Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 August 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Factors beyond Linguistic and Metalinguistic Skills That Are Associated with Learning to Read in Chinese View all 5 articles

The Moderation Effect of Processing Efficiency on the Relationship Between Visual Working Memory and Chinese Character Recognition

\r\nZhengye Xu&#x;Zhengye Xu1†Li-Chih Wang,,&#x;Li-Chih Wang1,2,3†Duo Liu,,*Duo Liu1,2,3*Yimei ChenYimei Chen4Li TaoLi Tao5
  • 1Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
  • 2Integrated Centre for Wellbeing, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
  • 3Centre for Child and Family Science, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
  • 4Shenzhen Tongle School, Shenzhen, China
  • 5Xuefu Middle School, Shenzhen, China

To investigate the underlying mechanism of the relationship between visual working memory (VWM) and Chinese character recognition, and the moderation effect of processing efficiency on this relationship, 154 first-grade students were administered a battery of tasks for VWM, rapid temporal processing, and Chinese character reading. In the VWM task, the children were asked to remember the jumping routes of a frog and report these routes in reverse sequence. The longest span for which each participant could respond correctly at least four times out of six was the VWM index. In the task of temporal order judgement, the participants were asked to select which of two balls was presented first, with stimulus onset asynchronies varying from 8 to 492 ms according to an adaptive psychophysical procedure. Visual temporal order threshold (VTOT) was utilized as an indicator of processing efficiency. The participants were asked to read 100 characters aloud to measure their word-level reading abilities in Chinese character recognition. After controlling age, non-verbal intelligence, visual short-term memory, morphological awareness, and orthographic awareness, the results of a moderation effect analysis showed that (1) both VWM and visual VTOT predicted Chinese character reading, and (2) the moderation effect of VTOT on the VWM-reading link was significant (p = 0.001). The correlation between VWM and Chinese character reading was positive and significant when VTOTs were above average (i.e., smaller than 87.14 ms); however, the correlation was negative at relatively poor levels of VTOTs (i.e., larger than 231.44 ms).

Introduction

Working memory (WM) is the ability to concurrently store and manipulate information that is necessary to perform mental tasks (Baddeley et al., 1986). WM capacity is generally measured by WM span tasks, in which participants are asked to maintain items to be recalled while they perform a demanding secondary task such as counting (Baddeley, 2003). Compared with the literature on phonological WM (Hansen and Bowey, 1994; Calvo, 1996; Chung et al., 2010), the literature on visual WM (VWM) and reading is limited, and the findings are controversial (e.g., Olson and Datta, 2002; Jeffries and Everatt, 2004; Smith-Spark and Fisk, 2007). VWM, as an acritical component of WM (Baddeley and Lieberman, 2017), is thought to be important for beginning readers’ word recognition, especially in languages with logographic writing systems, such as Chinese (Huang and Hanley, 1995; Wang et al., 2014, 2015).

The inconsistent findings regarding the association between VWM and reading might be due to individual differences in aspects related to WM and/or reading, such as age and reading experience (e.g., Gottardo et al., 1996; Berninger et al., 2010; Sullivan et al., 2015). The capacity of VWM can be improved with the increase of processing efficiency (Olson and Jiang, 2004; Chung et al., 2008; Brady et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2019). Children with the same VWM capacity, but different levels of processing efficiency, should utilize different cognitive resources on the same task (Barrouillet et al., 2004). In addition to influencing VWM performance, children’s processing efficiency might further impact higher-level cognitive processing that is related to VWM, such as reading. To address these issues, the present study investigated the association between VWM and Chinese character reading in first-grade Chinese students. With an index of visual processing efficiency (i.e., rapid temporal processing, the possible role of processing efficiency in this association was explored. The findings can extend our understanding of the underlying mechanism of the relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition. In turn, understanding the mechanism could help us develop efficient means to improve children’s reading.

WM and Chinese Character Recognition

Working memory consists of multicomponent memory systems. Phonological WM, i.e., the phonological loop, has a phonological store and an articulatory rehearsal system (Baddeley et al., 1998). VWM, also known as the visual-spatial sketchpad, is used for immediate maintaining and processing of visual non-verbal information, including visual features and spatial analysis (Peng et al., 2013). These two WM systems, together with the central executive system and a newly proposed episodic buffer, form a complete system and are effective in explaining reading and other cognitive processes (Baddeley, 2003). The effect of WM on reading is demonstrated by findings that children with reading disabilities show significant and marked decrements on WM tasks compared to typically developing children in alphabetic languages (e.g., Siegel and Ryan, 1989; Swanson et al., 1996; Swanson, 1999) and in Chinese (e.g., Luo et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2015). One explanation of how deficits of WM affect reading is that poor WM capacities impede a crucial reading process, i.e., maintaining recently retrieved knowledge and integrating it with the present inputs (Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004).

The association between WM and word recognition has been investigated extensively, mainly by focusing on phonological WM (e.g., Gathercole and Baddeley, 1993; Baddeley, 2003; Chee et al., 2004; Leong et al., 2008) rather than VWM (Opitz et al., 2014; Peng et al., 2013). More importantly, findings about the association between VWM and reading are inconclusive in alphabetic languages. Some studies have documented deficits in VWM associated with poor reading performance (Olson and Datta, 2002; Smith-Spark and Fisk, 2007), but others failed to find any such association (Jeffries and Everatt, 2004; Kibby et al., 2004).

