Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 08 September 2020
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Current Perspectives in Cognitive Processing by Domesticated Animals View all 11 articles

Where Do We Stand in the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris) Positive-Emotion Assessment: A State-of-the-Art Review and Future Directions

\r\nErika Csoltova*Erika Csoltova*Emira MehinagicEmira Mehinagic
  • Diana Pet Food, Elven, France

Although there have been a growing number of studies focusing on dog welfare, the research field concerning dog positive-emotion assessment remains mostly unexplored. This paper aims to provide a state-of-the-art review and summary of the scattered and disperse research on dog positive-emotion assessment. The review notably details the current advancement in dog positive-emotion research, what approaches, measures, methods, and techniques have been implemented so far in emotion perception, processing, and response assessment. Moreover, we propose possible future research directions for short-term emotion as well as longer-term emotional states assessment in dogs. The review ends by identifying and addressing some methodological limitations and by pointing out further methodological research needs.

Introduction

There has been a growing interest in animal emotion research in recent years. However, positive-emotion has received much less attention than negative emotion thus far. Understanding how an animal perceives, processes, and expresses positive-emotion is crucial not only from a theoretical point of view to improve our knowledge about animal emotion, but also from a practical point of view to improve the animal’s life. Concepts such as satisfaction with life, well-being, and welfare refer to more than a mere absence of negative emotions linked to stress and suffering (Mench, 1998; Boissy et al., 2007; Mellor and Beausoleil, 2015; Lawrence et al., 2019); therefore, it is imperative to be able to objectively assess animals perception and processing of, and responses to, positive-emotions.

Specifically, this review focuses on positive-emotion perception, processing, and response assessment in dogs. The review starts with a brief overview of emotion, including its definition, functions of emotion, selected interdisciplinary approaches in the study of emotion, elements associated with and affecting emotion processing, and responses. It is not our intention to dive deeply into the wide and complex field of animal emotion, given the existence of rather extensive literature on the topic (e.g., Harding et al., 2004; Paul et al., 2005, 2020; Burgdorf and Panksepp, 2006; Boissy et al., 2007; Mendl et al., 2009, 2010; de Vere and Kuczaj, 2016; Massen et al., 2019; Kremer et al., 2020). Rather, we emphasize several important key aspects in the study of emotion. Next, we discuss positive assessment in dogs, including the definition of terms such as happiness and pleasure. Thereafter, we continue reviewing measures, approaches, methods, and techniques that have been implemented so far to identify positive-emotion perception, processing, and responses in dogs. Next, we propose possible future directions for research both in short-term emotional responses, as well as for longer-term emotional states assessment in dogs. The review ends by summarizing the main findings, identifying major methodological limitations, and suggesting possible solutions.

A Brief Overview of Emotion

Even though there is ample literature on emotion ranging from psychology and neuroscience to animal welfare science, little agreement has been achieved on the concept of emotion. Conceptualization issues have been prevalent over the years and there has been a lot of debate about the definition of emotion. The ability to gain a better understanding of emotion and its complexity is complicated by several different factors. Major debates and disagreements have occurred, regarding the various components that make up emotions, how many emotions exist, the underlying structure of emotions, whether emotions require conscious experience, and how they should be measured (Daroff and Aminoff(eds), 2014; de Vere and Kuczaj, 2016). Furthermore, there are often discrepancies in how the authors define and use the terms across a given manuscript (de Vere and Kuczaj, 2016).

Although a widely accepted definition is missing, emotions are often described as relatively intense, rapid, affective responses to an external stimulus, causing a specific physiological change (Dantzer, 2002; Boissy et al., 2007).

One of the crucial roles of emotion from an evolutionary perspective is to facilitate behavioral and physiological adaptation to a changing environment. The essential survival function of emotions is to seek out rewards and resources while avoiding harm and punishment. The reward is associated with experiencing positive-emotion, while the consequence of reward omission or punishment is a negative emotional experience (Rolls, 2000, 2005).

