Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 04 August 2020
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Consumer’s Behavior beyond Self-Report View all 15 articles

Mental Shopping Calculations: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study

  • 1Faculty of Educational Studies, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
  • 2Action and Cognition Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland

One of the most critical skills behind consumer’s behavior is the ability to assess whether a price after a discount is a real bargain. Yet, the neural underpinnings and cognitive mechanisms associated with such a skill are largely unknown. While there is general agreement that the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) on the left is critical for mental calculations, and there is also recent repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) evidence pointing to the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) of the right PPC as crucial for consumer-like arithmetic (e.g., multi-digit mental addition or subtraction), it is still unknown whether SMG is involved in calculations of sale prices. Here, we show that the neural mechanisms underlying discount arithmetic characteristic for shopping are different from complex addition or subtraction, with discount calculations engaging left SMG more. We obtained these outcomes by remodeling our laboratory to resemble a shop and asking participants to calculate prices after discounts (e.g., $8.80–25 or $4.80–75%), while stimulating left and right SMG with neuronavigated rTMS. Our results indicate that such complex shopping calculations as establishing the price after a discount involve SMG asymmetrically, whereas simpler calculations such as price addition do not. These findings have some consequences for neural models of mathematical cognition and shed some preliminary light on potential consumer’s behavior in natural settings.

Introduction

Neuropsychological studies in patients and neuroimaging reports in healthy individuals show that the neural circuits for mental calculations form a complex network of interacting areas. They involve the parietal lobes, e.g., the bilateral intraparietal sulcus, the posterior subdivisions of the superior parietal lobules, and the left angular and supramarginal gyri of the inferior parietal lobule (Gobel et al., 2001; Naccache and Dehaene, 2001; Dehaene et al., 2003, 2004; Eger et al., 2003; Fias et al., 2003; Piazza et al., 2004; Pinel et al., 2004; Nieder, 2005; Sato et al., 2007; Andres et al., 2008; Brozzoli et al., 2008; Domahs et al., 2008, 2010; Kaufmann et al., 2008), as well as the temporal lobes, e.g., the posterior inferior temporal gyrus (Daitch et al., 2016; Hermes et al., 2017; Yeo et al., 2017; Pinheiro-Chagas et al., 2018, 2019) and even the left frontal lobe, with the Broca’s area and its vicinity (Schmithorst and Brown, 2004; Shuman and Kanwisher, 2004; Majerus et al., 2010). While it is still unknown what subdivision of this network is the most critical for mental arithmetic, there is some agreement that such an area should be located in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) on the left (Dehaene et al., 2004; Nieder, 2005). However, recent studies also suggest that the right PPC may exert some role in such mental operations too (Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Knops and Willmes, 2014; Sokolowski et al., 2017; Mock et al., 2018). Indeed, there is evidence that the more difficult/complex the arithmetic task, the more profound the engagement of the right PPC (Fehr et al., 2008; Hamid et al., 2011; Vansteensel et al., 2014; Klichowski and Kroliczak, 2017; Mock et al., 2018; cf. Semenza and Benavides-Varela, 2017; Semenza et al., 2017). Whether or not these earlier findings are relevant for more down-to-earth tasks such as shopping arithmetic, especially discount calculations, still remains to be seen.

The key evidence to resolve this issue could come from the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), in particular repetitive TMS (rTMS), because TMS makes it possible to demonstrate the causal role of a selected brain area in the operations in question. Meanwhile, rTMS has been used only in studies on simple mental calculations (i.e., based on single digits; Salillas et al., 2012; Maurer et al., 2015) that are far removed from such daily chores as calculating prices in consumers’ heads. To the best of our knowledge, only one rTMS study thus far has examined multi-digit mental addition (e.g., 23 + 26) and subtraction (e.g., 49–26), which are much closer to numbers that consumers are likely to operate on, showing an atypical lateralization of the rTMS effect (right > left) for the supramarginal gyrus (SMG), a part of PPC (Montefinese et al., 2017). This effect is probably related to the fact that in the case of complex mental calculations on rather unfamiliar or accidental numbers, the visual working memory is strongly burdened, and there is also a need for maintaining intermediate results, and therefore, increased attentional resources (Majerus et al., 2010; Fias et al., 2013; Sokolowski et al., 2017). In fact, such difficult, memory-based, processes are supported by the right SMG (Mock et al., 2018). However, it is not known whether or not the right SMG is involved in other types of complex arithmetic, e.g., calculations of sale prices that are characteristic of everyday human activities (Dastjerdi et al., 2013). An example is shopping, wherein for discounts one typically subtracts a percentage value from small multi-digit numbers, i.e., the ones with values after the decimal point (e.g., 8.80–50%).

