Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 18 September 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Children's Competencies Development in the Home Learning Environment View all 24 articles

Probing the Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children's Mathematical Skills: A Systematic Review

  • 1Brain and Cognition, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 2Research Centre for Learning in Diversity, HOGENT, Ghent, Belgium
  • 3Parenting and Special Education, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 4Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven Kulak, Kortrijk, Belgium

The concept of home numeracy has been defined as parent–child interactions with numerical content. This concept started to receive increasing attention since the last decade. Most of the studies indicated that the more parents and their children engage in numerical experiences, the better children perform in mathematical tasks. However, there are also contrasting results indicating that home numeracy does not play a role or that there is a negative association between the parent–child interactions and children's mathematics performance. To shed light on these discrepancies, a systematic review searching for available articles examining the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills was conducted. Thirty-seven articles were retained and a p-curve analysis showed a true positive association between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. A more qualitative investigation of the articles revealed five common findings: (1) Advanced home numeracy interactions but not basic ones are associated with children's mathematical skills. (2) Most participants in the studies were mothers, however, when both parents participated and were compared, only mothers' reports of formal home numeracy activities (i.e., explicit numeracy teaching) were linked to children's mathematical skill. (3) Formal home numeracy activities have been investigated more commonly than informal home numeracy activities (i.e., implicit numeracy teaching). (4) The number of studies that have used questionnaires to assess home numeracy is larger compared with the ones that have used observations. (5) The majority of the studies measured children's mathematical skills with comprehensive tests that index mathematical ability with one composite score rather than with specific numerical tasks. These five common findings might explain the contradictory results regarding the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills. Therefore, more research is necessary to draw quantitative conclusions about these five points.

Introduction

According to Vygotsky's sociocultural theory (Vygotsky, 1978), culture, community, and social interactions play a fundamental role in children's learning and development. This theoretical framework indicates that people, such as teachers, peers, siblings, and parents, who are present in the environment of children have the power to (educationally) stimulate the children by social interactions, and this process is influenced by environmental factors such as culture and socio-economic status. Especially, the past few years, increasing attention has been paid to understand the learning opportunities brought by parents that affect children's mathematical skills. More specifically, the concept of “home numeracy” has been introduced to describe the various ways in which parents can influence their children's mathematical skills (Blevins-Knabe and Austin, 2016). Most commonly, home numeracy has been operationalized as parent–child interactions related with numerical “activities,” such as the frequency of engaging in certain numerical activities or the frequency of using numerical words during certain activities. To a lesser extent, as a part of a home numeracy measure, some researchers also included other indexes, such as parents' academic expectations from their children (e.g., Kleemans et al., 2012) and their own attitudes toward mathematics (e.g., Skwarchuk et al., 2014). However, in this review we use the term “home numeracy” to specifically refer to the parent–child interactions related with numerical “activities” and report findings related to activities only, and not on expectations or attitudes.

Many studies have demonstrated that home numeracy positively correlates with how well-children perform in mathematical tasks (e.g., Blevins-Knabe and Musun-Miller, 1996; LeFevre et al., 2009; Kleemans et al., 2012). On the other hand, some studies reported that home numeracy negatively correlates with children's mathematical abilities (e.g., Blevins-Knabe et al., 2000; Ciping et al., 2015) or has no relation with these at all (e.g., Zhou et al., 2006; Missall et al., 2015). Bearing in mind these contradictory findings, revealing a clear picture of the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills is especially important because of the educational impact home numeracy has been suggested to have. First, home numeracy is assumed to explain the variation in kindergartners' early mathematical performance (e.g., LeFevre et al., 2009), which in turn has been shown to affect later mathematical achievement (e.g., Duncan et al., 2007; Gilmore et al., 2010; Sasanguie et al., 2012, 2013). Yet, literature on home numeracy is still in its infancy and lacks a systematic analysis of the available data. Because in the literature there is an abundance of definitions of both home numeracy and mathematical skills, we will first present a clear description of the concepts of home numeracy and mathematical skills.

Home Numeracy

Different types of home numeracy activities were introduced by LeFevre et al. (2009) based on an analogy with their previous work studying the relation between home literacy activities and children's early literacy skills (e.g., Sénéchal and LeFevre, 2002). LeFevre et al. (2009) developed a comprehensive questionnaire to assess home numeracy via parental self-reports on the frequency of various activities they performed with their child within in a given time frame, e.g., during the last month. Meanwhile, this questionnaire has been used widely. Based on a principal components analysis, the authors suggested that home numeracy activities can be divided into two broad categories, i.e., formal and informal activities. Formal activities are defined as parents' intentional teaching efforts, such as counting objects, practicing simple sums, or reading number story books. Informal activities, on the other hand, consist of parents' unintended teaching that takes place during activities such as playing board or card games, using calendars and reading clocks. LeFevre et al. (2009) reported that both types of activities positively correlate with children's performance in mathematics.

As already mentioned, the origin of the interest in home numeracy goes back to Vygotsky's (1978) sociocultural theory stating that social interactions play a fundamental role in the development of children's cognitive skills. Hereby, Vygotsky emphasized that children's learning is most efficient when knowledgeable others, such as teachers or parents, can identify the level of children's achievement and build their interactions on top of that level, an idea better known as the “Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD).” In accordance with the idea of ZPD, it has been suggested that home numeracy activities can be further divided into two categories based on difficulty level: basic and advanced activities (Skwarchuk, 2009; Skwarchuk et al., 2014). The distinction between basic and advanced activities of course depends on children's age and performance level. For instance, basic activities describe easier number practices, such as counting or recognizing written numbers, whereas advanced activities rather refer to more difficult number practices, such as teaching calculations, for ~5-year-olds. Therefore, it can be expected that practicing basic activities that children can already do by themselves does not result in improvement; however, practicing advanced activities that are just beyond children's achievement level provides opportunities for improvement.

Another common method to assess home numeracy is through observation (e.g., Levine et al., 2010). Typically, in an observation study, parent–child dyads are observed while they engage in either daily routine activities at home, such as making dinner (i.e., unstructured observations), or during pre-specified activities, such as book reading or playing with Lego blocks, that are preset by researchers at home or in laboratories (i.e., semi-structured observations). In a next step, the recordings are transcribed to reveal the frequency of the numeracy talk. In other words, observation studies do not investigate how often parents and their children engage in numerical activities; instead they focus on how often parents and their children utter numerical words during certain activities. Furthermore, the numeracy talk can be classified based on its content. For instance, Gunderson and Levine (2011) categorized different types of numeracy talk, such as counting objects and naming cardinal values of objects. Classification of numeracy talk also allows for a distinction similar to basic and advanced activities. For instance, basic numeracy talk (e.g., about numbers smaller than 4) and advanced numeracy talk (e.g., about numbers larger than 4) can be distinguished relative to children's age (e.g., Gunderson and Levine, 2011).

Although numeracy-related interactions have been proposed as a unique predictor of children's mathematical skills, Anders et al. (2012) observed that not only home numeracy but also home literacy–related interactions were associated with children's mathematical skills. Therefore, some researchers argued that home literacy and numeracy environments are not completely independent from one another, but rather form a more global construct, i.e., the Home Learning Environment (HLE; Melhuish et al., 2008; Dearing et al., 2012; Baker, 2015; Niklas and Schneider, 2017). The HLE is most commonly assessed via questionnaires asking about a wider range of activities, both numerical and non-numerical (e.g., frequency of going to library, painting, drawing, or playing dice games), and family possessions (e.g., number of books or access to educational software at home).

Another factor that affects both parents' engagement in home numeracy activities (e.g., Starkey et al., 1999) and children's mathematical skills (e.g., Siegler and Ramani, 2008) is socio-economic status (SES), mostly operationalized as parents' education level and household income (e.g., Jordan et al., 2006; Dubow et al., 2009). It has been suggested that home numeracy activities are related with SES (Niklas and Schneider, 2017; see for a review, Elliott and Bachman, 2018). A recent meta-analysis has documented that the positive associations of home numeracy activities were larger in high-SES families compared with low-SES families (Dunst et al., 2017). However, the direction of the relation is not very clear, as also negative associations between parents' education level and their engagement in home numeracy activities have been reported (e.g., LeFevre et al., 2010a; Niklas and Schneider, 2014). Therefore, we will include SES as a sample characteristic in this review.

