use (Green and Abutalebi, 2013). These variables were not examined systematically in this meta-analysis because this information was not available or was not reported with sufficient consistency to be analyzed. Consistent reporting of participant background characteristics will permit moderator analyses in future meta-analyses of this topic.

Conclusion

The results of this report indicate that bilingualism can enhance cognitive functioning. However, the bilingual advantage is more likely to be observed on the Stroop, Simon, and Attentional Network tasks, and in participants over 50-years of age. Understanding why the advantage is most pronounced on these tasks and in this age group could advance our understanding of how learning and using multiple languages influences neural and cognitive functioning.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://osf.io/ykp5w/?view_only=1d90f8c86146466e8dcfb6c4d23a334f.

Author Contributions

JL and AW were responsible for the acquisition and analysis of data. MK, JL, and AW were all responsible for the interpretation of data and preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

*Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., et al. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cereb.Cortex 22, 2076–2086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., et al. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia 69, 201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., and Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Rev. Educ. Res. 80, 207–245. doi: 10.3102/0034654310368803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Anderson, J. A. E., Saleemi, S., and Bialystok, E. (2017). Neuropsychological assessments of cognitive aging in monolingual and bilingual older adults. J. Neurolinguist. 43, 17–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ansaldo, A. I., Ghazi-Saidi, L., and Adrover-Roig, D. (2015). Interference control in elderly bilinguals: appearances can be misleading. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 37, 455–470. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.990359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Antón, E., Carreiras, M., and Duñabeitia, J. A. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS ONE 14:e0206770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., et al. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Front. Psychol. 5:398. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Antón, E., García, Y. F., Carreiras, M., and Duñabeitia, J. A. (2016). Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? J. Mem. Lang. 90, 147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antoniou, K., Grohmann, K. K., Kambanaros, M., and Katsos, N. (2016). The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition 149, 18–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A., and Rosselli, M. (1989). Neuropsychology characteristics of normal aging. Dev. Neuropsychol. 5, 307–230. doi: 10.1080/87565648909540441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Arredondo, M. M., Hu, X. S., Satterfield, T., and Kovelman, I. (2017). Bilingualism alters children's frontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Dev. Sci. 20, 1–25. doi: 10.1111/desc.12377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Badzakova-Trajkov, G. (2008). A behavioural and functional imaging investigation of Stroop task performance in late proficient bilinguals (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Available online at: https://researchspace.auckland.ac.nz/handle/2292/3365

Google Scholar

*Barac, R., and Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Dev. 83, 413–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Barac, R., Moreno, S., and Bialystok, E. (2016). Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Dev. 87, 1277–1290. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Batres, K. M. (2013). Bilingual cognitive control and perspective-monitoring in dialogue (Doctoral dissertation). The Graduate School, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States. Available online at: https://ir.stonybrook.edu/jspui/handle/11401/77596

Google Scholar

Baugh, F. (2002). Correcting effect sizes for score reliability: a reminder that measurement and substantive issues are linked inextricably. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 62, 254–263. doi: 10.1177/0013164402062002004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrmann, M., Geng, J. J., and Shomstein, S. (2004). Parietal cortex and attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Berroir, P., Ghazi-Saidi, L., Dash, T., Adrover-Roig, D., Benali, H., and Ansaldo, A. I. (2017). Interference control at the response level: functional networks reveal higher efficiency in the bilingual brain. J. Neurolinguist. 43, 4–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Dev. 70, 636–644. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 68–79. doi: 10.1037/cjep2006008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: beyond inhibition. Dev. Psychol. 46, 93–105. doi: 10.1037/a0015466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (2011). Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 110, 461–468. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. J. Cogn. Dev. 11, 485–508. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2010.516420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Craik, F., and Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. 34, 859–873. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., et al. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the simon task: evidence from MEG. Neuroimage 24, 40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., and Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon Task. Psychol. Aging 19, 290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., and DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. J. Exp. Psychol. 35, 565–574. doi: 10.1037/a0012735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Hawrylewicz, K., Wiseheart, M., and Toplak, M. (2017). Interaction of bilingualism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adults. Bilingualism 20, 588–601. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., and Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Dev. Sci. 7, 325–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., and Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychol. Aging 29, 696–705. doi: 10.1037/a0037254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bialystok, E., and Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112, 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bice, K., and Kroll, J. F. (2015). Native language change during early stages of second language learning. Neuroreport 26:966. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Billig, J. D., and Scholl, A. P. (2011). The impact of bilingualism and aging on inhibitory control and working memory. Organon 26, 39–52. doi: 10.22456/2238-8915.28833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biss, R. K., Campbell, K. L., and Hasher, L. (2012). Interference from previous distraction disrupts older adults' memory. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 68, 558–561. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Blackburn, A. M. (2013). A study of the relationship between code switching and the bilingual advantage: evidence that language use modulates neural indices of language processing and cognitive control (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Texas, San Antonio, TX United States. Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net