Depending on the type of task, as well as on the conditions in which a given task is performed, different neural mechanisms might be invoked for disparate kinds of mental calculations (Curtis et al., 2016; Yusoff et al., 2016; Beller et al., 2018; Hickendorff et al., 2019). For example, complex addition or subtraction is based on mechanisms that are distinct from the ones for complex multiplication or division. Similarly, school math calculation may engage different neural resources from those used in a store. Indeed, different complex calculations can be associated with disparate neural processes (Li et al., 2018), and therefore, can differentially/asymmetrically engage different subdivisions of the relevant neural circuits (Yusoff et al., 2016; Hawes et al., 2019). Thereby, one cannot assume that in the case of sale price calculations the role of the right SMG will be the same as in the case of multi-digit mental addition or subtraction (Montefinese et al., 2017). In fact, operating on familiar numbers that are often seen on shop labels (e.g., −50% or −25%) – even though they are multi-digit, and therefore, complex – may not require right-hemisphere resources such as visual working memory to the extent than does operating on unfamiliar numbers (which put greater load on memory capacity, and consequently, require critical engagement of right SMG; Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2011; Bloechle et al., 2016; Nemati et al., 2017; Mock et al., 2018). Therefore, we hypothesized that the right, as compared to left SMG, might be less critical for calculations of sale prices characteristic for consumers’ daily behavior. We did not expect to see any differential engagement of SMG from the two hemispheres in our control task, that is, price addition.

The outcomes we obtained do not rule out that the right SMG is involved in complex mental calculation, but its greater engagement is not specific for all complex arithmetic. Indeed, in discount arithmetic its role is less pronounced and the left SMG dominates instead. While these findings may have some consequences for neural models of mathematical cognition too, they provide one of the first pieces of evidence unveiling the neural substrates critical for consumer’s behavior.

Materials and Methods

Our study was assessed and approved by the local Ethics Committee for Research Involving Human Subjects (opinion #9/10/01/2018). As such, all procedures and manipulations were carried out in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki 2013 and its recent amendments.

Participants

Twenty healthy volunteers (14 women, age: 20–27, mean = 20.9, SD = 1.6) who took part in this study gave their written informed consents prior to participation. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. As confirmed by the results of the revised version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Dragovic, 2004), nearly all volunteers (18) declared themselves as right-handed (laterality quotient = 93.6, SD = 16.3, laterality score = 60.0, SD = 12.4), whereas two of them were left-handed (laterality quotient = −58.3, SD = 58.9, laterality score = −37.5, SD = 38.9). All participants were reimbursed for their time and efforts, obtaining 50 PLN (approximately $13 USD) in the form of a gift card to redeem at a shop with books and CDs.

Stimuli and Procedure

To emulate the processes characteristic for shopping, we remodeled our laboratory so that it resembled a shop (see Figure 1). For example, at the entrance to the lab there was a shop sign with the opening hours. Next to the TMS chair, we put a shopping basket full of to-be-purchased items, and a supermarket shelf displaying different products and their prices.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup. (A) The tracking system for the head and coil. (B) A supermarket shelf. (C) One of the possible positions of the TMS coil (here over the right SMG). (D) A shopping basket. The translations of labels in Polish are the following: “cena” = “price,” “1 szt.” = “one piece,” “prosimy o przemyslane zakupy” = “please purchase carefully,” and “otwarte” = “open”.