Studies of the impact of VWM on word recognition in typically developing Chinese children are scarce (e.g., So and Siegel, 1997). However, Chinese has unique features that might highlight the role of VWM in reading. Specifically, different from alphabetic languages, Chinese characters map onto phonology at the syllable level, which means that the usage of grapheme-phoneme correspondence rules is not possible in Chinese (Tan et al., 1996; Perfetti et al., 2005). That is, it is difficult for Chinese readers to rely on grapheme-phoneme conversion rules to transfer written words into a phonological code automatically. When processing characters, the sounds need to be retrieved through their links with corresponding visual forms in the mental lexicon (Shu et al., 2003; Ho et al., 2007).

In addition, the basic unit of Chinese, the character, is composed of strokes that are packed into a square configuration, processing a high, non-linear visual complexity. A simplified Chinese character has 10.3 strokes on average (Srihari et al., 2007). As a system for processing visual information, VWM may benefit the acquisition and recognition of the complex visual features of characters, which can further influence reading performance in Chinese (Tan et al., 1996; Siok et al., 2009). A mainly effective way for Chinese children to learn characters is repetitive writing (Tan et al., 2005). During this process, VWM should help children to maintain the current stimuli (e.g., strokes) and retrieve their stroke knowledge, and then follow the specific sequence to construct the form of a character. For example, when learning the character止/zhi3/ (stop), children need to process the form and stroke order at the same time to retrieve the knowledge of how to write the strokes, i.e., 丨一, and then to construct the character properly (see Figure 1). The contribution of VWM to t character acquisition should be reflected further as a relationship between VWM and character recognition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The stoke order of the Chinese character /zhi3/ (stop).

As well, the literature demonstrates that the segments of forms (e. g., strokes and radicals) can be processed to facilitate character recognition (Li et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2010). Many characters look very similar but have totally different meanings, such aswww.frontiersin.org\da4\(big), 太\tai4\(too), and 犬\quan3\(dog). To reach reading, children need to process the visual features and, simultaneously, to distinguish them from others with similar visual forms in their mental lexicons, which is related to VWM (Just and Carpenter, 1992; Shu et al., 2006). Considering that both Chinese character acquisition and recognition require simultaneous processing and storage of these visual segments of characters, which are the core nature of WM (Baddeley et al., 1998; Gathercole and Baddeley, 1993), it is reasonable to postulate the association between them.

Processing Efficiency in VWM and Chinese Character Recognition

It has been suggested that VWM is a limited-capacity system in which some resource is shared between processing and storage (Baddeley, 2003). Such a limited-capacity system leads to a phenomenon that the performance of VWM decreases when the concurrent memory load increases (Anderson et al., 1996; Baddeley, 2012). Nevertheless, it was suggested that the capacity of VWM can be influenced by processing efficiency (Brady et al., 2009). For instance, generally, an age-related increase occurs in the counting span task due to an enhanced efficiency to process sequences of numbers. Interestingly, this developmental increase of VWM capacity disappeared when participants were asked to count using unfamiliar terms, which reduced the processing efficiency in the counting task (e.g., Case et al., 1982; Bull and Scerif, 2001). On the other hand, some studies found that enhancing the processing efficiency of input (e.g., involving statistical structure and regularities) can lead to the improvement of VWM capacity (Olson and Jiang, 2004; Olson et al., 2005).

The time-based resource-sharing (TBRS) model (Barrouillet et al., 2004) assumed that, in tasks for VWM capacity, both processing and maintenance of information rely on the same limited cognitive resource (e.g., attention). High processing efficiency facilitates the VWM performance by assisting a rapid and incessant switching of cognitive resources from processing to maintenance. This rapid switching occurs during short pauses, which occurs when concurrent processing is running. Accordingly, compared with continuously occupying attention, the frequent pauses and switching can decrease demanding. Hence, the high processing efficiency improves VWM performance through lightening the cognitive load (Barrouillet et al., 2007). In turn, it is reasonable to assume that, if individuals have equal VWM capacities, these with higher processing efficiency can use fewer cognitive resources to process the same content.

It should be noted that, in addition, to processing visual features, other cognitive processing (e.g., processing of phonology) is required in Chinese character recognition. However, humans’ cognitive resources are limited (Paas et al., 2003); the more they are used for visual processing and maintenance, the less remains for the other cognitive processing. According to the TBRS model (Barrouillet et al., 2004), although children with different levels of processing efficiency can have the same VWM capacities, the cognitive resources that they use for the task of VWM are different. Of children with the same VWM capacity, these with high processing efficiency will use less resources than those with low processing efficiency to process and maintain visual features of characters. Thus, these children with high processing efficiency can have more cognitive resources available for cascade processing, such as activating the corresponding sounds of the visual forms (Baddeley, 2003). As a consequence, their performances in Chinese character recognition might be improved to larger extents (van Merrienboer and Sweller, 2005).

In brief, according to the TBRS model (Barrouillet et al., 2004), the current study assumed that children with the equal VWM capacity but different levels of processing efficiency could process visual features to similar extents, but that the cognitive resources that they use for processing these visual features would be different. That is, their available cognitive resources for other aspects of Chinese character recognition should be different (Barrouillet et al., 2007). With more remaining cognitive resources, the children with high processing efficiency could score better on Chinese character recognition tasks. In contrast, low processing efficiency might lead to a lack of resources for the cascade processing of character recognition. As a consequence, the association between VWM and Chinese character recognition could be strengthened in children with high processing efficiency, whereas it might be weakened in those with low processing efficiency.