Several theories have been proposed to explain emotion and emotion processing, both in humans and animals. Among them, is the discrete emotion theory, inspired by Darwin’s ideas on the existence of primary emotions reflected in a universal facial expression (Darwin, 1872; Tomkins, 1962; Ekman et al., 1987; Izard, 1994, 2007; Ekman and Cordaro, 2011) and affective neuroscience, which assumes a central role of specific hard-wired neural brain system responses in emotion processing (Berridge, 1996; LeDoux, 1998, 2000, 2012; Panksepp, 2004, 2005, 2011; Burgdorf and Panksepp, 2006; Damasio, 2011).

On the other hand, dimensional approaches, such as the theory of constructed emotion, consider emotions not as universal signals but as potential combinations of external and internal sensations evolved through various distributed brain circuits, which create subjective emotional experiences and perceptions (Barrett, 2006, 2017; Barrett and Wager, 2006). The theory assumes the universal existence of a fundamental conscious neurophysiological affective state called core affect consisting of two dimensions, vertical degree of activation (arousal) and horizontal degree of pleasure (valence) (Russell and Barrett, 1999; Russell, 2003; Barrett, 2006; Duncan and Barrett, 2007).

To bring the discrete and dimensional approaches together, Mendl et al. (2010) have proposed an overarching integrative and functional psychobiological approach for the study of animal emotion. This framework offers a structure for different discrete emotions integration and suggests causal bidirectional interaction between short-term discrete emotions and longer-term mood states. It emphasizes the strong influence of emotional responses to environmental situational factors on an individual’s emotional mood and related behavioral, physiological, and cognitive changes. The integrative functional framework also proposes how to measure and predict the impact of emotion-induced cognitive biases on mood states and how these mood states, in turn, impact the stimulus appraisal and decision-making processes (Mendl et al., 2010).

Apart from taking into consideration the mood state when studying animal emotion, here we would like to stress the possible importance of temperament traits in animal emotion assessment studies. Temperament has been considered as a consistent and long-lasting disposition to qualitatively affect both the mood state and the susceptibility to emotional stimuli, speed, and strength of emotion responses (Allport, 1961). Temperament has been defined by certain researchers as a biogenetically determined, relatively stable emotional predisposition to process and express emotions—one that serves as a foundation for personality1 (Goldsmith et al., 1987). Given that, it is plausible to assume a significant role of certain temperament traits in animal emotion perception, processing, and responses (Boissy, 1995; Boissy et al., 2007; Coleman, 2020; Kremer et al., 2020).

For a long time, it was believed that emotion is independent of cognition (Zajonc, 1980). However, recent neurobiology research has offered proof about the interconnected relationship between emotion and cognition (Okon-Singer et al., 2015).

Besides, research on animals provides further evidence for interactions between emotion and cognitive processes such as learning, memory, attention, judgment, decision making, and social cognition (Harding et al., 2004; Paul et al., 2005; Boissy et al., 2007; Mendl et al., 2009, 2010; Burman et al., 2011; Burman and Mendl, 2018; Hintze et al., 2018; Massen et al., 2019). Hence, examining cognitive processes may prove particularly valuable when studying dogs’ positive-emotion processing and responses.

It has been suggested that emotion contributes to cognitive processing, while cognition can play a role in the regulation of emotion (Izard et al., 1984, 2008; Harding et al., 2004; Mendl et al., 2009). For instance, a cognitive appraisal is an assessment that is made about the relevance of a stimulus to oneself at a particular point in time (Lazarus, 1966). The appraisal needs to be made to assign emotional value to stimuli by making use of available information, to determine the appropriate response (e.g., to fear, flee, or feel pleasure). How the animal appraises the information is going to affect the valence and arousal of the emotional response (Paul et al., 2005). However, emotional factors may interfere with correct information processing and create so-called cognitive biases. Cognitive bias is a pattern of a systematic error in cognitive processing by creating a subjective representation of reality that differs from an objective input (Haselton et al., 2015). It is influenced by the emotional state and directly impacts attention, judgment, and decision making. Therefore, cognitive bias can be a useful indicator of emotion in animals (Harding et al., 2004; Mendl et al., 2009, 2010; Bateson et al., 2011; Roelofs et al., 2016; Burman and Mendl, 2018; Clegg, 2018).