We used stimuli in the form of shop labels with numbers consisting of values with decimal points and values of discounts. Example stimuli are shown in Figures 2A,B. A participant’s task was to state the price after a discount (e.g., 8.80 PLN − 50% = 4.40 PLN, see Figure 2D), and in the control task, a sum of two prices (e.g., 2.50 PLN + 3.50 PLN = 6 PLN, see Figure 2C). The two tasks were presented in counterbalanced blocks of trials. The order of trials within blocks was pseudorandomized for different participants. Each block consisted of 40 trials, including 20 trials with rTMS. The whole study consisted of two sessions: one with stimulation over the right SMG and the other over the left SMG, with the order of stimulation sites counterbalanced across participants. Before the actual experiment, a noTMS training session (i.e., two blocks consisting of 10 trials) was administered. The stimuli used during the training session did not appear in the subsequent experiment.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Example stimuli and trial structures. (A) Examples of stimuli for adding two prices. (B) Examples of stimuli for calculating the price after the discount. (C) Trial structure for adding two prices: In each trial, after a fixation point presented on a black screen for 500 ms, two shop labels were shown for a maximum of 6 s. After the onset of a participant’s vocal response, a black screen was introduced for a period of “6.5 s minus a response time” prior to the next trial (or in its absence, after a 6-s stimulus duration, for additional 500 ms). (D) Trial structure for calculating the prices after a discount: In each trial, after a fixation point presented on a black screen for 500 ms, one shop label and a discount value was shown for a maximum of 6 s (in the case of no response). As before, after the onset of a participant’s vocal response, a black screen was introduced for a period of 6.5 s minus a response time or minus stimulus duration. The translations of labels in Polish are the following: “cena” = “price”, “1 szt.” = “one piece”.

A given stimulus was displayed on a 119.38 cm (47-inch) LG® TV screen (60 Hz, LG Electronics Inc., Seoul, Korea) and placed in front of the TMS chair at a distance of 2.28 m. Stimulus presentation was controlled by Cedrus® SuperLab for Windows version 4.5.4 (Cedrus® Corp., San Pedro, CA, USA) installed on a Dell® OptiPlex® 7010 computer (Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA) and synchronized with the TMS stimulator using National Instruments PCI-DIO24 Digital I/O Card. Participants provided their answers vocally, and the experimenter constantly monitored their accuracies. Response times (RTs) – as measured by the onsets of vocal reactions – were detected by the SV-1 Smart Voice Key (Cedrus® Corp., San Pedro, CA, USA). A given stimulus was preceded by a fixation point in the form of a white cross over a black background and displayed for 500 ms. Following a response or after 6 s in the case of no response, a black screen was introduced between successive trials. Its duration was equal to 6.5 s minus RTs for the stimulus preceding it or 500 ms, if the participant did not provide an answer. Thus, the duration time for one trial was 7 s in total, but given that it was the experimenter who initiated the next trial only after confirming that the coil is still in the proper place, there was no risk of accumulation effects for subsequent series of stimulations (Rossi et al., 2009).