Present Study

In sum, the current study recruited children to complete tasks of visual temporal order judgement (TOJ), backward Corsi, and Chinese character reading for their visual processing efficiency, VWM, and Chinese character recognition, respectively. The visual TOJ task involves a paradigm for measuring visual rapid temporal processing. Visual temporal processing is a more fundamental cognitive ability that relates to processing brief components and rapid sequences of information, which has a facilitating effect on Chinese character recognition (e.g., Chung et al., 2008; Wang and Yang, 2018; Wang et al., 2019). The relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition and the role of processing efficiency in the relationship were investigated.

Since there are close correlations between beginning readers’ basic cognitive abilities (e.g., WM and temporal processing) and their reading (Schneider and Pichora-Fuller, 2001; Cain et al., 2004), first grade could be a good starting point for exploring the relationships between VWM, processing efficiency and reading in Chinese. Considering their impacts on VWM and reading in children (Swanson et al., 2009), age and non-verbal intelligence were controlled. In addition, as the current study focused on the core component of VWM, i.e., the ability to process and maintain concurrently (Baddeley, 2003), visual short-term memory (VSTM) was measured. Hence, the role of VWM in character recognition was able to be investigated independently of the influence of VSTM (Cowan, 2017), which reflects storage of information in a short time (Engle et al., 1999). In addition, in order to further control the influence of children’s metalinguistic abilities on Chinese character recognition (Liu and McBride-Chang, 2010; Li et al., 2012), morphological awareness and orthographic awareness were taken into account as covariates.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 154 Chinese children (90 boys and 64 girls) in Grade 1 (mean age = 7.17 years, SD = 0.35) participated in this study. All participants were native speakers of Chinese and were recruited from a primary school located in an urban community with a low-to-middle socioeconomic level, in Shenzhen, China. Written informed parental consent was obtained for the participants. The schools and parents reported no physical or mental problems in any of the children.

Procedure

Each participant was asked to complete the VWM task, the visual TOJ task, the Chinese character reading task, and the control measures individually, in a quiet classroom. These tasks were administered by trained experimenters and required approximately 1 h to complete.

Measures

Corsi Forward and Backward Tasks

A Forward Corsi task was adopted to test the participants’ VSTM, and a backward Corsi task was adopted to measure VWM (Ang and Lee, 2008). Both tasks were performed using an E-prime program. First, a picture with nine lily pads was presented, and then, a frog jumped across the lily pads to go somewhere. In the Corsi forward task, the participants were asked to remember and repeat the order by clicking the corresponding lily pads sequentially, while, in the backward task, they were asked to indicate the backward order for each trial. The forward task was started from one lily pad, whereas the backward task was started from two lily pads. The number of lily pads increased by one when a participant answered correctly four times for the six sequences; otherwise, the task was terminated. The highest number of lily pads that participants could remember (i.e., four correct responses out of six sequences) in the Corsi forward and backward tasks were the scores for VSTM and VWM, respectively. The maximum score for each task was nine.

Visual TOJ Task

A visual TOJ task that was used in a previous study (Steinbrink et al., 2014) was applied to measure children’s visual rapid temporal processing. The task utilized visual temporal order thresholds (VTOTs) as the indicator of visual temporal processing, which increased the efficiency and reliability of the measurement (Leek, 2001; Wittmann and Fink, 2004). The smaller the value of VTOT, the higher the performance in the visual TOJ task, that is, the higher the temporal sensitivity. An adaptive psychophysical procedure was used to measure individual thresholds in this task. The distance between the participant and screen was 30 cm. A scenario with a seal was presented in the middle of the scene at first and, after a delay of 500 ms, one of two colored balls (luminance 62 cd m–2, background 0.7 cd m–2) was presented either on the right or the left side of the seal. These two balls were identical, their diameter is 7.7°, and their distance to the seal’s head 14° each. The alternative ball was presented on the other side of the seal after a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 8 ms up to 492 ms, i.e., 1–60 frames. Then, the participants were asked to hit the first presented ball on the tablet. There were 10 presentations for the practice, whose SOA varied from 328 to 115 ms. Then, in the experimental run, the SOA of the first presentation was 115 ms (14 frames), with two-down-one-up staircases randomly interleaved in linear steps of 33 ms (4 frames). A failsafe “bonus” trial (i.e., SOA of 492 ms) was interleaved to each of 7th to 13th trial. The experimental run was terminated after 30 trials.

Chinese Character Reading

A Chinese character reading task, which was utilized previously by Liu and McBride-Chang (2010), was used to examine the children’s reading ability at the word level. There were 100 simplified Chinese characters, listed in order of increasing difficulty level. Twenty characters were selected from Chinese language textbooks each of for Grades 2 to 6 (Shu et al., 2003). In this task, the participants were required to read 100 characters aloud one-by-one, and testing stopped when the child failed to recognize 15 consecutive characters. One point was awarded for each correct character; the maximum score for Chinese character reading was 100. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.97.

Control Measures

Raven’s standard progressive matrices

The children’s non-verbal intelligence was tested by Sets A and B of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1996). For each test item, the children were asked to select an element from six options according to the pattern. Each correct was assigned one score, and the maximum score was 24. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.78.

Morphological awareness

The compounding production task was used to test the participants’ morphological awareness (Liu et al., 2017). The participants were asked to listen to a scenario and were asked to use their imagination to create a novel word to represent the scenario properly. For example, one item in this task was “www.frontiersin.org (What should we call an umbrella that is made of grass?” The answer for this item waswww.frontiersin.org (grass umbrella). A five-point scale (0–4) was utilized to rate the answer of participants. There was a total of 31 items in this task, and the maximum score was 124. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86.