Mathematical Skills

In the present review, “mathematical skills” refer to a wide range of skills (e.g., non-symbolic and symbolic number discrimination, counting, and arithmetic) that studies have used to correlate with home numeracy. Theoretically, mathematical skills can be divided into two categories; informal and formal (Baroody and Ginsburg, 1990). Informal skills are basic skills that are learned through everyday activities, such as non-symbolic number processing and counting skills (e.g., Purpura et al., 2013). Non-symbolic number processing is suggested to rely on an innate ability to approximately represent, understand and manipulate non-symbolic (e.g., dot arrays) magnitudes (Dehaene, 2001). This representation is typically measured with a non-symbolic comparison task, i.e., a task in which participants have to indicate the larger of two dot arrays (e.g., Piazza et al., 2004) or with a non-symbolic number line estimation task, i.e., a task in which participants have to place a dot array on an empty line going from, e.g., 0–10 dots (e.g., Sasanguie et al., 2012). Another informal basic skill is counting. It is assumed that verbal rote counting is first practiced as a routine by mimicking others such as parents and teachers, or by singing counting songs (Wynn, 1990) at an age of around 2. However, understanding that each number in the counting list represents one and only one entity in the sequence, i.e., the so-called “one-to-one correspondence,” and that the last number in the counting list represents the total number of entities in a set, i.e., the “cardinality principle,” are skills that are only acquired from around the age of three and a half (Gelman and Meck, 1983; Wynn, 1990). All these different subskills have been commonly measured with different tasks, such as verbal counting, i.e., counting as high as possible (e.g., Geary et al., 2018) and give-a-number tasks, i.e., choosing a given number of entity from a set (Wynn, 1990).

Formal skills are more advanced and require knowledge of symbolic numbers, such as Arabic digits and written number words, which are learned through cultural education and/or direct instructions. Examples of formal skills are for instance symbolic number processing and arithmetic (Baroody and Ginsburg, 1990). Symbolic number processing is the ability to understand and manipulate digits and number words (Dehaene, 2011). Often, this skill is measured with symbolic versions of the comparison task (e.g., Bugden and Ansari, 2011) and number line estimation task (e.g., Siegler and Booth, 2004) that were explained earlier. Another formal, mathematical skill—which is also commonly addressed in home numeracy studies—is arithmetic. Measures of arithmetic skills can tap into accuracy, i.e., solving as many questions correctly, and/or fluency, i.e., solving questions correctly as fast as possible, or making calculations. All these formal and informal skills can be examined with separate specific tasks, but they also can be part of a mathematical test of which performance is then averaged into a composite score. For instance, the Test of Early Mathematics Ability (TEMA, Ginsburg and Baroody, 1990) consists of two subtests: one for formal and one for informal mathematical abilities. The informal subtest measures concepts of relative magnitude and counting skills, whereas the formal subtest measures skills, such as calculation, reading and writing numerals, knowledge of number facts, calculation algorithms, and base 10 concepts. The majority of studies have investigated the role of home numeracy evaluating children's performance based on a composite score of either each subtest separately or both subtests combined.

The Present Study

Home numeracy on the one hand and mathematical skills on the other hand have been operationalized and measured in various ways in different studies. Therefore, in the current systematic literature review, we aimed to systematically investigate the relationship between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills, by taking the diversities in how both constructs were measured into consideration. Furthermore, to test whether this relationship has a true effect or the findings of the reviewed studies were result of a publication bias, we conducted a p-curve analysis (Simonsohn et al., 2013, 2014).

Method

A systematic review study (Grant and Booth, 2009) was conducted to obtain a comprehensive overview of the studies that investigated the relationship between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. Articles were selected in four steps: First, the two most important databases in the field of psychological and educational research were scanned to reach all relevant articles as possible. Second, the abstracts of the articles were screened by three independent raters (i.e., the first, second and fourth author of this article) taking into account the inclusion and exclusion criteria that are described later. Third, the selected articles were read in full and screened taking into account the additional exclusion criteria described below. Lastly, the final subset of articles was analyzed in detail. An overview of the selection process is presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart illustrating the article selection procedure through the systematic review process.

Systematic Search

The online databases ERIC (EBSCOhost) and Web of Science (SSCI) were scanned to reach potentially relevant articles. Our search strategy was limited to peer-reviewed journal articles written in English. We searched for all articles that were published up to November 2019. The search string was as follows: (child* OR kindergart* OR preschool*) AND (numer* OR “number sense”) AND (“home numeracy” OR “home learning” OR “parent talk” OR parent* OR SES* OR socio*) AND (math* OR arithmet* OR calculation OR performance). This yielded 714 hits in total (530 from Web of Science and 184 from ERIC). From these, 62 articles were overlapping and therefore eliminated. Consequently, we ended up with 652 articles for abstract screening.

Abstract Screening

The abstracts were read and evaluated by three independent raters: The first author of this study screened all the abstracts, in combination with the second and the fourth authors who shared and screened the abstracts in halves. In other words, every abstract was read by two raters. The raters scored the relevancy for the review on a three-point scale: 1 = “relevant” if home numeracy (i.e., frequency of activities and talk, but not parents' expectations, attitudes, or SES) was measured as the independent variable and children's mathematical skills were examined as the dependent variable (n = 74); 2 = “maybe relevant” if the assessment of one of the independent or dependent variables was not clearly mentioned in the abstract (n = 71); 3 = “not relevant” if the study was not about home numeracy and mathematical skills at all (n = 507). Cohen's Kappa (κ) was calculated to determine the level of agreement between the raters (Cohen, 1960). This agreement was substantial, κ = 0.736, p < 0.001 (e.g., McHugh, 2012). The disagreements on the ratings of the articles were overcome via discussions. All papers that were categorized as “related” and “maybe related” (n = 145) were entered into the next phase of full-text screening.

Full-Text Screening

In the full-text screening, all the articles were read in full by the first author and additional inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied: First, in the current study, “home numeracy” was defined as the parent–child interactions that include activities with numerical content. Therefore, the articles that operationalized “home numeracy” in another way (e.g., as parents' attitudes, expectations, perceived parental involvement, or family socio-demographics) were excluded (n = 34). Second, articles that did not measure any form of children's mathematical skills but tested for example parents' or teachers math skills were excluded (n = 23). Third, articles that reported only intervention but not pretest data were excluded (n = 22). Fourth, articles that did not present original, empirical data (e.g., reviews, project reports) were excluded (n = 15). Fifth, articles of which the full text could not be retrieved online were not included (n = 14). Hence, after full-text screening, 108 articles were additionally eliminated. As a result, 37 articles were analyzed in this review.

Results

Some commonalities were observed regarding the operationalization of both mathematical skills and home numeracy. Therefore, the studies were reviewed in two hierarchical levels (see Figure 2): At the higher level, we distinguished between the ways mathematical skills were operationalized, i.e., (A) comprehensive mathematical tests, which reveal a composite score from more than one task, (B) specific mathematical task(s), which reveal a single score of one task, or (C) both. At the next level, we distinguished between the methods used to evaluate home numeracy, i.e., (1) questionnaire, (2) observation, or (3) both, and elaborated on the assessment details. Finally, all the results were compiled and a p-curve analysis was run. Table 1 presents an overview with all the detailed information of the articles included in the review (e.g., sample characteristics, measures, and results).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Summary and organizational chart of the reviewed articles.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Detailed overview of the reviewed studies.

A. The Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children's Performance as Measured by Comprehensive Mathematical Tests

A.1. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Questionnaires

In total, 22 studies examined the relationship between home numeracy and comprehensive mathematical tests. Fifteen of these studies used questionnaires to assess home numeracy. Most of these questionnaires were inspired by the work of LeFevre et al. (2009), specifically targeting numeracy activities (n = 11). A minority of the studies assessed the broader home learning environment of which home numeracy is a part (n = 4).

Of the 11 studies that specifically addressed numeracy activities, 8 investigated the effects of formal home numeracy activities on mathematical skills. Huntsinger et al. (1997) recruited families from different ethnic backgrounds with a high socio-economic status and showed that children (Mage = 5.5 years) with a higher composite mathematical score (i.e., TEMA-2) were the ones whose parents spent more time in formal teaching practices, such as helping with mathematics homework, regardless of the ethnicity. They also showed however that Chinese-American and Taiwan-Chinese parents engaged more in formal activities than Euro-American parents and that this higher engagement was associated with higher mathematics scores in Chinese-American and Taiwan-Chinese children. Two studies with participants from middle- to high-SES families showed that formal numeracy activities were positively associated with children's (Mage = 6 years) composite mathematical score (i.e., Utrecht Early Numeracy Test-Revised) above and beyond children's cognitive skills, linguistic skills, and their home literacy environment (Kleemans et al., 2012; Segers et al., 2015). However, one study with participants from low- to middle-SES families reported that children's (Mage = 4.3 years) composite mathematical scores were not associated with formal home numeracy activities, but positively related with SES (Cheung et al., 2018). Next to these cross-sectional designs, several studies used a longitudinal approach. Anders et al. (2012) found that both families' SES and formal home numeracy activities were associated with kindergartner's (Mage = 3.08) concurrent mathematical score and also its growth in the following 2 years. Another study showed that in a group of middle- to high-SES participants formal numeracy activities were the most stable (positive) predictors of kindergartners' (Mage = 4.09 years) composite mathematical score one semester later, even after accounting for control variables, such as a child's age, gender, and parental education (Napoli and Purpura, 2018).