rTMS Protocol

We adopted an rTMS protocol from an earlier study by Montefinese et al. (2017). Following the onset of a stimulus, namely at 100 ms, the rTMS train consisting of three (10 Hz) pulses – i.e., the first pulse at 100 ms, the second at 200 ms, and the last one at 300 ms – was delivered with a Magstim® Rapid 2 Plus 1 stimulator using a figure-of-eight coil (with the inner diameter of 70 mm). The angle of the coil relative to the mid-sagittal plane of the participant’s head was maintained at 45°, as it is the optimal coil orientation for stimulating PPC areas (Janssen et al., 2015). As in previous studies, a fixed stimulation intensity of 65% of the maximum stimulator output was used (for a review of the justifications for using fixed stimulation intensity, see Robertson et al., 2003; Vesia et al., 2010; Montefinese et al., 2017). Such stimulation protocol should result in a slowdown of RTs following rTMS. The TMS targets (the right and left SMG) were located with frameless stereotaxic neuronavigation (Brainsight® Frameless, Rogue Research Inc., Montreal, QC) and marked on the MNI ICBM 152 average brain template. Subsequently, participants’ brains were warped to this standard space with the Brainsight 2.3.9 registration algorithm for the MNI model head, utilizing five best landmarks, such as the frontmost, backmost, topmost, leftmost, and rightmost points on the skull in the scaling-step procedure. The coordinates of the SMG were selected based on previous studies (e.g., Potok et al., 2019; for details, see Figure 3). The positions of the coil and the head were constantly monitored with the Polaris® Vicra Optical Tracking System (Northern Digital Inc., Waterloo, ON, Canada) in real time (see Figure 1A). The coil was held tangentially and perpendicular to the surface of the scalp, its position was secured with a dual rod articulated arm (Manfrotto® 244N Variable Friction Magic Arm, Bassano del Grappa, Italy, see Figure 1C), and if necessary fine-tuned on-line by the experimenter. Moreover, to minimize head movements, participants’ heads were immobilized with the headrest and side support (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The coordinates of our rTMS targets in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template-brain space. (A) The sagittal view of the right SMG site. (B) The sagittal view of the left SMG site. (C) The frontal-plane view. (D) The transverse-plane view. (E) TMS target site rendered over the standard brain with TMS coil orientation for the right SMG. (F) TMS target site rendered over the standard brain with TMS coil orientation for the left SMG.

Data Analyses

In addition to comparisons between means, the main dependent variable in the critical analyses described below was the difference in RTs between rTMS over the left SMG and rTMS over the right SMG, referenced to noTMS baseline. In such comparisons, negative values indicate response facilitation and positive values indicate response delay following rTMS. The adopted level of significance was α = 0.05. For RTs accompanying correctly performed tasks, outliers greater than 6 s were discarded. All statistical analyses were carried out using IBM® SPSS Statistics® for Mac Version 26.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). To compare RTs in disparate conditions and differences between rTMS effects over the left and right SMG, the following analyses were performed. Because the two tasks require quite different calculations, we ran two separate repeated-measures analyses of variance (rmANOVAs), one for adding prices and one for calculating discounts, with neural state (noTMS, rTMS-over-the-left-SMG, and rTMS-over-the-right-SMG) as the within-subjects factor. The necessary post hoc tests were performed with additional Bonferroni correction.

Results

Neither the accuracy for adding prices nor calculation of discounts was affected by rTMS applied to the left or right SMG. Therefore, further analyses are limited to RTs for correctly performed calculations in each task.

The following rmANOVA for adding two prices revealed no significant effect whatsoever, regardless of the stimulation side (F(2, 38) = 0.02, p = 0.98, see Figure 4A, where these results are expressed as difference scores, relative to noTMS baseline). Yet, for discount calculations rTMS affected RTs differently (F(2, 38) = 3.6, p < 0.05), and the effect was such that only rTMS applied to the left SMG, as compared to right SMG, resulted in a significant slowdown of RTs (the difference between means = 92 ms, SE = 34 ms, Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.05). These results are, again, expressed as difference scores in Figure 4B. None of the results changed when the two left-handed participants were removed.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Results. (A) Adding two prices on shop labels: When we contrasted rTMS-over-the-left-SMG trials and rTMS-over-the-right-SMG trials, there were no significant differences in response times. (B) Calculating the prices after a typical shopping discount: When we contrasted rTMS-over-the-left-SMG trials with rTMS-over-the-right-SMG trials, there was a significant delay in response times following left SMG stimulations. Asterisk (*) indicates significant p (p < 0.05) and “0” the no-stimulation baseline. Error bars depict standards errors of the means.