To sum up, emotions are short-term reactive responses to emotion-eliciting stimuli (potentially rewarding or punishing) accompanied by physiological changes in the body (Dantzer, 2002; Rolls, 2005; Boissy et al., 2007). They have biological and cognitive foundations. Emotions consist of either positive or negative valence and arousal, ranging from low to high (Russell and Barrett, 1999; Mendl et al., 2010). From a temporal perspective, emotions are closely associated with longer-term mood states and long-lasting, relatively stable temperament traits. Mood, temperament, and cognition seem to play significant roles in emotion processing and responses (Goldsmith and Campos, 1982; Gross(ed.), 2007; Mendl et al., 2009, 2010).

Positive-Emotion Assessment in Dogs

Assessment of positive-emotions in dogs has received only limited attention thus far. Research studies have been focused mainly on negative emotion assessment (e.g., stress, fear, and anxiety) (Dreschel and Granger, 2005; Bergamasco et al., 2010; Travain et al., 2015; Csoltova et al., 2017). One of the possible reasons for this bias is that indicators of negative emotions are much more intense and therefore more easily observed and studied. Positive-emotional states associated with an animal’s well-being are usually much more subtle, often less expressive, and often difficult to reliably assess and distinguish from negative emotional states (Boissy et al., 2007). Even though dog well-being and welfare2 topics have received increased attention in recent years, most of the studies have focused on indicators of compromised dogs’ welfare (e.g., Beerda et al., 2000; Rooney et al., 2007; Mariti et al., 2015) or indicators related to the improvement of dogs’ already compromised welfare (Hennessy et al., 1998, 2006; Coppola et al., 2006; Bergamasco et al., 2010; Shiverdecker et al., 2013; Csoltova et al., 2017).

Understanding emotions, particularly positive-emotions, is crucial for dogs’ well-being. Therefore, there is a huge need for studies focused on the positive well-being and welfare of dogs. Concepts of positive well-being, welfare, and quality of life are closely linked to positive-emotions associated with concepts such as happiness and pleasure (Duncan, 2005; Boissy et al., 2007; Yeates and Main, 2008; Lawrence et al., 2019).

Happiness can be defined as an emotional state that is characterized as a longer-lasting, steady, persistent, and yet less intense positive-emotional experience (Boissy et al., 2007). Animal happiness might include everyday experiences of pleasure, opportunities to interact with their environment, conspecifics, caretakers, and having the freedom to achieve one’s own goals (Yeates and Main, 2008).

To the best of our knowledge, pleasure has been the most thoroughly researched positive-emotion so far (e.g., Cabanac, 1971, 1979; Panksepp, 1982; Berridge, 1996; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008, 2011). Pleasure, although rather a complex phenomenon, is usually associated with subjective hedonic experience (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2011). Pleasure is a passive experience, evoked by an anticipated or received reward, which affects learning, approach behavior, and decision making, as well as possibly contributing to a longer-lasting state of happiness (Schultz, 2015). Pleasure is intertwined with reward-related processes such as wanting, liking, and learning (Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Berridge et al., 2009). These motivational, emotional, and cognitive processes alternate and co-appear at any time during the pleasure cycle and can be both conscious and unconscious (Berridge, 1996; Berridge and Robinson, 2003; Finlayson et al., 2007; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Berridge et al., 2009). The pleasure cycle (Sherrington, 1907; Craig, 1917; Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2011) consists of the following time phases (Figure 1):

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Pleasure cycle phases (adapted from Berridge and Kringelbach, 2011; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015). Pleasures associated with different social and non-social rewards, follow a cyclical pattern, represented by appetitive, consummatory, and satiety phases (Sherrington, 1907; Craig, 1917). Rewards play a crucial role in initiating, sustaining, and switching between the phases and consist of processes supported by multiple brain networks, each with a specific function in the processing of wanting, liking, and learning elements of the reward (Berridge, 1996; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013).