Google Scholar

*Blom, E., Boerma, T., Bosma, E., Cornips, L., and Everaert, E. (2017). Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Front. Psychol. 8:522. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Blumenfeld, H. K. (2008). Suppression mechanisms in monolingual and bilingual language comprehension: Evidence from eye-tracking, priming, and inhibitory control tasks (Doctoral dissertation). Northwestern University. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Database

Google Scholar

*Blumenfeld, H. K., and Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 547–567. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.812093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Blumenfeld, H. K., and Marian, V. (2014). Cognitive control in bilinguals: advantages in stimulus–stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism 17, 610–629. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bogulski, C. A., Rakoczy, M., Goodman, M., and Bialystok, E. (2014). Executive control in fluent and lapsed bilinguals. Bilingualism 18, 561–567. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2011). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

*Borsa, V. M., Perani, D., Della Rosa, P. A., Videsott, G., Guidi, L., Weekes, B. S., et al. (2018). Bilingualism and healthy aging: aging effects and neural maintenance. Neuropsychologia 111, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowles, R. P., and Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Assessing the age-related effects of proactive interference on working memory tasks using the Rasch model. Psychol. Aging 18:608. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Brito, N. H., Murphy, E. R., Vaidya, C., and Barr, R. (2016). Do bilingual advantages in attentional control influence memory encoding during a divided attention task? Bilingualism 19, 621–629. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Brown, J. M. (2015). Task-Switching in bilinguals: Further investigation of the bilingual advantage (Doctoral dissertation). Illinois State University. Available online at: https://ir.library.illinoisstate.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1415&context=etd

Google Scholar

*Buac, M., and Kaushanskaya, M. (2014). The relationship between linguistic and non-linguistic cognitive control skills in bilingual children from low socio-economic backgrounds. Front. Psychol. 5:1098. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo, A., and Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 130, 278–288. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Carlson, S. M., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 11, 282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Chabal, S., Schroeder, S. R., and Marian, V. (2015). Audio-visual object search is changed by bilingual experience. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 2684–2693. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0973-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Clare, L., Whitaker, C. J., Martyr, A., Martin-Forbes, P. A., Bastable, A. J. M., Pye, K. L., et al. (2016). Executive control in older welsh monolinguals and bilinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 412–426. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1148041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Coderre, E. L., Smith, J. F., Van Heuven, W. J., and Horwitz, B. (2016). The functional overlap of executive control and language processing in bilinguals. Bilingualism 19, 471–488. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Coderre, E. L., and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2014). The effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 5:1070. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coderre, E. L., van Heuven, W. J. B., and Conklin, K. (2013). The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism 16, 420–441. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Costa, A., Hernández, M., and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106, 59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Crivello, C., Kuzyk, O., Rodrigues, M., Friend, M., Zesiger, P., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers' executive function. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, C. E., and D'Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*D'Souza, A. A., Moradzadeh, L., and Wiseheart, M. (2018). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: working memory and inhibitory control. Cogn. Res. 3, 11–11. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0095-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Damian, M. F., Ye, W., Oh, M., and Yang, S. (2018). Bilinguals as 'experts'? Comparing performance of mono- to bilingual individuals via a mousetracking paradigm. Bilingualism 22, 1–18. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*de Bruin, A., Bak, T. H., and Della Sala, S. (2015a). Examining the effects of active versus inactive bilingualism on executive control in a carefully matched non-immigrant sample. J. Mem. Lang. 85, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., and Della Sala, S. (2015b). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: an example of publication bias? Psychol. Sci. 26, 99–107. doi: 10.1177/0956797614557866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*De Leeuw, E., and Bogulski, C. A. (2016). Frequent L2 language use enhances executive control in bilinguals. Bilingualism 19, 907–913. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*De Pape, A. M. R. (2007). Musical expertise, bilingualism and executive functioning (Master's thesis). York University, Toronto, ON, Canada.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