Orthographic knowledge

A Chinese character decision task (Li et al., 2012) was included to assess the participants’ awareness of orthographic patterns. There were four types of stimuli according to the radical and orthographic rules. The first type was “real” characters (i.e., 45 items) with correct radical and legal orthographic rules in Chinese (e.g., www.frontiersin.org). In addition, there were 15 pseudocharacters with correct radical but illegal orthographic rules (e.g., www.frontiersin.org), 15 items with incorrect radical but legal orthographic rules (e.g., www.frontiersin.org), and 15 items with random combinations of strokes (e.g., www.frontiersin.org). The children were instructed to select “real” characters from the 90 characters, although they had not learned any of these characters before. The 45 items with correct radical and legal orthographic rules (i.e., “real” character) should be judged as characters since they consist the correct radical and legal orthographic rules. In contrast, the other 45 characters should be judged as non-characters due to their incorrect components or illegal orthographic rules. One score was recorded if the child judged the pseudocharacter as a character or rejected others as a character. There was a total of 90 items, and the maximum score was 90. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.81.

Results

Estimation of VTOTs

The visual thresholds were estimated for each run separately. The MATLAB toolbox psignifit (2.5.61; cf. Wichmann and Hill, 2001) was utilized to analyse the SOAs with 75% correct responses. The thresholds were labelled as misses if (a) the threshold value and/or slope value was negative; (b) the threshold value was larger than 492 ms (i.e., SOA of a “bonus” trial); and (c) slope > 500 or slope > mean slope + 1 SD. In the final analysis, only the best threshold value (lowest SOA) of the two independent runs was included (Steinbrink et al., 2014). Accordingly, 79 VTOTs could be determined in 154 children. The correlations between all measures and the mean age and results of non-verbal intelligence, VWM, VTOTs, Chinese character reading, VSTM, morphological awareness, and orthographic knowledge are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of correlations, means and standard deviations on age, non-verbal intelligence, visual working memory (VWM), visual temporal order threshold (VTOT), Chinese character reading, visual short-term memory (VSTM), morphological awareness, and orthographic knowledge.

To investigate the effects of VTOTs and WM on Chinese character reading, we conducted a moderation effect analysis. Model Number 1 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS with 5,000 bootstrap samples was used, as suggested by Hayes (2017). The results are summarized in Table 2 and show significant predictive effects of VWM (p < 0.001), VTOTs (p = 0.02), and morphological awareness (p = 0.004) on Chinese character reading. Children with better performances in VWM, VTOTs, or morphological awareness had higher scores for Chinese character reading. More importantly, a significant interaction between VTOTs and VWM (p = 0.001) was found, which explained 11.45% of the variance in Chinese character recognition.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The results of moderation effect analysis.

To characterize the nature of this interaction further, the Johnson–Neymann (J–N) technique with 5,000 bootstrap samples was used, following suggestions and using the SPSS script provided by Hayes and Matthes (2009). The J–N technique allowed us to identify points directly in the range of the moderator variable (i.e., processing efficiency) where the effect of the predictor on the outcome transitions from being statistically significant to insignificant by finding the value of the moderator variable for which the ratio of the conditional effect to its standard error was equal to the critical t score. The conditional effect of VWM on Chinese character reading was (marginally) significant at a VTOT of 87.14, ß = 2.54, SE = 1.27, t = 2.00, p = 0.05, 95% CIs [0.00, 5.08], i.e., the 61th percentile of the distribution in our sample, as well as at a VTOT of 231.44, ß = −5.86, SE = 2.93, t = −2.00, p = 0.05, 95% CIs [−11.72, 0.00], i.e., the 22th percentile of the distribution in our sample. The relationship between WM and Chinese character reading was positively significant at VTOTs smaller than 87.14 ms, and negatively significant at VTOTs larger than 231.44 ms. This relationship was insignificant at VTOTs between these two thresholds (i.e., from 87.14 to 231.44) (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The relationships between working memory and Chinese character reading of children with different levels of VTOTs. VTOTs, visual temporal order thresholds.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition and the effect of processing efficiency on this relationship. Consistent with previous studies (Olson and Datta, 2002; Smith-Spark and Fisk, 2007; Chung et al., 2008; Peng et al., 2013; Wang and Yang, 2018; Wang et al., 2019), after controlling age, non-verbal intelligence, VSTM, morphological awareness, and orthographic awareness, the main effects of VWM and processing efficiency on Chinese character recognition were significant, which demonstrated their facilitating roles in Chinese children’s reading. Meanwhile, the current findings suggested a moderation effect of processing efficiency on the relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition. To illustrate, among children with average or above processing efficiency, there was a positive association between VWM and Chinese character recognition. In contrast, for children with poor processing efficiency, the association became negative.

VWM and Chinese Character Recognition

The results of the moderation effect analysis showed that the main effect of VWM on Chinese character reading was significant. The participants with better VWM capacity had higher scores for the Chinese character reading task, which supported a facilitating role of VWM capacity in Chinese character recognition. The VSTM did not predict the character recognition in the current sample, which suggested that the contribution of VWM to reading in Chinese should be due to the ability to process and maintain visual features simultaneously, rather than the ability to store information over a short period. An interpretation of these findings was that Chinese characters are constructed by visual components (e.g., strokes), which can be processed during the character recognition (Li et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2010). Keeping this visual information in the STM might be not enough for recognizing characters. As mentioned before, there are many similar characters that share visual components, such as www.frontiersin.org\da4\(big) and www.frontiersin.org\tai4\(too). In contrast, VWM enables children to distinguish the target character from other similar ones in their mental lexicon while the processing of visual forms is running (Baddeley, 2003; Barrouillet et al., 2004).