1. The appetitive phase is characterized by wanting, motivational processes of incentive salience, desire to obtain the reward (Berridge, 1996; Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Berridge et al., 2009; Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009). Wanting is mainly triggered by innate unconditioned or classically conditioned stimuli (Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Berridge et al., 2009). Dopamine seems to be a reward predictor, reflecting wanting (Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Wise, 2004; Robinson et al., 2005; Arias-Carrión and Pöppel, 2007; Berridge, 2007; Newquist and Gardner, 2015). This phase is characterized by increased activity, motivation, approach behavior, and associative learning (e.g., remembering food-associated stimuli) (Craig, 1917; Berridge, 1996; Berridge et al., 2009; Schultz, 2015).

2. The consummatory phase is characterized by liking. During this stage, the subject experiences sensory pleasure (Berridge, 1996; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Berridge et al., 2009). Opioids, endocannabinoid, and orexin seem to mediate the hedonic liking responses to food reward (Peciña and Berridge, 2005; Smith and Berridge, 2005; Mahler et al., 2007; Ho and Berridge, 2013; Castro and Berridge, 2014).

3. The satiety phase is characterized by strong learning about reward predictions based on previous experiences (although learning happens at any phase during the reward cycle). Learning includes explicit cognitive predictions as well as implicit classical and operant conditioning (Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009). Leptin, ghrelin, and serotonin have been implicated, among others, in satiation and satiety (de Graaf et al., 2004; Voigt and Fink, 2015; Cassidy and Tong, 2017). It has been suggested that social rewards are processed at the same reward network as nonsocial rewards and addiction (Wang and Aragona, 2004; Young and Wang, 2004; Krach et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2010).

In the following subsections, we are going to review in more detail what methodological and technical advancements have been achieved and what positive-emotion measures have been identified so far in dogs’ positive-emotion perception, processing, and response assessment. We start with behavioral measures, including research on brain lateralization of emotion processing, facial expression analysis, and qualitative approaches. Thereafter, we discuss specific noninvasive physiological and endocrine measures applied in positive-emotion assessment. Finally, we discuss neurobiological methods applied in dog emotion research.

Behavioral Measures

Behavioral signals play an important role in intra- and interspecies emotional communication. Expressing emotions through facial and body movements has been documented in several studies (Beerda et al., 1997, 1998; Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Shiverdecker et al., 2013). However, some of the behavioral indicators mentioned below have been associated with both negative and positive-emotions.

Thus far, research has focused mainly on identifying behaviors associated with negatively connotated emotions. Although there exists a high within-individual variability in behavior expression, behaviors such as increased activity, repetitive movements, auto-grooming, lowered body posture, lip licking, panting, yawning, crouching, shaking, vocalization (barking, whining), scratching, paw lifting, paw sweating, increased salivation, blinking of eyes have been identified as stress indicating behaviors in dogs (Hetts et al., 1992; Beerda et al., 1997, 1998, 1999; Dreschel and Granger, 2005; Tod et al., 2005; Hennessy et al., 2006; Siniscalchi et al., 2008; Stracke et al., 2011; Hekman et al., 2012; Shiverdecker et al., 2013; Kuhne et al., 2014b; Csoltova et al., 2017).

On the other hand, increased physical activity (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; McGowan et al., 2014), attentive behavior (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Westerback, 2011), tail wagging (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Takahashi, 2011; McGowan et al., 2014), lip licking (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Westerback, 2011; Gygax et al., 2015), vocalization, and shake off (Rehn and Keeling, 2011) have been associated with positive-emotional states in dogs.

Vocalization

Dogs use a wide range of different context-specific subunits of barks and mixed sounds as a means for communication of emotional arousal and both positive and negative emotional states. However, high variability has been found among, as anywhere from 2 to 12 types of subunit barks were identified in different breeds (Feddersen-Petersen, 2000), consisting of acoustic parameters such as frequency, tonality, and rhythmicity (Pongrácz et al., 2010).