Two studies explicitly distinguished between basic and advanced formal numeracy activities (Del Rio et al., 2017; Zippert and Ramani, 2017). In a group of middle- to high-SES American participants advanced formal number activities (e.g., solving simple sums), but not basic ones (e.g., counting), were positively related with children's (Mage = 4.5 years) performance in composite score of advanced (i.e., symbolic comparison and arithmetic) mathematical skills (Zippert and Ramani, 2017). The other study recruited Chilean families from low and high socio-economic backgrounds and assessed mothers' and fathers' home numeracy separately (Del Rio et al., 2017). These authors found that children's (Mage = 5.6 years) composite mathematical score (i.e., Applied Problems subtest of the Woodcock–Muñoz Batería III) was positively associated with SES and mothers' advanced formal activities at home, but not with the ones of the fathers. However, the relationship between SES and mothers' advanced home numeracy activities was negative.

Niklas and Schneider (2014) recruited participants from diverse socio-economic backgrounds in Germany and assessed only informal activities, the authors reported that the frequency of playing games at the end of kindergarten (Mage = 6.4 years) positively predicted children's mathematical composite score at the end of Grade 1 even after controlling for SES. Furthermore, similar to Del Rio et al. (2017), it was found that SES was negatively related to informal activities whereas it was positively associated with children's mathematical composite score. However, two studies that measured both formal and informal home numeracy activities with a longitudinal approach (Ciping et al., 2015; Huntsinger et al., 2016) reported conflicting results. Huntsinger et al. (2016) showed that in a group of middle- to high-SES American families, formal numeracy activities at the mean age of 4.48 years were the most consistent (positive) predictor of kindergartners' composite mathematical score (TEMA-2) 1 year later (Mage = 5.6 years), but informal activities were not. Ciping et al. (2015) took another approach and investigated the effect of children's mathematical skills on home numeracy in a group of Chinese participants with diverse socio-economic backgrounds. These authors observed that the composite mathematical score of two tasks (i.e., calculation fluency and omitted number) at Grade 1 (Mage = 6.7 years) negatively predicted the formal, but not the informal numeracy activities at Grade 2. Furthermore, no moderation effect from SES was found.

In addition to studies that focused on home numeracy activities, four studies investigated the relation between the general home learning environment (HLE) and mathematical skills. Baker (2015) demonstrated in a sample of African-Americans from diverse socio-economic backgrounds that boys' (Mage = 5.6 years) composite mathematical score (i.e., National Assessment of Educational Progress) were positively associated with SES and also with mothers' home learning activities even after controlling for children's age and family demographics (e.g., income, maternal age, and education). Three other studies investigated the longitudinal associations of HLE on mathematics (Melhuish et al., 2008; Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Visser et al., 2019). Home learning environment of participants in the UK from diverse socio-economic backgrounds, assessed at age 3, was the strongest predictor of children's composite mathematical score (i.e., the Early Number Concepts subscale of the British Ability Scale II) both at ages 5 and 7, over and above the effects of maternal education and social class (Melhuish et al., 2008). Niklas and Schneider (2017) recruited families from low- to middle-SES in Germany and found that the composite mathematical scores at the middle of Grade 4 were positively predicted by the home learning environment assessed at kindergarten, even when earlier mathematical skills, other child (i.e., age, sex, and intelligence) and family characteristics (i.e., socio-economic status and migration background) were accounted for. Families' SES was not a direct predictor of composite mathematical scores; however, SES was associated with HLE. Similar findings were reported by Visser et al. (2019) in a sample of South African families from diverse socio-economic backgrounds. Home learning environment measured in kindergarten was positively associated with Grade 5 children's standardized mathematical scores, even after accounting for earlier mathematical skills and SES.

A.2. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Observations

Five studies used observations to index home numeracy, and two of them focused on mothers only. Elliott et al. (2017) observed that highly educated American mothers' numeracy talk of large numbers (>10) was positively associated with children's (Mage = 5.7 years) composite mathematical score (i.e., TEMA-3), whereas this was not true for talk about small (1–5) and medium (6–10) numbers, or overall number talk. Two longitudinal studies showed that the frequency of numeracy talk was predictive for children's mathematical skills (Susperreguy and Davis-Kean, 2016; Leyva, 2019). Leyva (2019) showed that low-income Chilean parents' math talk when children were in the beginning of pre-kindergarten (Mage = 4.5 years) was predictive for children's composite mathematical scores (Applied Problems subset of the Woodcock-Muñoz Bateria III) at the end of kindergarten, even after controlling for age, earlier mathematical skills, and parents' education. In a sample of children from diverse socio-economic backgrounds, Susperreguy and Davis-Kean (2016) found that American mothers' numeracy talk with their children (Mage = 4.5 years) was predictive for children's composite mathematical scores (TEMA-3) 1 year after the recordings, even after controlling for maternal education and children's self-regulation (i.e., inhibitory control) and working memory. By contrast, two studies reported the absence of a relationship between numeracy talk and mathematical skills (Zhou et al., 2006; Leyva et al., 2019). Leyva et al. showed that the frequency of numeracy talk in low-income families with diverse ethnic backgrounds from the USA did not relate to children's (Mage = 6.6 years) composite math scores (Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement, WJ-III; Woodcock et al., 2001). Ethnicity or mothers' education did not have any effect on children's mathematical skills either. Zhou et al. (2006) found that the frequency of numeracy talk in Chinese families from diverse socio-economic background was equal in “high” and “low” math achievers (Mage = 4.1 years).

A.3. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Both Questionnaires and Observations

There are two studies that used both a questionnaire and semi-structured observations to assess the frequency of numeracy activities and the frequency of numeracy talk, respectively (Ramani et al., 2015; Lehrl et al., 2019). Ramani et al. (2015) recruited children and their parents from Head Start centers (low-SES) in the USA and found that children's (Mage = 4.3 years) composite score of basic mathematical skills (i.e., verbal counting and numeral identification) was positively associated with formal home numeracy activities as indexed by the questionnaire, but not with the observed basic (i.e., counting and identifying numbers) or advanced (i.e., labeling number of elements in a set, ordering numbers, and arithmetic) numeracy talk. By contrast, the composite score of advanced mathematical skills (i.e., counting principles, enumeration and cardinality, number line estimation, and numeral magnitude comparison) was positively related with the observed advanced numeracy talk—but not with basic talk or formal activities. Lehrl et al. (2019) investigated the longitudinal associations between home numeracy and mathematics in participants from Germany from diverse socio-economic backgrounds and showed that numeracy talk, but not home numeracy activities, at ages 3, 4, and 5, positively predicted children's mathematical achievement at age 12 even when SES was controlled for.

B. The Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children's Performance on Specific Mathematical Tasks

B.1. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Questionnaires

Eleven studies used specific tasks to examine children's mathematical skills. Seven of them made use of questionnaires, of which six specifically focused on home numeracy activities and one on both numeracy activities and the broader concept of home learning environment.