Another possible reason for this relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition might be related to the stroke order. The stroke order is required to achieve complete learning in Chinese (Tsai et al., 2012). The representation of stoke orders has been suggested as an independent representation for Chinese character reading (Shimomura, 1980; Flores d’Arcais, 1994). In addition to storing information, encoding and maintaining serial order information of perceived events is another important feature of WM (Ginsburg et al., 2017). With regard to the mechanism of the Corsi backward task for measuring children’s VWM in the current study (i.e., recalling the items in reverse sequence), in addition to remembering the items, the children were also required to remember and retrieve the visual sequences between items. Together, the reasons why VWM is beneficial to Chinese character acquisition and recognition may not only be that VWM can facilitate the storage of complex visual features of Chinese characters but also that it can facilitate the processing of serial information. In other words, VWM can help readers to process and maintain the forms and strokes of Chinese characters, and at the same time can assist them to integrate the strokes to construct the forms according to specific sequences.

Processing Efficiency in VWM and Chinese Character Recognition

Consistent with previous studies performing the TOJ task in Chinese (e.g., Chung et al., 2008; Wang and Yang, 2018), in general, the present findings indicated that the children with higher visual processing efficiency (i.e., smaller VTOTs) had better performances on the Chinese character reading task. The positive link between visual temporal processing and reading in Chinese suggested that the ability of typically developing children to integrate visual information that converges in rapid succession is critical in orthographic processing in Chinese. Because of the opaque orthography of Chinese characters, character reading requires processing the elements and spatial configurations of a character. The literature has reported the importance of visual-orthographic processing in Chinese reading (McBride-Chang et al., 2005; Siok et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2015). Due to the aforementioned weak correspondence between orthography and phonology in Chinese, Chinese readers must recognize and integrate the orthographic segments of Chinese characters, and then further match the pronunciations stored in the mental lexicon with these Chinese characters. This process requires speed and relies heavily on visual processing at the same time, so it is reasonable to see its connection to visual temporal processing.

In addition, as mentioned, the literature reports that the specific sequence of strokes is a part of mental representations of Chinese characters (Giovanni, 1994; Parkinson et al., 2010). The current results imply a role for beginning readers’ sensitivity to determine the order of two rapidly presented non-literal visuals in reading (i.e., first graders). Consistently, it has been suggested that a visual-order processing deficit may be related to reading difficulty (e.g., Farmer and Klein, 1995; Booth et al., 2000). Together, these findings suggest that the rate of processing and retrieving visual features sequentially might influence orthographic coding, which could subsequently impact character recognition (Chung et al., 2008).

More importantly, a moderation effect of processing efficiency on the relationship between VWM and Chinese character recognition was revealed. Consistent with our assumption, the facilitating effect of VWM on Chinese character recognition was observed only in children with average or high processing efficiency. This finding supports that the correlation between VWM and Chinese character recognition should be due to an overlapping component between them, which is related to the efficient processing. According to the TBRS model (Barrouillet et al., 2007), this component should be the ability to switch between processing and maintenance. To illustrate, Chinese character recognition requires holding segments of orthography, but this information is subject to loss (due to either decay or interference). The TBRS model (Barrouillet et al., 2007) suggests that children with higher processing efficiency have more flexibility to allocate their cognitive resources to the stimuli in processing and maintenance. Thus, these children should perform better at permitting decoding, i.e., the ability to map written language onto speech with accuracy (Gough and Tunmer, 1986). The efficient processing could help the children to reach completion of the cognitive task, such as Chinese character recognition, before the requisite information is lost (Jensen, 1993; Miller and Vernon, 1996).

On the other hand, an inhibiting effect of VWM on Chinese character recognition was found in children with poor processing efficiency. One possible interpretation of the inhibiting effect might be that, as per our assumption, the children with low processing efficiency used more cognitive resources to process and maintain visual features of characters. Consequently, they did not have enough resources to activate the corresponding sounds, thus leading to a negative link between VWM and reading (Paas et al., 2003). In addition, the inhibiting effect might be related to the ability to inhibit distractors during processing. Specifically, as we mentioned before, many characters have similar orthography, so children with poor processing efficiency might have difficulty distinguishing them during character recognition. In other words, poor processing efficiency may elicit difficulties in locating their attention to target information and inhibiting noise during learning. Subsequently, their learning effect decreased (van der Sluis et al., 2007). For example, in Chinese, many characters can be components of other characters; for example, www.frontiersin.org\hai4\(scare) can be a component of www.frontiersin.org\ge1\(cut). However, their pronunciations are distinctive. There is a possibility that the information of the component www.frontiersin.org\hai4\(scare) is activated during the recognizing of the character www.frontiersin.org\ge1\ (cut).

In addition, the results of the participants from the 21th to 60th percentile of the distribution in our sample showed a non-significant correlation between VWM and Chinese character recognition. It should be noted that these participants’ processing efficiency was not defined as poor, but their performances still did not reach the average in the present sample. The non-significant correlation supports a possibility that, for these children with relative low processing efficiency, the processing and maintenance of visual features did not use too many cognitive resources to impede character recognition. However, the remaining cognitive resources did not produce any significant benefit to the following processing, such as activating sounds and inhibiting distractors in the mental lexicon.