Apart from intraspecies communication, research also shows that dogs’ barking represents an important active communication tool also with humans (Feddersen-Petersen, 2000; Yin, 2002; Pongrácz et al., 2005, 2006). In addition, humans seem to be able to correctly identify the emotional state of the dog based on context-specific barks. Short inter-bark time lapses are perceived as aggressive, while longer time lapses between barks indicate low aggression. Higher pitched barks combined with longer between bark sequences are associated with happiness and playfulness (Yin and McCowan, 2004; Pongrácz et al., 2005, 2006). However, excessive barking has been linked to the excitement, boredom, disturbances, anxiety, and pain in dogs (Righetti, 2005).

Whining and yelping sounds were recorded when the owner returned after separation, suggesting increased positive arousal and motivation to approach the owner (Rehn et al., 2014). Another study found that early onset of whining after short separation from the owner was the most typical vocal response of dogs diagnosed with a separation-related disorder. According to the authors of the study, barking encompasses a wide range of both positive and negative emotional states, while, on the other hand, whines are more associated with negative emotions (Pongrácz et al., 2017).

Activity

Activity, in a form of exploration or information gathering, might be a relevant indicator of the animal’s well-being (Boissy et al., 2007). Increased activity was shown as a sign of positive excitement in dogs when reunited with the owner after separation (Rehn et al., 2014) or when the dogs were solving a cognitive task (McGowan et al., 2014). On the other hand, this behavioral indicator is also context specific, since excitation was also documented as an indicator of moderate stress in a social setting (Beerda et al., 1998, 2000) and shelter environment (Part et al., 2014; Cozzi et al., 2016).

However, activity measured as speed or latency to approach food in subsequent runway tests might be a relatively easy and effective tool to measure incentive salience in dogs. The operant runway method has been proved to be a useful approach to study the behavioral and neurobiological basis of reward-seeking motivation in rodents (Hull, 1934; Crespi, 1942; Ettenberg, 2009). Research suggests that the time necessary to cross the runway is a reliable indicator of the animal’s incentive salience (Ettenberg, 2009). The runway method may, therefore, be an interesting alternative for exploring food-related behavior in dogs.

Recently, two studies implemented the runway task as a tool to test its usefulness in food preference in dogs. Both research groups found that dogs moved faster through the runway to obtain a more preferred food compared to the less preferred food (Riemer et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019). However, the food quantity did not affect the running speed, suggesting a higher incentive value of the more preferred food over a greater quantity of less preferred food (Riemer et al., 2018).

Lip Licking

Observed higher frequencies of oral behavior in a form of lip/nose licking and/or tongue flicking have previously been proposed to indicate acute stress in a social context (e.g., Beerda et al., 1997, 1998; Csoltova et al., 2017). Nose licking has been also observed in frustration-provoking situations when access to a food reward was denied to dogs (Bremhorst et al., 2019).

Albuquerque et al. (2018) studied dogs’ lip licking responses to both positive and negative stimuli. They have found more prevalent frequency of lip licking in response to negative visual stimuli (human’s and dogs’ angry and aggressive facial expressions) compared to positive ones (humans’ and dogs’ happy and playful expressions). In addition, authors have found higher rates of lip licking in response to human stimuli.

On the other hand, other research findings suggest that lip licking could be associated with positive-emotion, performed in response to verbal and physical human interactions and seeing an owner after separation (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Westerback, 2011; Gygax et al., 2015).

Tail Wagging

A higher frequency of tail wagging recorded in social settings has been proposed as an indicator of emotion reflecting positive valence and contact-seeking behavior (Rehn and Keeling, 2011; Rehn et al., 2014). Tail wagging related to positive expectation was observed during treat offerings (Travain et al., 2016). Also, dogs were observed to wag their tails more as a reaction toward food reward or human contact and less to conspecifics (McGowan et al., 2014).