Results from a sample with children from diverse socio-economic backgrounds living in the USA showed that SES was positively related with the HLE. In turn, both the HLE and formal home numeracy activities were positively associated with girls' (Mage = 6.7 years) addition and subtraction skills (Dearing et al., 2012). Kleemans et al. investigated the long-term effects of formal numeracy activities on children's calculation skills in families from middle- to high-SES living in the Netherlands (Kleemans et al., 2013, 2018). Results showed that formal numeracy activities when children were in kindergarten (Mage = 6 years) positively predicted children's addition and subtraction scores 1 year later in first grade, even when cognitive and linguistic child factors were controlled for (Kleemans et al., 2013). Following the same children, Kleemans et al. (2018) found that formal home numeracy activities positively predicted children's arithmetic performance with large but not small problem sizes 2 years later (Kleemans et al., 2018). Huang et al. (2017) assessed mothers and fathers from China separately and found that children's (Mage = 5 years) word problem solving skills (e.g., “Emma has four pens. Her sister gives two more. How many pens does Emma have now?”) were positively correlated with mothers' formal number practices and negatively with mothers' number book activities. Symbolic calculation (e.g., 1 + 3) scores were positively related with mothers' number practices and fathers' informal (games and application) activities. There are three studies that showed that home numeracy is differentially related with specific mathematical tasks (Benavides-Varela et al., 2016; Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018a; Vasilyeva et al., 2018). In a sample of Italian families from diverse socio-economic backgrounds, Benavides-Varela et al. (2016) found that counting skills in children (Mage = 5.9 years), but not one-to-one correspondence, everyday numerical problems, non-symbolic magnitude comparison, and number line estimation, were positively correlated with the frequency of playing board games, but not with any other type of home numeracy activities, such as playing videogames or reading book even after controlling for SES. Mutaf-Yildiz et al. (2018a) reported that formal home numeracy activities in Belgian families from middle- to high-SES backgrounds were positively associated with children's (Mage = 5.4 years) enumeration (i.e., sequentially tapping the correct number of dots indicated by a digit), but not with symbolic and non-symbolic comparison or number line estimation tasks, and not with a non-symbolic symbolic connecting task (i.e., choosing a set of dots equivalent to a target digit) or pictorial calculations even after taking SES into account. This study further demonstrated that informal home numeracy was positively associated with pictorial calculations and performance on a symbolic number line estimation task, but not with the other tasks. Finally, in a longitudinal study, Vasilyeva et al. (2018) observed that Russian families' formal home numeracy activities, at age 5.8 on average, positively predicted children's number identification skills and informal activities positively predicted symbolic comparison skills, whereas both types of activities were associated with arithmetic performance 1 year later.

B.2. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Observations

Three longitudinal studies used observations to index home numeracy and examined its association with specific numerical skills in families from the US with diverse socio-economic backgrounds. Levine et al. (2010) administered the Point-to-X task, i.e., a task in which participants have to indicate which of the two pictures contains a certain number of items (Wynn, 1992) to measure the cardinal-principle knowledge of 3.8-year-old children. Parent–child dyads were observed in an unstructured way during five home visits, each lasting 90 min. Especially parents' number talk positively predicted children's cardinal-principle knowledge, even after controlling for SES (education level of the primary caregiver who interacted with the child during the observations and income) and for parents' total amount of talk (Levine et al., 2010; see also Glenn et al., 2018). A more detailed analysis (Gunderson and Levine, 2011) of the same data revealed that specifically the talk about large sets (4–10) of present objects positively predicted the children's score on a Point-to-X task, even after controlling for SES and other types of parental numeracy and non-numeracy talk. In this sample, SES was positively related with parents' number talk which in turn was positively associated with children's mathematical skills.

B.3. Home Numeracy Operationalized With Both Questionnaires and Observations

Only one study used both a questionnaire and semi-structured observations to measure home numeracy in Belgian families from middle to high socio-economic backgrounds (Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018b). Results showed that home numeracy assessed with a questionnaire on the one hand and with observations on the other were not correlated with each other. Moreover, this study showed that children's (Mage = 5.6 years) calculation skills positively correlated with the frequency of formal home numeracy activities measured with a questionnaire, whereas these calculation skills negatively correlated with parents' observed numeracy talk (Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018b).

C. The Relation Between Home Numeracy and Children's Performance in Both Comprehensive Mathematical Tests and Specific Mathematical Tasks in One Study

In four studies, home numeracy was indexed with questionnaires and children's mathematical skills were examined with both a composite score and specific mathematical measures (LeFevre et al., 2009; Manolitsis et al., 2013; Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Missall et al., 2015). These studies revealed conflicting results. In a Canadian sample of children from diverse socio-economic backgrounds, LeFevre et al. (2009) found that a composite mathematical score (i.e., KeyMath–Revised) was positively associated with informal activities (games) over and above the effects of children's (Mage = 6.5 years) vocabulary, spatial memory span, and SES. On the other hand, in another similar Canadian sample, Skwarchuk et al. (2014) observed that children's (Mage = 5.8 years) composite mathematical score (i.e., KeyMath–Revised) was not associated with informal home numeracy or basic formal activities, but that it was only positively related with advanced formal practices even after accounting for SES. In this study, SES also positively correlated with mathematical skills. Moreover, LeFevre et al. (2009) showed that a specific measure of calculation fluency (solving problems correctly as fast as possible) was positively related to both formal and informal activities, even when SES was accounted for, whereas Manolitsis et al. (2013) reported that formal number activities in Greek families from diverse socio-economic backgrounds were only positively correlated with specific verbal counting skills (counting from 1 to highest number children could) of children (Mage = 5.4 years), but not with math fluency or a composite mathematical score (i.e., TEMA-3). In this study, SES was not related with home numeracy activities or mathematical skills. However, Skwarchuk et al. (2014) reported that a specific measure of non-symbolic calculation was positively associated with informal home numeracy (i.e., parents' number game knowledge) but not with formal activities, even after controlling for family income. By contrast, Missall et al. (2015) recruited families from diverse ethnicities and SES living in the USA and documented that home numeracy—which was calculated as a sum score of both formal and informal activities—did not predict any type of (composite or specific) mathematical skill in children (Mage = 4.4 years). The authors also reported that neither ethnicity nor income were related to home numeracy activities.

P-Curve Analysis

It became clear that 33 studies out of 37 observed significant associations between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. However, one should remember that there is a bias to publish studies with positive findings compared with the ones with negative findings (e.g., Joober et al., 2012). Therefore, we tested whether these significant (p < 0.05) relations were the result of a publication bias, or whether they indicate a true effect, by means of a p-curve analysis (Simonsohn et al., 2013, 2014). A p-curve analysis answers this possibility of a publication bias by producing a distribution of statistically significant p-values for a set of studies. Simonsohn et al. (2014) reasoned that a right-skewed p-curve would be generated if there is a true effect because more low (e.g., p < 0.01) significant p-values are expected than high significant p-values (e.g., p = 0.04) in a set of studies. On the contrary, if the significant relations in a set of studies were the result of a publication bias, a left-skewed p-curve would be revealed because more high p-values (e.g., p = 0.05) and less low p-values (e.g., p < 0.01) would be observed in such a set of studies.

Simonsohn et al. (2014) developed an online application “p-curve app 4.06” (http://www.p-curve.com/app) to generate a p-curve. The app uses available test statistics (r, t, z, f, F, or χ2) from each study, along with the degrees of freedom, and recalculates the p-values (see the last column of Table 1). Because the p-curve analysis assumes that all the p-values that are to be analyzed are statistically independent from each other, including only one statistical test from each reviewed paper in the analysis is advised. Consequently, in the papers where two or more results were reported, a decision must be made to select only one of them. To keep this choice objective, we followed Simonsohn's (2014) recommendation and chose the result that was reported first in the manuscript to be included in our analysis. The p-curve analysis was run on only 28 papers because in four studies no significant relations were observed and in five other studies the required test statistics were not reported. Gunderson and Levine (2011) for instance documented a scatter plot displaying the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills, but did not report the actual effect size of the correlation.

Figure 3, revealed by the “p-curve app 4.06,” displays the results of the p-curve analysis. The two dashed lines are reference lines; the shorter-dashed line represents an expected distribution of p-values when there is no true effect and the longer-dashed line represents an expected distribution of p-values when there is a true effect. The observed p-curve of this set of studies, represented by the straight line, shows that there are more low (e.g.,0.01 s) p-values compared with higher (e.g.,0.04 s) ones, as we would expect when there is a true effect, with a high statistical power (89%). This result indicates that the positive relation between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills reported in the reviewed studies indeed reflects a true effect and therefore has evidential value.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the p-curve analysis.

Discussion

The purpose of the current literature review was to provide a synthesized and systematic overview of the studies focusing on the relationship between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. We observed that both concepts have been operationalized in a variety of ways. However, despite the use of diverse measures for home numeracy and mathematical skills, we found that 32 studies out of 37 revealed a positive association between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. In addition, a p-curve analysis confirmed that this association reflects a true effect and cannot be explained by a publication bias. The present review holds, however, also some limitations. First, the search for relevant articles was conducted in only two databases, albeit the most important ones in the field of psychological and educational research. Second, owing to the timing of the actual search, only articles published up to November 2019 were analyzed. Third, intervention studies were not included because they felt out of the scope of our research. However, to complete the picture presented, these limitations were defeated by adding relevant papers here the current discussion section that were published recently, not indexed in the databases we scanned, and discussing interventions.