Discussion

The outcomes of this study, based on the selected stimulation coordinates, as well as our shop-price label set, clearly show that it was the left SMG, as compared to its counterpart on the right, that was involved more (i.e., was asymmetrically engaged) in such shopping arithmetic as calculating discounts. The same type of stimulation, on the other hand, did not show any hemispheric asymmetries for adding prices. Discount calculation, a kind of task which is frequently performed by consumers before purchasing items in large superstores must, therefore, be dependent on a different involvement of key areas of the neural circuits underlying complex mental arithmetic (cf. Montefinese et al., 2017). One can only speculate that in less familiar settings and with less common prices/labels, a different asymmetric engagement of SMG would be revealed. Whether or not, under such circumstances, consumers are likely to calculate real prices after discounts, rather than trusting the sellers that there are bargains behind the “–XX%” labels, is an open question. Yet, consistent with some earlier evidence discussed above, if the size of the discount (the real price) was to be compared across different stores, the neural asymmetry for such processing could easily change its direction.

Importantly, our results indicate that the specific stimulation site of our choice, located in a rather anterior subdivision of the left SMG, plays a causal role in such highly complex arithmetic as discount price calculations. The greater engagement of left SMG, as compared to the right one, is consistent with a notion that this subdivision of PPC is also a part of the praxis representation network (PRN) for representing and sequencing highly complex and skilled actions in the typical brain (Przybylski and Kroliczak, 2017; see also Klichowski et al., 2020). After all, mathematical cognition is often linked to skilled manual/finger operations, and thus praxis (Rugani et al., 2017; Ras et al., 2019). Such embodiment of mental calculation may partly result from counting out loud and using fingers in learning arithmetic in childhood, when counting is a huge challenge (Brozzoli et al., 2008; Domahs et al., 2008). Because it takes longer for children to use fingers for more complex calculations than for less complex ones, therefore, the asymmetrically organized, more left-lateralized PRN can be a common framework for these types of mental arithmetic (Rapin, 2016).

Consistent with some of the above-mentioned arguments, it is quite unlikely that disparate kinds of mathematical, algebraic, or even shopping operations on price label contents or money, would be critically contingent on one specific mechanism, whether or not linked to SMG as a dominant node in a neural circuit. In fact, earlier research (e.g., Fias et al., 2003; Harvey et al., 2017; Kersey and Cantlon, 2017) shows the involvement of the intraparietal sulcus, the nearby subdivisions of the superior, as well as the inferior parietal lobule (including the angular gyrus) in different tasks involving numerosity processing and numbers. Thus, our null effect following right SMG TMS in consumers’ discount calculations does not preclude that more posterior or superior stimulation sites would reveal their causal contributions to such a task. Indeed, the outcomes of an earlier study by Montefinese et al. (2017) give pointers as to what kinds of calculations and stimulation sites should be used to disclose disparate neural mechanisms contributing to higher-level processing of numbers or supporting operations for them. Further questions to be also considered are differences in the timing when specific nodes of such a network contribute to disparate calculation tasks. It is, for example, quite possible that a selection of a different stimulation interval could interfere more with the discount-calculation task studied here. Whether or not this would also require different stimulation coordinates is yet another question to be addressed in future studies.

In sum, based on evidence from rTMS, our report demonstrates that left SMG, as compared to its right counterpart, plays a greater role in calculating prices after discounts. No effect of rTMS applied to left or right SMG was observed in our control task, that is, price addition. Yet, following study conclusion our participants claimed that TMS affected their ability to perform both tasks. This was clearly not the case. A change in protocol – including different timing of stimulation intervals, e.g., better correlated with proper chunking of responses leading to successful price calculations, would be required to show a casual contribution of SMG (especially SMG on the right) or other brain areas to both of the studied, but also related, tasks. While our positive findings add new knowledge to basic understanding of parietal lobe contributions to shopping mental calculations, further research is no doubt needed to elucidate the complexities of neural contributions to more sophisticated consumers’ behavior, both in and well beyond their self-reports.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding authors.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics committee of Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