Play Behavior

Play behavior is a pleasurable experience and thus a possible indicator of a positive-emotional state. Play behavior includes a complex set of different motor activities and play patterns. Through play, dogs acquire specific skills necessary for survival (Bekoff, 1974a). Social play has an important role in developing social experiences and skills (Bekoff, 1974b, 1984). The high prevalence of social play in adult dogs indicates its social cohesion and bonding function (Bradshaw et al., 2015; Palagi et al., 2015; Sommerville et al., 2017). In addition, it was found that the affiliative behavior of the human handler during play decreased cortisol levels in working dogs (Horváth et al., 2008).

However, there have been some debates regarding whether play behavior is a reliable indicator of positive welfare since the play occurs under less favorable environmental conditions as well (Held and Špinka, 2011; Sommerville et al., 2017).

Behavioral Lateralization of Emotion Processing

Extensive evidence exists about brain lateralization in perception and response to emotion-eliciting stimuli in different animal species (e.g., Fernández-Carriba et al., 2002; De Boyer Des Roches et al., 2008; Rogers, 2010; Wallez and Vauclair, 2011; Leliveld et al., 2013; Rogers et al., 2013; Siniscalchi et al., 2016c). The main existing brain lateralization theories propose different explanations of emotional processing. The right hemisphere hypothesis states that the right cerebral hemisphere dominates the perception and regulation of emotional processes independent of the emotion valence (Borod et al., 1998). On the other hand, the valence-specific hypothesis suggests that each cerebral hemisphere is responsible for different emotional valence perception and processing, with the left hemisphere being dominant in positively connotated emotion processing, while the right hemisphere predominantly processes negative emotions, such as fear and aggression (Ahern and Schwartz, 1979; Wedding and Stalans, 1985; Adolphs et al., 2001).

In the following subsections, we review research done on behavioral lateralization of emotion processing, including emotional information perception and emotional expression.

Visual lateralization

Emotional facial expressions communicate one’s desires and intentions and may influence others’ emotional state. Therefore, they play an important role in social interactions. The animal’s ability to accurately recognize and discriminate emotional information, including the facial emotions of others, is a core element of social competence promoting adaptive behavior in response to perceived information (Lindell, 2013).

Research evidence exists that dogs are capable of facial recognition (Racca et al., 2010; Somppi et al., 2014). In addition, further studies revealed pronounced specificities regarding facial processing, demonstrating dogs’ ability to discriminate between different emotional facial expressions in humans (Nagasawa et al., 2011; Müller et al., 2015; Turcsán et al., 2015; Somppi et al., 2016). Besides, dogs looked significantly longer at both human and dog faces whose facial expression reflected the same emotional valence as the vocalization. These results suggest that dogs’ brains process, differentiate, and integrate multimodal sensory inputs of different emotional valence (Albuquerque et al., 2016).

Apart from dogs’ ability to recognize individual faces visually and discriminate among different emotional states, further studies also revealed gaze bias and lateralized cerebral hemispheric processing of facial expressions. Strong left-gaze bias was recorded in dogs toward images of human faces, but not toward images of monkeys, dogs, and inanimate objects (Guo et al., 2009). Further study revealed that dogs looked significantly more into the right side of the face, regardless of the emotion expressed in their left visual field, indicating the right cerebral hemispheric dominance for processing all emotions. Facial expressions depicting positive-emotion resulted in forehead gaze fixation, while gaze fixation on the eye and mouth region was recorded in response to negative facial expression (Barber et al., 2016).

Within the framework of the valence-specific hypothesis, left-gaze bias was observed in dogs in response to pictures depicting negative facial expressions of conspecifics and a right-gaze bias in response to positive expressions. No gaze bias was observed in response to neutral dog facial expressions. In response to pictures depicting human facial expressions, the right cerebral dominance (left-gaze bias) was found in response to negative and neutral expressions; however, no gaze bias was recorded toward positive expressions (Racca et al., 2012).