On the other hand, these correlations between VWM and Chinese character recognition in children with different levels of processing efficiency might be due to learning of characters. It is possible that children with higher processing efficiency may have more efficient strategies for learning. The efficient strategies may both enhance their VWM capacity and character acquisition, which would further lead to a positive association between VWM and Chinese character recognition. There is evidence that Chinese readers do chunk strokes of a character into components, radicals, or even a whole to benefit memory. It has been reported that, at the early stage of learning to read Chinese, visual inputs of complex characters are decomposed into radicals when characters are being processed (Chua, 1999; Wu et al., 1999). In addition to improving the acquisition of characters, these visual chunking skills can save memory resources and improve VWM capacity (Anderson et al., 2013). To examine Chinese children’s character processing, one study utilized a delay-coding task in which children were shown a Chinese character briefly and were asked to reproduce it by writing it on paper with a pencil after the sample was removed (Pak et al., 2005). The results showed that children with more efficient approaches to chunking visual orthographic information had higher accuracies in the copying task, as well as reading ability, than those with less-developed visual chunking skills. Accordingly, it is possible that, with more efficient visual chunking strategies, such as chunking strokes as radicals, children’s VWM capacity and character learning are improved simultaneously. In turn, the positive correlation between VWM and character reading could be established. However, since we did not investigate the learning strategy directly in the present study, this interpretation should be explored in future studies.

Limitations and Future Studies

First, students of only one grade were involved in the current study, and it has been reported that the relationship between VWM and reading may vary across grades (Gottardo et al., 1996; Berninger et al., 2010). With increasing grade levels, children’s reading experience accumulates, which may lead to automatic processing of characters. In other words, fewer cognitive resources are required for character reading. Thus, the relationships among processing efficiency, VWM, and Chinese character recognition might be different. To determine the role of grade in the relationships among these three variables, students of more grades should be involved in future studies. Additionally, a longitudinal study regarding the processing efficiency in VWM and Chinese character recognition should be conducted to help determine a more complete model of reading. Also, according to the criteria for VTOTs (Steinbrink et al., 2014), only 79 participants were taken into the moderation effect analysis. Although the J–N technique in PROCESS (Hayes, 2017) analyzed the data with 5,000 bootstrap samples to reduce the influence of the relative small sample size, a larger number of participants should be included in future studies to investigate whether the current findings would be the same.

Second, the current study focused on visual modality and orthographic processing in Chinese. Other modalities (e.g., auditory processing) and other aspects of characters (e.g., phonological processing) could be considered in the future. The findings regarding different modalities and character-related representations could provide a more complete understanding of the relationship between WM and reading. Third, only one reading ability was measured in the current study. Different reading tasks require different cognitive loads (Christopher et al., 2012; Oakhill and Cain, 2012), which may influence the relationship between VWM and reading and the moderation effect of processing efficiency. Other measures of reading at different levels, such as reading comprehension and reading fluency, should be considered in future studies. Finally, since (V)WM capacity and processing efficiency were measured in different tasks in the present study, the role of (V)WM efficiency in reading was not examined. Future studies should develop a task for measuring (V)WM capacity and efficiency simultaneously, which could improve the understanding of the mechanism of the relationship between WM and reading.

Theoretical and Educational Implications

The present study found the facilitating effect of VWM on Chinese character recognition. In addition, the moderation effect of processing efficiency on the relationship between VWM and reading was revealed. Consistent with the view of TBRS model (Barrouillet et al., 2004), the current findings demonstrated that high processing efficiency could alleviate cognitive load and save memory space for further processing and, in turn, reading performance could be improved. On the other hand, to ensure accuracy, low processing efficiency may require more cognitive resources. This means that the cognitive load could be heavier for children with low processing efficiency, which may impair their subsequent processing.

According to the current findings, to improve children’s reading ability at the word level, strategies for enhancing children’s processing efficiency should be introduced in learning. Although WM capacity is limited, higher processing efficiency could allow more items to be held in memory for a longer time (Gough and Tunmer, 1986; Jensen, 1993; Miller and Vernon, 1996). In addition to presenting stokes and forms of characters, the regulations of Chinese characters, which can enhance the processing efficiency, should be highlighted in teaching. For example, for most Chinese compound characters, the phonetic radical, which is related to the pronunciation of characters, is always presented on the left side of a character, whereas the semantic radical, which is related to the meanings of characters, is always present on the other side. With this regulation, children can save cognitive resources required to remember the location of the phonetic and semantic radicals. They will not confuse a components’ location within a character, and then, they can use more cognitive resources for further processing to benefit their acquisition and recognition.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Human Research Ethics Committee, The Education University of Hong Kong. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

ZX, L-CW, and DL designed and conducted tasks, analyzed the data, and co-wrote the manuscript. YC and LT conducted tasks, and contributed to the testing material design and preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by RGC Grant 18605019 of the Education University of Hong Kong in Hong Kong Special Administrative Region.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the children, their parents and teachers, for their participation.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://bootstrap-software.org/psignifit