During the preparation of this manuscript, MK was supported by scholarship for young outstanding scientists funded by the Ministry of Science and Higher Education in Poland (Ministerstwo Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyzszego, MNiSW, award #0049/E-336/STYP/11/2016, https://www.gov.pl/web/nauka/) and by European Cooperation in Science and Technology grant: European Network on Brain Malformations (Neuro-MIG; CA COST Action CA16118, https://www.cost.eu). COST is supported by the EU Framework Programme for Research and Innovation Horizon 2020. All equipment used was funded by the Ministry of Science and Higher Education (MNiSW, https://www.gov.pl/web/nauka/) grant 6168/IA/128/2012 to GK. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Agnieszka Cybal-Michalska, Dean of Faculty of Educational Study at Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznan (https://wse.amu.edu.pl), for purchasing gift cards for our participants.

References

Andres, M., Olivier, E., and Badets, A. (2008). Actions, words, and numbers: a motor contribution to semantic processing? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 313–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00597.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsalidou, M., and Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2 + 2= 4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage 54, 2382–2393. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beller, S., Bender, A., Chrisomalis, S., Jordan, F. M., Overmann, K. A., Saxe, G. B., et al. (2018). The cultural challenge in mathematical cognition. J. Numer. Cogn. 4, 448–463. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v4i2.137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloechle, J., Huber, S., Bahnmueller, J., Rennig, J., Willmes, K., Cavdaroglu, S., et al. (2016). Fact learning in complex arithmetic—the role of the angular gyrus revisited. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3061–3079. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brozzoli, C., Ishihara, M., Gobel, S. M., Salemme, R., Rossetti, Y., and Farne, A. (2008). Touch perception reveals the dominance of spatial over digital representation of numbers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 5644–5648. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708414105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, E. T., Huebner, M. G., and LeFevre, J. A. (2016). The relationship between problem size and fixation patterns during addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. J. Numer. Cogn. 2, 91–115. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v2i2.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daitch, A. L., Foster, B. L., Schrouff, J., Rangarajan, V., Kasikci, I., Gattas, S., et al. (2016). Mapping human temporal and parietal neuronal population activity and functional coupling during mathematical cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, E7277–E7286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608434113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dastjerdi, M., Ozker, M., Foster, B. L., Rangarajan, V., and Parvizi, J. (2013). Numerical processing in the human parietal cortex during experimental and natural conditions. Nat. Commun. 4:2528. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., and Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., and Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domahs, F., Krinzinger, H., and Willmes, K. (2008). Mind the gap between both hands: evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children’s mental calculation. Cortex 44, 359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., and Nuerk, H. -C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition 116, 251–266. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dragovic, M. (2004). Towards an improved measure of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory: a one-factor congeneric measurement model using confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality 9, 411–419. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M. O., Giraud, A. L., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2003). A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 37, 719–725. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00036-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fehr, T., Code, C., and Herrmann, M. (2008). Auditory task presentation reveals predominantly right hemispheric fMRI activation patterns during mental calculation. Neurosci. Lett. 431, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.11.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., and Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 47–56. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fias, W., Menon, V., and Szucs, D. (2013). Multiple components of developmental dyscalculia. Trend. Neurosci. Educ. 2, 43–47. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobel, S., Walsh, V., and Rushworth, M. F. (2001). The mental number line and the human angular gyrus. NeuroImage 14, 1278–1289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamid, A. I. A., Yusoff, A. N., Mukari, S. Z. -M. S., and Mohamad, M. (2011). Brain activation during addition and subtraction tasks in-noise and in-quiet. Malays. J. Med. Sci. 18, 3–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Harvey, B. M., Ferri, S., and Orban, G. A. (2017). Comparing parietal quantity-processing mechanisms between humans and macaques. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 779–793. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawes, Z., Sokolowski, H. M., Ononye, C. B., and Ansari, D. (2019). Neural underpinnings of numerical and spatial cognition: an fMRI meta-analysis of brain regions associated with symbolic number, arithmetic, and mental rotation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 103, 316–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermes, D., Rangarajan, V., Foster, B. L., King, J. R., Kasikci, I., Miller, K. J., et al. (2017). Electrophysiological responses in the ventral temporal cortex during reading of numerals and calculation. Cereb. Cortex 27, 567–575. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickendorff, M., Torbeyns, J., and Verschaffel, L. (2019). “Multi-digit addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division strategies” in International handbook of mathematical learning difficulties. eds. A. Fritz, V. Haase, and P. Rasanen (Cham: Springer), 543–560.