*Dell'Armi, M. (2015). L'impact du bilinguisme sur les capacités inhibitrices des enfants: une étude-pilote/impact of bilingualism on children's inhibitory capacities: a pilot-study. Enfance 67, 245–259. doi: 10.4074/S0013754515002050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Dev. Rev. 12, 45–75. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90003-K

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Desideri, L., and Bonifacci, P. (2018). Verbal and nonverbal anticipatory mechanisms in bilinguals. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 47, 719–739. doi: 10.1007/s10936-017-9556-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Desjardins, J. L., and Fernandez, F. (2018). Performance on auditory and visual tasks of inhibition in english monolingual and spanish-english bilingual adults: do bilinguals have a cognitive advantage? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 410–419. doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-H-17-0160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, T., Wahl, A., Buytenhuijs, F., Van Halem, N., Al-Jibouri, Z., De Korte, M., et al. (2019). Multilink: a computational model for bilingual word recognition and word translation. Bilingualism 22, 657–679. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., and Homer, B. D. (2019). Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 1122–1147. doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01567-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Dunabeitia, J. A., Hernandez, J. A., Anton, E., Macizo, P., Estevez, A., Fuentes, L. J., et al. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Exp. Psychol. 61, 234–251. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Duncan, H. D. (2010). The effects of aging and bilingualism on language-specific attention control (Doctoral dissertation). Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada. Available online at https://spectrum.library.concordia.ca/979449/

Google Scholar

*Duncan, H. D., Segalowitz, N., and Phillips, N. A. (2016). Differences in L1 linguistic attention control between monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism 19, 106–121. doi: 10.1017/S136672891400025X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easterbrook, P. J., Gopalan, R., Berlin, J., and Matthews, D. R. (1991). Publication bias in clinical research. Lancet 337, 867–872. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)90201-Y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J. 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., and Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1201–1206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., and Puglisi, M. L. (2014). Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low income families. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 736–747. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

*Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., and Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1364–1371. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Estanga, A., Ecay-Torres, M., Ibañez, A., Izagirre, A., Villanua, J., Garcia-Sebastian, M., et al. (2017). Beneficial effect of bilingualism on alzheimer's disease CSF biomarkers and cognition. Neurobiol. Aging 50, 144–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiszer, C. (2008). The Effect of Bilingualism on Cognition: Evidence from early and late bilinguals. (Master's Thesis). Paris, France.

Google Scholar

*Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., and Marian, V. (2017). Cross-linguistic phonotactic competition and cognitive control in bilinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 783–794. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2017.1321553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., et al. (2010). Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. Neuroimage 53, 1272–1278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Garraffa, M., Beveridge, M., and Sorace, A. (2015). Linguistic and cognitive skills in Sardinian-Italian bilingual children. Front. Psychol. 6:1898. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gervais, W. (2015). Putting PET-PEESE to the test. Will Gervais Blog. Available online at: https://web.archive.org/web/20170326200626/http://willgervais.com/blog/2015/6/25/putting-pet-peese-to-the-test-1

*Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., and Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. J. Neurosci. 33, 387–396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Goldsmith, S. F., and Morton, J. B. (2018). Sequential congruency effects in monolingual and bilingual adults: a failure to replicate Grundy et al. (2017). Front. Psychol. 9:2476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., and Nadig, A. S. (2017). Can bilingualism mitigate set-shifting difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorders? Child Dev. 90, 1043–1060. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F. I., and Bialystok, E. (2015). Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia 66, 170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Grant, A., and Dennis, N. (2017). Increased processing speed in young adult bilinguals: evidence from source memory judgments. Bilingualism 20, 327–336. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism 1, 67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. W., and Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: the adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 515–530. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Grundy, J. G., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Friesen, D. C., Mak, L., and Bialystok, E. (2017). Sequential congruency effects reveal differences in disengagement of attention for monolingual and bilingual young adults. Cognition 163, 42–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Guido Mendes, C. M. (2015). The impact of bilingualism on conflict control (Doctoral dissertation). University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. Available online at: https://otago.ourarchive.ac.nz/handle/10523/6030

Google Scholar

*Gutierrez, M. (2009). A study of possible pre-cognitive advantages of bilingualism (Master's thesis). The University of Texas, El Paso, TX, United States. Available online at: https://digitalcommons.utep.edu/dissertations/AAI1473867/

Google Scholar

*Gutierrez, M. (2013). Strengthening cognitive development in minority populations: a study of the beneficial effects of bilingualism. (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Texas, El Paso, TX, United States. Available online at: https://digitalcommons.utep.edu/dissertations/AAI3609486/

Google Scholar

*Hamlet, A. L. (2015). Executive functioning in mono vs. bilingual young adults: Assessment using language-based tools (Doctoral dissertation). St. John's University. New York, NY, United States. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Database.