References

Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., and Lebiere, C. (1996). Working memory: activation limitations on retrieval. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 221–256. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1996.0007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y. M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., and Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Sci. Stud. Read. 17, 41–56. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2012.689789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ang, S. Y., and Lee, K. (2008). Central executive involvement in children’s spatial memory. Memory 16, 918–933. doi: 10.1080/09658210802365347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., and Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol. Rev. 105, 158–173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., and Lieberman, K. (2017). “Spatial working memory,” in Exploring Working Memory, ed. A. Baddeley (London: Routledge), 206–223. doi: 10.4324/9781315111261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Sala, S. D., and Spinnler, H. (1986). Dementia and working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sec. A 38, 603–618. doi: 10.1080/14640748608401616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., and Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 83–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., and Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 570–585. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Swanson, H. L., Lovitt, D., Trivedi, P., Lin, S. J. C., et al. (2010). Relationship of word-and sentence-level working memory to reading and writing in second, fourth, and sixth grade. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 41, 179–193. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0002)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Mac Whinney, B., and Harasaki, Y. (2000). Developmental differences in visual and auditory processing of complex sentences. Child Dev. 71, 981–1003. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., and Alvarez, G. A. (2009). Compression in visual working memory: using statistical regularities to form more efficient memory representations. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 138, 487–502. doi: 10.1037/a0016797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., and Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain, K., Oakhill, J., and Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 31–42. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo, M. G. (1996). Phonological working memory and reading in test anxiety. Memory 4, 289–306. doi: 10.1080/096582196388960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Case, R., Kurland, D. M., and Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 33, 386–404. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(82)90054-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chee, M. W., Soon, C. S., Lee, H. L., and Pallier, C. (2004). Left insula activation: a marker for language attainment in bilinguals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 15265–15270. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403703101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., et al. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: a latent variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 470–488. doi: 10.1037/a0027375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chua, F. K. (1999). Phonological recoding in Chinese logograph recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25, 876–891. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, K. K., Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W., Tsang, S. M., and Lee, S. H. (2010). Cognitive profiles of Chinese adolescents with dyslexia. Dyslexia 16, 2–23. doi: 10.1002/dys.392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, K. K., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., and Ho, C. S. H. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 58, 15–35. doi: 10.1007/s11881-008-0015-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, N. (2017). The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 1158–1170. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1191-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., and Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, 309–331. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, M. E., and Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 460–493. doi: 10.3758/BF03210983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 6, 337–355.

Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: a critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 8, 259–272. doi: 10.1007/BF03174081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsburg, V., Archambeau, K., van Dijck, J.-P., Chetail, F., and Gevers, W. (2017). Coding of serial order in verbal, visual and spatial working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 146, 632–650. doi: 10.1037/xge0000278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovanni, B. G. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 6, 337–355. doi: 10.1080/09541449408406519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., and Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 63, 563–582. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.0062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, P. B., and Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remed. Spec. Educ. 7, 6–10. doi: 10.1177/074193258600700104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, J., and Bowey, J. A. (1994). Phonological analysis skills, verbal working memory, and reading ability in second-grade children. Child Dev. 65, 938–950.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., and Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 924–936. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K., Lee, S. H., and Tsang, S. M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 97, 61–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, H. S., and Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition 54, 73–98. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00641-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeffries, S., and Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: its role in dyslexia and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia 10, 196–214. doi: 10.1002/dys.278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, A. R. (1993). Why is reaction time correlated with psychometric g. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2, 53–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Just, M. A., and Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychol. Rev. 99, 122–149. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kibby, M. Y., Marks, W., Morgan, S., and Long, C. J. (2004). Specific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: a working memory approach. J. Learn. Disabil. 37, 349–363. doi: 10.1177/00222194040370040601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leek, M. R. (2001). Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research. Percept. Psychophys. 63, 1279–1292. doi: 10.3758/BF03194543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, C. K., Tse, S. K., Loh, K. Y., and Hau, K. T. (2008). Text comprehension in Chinese children: relative contribution of verbal working memory, pseudoword reading, rapid automatized naming, and onset-rime phonological segmentation. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 135–149. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H., and Peng, H. (2012). Chinese children’s character recognition: visuo-orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. J. Res. Read. 35, 287–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Fu, X., and Lin, Z. (2000). Study on the development of Chinese orthographic regularity in school children. Acta Psychol. Sin. 32, 121–126.

Google Scholar

Liu, D., Chen, X., and Chung, K. K. (2015). Performance in a visual search task uniquely predicts reading abilities in third-grade Hong Kong Chinese children. Sci. Stud. Read. 19, 307–324. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2015.1030749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Chung, K. K., McBride-Chang, C., and Tong, X. (2010). Holistic versus analytic processing: evidence for a different approach to processing of Chinese at the word and character levels in Chinese children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 107, 466–478. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Li, H., and Wong, K. S. R. (2017). The anatomy of the role of morphological awareness in Chinese character learning: the mediation of vocabulary and semantic radical knowledge and the moderation of morpheme family size. Sci. Stud. Read. 21, 210–224. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2017.1278764

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., and McBride-Chang, C. (2010). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 62–73. doi: 10.1037/a0016933