The majority of the studies revealed by our systematic review (n = 22/37) used comprehensive tests to measure children's mathematical skills, compared with only 11 studies that used specific tasks. Four included both types of mathematical measures. Among the 22 studies that tested children with comprehensive batteries, 15 of them used questionnaires, five used observations, and two included both measures to assess home numeracy. Studies have shown that home learning environment (Melhuish et al., 2008; Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Visser et al., 2019; n = 4, Baker, 2015), informal (n = 1, Niklas and Schneider, 2014), and formal home numeracy (Huntsinger et al., 1997, 2016; Kleemans et al., 2012; Segers et al., 2015; Napoli and Purpura, 2018; n = 6, Anders et al., 2012) were positively associated with composite mathematical scores. Furthermore, two studies distinguished between basic and advanced formal activities and showed that advanced but not basic activities were related with children's composite mathematical scores (Del Rio et al., 2017; Zippert and Ramani, 2017). In line with the findings from the questionnaires, studies based on observational methods also demonstrated that home numeracy talk (n = 3, Susperreguy and Davis-Kean, 2016; Lehrl et al., 2019; Leyva, 2019), especially the advanced talk but not the basic talk (n = 2, Ramani et al., 2015; Elliott et al., 2017), was positively associated with children's composite math score.

Out of the 11 studies that tested children with specific mathematical measures, seven used questionnaires, three used observations, and only one used both measures to index home numeracy. The home learning environment (n = 1, Dearing et al., 2012), informal (n = 4, Benavides-Varela et al., 2016; Huang et al., 2017; Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018b; Vasilyeva et al., 2018), and formal home numeracy (n = 5, Kleemans et al., 2013, 2018; Huang et al., 2017; Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018a; Vasilyeva et al., 2018) were positively associated with specific mathematical tasks (i.e., calculations and counting). Moreover, studies using observations showed that home numeracy talk (n = 2, Levine et al., 2010; Glenn et al., 2018), especially the advanced talk (n = 1, Gunderson and Levine, 2011), was related with the specific mathematical skills, i.e., cardinal-principle knowledge.

All of the four studies that examined children with both comprehensive mathematical tests and specific mathematical tasks used questionnaires to assess home numeracy. Their results showed that informal (LeFevre et al., 2009) and advanced formal activities (Skwarchuk et al., 2014) were associated with composite mathematical scores. Moreover, both formal (LeFevre et al., 2009; Manolitsis et al., 2013) and informal activities (LeFevre et al., 2009; Skwarchuk et al., 2014) were related with some specific mathematical measures (i.e., math fluency, counting, and non-symbolic calculation).

Overall, only four studies (Zhou et al., 2006; Missall et al., 2015; Cheung et al., 2018; Leyva et al., 2019) did not observe any relationship between home numeracy (both questionnaire and observations) and mathematical skills (both comprehensive and specific mathematical measures). In addition, one study showed that the relationship between formal home numeracy and composite mathematical score was negative (Ciping et al., 2015). Although there are some commonalities, such as age, socio-economic status, and country in which the study was conducted, among these studies, these factors are probably not conclusive enough to explain the absence and negative direction of the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills. With respect to age, at first sight, in the five studies mentioned earlier, children were, on average, either slightly younger or older compared with the children in other studies (~5.5 years on average) that did observe a positive relation. In Cheung et al. (2018), Missall et al. (2015), and Zhou et al. (2006), children's mean age was 4.3, 4.5, and 4 years, respectively. The children in Leyva et al. (2019) were 6.6 years on average, which is older than most of the other studies. The age factor applies to the study of Ciping et al. (2015) that found a negative relationship and assessed home numeracy when children were on average 7.7 years old. This is later than all the other studies that collected home numeracy data when children were attending kindergarten. This seems to suggest there is a kind of critical period for home numeracy activities to result in a positive effect. However, this picture is not confirmed by all studies: two other studies (Ramani et al., 2015; Zippert and Ramani, 2017) that examined children's math skills and home numeracy at the age of around 4 and a study by Dearing et al. (2012), examining children's math skills and home numeracy at the age of around 6.7 reported positive associations (see also, Cheung et al., 2020). The number of studies examining relatively younger and older children is too small to establish reliable conclusions about the role of children's age.

Other commonalities between the studies in which a negative or no relation was found between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills are the socio-economic status of the family and the country where the research was conducted. The SES of the samples in those studies was low (Cheung et al., 2018; Leyva et al., 2019) or at least very diverse, with approximately half of the families having a low-SES background (Zhou et al., 2006; Ciping et al., 2015; Missall et al., 2015). Moreover, three (Zhou et al., 2006; Ciping et al., 2015; Cheung et al., 2018) of these five studies were conducted in Asian countries, i.e., Philippines and China, and one study included a sample living in the USA and having diverse ethnic backgrounds, 28% of which were Chinese (Leyva et al., 2019). However, the moderating effect of SES/country on the relation between home numeracy and mathematical skills is, just as age, not consistent. Huang et al. (2017) reported a positive relationship between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills in a Chinese sample [Huang et al., 2017; see also Zhang et al. (2020) for a recent replication]. Moreover, three studies with families from low socio-economic backgrounds (Ramani et al., 2015; Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Leyva, 2019; see also Harris et al., 2014) showed a positive association between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills.

Although the pattern revealed by our systematic review with respect to the moderating effect of SES/country on the relation between home numeracy and mathematical skills is not consistent and needs to be investigated further, it is clear that these factors are related to the frequency of home numeracy activities and children's mathematical skills. The majority of studies found a positive relation between SES and home numeracy activities (except Niklas and Schneider, 2014; Del Rio et al., 2017). In two studies by LeFevre et al. that were not included in our systematic analysis because they were not mentioned in the two databases we examined, it was shown that cultural factors indeed play a role in the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills. In one study, LeFevre et al. (2010b) found that Greek parents reported numeracy activities less frequently than Canadian parents, although formal home numeracy activities were associated with children's mathematical skills in both samples. In the other study, LeFevre et al. (2002) showed that French-speaking Canadian parents reported less frequent home numeracy activities than English-speaking Canadians.

In sum, the interactions between age, culture, home numeracy, and mathematical skills is worthy of further investigation, and may help to disentangle the precise moderating effects of age, SES, and culture on the relation between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills.

Beyond the fact that most of the studies have reported a positive relationship between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills, a more qualitative screening revealed five common findings and additional avenues for further research. First, six studies distinguished between basic and advanced home numeracy (either activities or talk) and all these studies have showed that advanced but not basic home numeracy plays an important role in children's mathematical skills (Gunderson and Levine, 2011; Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Ramani et al., 2015; Del Rio et al., 2017; Elliott et al., 2017; Zippert and Ramani, 2017). In these studies, examples of basic home numeracy included counting, reciting numbers, and identifying numerals, especially smaller than four. In combination with the fact that the children's age in these studies ranged from 4 to 6 (see Table 1), it can be assumed that these children already possessed this basic number knowledge. Based on this assumption, our results are in line with Vygotsky's theory of Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD, 1978): Practicing skills that children can already do by themselves does not result in improvement; by contrast, practicing skills that are just above the expertise level of the children provides opportunities for improvement. Following this argument, it can be hypothesized that basic home numeracy activities are more related to mathematical skills than advanced activities in relatively younger samples, whereas the opposite pattern is to be expected in relatively older samples. This needs to be confirmed in future research.

The second common point we observed is that six studies acquired home numeracy data from mothers, whereas only two of them also recruited fathers. The other studies did not explicitly distinguish between mothers and fathers, and gathered data from any parent whoever is the respondent which were mostly mothers. Five studies with mothers found associations between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills (Baker, 2015; Susperreguy and Davis-Kean, 2016; Del Rio et al., 2017; Elliott et al., 2017; Huang et al., 2017), and only one did not observe a relationship (Leyva et al., 2019). However, the two studies (Del Rio et al., 2017; Huang et al., 2017) that compared mothers and fathers showed that only mothers' but not fathers' formal home numeracy activities were linked with the children's mathematical skills. Del Rio et al. (2017) also reported that mothers and fathers did not significantly differ in the frequency of engaging in formal numeracy activities, whereas Huang et al. (2017) observed that mothers' engagement in formal numeracy activities was significantly more frequent than fathers' engagement. Furthermore, Huang et al. (2017) showed that fathers' engagement in informal activities was significantly more frequent than mothers' and that it was related with children's written calculation skills. To date, there are only two studies that compared mothers' and fathers' home numeracy explicitly and more research comparing mothers' and father's home numeracy activities is needed before drawing conclusions. However, it is an interesting topic worthwhile to explore further in future studies.