Google Scholar

Hedges, L., and Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Models for Meta-Analysis. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V., and Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed-and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol. Methods 3, 486–504. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Heidlmayr, K., Hemforth, B., Moutier, S., and Isel, F. (2015). Neurodynamics of executive control processes in bilinguals: evidence from ERP and source reconstruction analyses. Front. Psychol. 6:821. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Heidlmayr, K., Moutier, S., Hemforth, B., Courtin, C., Tanzmeister, R., and Isel, F. (2014). Successive bilingualism and executive functions: the effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop Colour Word task. Bilingualism 17, 630–645. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hernandez, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A. B., and Sebastian-Galles, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism 13, 315–325. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilchey, M. D., and Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 625–658. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilchey, M. D., Saint-Aubin, J., and Klein, R. M. (2015). “Does bilingual exercise enhance cognitive fitness in traditional non-linguistic executive processing tasks?,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing, ed J. Schwieter (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press), 586–613. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781107447257.026

CrossRef Full Text

Hommel, B. (2011). The simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychol. 136, 189–202. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Houtzager, N., Lowie, W., Sprenger, S., and de Bot, K. (2017). A bilingual advantage in task switching? Age-related differences between German monolinguals and Dutch-Frisian bilinguals. Bilingualism 20, 69–79. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hutchison, L. A. (2012). Relations between executive functioning, second language fluency, and externalizing behavior problems in early childhood (Doctoral dissertation). George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, United States. Available online at: http://ebot.gmu.edu/handle/1920/7955

Google Scholar

Idris, N., and Ruzni, N. (2012). Performance of the trim and fill method in adjusting for the publication bias in meta-analysis of continuous data. Am. J. Appl. Sci. 9, 1512–1517. doi: 10.3844/ajassp.2012.1512.1517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Jalali-Moghadam, N., and Kormi-Nouri, R. (2015). The role of executive functions in bilingual children with reading difficulties. Scand. J. Psychol. 56, 297–305. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Jiao, L., Liu, C., Wang, R., and Chen, B. (2017). Working memory demand of a task modulates bilingual advantage in executive functions. Int. J. Biling. 23, 102–117. doi: 10.1177/1367006917709097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Johns, B. T., Sheppard, C. L., Jones, M. N., and Taler, V. (2016). The role of semantic diversity in word recognition across aging and bilingualism. Front. Psychol. 7:703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kalashnikova, M., and Mattock, K. (2014). Maturation of executive functioning skills in early sequential bilingualism. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 17, 111–123. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2012.746284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kapa, L. L., and Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cogn. Dev. 28, 233–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., and Buac, M. (2014). Effects of classroom bilingualism on task-shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in children. Dev. Sci. 17, 564–583. doi: 10.1111/desc.12142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kazemeini, T., and Fadardi, J. S. (2015). Executive function: comparing bilingual and monolingual Iranian university students. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 45, 1315–1326. doi: 10.1007/s10936-015-9403-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Keijzer, M. (2013). Working memory capacity, inhibitory control and the role of L2 proficiency in aging L1 Dutch speakers of near-native L2 English. Brain Sci. 3, 1261–1281. doi: 10.3390/brainsci3031261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Keijzer, M. C., and Schmid, M. S. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control advantages of elderly late dutch-english bilinguals. Linguist. Appr. Bilinguist. 6, 64–85. doi: 10.1075/lab.14032.kei

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keye, D., Wilhelm, O., Oberauer, K., and Van Ravenzwaaij, D. (2009). Individual differences in conflict-monitoring: testing means and covariance hypothesis about the simon and the eriksen flanker task. Psychol. Res. 73, 762–776. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0188-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., and Kempe, V. (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 640–648. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.929580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Knyshev, E. A. (2018). Is there a bilingual advantage: testing the role of language mode (Doctoral dissertation). Northwestern State University of Louisiana. Available online at: https://krex.k-state.edu/dspace/bitstream/handle/2097/39243/ElenaKnyshev2018.pdf?sequence=3