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Y., Wang, J., Wu, H., Zhu, D., and Zhang, Y. (2013). Working-memory training improves developmental dyslexia in Chinese children. Neural Regen. Res. 8, 452–460. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1673-5374.2013.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., and Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Read. Writ. 18, 99–128. doi: 10.1007/s11145-004-7343-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. T., and Vernon, P. A. (1996). Intelligence, reaction time, and working memory in 4-to 6-year-old children. Intelligence 22, 155–190. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90014-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakhill, J. V., and Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Sci. Stud. Read. 16, 91–121. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2010.529219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, I. R., and Datta, H. (2002). Visual-temporal processing in reading-disabled and normal twins. Read. Writ. 15, 127–149. doi: 10.1023/A:1013872422108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, I. R., and Jiang, Y. (2004). Visual short-term memory is not improved by training. Mem. Cogn. 32, 1326–1332. doi: 10.3758/BF03206323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, I. R., Jiang, Y., and Moore, K. S. (2005). Associative learning improves visual working memory performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 889–900. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opitz, B., Schneiders, J. A., Krick, C. M., and Mecklinger, A. (2014). Selective transfer of visual working memory training on Chinese character learning. Neuropsychologia 53, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paas, F., Renkl, A., and Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: recent developments. Educ. Psychol. 38, 1–4. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pak, A. K., Cheng-Lai, A., Tso, I. F., Shu, H., Li, W., and Anderson, R. C. (2005). Visual chunking skills of Hong Kong children. Read. Writ. 18, 437–454. doi: 10.1007/s11145-005-6575-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkinson, J., Dyson, B. J., and Khurana, B. (2010). Line by line: the ERP correlates of stroke order priming in letters. Exp. Brain Res. 201, 575–586. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2077-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, P., Tao, S., and Li, B. (2013). The deficit profile of working memory, inhibition, and updating in Chinese children with reading difficulties. Learn. Indiv. Differ. 25, 111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., and Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychol. Rev. 112, 43–59. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J. (1996). The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 1–48. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B. A., and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2001). Age-related changes in temporal processing: implications for speech perception. Semin. Hearing 22, 227–240. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-15628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimomura, T. (1980). “Informatics: input and output: science of the stroke sequence of Kanji,” in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Computational Linguistics, ed. A. Gelbukh (Berlin: Springer-Verlag), 270–273.

Google Scholar

Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., and Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: implications for learning to read. Child Dev. 74, 27–47. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., and Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 122–133. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, L. S., and Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Dev. 60, 973–980.

Google Scholar

Siok, W. T., Spinks, J. A., Jin, Z., and Tan, L. H. (2009). Developmental dyslexia is characterized by the co-existence of visuospatial and phonological disorders in Chinese children. Curr. Biol. 19, R890–R892. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith-Spark, J. H., and Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory 15, 34–56. doi: 10.1080/09658210601043384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

So, D., and Siegel, L. S. (1997). Learning to read Chinese: semantic, syntactic, phonological and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Chinese readers. Read. Writ. 9, 1–21. doi: 10.1023/A:1007963513853

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srihari, S. N., Yang, X., and Ball, G. R. (2007). Offline Chinese handwriting recognition: an assessment of current technology. Front. Comp. Sci. China 1:137–155. doi: 10.1007/s11704-007-0015-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinbrink, C., Zimmer, K., Lachmann, T., Dirichs, M., and Kammer, T. (2014). Development of rapid temporal processing and its impact on literacy skills in primary school children. Child Dev. 85, 1711–1726. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, J. R., Osman, H., and Schafer, E. C. (2015). The effect of noise on the relationship between auditory working memory and comprehension in school-age children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58, 1043–1051. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L. (1999). What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Dev. Psychol. 35, 986–1000. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L., Ashbaker, M. H., and Lee, C. (1996). Learning-disabled readers’ working memory as a function of processing demands. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 61, 242–275. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.0016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L., and Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004). The relationship between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 471–491. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.3.471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L., Zheng, X., and Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: a selective meta-analysis of the literature. J. Learn. Disabil. 42, 260–287. doi: 10.1177/0022219409331958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. H., Hoosain, R., and Siok, W. W. T. (1996). Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22, 865–882. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., and Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 8781–8785. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503523102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. H., Kuo, C. H., Horng, W. B., and Chen, C. W. (2012). Effects on learning logographic character formation in computer-assisted handwriting instruction. Lang. Learn. Technol. 16, 110–130.

Google Scholar

van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., and van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence 35, 427–449. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Merrienboer, J. J., and Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 17, 147–177. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L. C., Liu, D., and Xu, Z. (2019). Distinct effects of visual and auditory temporal processing training on reading and reading-related abilities in Chinese children with dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 69, 166–185. doi: 10.1007/s11881-019-00176-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L. C., and Yang, H. M. (2018). Temporal processing development in Chinese primary school–aged children with dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 51, 302–312. doi: 10.1177/0022219416680798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., McBride-Chang, C., and Chan, S. F. (2014). Correlates of Chinese kindergarteners’ word reading and writing: the unique role of copying skills. Read. Writ. 27, 1281–1302. doi: 10.1007/s11145-013-9486-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Yin, L., and McBride, C. (2015). Unique predictors of early reading and writing: a one-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergarteners. Early Childhood Res. Q. 32, 51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichmann, F. A., and Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept. Psychophys. 63, 1293–1313. doi: 10.3758/BF03194544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittmann, M., and Fink, M. (2004). Time and language-critical remarks on diagnosis and training methods of temporal-order judgment. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 64, 341–348.

Google Scholar

Wu, N., Zhou, X., and Shu, H. (1999). Sublexical processing in reading Chinese: a development study. Lang. Cogn. Process. 14, 503–524. doi: 10.1080/016909699386176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, M., Yang, J., Siok, W. T., and Tan, L. H. (2015). Atypical lateralization of phonological working memory in developmental dyslexia. J. Neuroling. 33, 67–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: processing speed, serial order, temporal sensitivity, visual processing, word recognition

Citation: Xu Z, Wang L-C, Liu D, Chen Y and Tao L (2020) The Moderation Effect of Processing Efficiency on the Relationship Between Visual Working Memory and Chinese Character Recognition. Front. Psychol. 11:1899. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01899

Received: 22 January 2020; Accepted: 09 July 2020;
Published: 05 August 2020.

Edited by:

Lawrence Jun Zhang, The University of Auckland, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Yunhua Shen, Jilin University, China
Min Wang, Xi’an Jiaotong University, China

Copyright © 2020 Xu, Wang, Liu, Chen and Tao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Duo Liu, duoliu@eduhk.hk

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.