Google Scholar

Janssen, A. M., Oostendorp, T. F., and Stegeman, D. F. (2015). The coil orientation dependency of the electric field induced by TMS for M1 and other brain areas. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 12:47. doi: 10.1186/s12984-015-0036-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, L., Vogel, S. E., Wood, G., Kremser, C., Schocke, M., Zimmerhackl, L. B., et al. (2008). A developmental fMRI study of nonsymbolic numerical and spatial processing. Cortex 44, 376–385. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersey, A. J., and Cantlon, J. F. (2017). Neural tuning to numerosity relates to perceptual tuning in 3–6-year-old children. J. Neurosci. 37, 512–522. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0065-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klichowski, M., and Kroliczak, G. (2017). Numbers and functional lateralization: a visual half-field and dichotic listening study in proficient bilinguals. Neuropsychologia 100, 93–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klichowski, M., Nowik, A. M., Kroliczak, G., and Lewis, J. W. (2020). Functional lateralization of tool-sound and action-word processing in a bilingual brain. Health Psychol. Rep. 8, 10–30. doi: 10.5114/hpr.2020.92718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knops, A., and Willmes, K. (2014). Numerical ordering and symbolic arithmetic share frontal and parietal circuits in the right hemisphere. NeuroImage 84, 786–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Liu, D., Li, M., Dong, W., Huang, Y., and Chen, Q. (2018). Addition and subtraction but not multiplication and division cause shifts of spatial attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majerus, S., D’Argembeau, A., Martinez Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., et al. (2010). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2570–2593. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, S., Tanigawa, N., Sollmann, N., Hauck, T., Ille, S., Boeckh-Behrens, T., et al. (2015). Non-invasive mapping of calculation function by repetitive navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 3927–3947. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1136-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mock, J., Huber, S., Bloechle, J., Dietrich, J. F., Bahnmueller, J., Rennig, J., et al. (2018). Magnitude processing of symbolic and non-symbolic proportions: an fMRI study. Behav. Brain Funct. 14:9. doi: 10.1186/s12993-018-0141-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montefinese, M., Turco, C., Piccione, F., and Semenza, C. (2017). Causal role of the posterior parietal cortex for two-digit mental subtraction and addition: a repetitive TMS study. NeuroImage 155, 72–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naccache, L., and Dehaene, S. (2001). The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobe. Cereb. Cortex 11, 966–974. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nemati, P., Schmid, J., Soltanlou, M., Krimly, J. T., Nuerk, H. C., and Gawrilow, C. (2017). Planning and self-control, but not working memory, directly predict multiplication performance in adults. J. Numer. Cogn. 3, 441–467. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i2.61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieder, A. (2005). Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 177–190. doi: 10.1038/nrn1626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., and Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44, 547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., and Dehaene, S. (2004). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron 41, 983–993. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00107-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Daitch, A., Parvizi, J., and Dehaene, S. (2018). Brain mechanisms of arithmetic: a crucial role for ventral temporal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1757–1772. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Piazza, M., and Dehaene, S. (2019). Decoding the processing stages of mental arithmetic with magnetoencephalography. Cortex 114, 124–139. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potok, W., Maskiewicz, A., Kroliczak, G., and Marangon, M. (2019). The temporal involvement of the left supramarginal gyrus in planning functional grasps: a neuronavigated TMS study. Cortex 111, 16–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, L., and Kroliczak, G. (2017). Planning functional grasps of simple tools invokes the hand-independent praxis representation network: an fMRI study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 108–120. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716001120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapin, I. (2016). Dyscalculia and the calculating brain. Pediatr. Neurol. 61, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ras, M., Nowik, A. M., Klawiter, A., and Kroliczak, G. (2019). When is the brain ready for mental actions? Readiness potential for mental calculations. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 79, 319–331. doi: 10.21307/ane-2019-036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, E. M., Theoret, H., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2003). Studies in cognition: the problems solved and created by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 948–960. doi: 10.1162/089892903770007344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg-Lee, M., Chang, T. T., Young, C. B., Wu, S., and Menon, V. (2011). Functional dissociations between four basic arithmetic operations in the human posterior parietal cortex: a cytoarchitectonic mapping study. Neuropsychologia 49, 2592–2608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., Pascual-Leone, A., The Safety of TMS Consensus Group (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rugani, R., Betti, S., Ceccarini, F., and Sartori, L. (2017). Act on numbers: numerical magnitude influences selection and kinematics of finger movement. Front. Psychol. 8:1481. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salillas, E., Semenza, C., Basso, D., Vecchi, T., and Siegal, M. (2012). Single pulse TMS induced disruption to right and left parietal cortex on addition and multiplication. NeuroImage 59, 3159–3165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rizzolatti, G., and Gallese, V. (2007). Numbers within our hands: modulation of corticospinal excitability of hand muscles during numerical judgment. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 684–693. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmithorst, V. J., and Brown, R. D. (2004). Empirical validation of the triple-code model of numerical processing for complex math operations using functional MRI and group independent component analysis of the mental addition and subtraction of fractions. NeuroImage 22, 1414–1420. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semenza, C., and Benavides-Varela, S. (2017). Reassessing lateralization in calculation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 373:20170044. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semenza, C., Salillas, E., De Pallegrin, S., and Della Puppa, A. (2017). Balancing the 2 hemispheres in simple calculation: evidence from direct cortical electrostimulation. Cereb. Cortex 27, 4806–4814. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuman, M., and Kanwisher, N. (2004). Numerical magnitude in the human parietal lobe: tests of representational generality and domain specificity. Neuron 44, 557–569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokolowski, H. M., Fias, W., Mousa, A., and Ansari, D. (2017). Common and distinct brain regions in both parietal and frontal cortex support symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing in humans: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. NeuroImage 146, 376–394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vansteensel, M. J., Bleichner, M. G., Freudenburg, Z. V., Hermes, D., Aarnoutse, E. J., Leijten, F. S., et al. (2014). Spatiotemporal characteristics of electrocortical brain activity during mental calculation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5903–5920. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesia, M., Prime, S. L., Yan, X., Sergio, L. E., and Crawford, J. D. (2010). Specificity of human parietal saccade and reach regions during transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 30, 13053–13065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1644-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, D. J., Wilkey, E. D., and Price, G. R. (2017). The search for the number form area: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yusoff, A. N., Ling, T. X., Abd Hamid, A. I., and Mukari, S. Z. M. S. (2016). Superior temporal gyrus (STG) and cerebellum show different activation profile during simple arithmetic addition task in quiet and in noisy environment: an fMRI study. Malays. J. Health Sci. 14, 119–127. doi: 10.17576/JSKM-2016-1402-14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mathematical cognition, arithmetic operation, functional lateralization, posterior parietal cortex, transcranial magnetic stimulation study

Citation: Klichowski M and Kroliczak G (2020) Mental Shopping Calculations: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study. Front. Psychol. 11:1930. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01930

Received: 06 December 2019; Accepted: 13 July 2020;
Published: 04 August 2020.

Edited by:

Kazuo Ueda, Kyushu University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Nicola Großheinrich, Catholic University of Applied Sciences of North Rhine – Westphalia, Germany
Qingbai Zhao, Central China Normal University, China

Copyright © 2020 Klichowski and Kroliczak. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michal Klichowski, klich@amu.edu.pl; klichowski.michal@gmail.com, orcid.org/0000-0002-1614-926X; Gregory Kroliczak, krolgreg@amu.edu.pl, orcid.org/0000-0001-6121-0536

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.