Google Scholar

Kondo, H., Osaka, N., and Osaka, M. (2004). Cooperation of the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for attention shifting. Neuroimage 23, 670–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Köpke, B., and Nespoulous, J.-L. (2006). Working memory performance in expert and novice interpreters. Interpreting 8, 1–23. doi: 10.1075/intp.8.1.02kop

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kousaie, S., and Phillips, N. A. (2012a). Ageing and bilingualism: absence of a 'bilingual advantage' in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 356–369. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.604788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kousaie, S., and Phillips, N. A. (2012b). Conflict monitoring and resolution: are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. 1446, 71–90. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kousaie, S., and Phillips, N. A. (2017). A behavioural and electrophysiological investigation of the effect of bilingualism on aging and cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 94, 23–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., and Taler, V. (2014). Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:250. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kramer, R. (2011). Effects of bilingualism on inhibitory control and working memory: a study with early and late bilinguals (Master's Thesis). Federal University of Satan Catarina, Florianopolis, Brazil. Available online at: https://repositorio.ufsc.br/handle/123456789/96068

Google Scholar

*Ladas, A. I., Carroll, D. J., and Vivas, A. B. (2015). Attentional processes in low-socioeconomic status bilingual children: are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience? Child Dev. 86, 557–578. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lee, T. M. C., and Chan, C. C. H. (2010). Stroop interference in Chinese and English. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 465–471. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4.1-0FT465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee Salvatierra, J., and Rosselli, M. (2011). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. Int. J. Biling. 15, 26–37. doi: 10.1177/1367006910371021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., and Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 144, 394–425. doi: 10.1037/bul0000142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Luk, G., Anderson, J. A., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., and Bialystok, E. (2010). Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain Cogn. 74, 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Luk, G., de Sa, E., and Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism 14, 588–595. doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lustig, C., May, C. P., and Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. J. Exp. Psychol. 130:199. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marian, V., and Spivey, M. (2003). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Appl. Psycholinguist. 24, 173–193. doi: 10.1017/S0142716403000092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marian, V., Spivey, M., and Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain Lang. 86, 70–82. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00535-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Martin-Rhee, M. M., and Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism 11, 81–93. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Martinez Reyes, D. A. (2013). The effect of multilingualism on executive functioning and memory in a sample of college-educated older adults (Doctoral dissertation). Fuller Theological Seminary, Pasadena, CA, United States. Available online at: https://digitalcommons.fuller.edu/phd-clinical-psychology/45/

Google Scholar

*Marzecová, A., Asanowicz, D., Kriva, L. U., and Wodniecka, Z. (2013a). The effects of bilingualism on efficiency and lateralization of attentional networks. Bilingualism 16, 608–623. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Marzecová, A., Bukowski, M., Correa, A., Boros, M., Lupiáñez, J., and Wodniecka, Z. (2013b). Tracing the bilingual advantage in cognitive control: the role of flexibility in temporal preparation and category switching. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 586–604. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.809348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mehrani, M. B., and Zabihi, R. (2017). A comparative study of shifting ability, inhibitory control and working memory in monolingual and bilingual children. Psychol. Stud. 62, 421–427. doi: 10.1007/s12646-017-0432-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mendes, C. G. (2015). The impact of bilingualism on conflict control (Doctoral dissertation). University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. Available online at: https://otago.ourarchive.ac.nz/handle/10523/6030

Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mohades, S. G., Struys, E., Van Schuerbeek, P., Baeken, C., Van De Craen, P., and Luypaert, R. (2014). Age of second language acquisition affects nonverbal conflict processing in children: an fMRI study. Brain Behav. 4, 626–642. doi: 10.1002/brb3.246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mor, B., Yitzhaki-Amsalem, S., and Prior, A. (2015). The joint effect of bilingualism and ADHD on executive functions. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 527–541. doi: 10.1177/1087054714527790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Moradzadeh, L., Blumenthal, G., and Wiseheart, M. (2015). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: a closer look at task switching and dual-task performance. Cogn. Sci. 39, 992–1020. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., and Bajo, M. T. (2013). Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 531–546. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.807812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Morini, G. (2014). Interactions between language experience and cognitive abilities in word learning and word recognition (Doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, MD. Available online at: https://drum.lib.umd.edu/handle/1903/15705