As the third common finding, we noticed that among the studies that used questionnaires, the number of investigations on informal home numeracy activities (n = 11) was smaller compared with investigations on formal home numeracy activities (n = 22). Some studies that investigated both formal and informal home numeracy in one sample showed that children's mathematical skills were related with formal home numeracy activities but not with informal ones (LeFevre et al., 2010b; Rosales et al., 2020), whereas others observed the reverse pattern: informal but not formal activities were associated with children's mathematical skills (Zhang et al., 2020). Still other studies reported that formal and informal activities were associated with different mathematical measures (e.g., LeFevre et al., 2009; Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Ciping et al., 2015; Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018b), or that the relation depends on the parent who completed the questionnaire (e.g., Del Rio et al., 2017). Moreover, studies that calculated home numeracy as the sum score of both formal and informal activities (Missall et al., 2015) found no relation between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills (see also Blevins-Knabe and Musun-Miller, 1996). Two studies reported that formal home numeracy activities were related with a wider range of mathematical skills in children compared with informal ones (Ramani et al., 2015; Huntsinger et al., 2016). However, a recent meta-analysis of 11 studies showed that informal activities are stronger predictors of mathematical skills compared with formal ones (Dunst et al., 2017). On the other hand, intervention studies (which were not analyzed in the current review) also revealed some conflicting results. Some studies showed playing number games, i.e., informal activities, improved children in mathematical skills (e.g., Ramani and Siegler, 2008; Siegler and Ramani, 2008); whereas others did not observe this effect (Vandermaas-Peeler et al., 2012; Zippert et al., 2019). Overall, both types of home numeracy activities seem to play a role in children's mathematical skills as supported by the recent findings of Susperreguy et al. (2020). Future research is encouraged to include both formal and informal home numeracy activities to better understand the reasons of the differential relations between formal and informal activities and children's mathematical skills.

Fourth, we noticed that observations of parent–child interactions (n = 8) were less frequently used to index home numeracy compared with questionnaires (n = 26). The more frequent use of questionnaires is understandable. It is less time consuming than observations, thus easily applicable to larger samples. However, responses on questionnaires are retrospective and rely on memory, thus might be influenced by social desirability bias or false memories (e.g., Gravetter and Forzano, 2006). On the other hand, observation studies are less influenced by memory and social desirability bias if a cover story is presented to hide the aim of the study (Harmon-Jones et al., 2007). These technical differences between the two methods call for more research directly comparing them to understand whether they can be used interchangeably or whether they measure different aspects of home numeracy. To date, only four studies used two methods of measuring home numeracy (questionnaire and observations) in one sample. Two of these studies showed that data from a questionnaire on the one hand and from observations on the other were not related (Missall et al., 2017; Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018b). In addition, Mutaf-Yildiz et al. (2018b) showed that children's mathematical skills were positively correlated with reported home numeracy activities, whereas it was negatively correlated with observed numeracy talk (see also, Zippert et al., 2019). Also, the other studies observed differences between both methods: Lehrl et al. (2019) found that children's mathematical skills were related with observed numeracy talk but not with formal numeracy activities. Ramani et al. (2015) found that both measures were differentially associated with various mathematical skills: Home numeracy measured with a questionnaire was related with a composite score of basic mathematics, whereas home numeracy measured with observations was related with a composite score of advanced mathematical skills. More research comparing the two methods is required to identify the underlying reasons of their unrelated outcomes and their differential relations to various math measures. It is especially important to reveal what aspects of home numeracy are exactly being measured via the two methods. For instance, it could be tested whether parents who indicate that they play board games with their children frequently on a questionnaire also have frequent number talk observed during a board game session with their children.

Finally, the fifth common finding we noticed is that most of the studies examined children with comprehensive mathematical tests (n = 22) compared with specific mathematical tasks (n = 11)—excluding the ones that used both methods (n = 4). However, the use of composite mathematical scores in home numeracy research makes it unclear to observe which specific skills are associated with home numeracy. For instance, Manolitsis et al. (2013) found that formal home numeracy activities were not associated with composite math score (TEMA-3); however, they were related to counting skills in kindergartners (see also Cheung et al., 2018). Furthermore, recent studies showed that it is important to dissociate between different mathematical skills as they found that formal and informal home numeracy activities were differentially related to specific mathematical tasks (Mutaf-Yildiz et al., 2018a; Vasilyeva et al., 2018).

There are two possible reasons for the differential associations. On the one hand, various numerical skills might rely on (partially) different underlying processes (e.g., Reynvoet and Sasanguie, 2016; Sasanguie et al., 2017). For example, when comparing effect sizes, the relation between mathematics and symbolic number processing is larger and more consistent compared with the effect size of the relation between mathematics and non-symbolic number processing (De Smedt et al., 2013; Schneider et al., 2017). On the other hand, different tasks assumed to measure one concept are not necessarily associated. It has for instance been shown that non-symbolic comparison and number line estimation tasks were not related to each other (Sasanguie and Reynvoet, 2013; Maertens et al., 2016). From these insights, it follows that home numeracy can be correlated with one specific skill but not another. Therefore, future research is needed to clarify which specific type of home numeracy activity is linked with which specific type of mathematical skills, by examining various specific types of mathematical skills.

The present systematic review comes with some limitations due to the decisions made in the search process. First, interest in home numeracy is increasing very fast and new papers are published frequently. The scope of the articles analyzed in this research is limited to the ones published until November 2019. Second, several studies—including some that we were aware of—have been missed in our search because we screened only two databases and they were not indexed in those databases. To stick to the search strategy, those articles were not included in the analysis. Third, the scope of our research excluded home numeracy intervention studies. In intervention studies, parents are informed about the role they play in the development of their children's mathematical skills and how they can improve their support. Results suggest that home numeracy interventions have a positive effect on mathematical skills (e.g., Starkey and Klein, 2000; Niklas et al., 2016). However, in order to present a complete picture, the discussion was expanded with those recently published, missed, and discussing interventions papers (LeFevre et al., 2002, 2010a,b; Vandermaas-Peeler et al., 2012; Harris et al., 2014; Dunst et al., 2017; Cheung et al., 2020; Susperreguy et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2020; Zippert et al., 2020). Finally, it should be noted that the current results did not inform us about the overall effect size of the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills in children.

In conclusion, the current systematic search and review demonstrated that a positively significant link between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills has been observed in the majority of the studies. In addition, a p-curve analysis confirmed that this relationship holds a true effect. Moreover, a qualitative inspection of all studies revealed some possible sources for the variance in the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills in children across studies. These sources include children's age, family SES, and location of the research, the distinction between basic and advanced activities, differences between mothers and fathers, differences between questionnaires and/or observations, and differences between formal and informal activities. Differences between studies also emerged due to differences in the measurement of mathematical skills, i.e., specific mathematical tasks or comprehensive mathematical tests. These sources all seem to have an impact on observed findings. Therefore, more research is necessary to draw quantitative conclusions about these possible sources of variance.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets presented in this study are included in the article and further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

BMY, DS, BDS, and BR conceived and designed the study and interpreted the results. BMY organized the data and ran the analyses, wrote the draft of the overall study, and revised the draft carefully. DS, BDS, and BR critically reviewed the draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Grant DBOF/12/009 ‘Early mediators of number sense’ from the Research Fund KU Leuven.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anders, Y., Rossbach, H.-G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., et al. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 27, 231–244. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. E. (2015). Does parent involvement and neighborhood quality matter for African American boys' kindergarten mathematics achievement? Early Educ. Dev. 26, 342–355. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.968238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baroody, A., and Ginsburg, H. (1990). Chapter 4: children's mathematical learning: a cognitive view. J. Res. Math. Educ. Monogr. 4, 51–210. doi: 10.2307/749912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benavides-Varela, S., Butterworth, B., Burgio, F., Arcara, G., Lucangeli, D., and Semenza, C. (2016). Numerical activities and information learned at home link to exact numeracy skills in 5-6 years-old children. Front. Psychol. 7:94. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blevins-Knabe, B., and Austin, A. B. (2016). Early Childhood Mathematics Skill Development in the Home Environment. Cham: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-43974-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blevins-Knabe, B., Austin, A. B., Musun, L., Eddy, A., and Jones, R. M. (2000). Family home care providers' and parents' beliefs and practices concerning mathematics with young children. Early Child Dev. Care 165, 41–58. doi: 10.1080/0300443001650104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blevins-Knabe, B., and Musun-Miller, L. (1996). Number use at home by children and their parents and its relationship to early mathematical performance. Early Dev. Paren. 5, 35–45. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199603)5:1<35::AID-EDP113>3.0.CO;2-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bracken, B. A. (2006). Bracken Basic Concepts Scale–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. doi: 10.1037/t14979-000

CrossRef Full Text

Bugden, S., and Ansari, D. (2011). Individual differences in children's mathematical competence are related to the intentional but not automatic processing of Arabic numerals. Cognition 118, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, B., and Bradley, R. (1984). Home observation for measurement of the environment (HOME). Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.