Google Scholar

*Morton, J. B., and Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Dev. Sci. 10, 719–726. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Naeem, K., Filippi, R., Periche-Tomas, E., Papageorgiou, A., and Bright, P. (2018). The importance of socioeconomic status as a modulator of the bilingual advantage in cognitive ability. Front. Psychol. 9:1818. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Nair, V. K., Biedermann, B., and Nickels, L. (2016). Effect of socio-economic status on cognitive control in non-literate bilingual speakers. Bilingualism 20, 999–1009. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000778

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Navarro-Torres, C. A., Garcia, D. L., Chidambaram, V., and Kroll, J. F. (2019). Cognitive control facilitates attentional disengagement during second language comprehension. Brain Sci. 9:95. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9050095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Nayak, S. (2018). Beyond bilingual advantages: Contexts, mechanisms, and correlates of executive function in bilingual and monolingual children (Doctoral dissertation). Boston University. Available online at: https://search-proquest-com.ezproxy-b.deakin.edu.au/docview/1972852127?pq-origsite=gscholar

Google Scholar

*Nicolay, A.-C., and Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for three years. Bilingualism 16, 597–607. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 126, 220. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeso, I., Robles, N., Muñoz-Marrón, E., and Redolar-Ripoll, D. (2013). Dissociating the role of the pre-SMA in response inhibition and switching: a combined online and offline TMS approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:150. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Okanda, M., Moriguchi, Y., and Itakura, S. (2010). Language and cognitive shifting: evidence from young monolingual and bilingual children. Psychol. Rep. 107, 68–78. doi: 10.2466/03.10.28.PR0.107.4.68-78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Olsen, R. K., Pangelinan, M. M., Bogulski, C., Chakravarty, M. M., Luk, G., Grady, C. L., et al. (2015). The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Res. 1612, 128–139. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., and Bak, T. H. (2018). From bilingualism to bilingualisms: bilingual experience in edinburgh and singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism 21, 867–879. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, K. R. (2019). “Bilingualism in cognitive science: the characteristics and consequences of bilingual language control,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism, eds A. De Houwer and L. Ortega (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 435–465.

*Paap, K. R., and Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 66, 232–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., and Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Paap, K. R., and Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Front. Psychol. 5:962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Papageorgiou, A., Bright, P., Periche Tomas, E., and Filippi, R. (2019). Evidence against a cognitive advantage in the older bilingual population. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 72, 1354–1363. doi: 10.1177/1747021818796475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkin, A. J., and Lawrence, A. (1994). A dissociation in the relation between memory tasks and frontal lobe tests in the normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 32, 1523–1532. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90124-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pelham, S. D., and Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. 40, 313–325. doi: 10.1037/a0035224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, J. L., Sutton, A. J., Jones, D. R., Abrams, K. R., and Rushton, L. (2007). Performance of the trim and fill method in the presence of publication bias and between-study heterogeneity. Stat. Med. 26, 4544–4562. doi: 10.1002/sim.2889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Leung, H. C., et al. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 427–440. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00054-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pino Escobar, G., Kalashnikova, M., and Escudero, P. (2018). Vocabulary matters! The relationship between verbal fluency and measures of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 170, 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Poarch, G. J., and Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Dev. Rev. 35, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Poarch, G. J., and van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 113, 535–551. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., and Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers' executive functioning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 567–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Prior, A., and Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 682–691. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Prior, A., and MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism 13, 253–262. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Purić, D., Vuksanović, J., and Chondrogianni, V. (2017). Cognitive advantages of immersion education after 1year: Effects of amount of exposure. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 159, 296–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rabkina, I. (2014). Examining the Intersection of the Cognitive Advantages and Disadvantages of the Bilingual Brain (Senior thesis). Scripps College. Available online at: https://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/376/

Google Scholar

*Raudszus, H., Segers, E., and Verhoeven, L. (2017). Lexical quality and executive control predict children's first and second language reading comprehension. Read. Writ. 31, 405–424. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-9791-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rietbergen, M. (2014). The bilingual neurocognitive network: an investigation of neuroplasticity and cognitive performance (Master's thesis). University of Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands. Available online at: https://dspace.library.uu.nl/handle/1874/298035

Google Scholar

*Rodrigues, L. R. (2013). Cognitive differences between monolinguals and bi/multilinguals: executive functions boosted by code-switching? (Master's thesis). Catholic University of Pelotas, Pelotas, Brazil.

*Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Sanjuan, A., Ventura-Campos, N., Roman, P., Martin, C., Barcelo, F., et al. (2013). Bilinguals use language-control brain areas more than monolinguals to perform non-linguistic switching tasks. PLoS ONE 8:e73028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Romano, J. C. (2009). Bilingualism, cognition, and successful aging. (Doctoral dissertation). The Catholic University of America, Washington DC, United States. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Database.