Cheung, S. K., Dulay, K. M., and McBride, C. (2020). Parents' characteristics, the home environment, and children's numeracy skills: how are they related in low- to middle-income families in the Philippines? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 192. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, S. K., Yang, X., Dulay, K. M., and McBride, C. (2018). Family and individual variables associated with young Filipino children's numeracy interest and competence. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36, 334–353. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciping, D., Silinskas, G., Wei, W., and Georgiou, G. K. (2015). Cross-lagged relationships between home learning environment and academic achievement in Chinese. Early Child. Res. Q. 33, 12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, A. J. (2000). KeyMath-Revised/Updated Canadian Norms. Richmond Hill, ON: PsyCan.

De Smedt, B., Noël, M.-P., Gilmore, C., and Ansari, D. (2013). The relationship between symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills and the typical and atypical development of mathematics: a review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends Neurosc. Educa. 2, 48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dearing, E., Casey, B. M., Ganley, C. M., Tillinger, M., Laski, E., and Montecillo, C. (2012). Young girls' arithemtic and spatial skills: the distal and proximal roles of family socioeconomics and home learning experiences. Early Child. Res. Q. 27, 458–470. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S. (2001). Precis of the number sense. Mind Lang. 16, 16–36. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S. (2011). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, Revised and Updated Edition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Del Rio, M. F., Susperreguy, M. I., Strasser, K., and Salinas, V. (2017). Distinct influences of mothers and fathers on kindergartners' numeracy performance: the role of math anxiety, home numeracy practices, and numeracy expectations. Early Educ. Dev. 28, 939–955. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2017.1331662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubow, E. F., Boxer, P., and Huesmann, L. R. (2009). Long-term effects of parents' education on children's educational and occupational success: mediation by family interactions, child aggression, and teenage aspirations. Merrill Palmer Q. 55, 224–249. doi: 10.1353/mpq.0.0030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., et al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1428–1446. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D. W., Wilkie, H., and Dunst, K. S. (2017). “Meta-analysis of the relationship between home and family experiences and young children's early numeracy learning,” in Engaging Families as Children's First Mathematics Educators, eds S. Phillipson, A. Gervasoni, and P. Sullivan (Singapore: Springer), 105–125. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-2553-2_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, C., Smith, P., and McCulloch, K. (1996). British Ability Scales Second Edition (BAS II). Windsor: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company Limited.

Elliott, L., and Bachman, H. J. (2018). SES disparities in early math abilities: the contributions of parents' math cognitions, practices to support math, and math talk. Dev. Rev. 49, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, L., Braham, E. J., and Libertus, M. E. (2017). Understanding sources of individual variability in parents' number talk with young children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 159, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geary, D. C., vanMarle, K., Chu, F. W., Rouder, J., Hoard, M. K., and Nugent, L. (2018). Early conceptual understanding of cardinality predicts superior school-entry number-system knowledge. Psychol. Sci. 29, 191–205. doi: 10.1177/0956797617729817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, R., and Meck, E. (1983). Preschoolers' counting: Principles before skills. Cognition 13, 343–359. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90014-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., and Spelke, E. E. (2010). Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and achievement in the first year of formal schooling in mathematics. Cognition 115, 394–406. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsburg, H., and Baroody, A. J. (1990). TEMA-2: Test of early mathematics ability. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

Glenn, D. E., Demir-Lira, Ö. E., Gibson, D. J., Congdon, E. L., and Levine, S. C. (2018). Resilience in mathematics after early brain injury: the roles of parental input and early plasticity. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 30, 304–313. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, M. J., and Booth, A. (2009). A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Info. Libr. J. 26, 91–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-1842.2009.00848.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gravetter, F. J., and Forzano, L.-A. B. (2006). Research methods for the behavioral sciences. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.

Google Scholar

Gunderson, E. A., and Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: relations between parents' input and children's cardinal-number knowledge. Dev. Sci. 14, 1021–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., Amodio, D. M., and Zinner, L. R. (2007). Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, viii, 91–105. 483pp.

Google Scholar

Harris, T., Sideris, J., Serpell, Z., Burchinal, M., and Pickett, C. (2014). Domain-specific cognitive stimulation and maternal sensitivity as predictors of early academic outcomes among low-income African American preschoolers. J. Negro Educ. 83, 15–28. doi: 10.7709/jnegroeducation.83.1.0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hojnoski, R. L., and Floyd, R. G. (2013). Individual Growth and Development Indicators of Early Numeracy (IGDIs-EN). Retrieved from: http://www.myigdis.com/

Huang, Q., Zhang, X., Liu, Y., Yang, W., and Song, Z. (2017). The contribution of parent–child numeracy activities to young Chinese children's mathematical ability. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 328–244. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., Liaw, F.-R., and Ching, W.-D. (1997). Cultural differences in early mathematics learning: a comparison of euro-American, Chinese-American, and Taiwan-Chinese families. Int. J. Behav. Dev 21, 371–388. doi: 10.1080/016502597384929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., and Luo, Z. (2016). Parental facilitation of early mathematicas and reading skills and knowledge through encouragement of home-based activities. Early Child. Res. Q. 37, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joober, R., Schmitz, N., Annable, L., and Boksa, P. (2012). Publication bias: what are the challenges and can they be overcome? J. Psych. Neurosci. 37, 149–152. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Olah, L. N., and Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Dev. 77, 153–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleemans, T., Peeters, M., Segers, E., and Verhoeven, L. (2012). Child and home predictors of early numeracy skills in kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 27, 471–477. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleemans, T., Segers, E., and Verhoeven, L. (2013). Relations between home numeracy experiences and basic calculation skills of children with and without specific language impairment. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleemans, T., Segers, E., and Verhoeven, L. (2018). Individual differences in basic arithmetic skills in children with andwithout developmental language disorder: role of home numeracyexperiences. Early Child. Res. Q. 43, 62–72, doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krajewski, K. (2005). “Vorschulische Mengenbewusstheit von Zahlen und ihre Bedeutung für die Früherkennung von Rechenschwäche [Preschool awareness for quantities of numbers and its importance for the early identification of dyscalculia],” in Diagnostik von Mathematikleistungen, eds M. Hasselhorn, H. Marx, and W. Schneider. (Göttingen: Hogrefe), 49–70.

Krajewski, K., Küspert, P., Schneider, W., and Visé, M. (2002). Deutscher Mathematiktest für erste Klassen (DEMAT 1+) [German mathematical test battery for Grade 1]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Kuger, D., Pflieger, K., and Rossbach, H. G. (2005). Familieneinschätzskala Forschungsversion [Family rating scale, research version]. Unpublished instrument, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany.

LeFevre, J. A., Clarke, T., and Stringer, A. P. (2002). Influences of language and parental involvement on the development of counting skills: comparisons of French- and English-speaking Canadian children. Early Child Dev. Care 172, 283–300. doi: 10.1080/03004430212127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S.-L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., et al. (2010a). Pathways to mathematics: longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Dev. 81, 1753–1767. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeFevre, J. A., Polyzoi, E., Skwarchuk, S., Fast, L., and Sowinski, C. (2010b). Do home numeracy and literacy practices of Greek and Canadian parents predict the numeracy skills of kindergarten children? Int. J. Early Years Educ. 18, 55–70. doi: 10.1080/09669761003693926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeFevre, J. A., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Fast, L., Kamawar, D., and Bisanz, J. (2009). Home numeracy experiences and children's math performance in the early school years. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 41, 55–66. doi: 10.1037/a0014532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., Blaurock, S., Rossbach, H.-G., and Weinert, S. (2019). Long-term and domain-specific relations between the early years home learning environment and students' academic outcomes in secondary school. School Effect. School Improv. 31, 102–124. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2019.1618346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenhard, W., Hasselhorn, M., and Schneider, W. (2011). Kombiniertes Leistungsinventar zur allgemeinen Schulleistung und für Schullaufbahnempfehlungen in der vierten Klasse [Combined test for the assessment of general school performance and the recommendation of subsequent school track in Grade 4]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Levine, S. C., Suriyakham, L. W., Rowe, M. L., Huttenlocher, J., and Gunderson, E. (2010). What counts in th development of young children's number knowledge? Dev. Psychol. 46, 1309–1319. doi: 10.1037/a0019671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyva, D. (2019). How do low-income Chilean parents support their preschoolers' writing and math skills in a grocery game? Early Educ. Dev. 30, 114–130. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2018.1540250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyva, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., and Yoshikawa, H. (2019). What parents bring to the table: Maternal behaviors in a grocery game and first-graders literacy and math skills in a low-income sample. Elem. Sch. J. 119, 629–650. doi: 10.1086/703104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maertens, B., De Smedt, B., Sasanguie, D., and Reynvoet, B. (2016). Enhancing arithmetic in pre-schoolers with comparison or number line estimation training: does it matter? Learn. Instru. 46, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., and Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects of home literacy and numeracy environment on early reading and math acquisition. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 692–703. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHugh, M. L. (2012). Interrater reliability: the kappa statistic. Biochem. Med. 22, 276–282. doi: 10.11613/BM.2012.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melchers, P., and Preuss, U. (2003). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (KABC). German Version (6th ed.). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., and Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environemtn and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. J. Soc. 64, 95–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missall, K., Hojnoski, R. L., Caskie, G. I. L., and Repasky, P. (2015). Home numeracy environments of preschoolers: examining relations among mathematical activities, parent mathematical beliefs, and early mathematical skills. Early Educ. Dev. 26, 356–376. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.968243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missall, K., Hojnoski, R. L., and Moreano, G. (2017). Parent-child mathematical interactions: examining self-report and direct observation. Early Child Dev. Care 187, 1896–1908. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1193731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F., Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., and Mather, N. (2005). Bateria III Woodcock-Muñoz: Pruebas de Aprovechamiento [Achievement Test]. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

Google Scholar

Mutaf-Yildiz, B., Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., and Reynvoet, B. (2018a). Frequency of home numeracy activities is differentially related to basic number processing and calculation skills in kindergartners. Front. Psychol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutaf-Yildiz, B., Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., and Reynvoet, B. (2018b). Investigating the relationship between two home numeracy measures: a questionnaire and observations during Lego building and book reading. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36, 354–370. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Napoli, A. R., and Purpura, D. J. (2018). The home literacy and numeracy environment in preschool: cross-domain relations of parent–child practices and child outcomes. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 166, 581–603. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., and Tayler, C. (2016). Improving preschoolers' numerical abilities by enhancing the home numeracy environment. Early Educ. Dev. 27, 372–383. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.1076676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., and Schneider, W. (2014). Casting the die before the die is cast: the importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool children. Europ. J. Psychol. Educ. 29, 327–345. doi: 10.1007/s10212-013-0201-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., and Schneider, W. (2017). Home learning environment and development of child competencies form kindergarten until the end of elementary school. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 49, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., and Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44, 547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purpura, D. J., Baroody, A. J., and Lonigan, C. J. (2013). The transition from informal to formal mathematical knowledge: mediation by numeral knowledge. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 453–464. doi: 10.1037/a0031753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purpura, D. J., Reid, E. E., Eiland, M. D., and Baroody, A. J. (2015). Using a brief preschool early numeracy skills screener to identify young children with mathematics difficulties. School Psych. Rev. 44, 41–59. doi: 10.17105/SPR44-1.41-59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramani, G. B., Rowe, M. L., Eason, S. H., and Leech, K. A. (2015). Math talk during informal learning activities in Head Start families. Cogn. Dev. 35, 15–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramani, G. B., and Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Dev. 79, 375–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01131.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynvoet, B., and Sasanguie, D. (2016). The symbol grounding problem revisited: a thorough evaluation of the ans mapping account and the proposal of an alternative account based on symbol–symbol associations. Front. Psychol. 7:1581. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosales, J., Ramos, M., Janez, A., and De Sixte, R. (2020). Home numeracy activites in relation to basic number processing in kindergartners. Revista de Educación 389, 45–66. doi: 10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2020-389-454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., and Reynvoet, B. (2017). Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number. Psychol. Res. 81, 231–242. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0734-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., and Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 418–431. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., and Reynvoet, B. (2013). Number comparison and number line estimation rely on different mechanisms. Psychol. Belg. 53, 17–35. doi: 10.5334/pb-53-4-17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., Van den Bussche, E., and Reynvoet, B. (2012). Predictors for mathematics achievement? Evidence from a longitudinal study. Mind Brain Educ. 6, 119–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01147.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M., Beeres, K., Coban, L., Merz, S., Schmidt, S., Stricker, J., et al. (2017). Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Dev. Sci 20. doi: 10.1111/desc.12372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segers, E., Kleemans, T., and Verhoeven, L. (2015). Role of parent literacy and numeracy expectations and activities in predicting early numeracy skills. Math Think Learn. 17, 219–236. doi: 10.1080/10986065.2015.1016819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sénéchal, M., and LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children's reading skills: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 73, 445–460. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R., and Booth, J. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev. 75, 428–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. Dev. Sci. 11, 655–661. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00714.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., and Simmons, J. P. (2013, April 24). P-Curve: A Key to the File Drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Forthcoming. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2256237

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., and Simmons, J. P. (2014, April 27). P-Curve and Effect Size: Correcting for Publication Bias Using Only Significant Results. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2377290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skwarchuk, S. L. (2009). How do parents support preschoolers' numeracy learning experiences at home? Early Childhood Educ. J. 37, 189–197. doi: 10.1007/s10643-009-0340-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., and LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children's early numeracy and literacy skills: the development of a home numeracy model. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 121, 63–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starkey, P., and Klein, A. (2000). Fostering parental support for children's mathematical development: an intervention with head Start families. Early Educ. Dev. 11, 659–680. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1105_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starkey, P., Klein, A., Chang, I., Dong, Q., Pang, L., and Zhou, Y. (1999). “Environmental supports for young children's mathematical development in Chine and the United States,” in Paper Presented at the Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development (Albuquerque, NM).

Susperreguy, M. I., and Davis-Kean, P. E. (2016). Maternal math talk in the home and math skills in preschool children. Early Educ. Dev. 9289, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1148480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susperreguy, M. I., Douglas, H., Xu, C., Molina-Rojas, N., and LeFevre, J. A. (2020). Expanding the home numeracy model to chilean children: relations among parental expectations, attitudes, activities, and children's mathematical outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 50, 16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tymms, P., and Albone, S. (2002). “Performance indicators in primary school,” in School Improvement Through Performance Feedback, eds A. J. Visscher and R. Coe (Lisse: Routledge; Taylor and Francis Group), 191–218.

Van Luit, J., and Van de Rijt, B. (2009). (Utrecht early numeracy test-revised) Utrechtse Getalbegrip Toets-Revised. Apeldoorn: Graviant.

Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Ferretti, L., and Loving, S. (2012). Playing the ladybug game: parent guidance of young children's numeracy activities. Early Child Dev. Care 182, 1289–1307. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2011.609617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasilyeva, M., Laski, E., Veraksa, A., Weber, L., and Bukhalenkova, D. (2018). Distinct pathways from parental beliefs and practices to children's numeric skills. J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 345–366. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1483371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, M. M., Juan, A. L., and Hannan, S. M. (2019). Early learning experiences, school entry skills and later mathematics achievement in South Africa. South Afr. J. Childhood Educ. 9:a597. doi: 10.4102/sajce.v9i1.597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Woodcock, R., McGrew, K., and Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.

PubMed Abstract

Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding of counting. Cognition 36, 155–193. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wynn, K. (1992). Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 220–251. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Hu, B. Y., Zou, X., and Ren, L. (2020). Parent-child number application activities predict children's math trajectories from preschool to primary school. J. Educ. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/edu0000457. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Huang, J., Wang, Z., Wang, B., Zhao, Z., Yang, L., et al. (2006). Parent–child interaction and children's number learning. Early Child Dev. Care 176, 763–775. doi: 10.1080/03004430500232680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zippert, E., Daubert, E. N., Scalise, N. R., Noreen, D. D., and Ramani, D. B. (2019). Tap space number three: “promoting math talk during parent-child tablet play.” Dev. Psychol. 55, 1605–1614. doi: 10.1037/dev0000769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zippert, E., Douglas, A. A., Smith, M. R., and Rittle-Johnson, B. (2020). Preschoolers' broad mathematics experiences with parents during play. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 192. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zippert, E. L., and Ramani, G. B. (2017). Parents' estimations of preschoolers' number skills relate to at-home number-related activity engagement. Infant Child Dev. 26. doi: 10.1002/icd.1968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: home numeracy, mathematical skills, children, systematic review, p-curve analysis

Citation: Mutaf-Yıldız B, Sasanguie D, De Smedt B and Reynvoet B (2020) Probing the Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children's Mathematical Skills: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 11:2074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02074

Received: 30 April 2020; Accepted: 27 July 2020;
Published: 18 September 2020.

Edited by:

Frank Niklas, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Reviewed by:

Jo-Anne LeFevre, Carleton University, Canada
Maureen Vandermaas-Peeler, Elon University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Mutaf-Yıldız, Sasanguie, De Smedt and Reynvoet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Belde Mutaf-Yıldız, belde.mutaf@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.