Google Scholar

*Ross, J., and Melinger, A. (2016). Bilingual advantage, bidialectal advantage or neither? Comparing performance across three tests of executive function in middle childhood. Dev. Sci. 20, 1–21. doi: 10.1111/desc.12405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638–664. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., and Vélez-Uribe, I. (2016). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism 19, 489–503. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Santisi, M. N., Arecco, M. D. R., Salvatierra, J., Conde, A., et al. (2002). Stroop effect in spanish-english bilinguals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 819–827. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702860106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rubio-Fernandez, P., and Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people's beliefs: bilinguals have an advantage. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 211–217. doi: 10.1037/a0025162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ryskin, R. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Yiu, L. K., and Nguyen, E. T. (2014). Visuospatial perspective-taking in conversation and the role of bilingual experience. J. Mem. Lang. 74, 46–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., and Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 763–797. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2011). What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance? Intelligence 39, 222–232. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Salvatierra, J. L., and Rosselli, M. (2016). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. Int. J. Bilinguist. 71, 1–12. doi: 10.1177/1367006910371021

CrossRef Full Text

*Sampson, M. (2015). An investigation of inhibitory control in bilingual aphasia (Doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States. Available online at: https://drum.lib.umd.edu/handle/1903/16645

Google Scholar

Scaltritti, M., Peressotti, F., and Miozzo, M. (2017). Bilingual advantage and language switch: what's the linkage? Bilingualism 20, 80–97. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, F. L., and Hunter, J. E. (2014). Methods of Meta-Analysis:Correcting error and Bias in Research Findings, 3rd Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. doi: 10.4135/9781483398105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schroeder, S., and Marian, V. (2012). A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 591–601. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.669367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schroeder, S. R., Marian, V., Shook, A., and Bartolotti, J. (2016). Bilingualism and musicianship enhance cognitive control. Neural Plast. 2016:4058620. doi: 10.1155/2016/4058620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Seçer, I. (2016). Skills of cognitive flexibility in monolingual and bilingual younger adults. J. Gen. Psychol. 143, 172–184. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2016.1200530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Shulley, L. J., and Shake, M. C. (2016). Investigating the relationship between bilingualism, cognitive control, and mind wandering. J. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 257–274. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1128438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanley, T. D., and Doucouliagos, H. (2014). Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias. Res. Synth. Methods 5, 60–78. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Stasenko, A., Matt, G. E., and Gollan, T. H. (2017). A relative bilingual advantage in switching with preparation: nuanced explorations of the proposed association between bilingualism and task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. 146, 1527–1550. doi: 10.1037/xge0000340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stins, J. F., Polderman, J., Boomsma, D. I., and de Geus, E. J. (2005). Response interference and working memory in 12-year-old children. Child Neuropsychol. 11, 191–201. doi: 10.1080/092970490911351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Struys, E., Duyck, W., and Woumans, E. (2018). The role of cognitive development and strategic task tendencies in the bilingual advantage controversy. Front. Psychol. 9:1790. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Suarez, P. A. (2013). The role of bilingualism on neuropsychological test performance among Spanish speakers tested in their native language (Doctoral dissertation). University of California, San Diego, CA, United States. Available online at: https://escholarship.org/content/qt92f4233q/qt92f4233q.pdf

Google Scholar

*Taler, V., Johns, B. T., Young, K., Sheppard, C., and Jones, M. N. (2013). A computational analysis of semantic structure in bilingual verbal fluency performance. J. Mem. Lang. 69, 607–618. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., and Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Front. Psychol. 2:123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tao, L., Taft, M., and Gollan, T. H. (2015). The bilingual switching advantage: sometimes related to bilingual proficiency, sometimes not. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 531–544. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Teubner-Rhodes, S. (2014). The neural bases of the bilingual advantage in cognitive control: An investigation of conflict adaptation phenomena (Doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States. Available online at: https://drum.lib.umd.edu/handle/1903/15283

Google Scholar

van den Noort, M., Struys, E., Bosch, P., Jaswetz, L., Perriard, B., Yeo, S., et al. (2019). Does the bilingual advantage in cognitive control exist and if so, what are its modulating factors? A systematic review. Behav. Sci. 9:27. doi: 10.3390/bs9030027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Vaughn, K. A., Archila-Suerte, P., and Hernandez, A. E. (2019). Parietal lobe volume distinguishes attentional control in bilinguals and monolinguals: a structural MRI study. Brain Cogn. 134, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Verhagen, J., Mulder, H., and Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Effects of home language environment on inhibitory control in bilingual three-year-old children. Bilingualism 20, 114–127. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Vivas, A. B., Ladas, A. I., Salvari, V., and Chrysochoou, E. (2017). Revisiting the bilingual advantage in attention in low SES Greek-Albanians: does the level of bilingual experience matter? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 743–756. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1271442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., and Gade, M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: a test of four hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. 145, 246–258. doi: 10.1037/xge0000120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Waldie, K. E., Badzakova-Trajkov, G., Miliivojevic, B., and Kirk, I. J. (2009). Neural activity during Stroop colour-word task performance in late proficient bilinguals: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychol. Neurosci. 2, 125–136. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Warmington, M. A., Kandru-Pothineni, S., and Hitch, G. J. (2018). Novel-word learning, executive control and working memory: a bilingual advantage. Bilingualism 22, 763–782. doi: 10.1017/S136672891800041X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*White, L. J., and Greenfield, D. B. (2016). Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking head start preschoolers. Dev. Sci. 20, 1–14. doi: 10.1111/desc.12502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wierzbicki, S. (2013). An investigation into the relationship between bilingualism and attentional networks with a focus on linguistic distance and language proficiency (Master's dissertation) University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa. Available online at: http://mobile.wiredspace.wits.ac.za/handle/10539/13400

Google Scholar

*Wiseheart, M., Viswanathan, M., and Bialystok, E. (2016). Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism 19, 141–146. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Woumans, E., Ceuleers, E., Van der Linden, L., Szmalec, A., and Duyck, W. (2015). Verbal and nonverbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. J. Exp. Psychol. 41, 1579–1586. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Woumans, E., Surmont, J., Struys, E., and Duyck, W. (2016). The longitudinal effect of bilingual immersion schooling on cognitive control and intelligence. Lang. Learn. 66, 76–91. doi: 10.1111/lang.12171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Woumans, E., Van Herck, S., and Struys, E. (2019). Shifting gear in the study of the bilingual advantage: language switching examined as a possible moderator. Behav. Sci. 9:86. doi: 10.3390/bs9080086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y. J., Zhang, H., and Guo, T. (2016). Does speaking two dialects in daily life affect executive functions? An event-related potential study. PLoS ONE 11:e0150492. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Yamasaki, B. L., and Prat, C. S. (2014). The importance of managing interference for second language reading ability: an individual differences investigation. Discourse Process. 51, 445–467. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2014.913415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Xie, Z., and Dong, Y. (2017). Contributions of bilingualism and public speaking training to cognitive control differences among young adults. Bilingualism 20, 55–68. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Yang, E. (2015). Cognitive effects of bilingualism: Executive functions and language practice (Doctoral dissertation). University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, New York, NY, United States. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Database.

Google Scholar

*Yang, S., and Yang, H. (2016). Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 146, 121–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Yang, S., Yang, H. J., and Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism 14, 412–422. doi: 10.1017/S1366728910000611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Yudes, C., Macizo, P., and Bajo, T. (2011). The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Front. Psychol. 2:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 1:297. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Zeng, Z., Kalashnikova, M., and Antoniou, M. (2019). Integrating bilingualism, verbal fluency, and executive functioning across the lifespan. J. Cogn. Dev. 20, 656–679. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1648267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Zhou, B., and Krott, A. (2018). Bilingualism enhances attentional control in non-verbal conflict tasks—evidence from ex-gaussian analyses. Bilingualism 21, 162–180. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*References included in the meta-analyses have been preceded by an asterisk.

Keywords: bilingual advantage, meta-analysis, executive functioning, publication bias, bilingualism

Citation: Ware AT, Kirkovski M and Lum JAG (2020) Meta-Analysis Reveals a Bilingual Advantage That Is Dependent on Task and Age. Front. Psychol. 11:1458. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01458

Received: 18 January 2020; Accepted: 02 June 2020;
Published: 24 July 2020.

Edited by:

Guillaume Thierry, Bangor University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Haoyun Zhang, Pennsylvania State University (PSU), United States
Katharina Spalek, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Ware, Kirkovski and Lum. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jarrad A. G. Lum, jarrad.lum@deakin.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.