In a different study, human facial expressions depicting the same emotion (happiness, surprise, disgust, fear, sadness, anger, or neutral) were simultaneously presented into the left and right visual fields of the dog during feeding. Faces depicting anger, fear, and happiness resulted in left-turn bias, while surprise facial expression resulted in right head-turn bias. Shorter head-turning reaction time was recorded for fear and anger facial expressions in comparison with other emotional stimuli. For the facial expression depicting disgust and the neutral facial expression, no head-turning bias was observed. Further, angry human facial expressions resulted in longer latency to resume eating compared to other emotional facial expressions. Increased stress-associated behavioral responses to angry and happy human facial expressions were observed. Lastly, fearful, angry, and happy emotional expressions resulted in significantly higher cardiac activity in dogs, indicating dogs’ sensitivity to human emotional facial expression (Siniscalchi et al., 2018).

Apart from studies on dogs’ lateralized emotional facial-expression perception, lateralized head-orienting responses were observed to other visual emotion-eliciting stimuli in dogs as well. Silhouettes depicting a dog, a cat, or a snake were simultaneously presented into the left and right visual hemifields of the dog while feeding. Cat and snake pictures resulted in a left head-turning bias and shorter reaction time, suggesting right hemispheric activation for threatening and alarming stimuli. No head-turning bias was observed for a silhouette of the dog. When stimuli were presented either to the left or right visual spaces, dogs were found to react more to the left than to the right-side presentations, regardless of the stimulus presented. Furthermore, both left head-turning bias and the presentation of a cat and a snake silhouettes into the left visual space required a longer latency to resume feeding (Siniscalchi et al., 2010).

Acoustic lateralization

It seems that dogs are able to differentiate between positive (laughing) and negative (crying) emotional auditory stimuli (Huber et al., 2017), and additional research has revealed contra-lateralized brain processing in response to human emotional vocalization (Siniscalchi et al., 2008). In particular, the analysis of human nonverbal vocalizations showed a clear left head-orienting toward negative emotional stimuli (fear and sadness), suggesting a prevalent activation of the right cerebral hemisphere. On the contrary, a clear right head-turning response was recorded in response to positive vocalization (happiness), suggesting dominant left cerebral hemisphere activation in dogs. Moreover, heart rate and behavioral responses have provided further confirmatory evidence that human emotional sounds induced emotional responses in dogs (Siniscalchi et al., 2008).

Olfaction lateralization

Contrary to other senses, olfactory information projects predominantly to the ipsilateral hemisphere, meaning that right nostril sensory input is processed mainly in the right cerebral hemisphere, while the left cerebral hemisphere processes mainly the input from the left nostril (Royet, 2004). Previous research on dogs’ olfactory behavior has found nostril bias when investigating and processing odors (Siniscalchi et al., 2011, 2016b; Brown and Reimchen, 2020). Novel and non-aversive stimuli resulted in an initial right nostril sniffing preference, followed by a switch to left nostril investigation during repeated exposures. Consistent right-nostril sniffing bias was observed for potentially arousing emotional stimuli such as the sweat of a veterinarian and adrenaline (Siniscalchi et al., 2011). The right nostril bias, in response to novel and arousal stimuli, suggests sympathetic activation, which is predominantly controlled by the right cerebral hemisphere (Craig, 2005).

Asymmetric nostril use was also observed in response to human and canine odors produced during emotional states such as joy, fear and anxiety, physical stress, and neutral stimuli. Right nostril bias was observed when dogs investigated conspecific stress-indicating odors (collected when the dog was isolated in an unfamiliar environment). Left nostril bias was documented when dogs sniffed human odors produced during fear and physical stress (running). These results, according to the authors, indicate that dogs might use different sensory pathways for conspecific versus heterospecific emotional signals processing (Siniscalchi et al., 2016b).

Facial expression lateralization

A study examining facial expression lateralization in dogs in response to positive social stimuli (the owner) found that dogs moved their left eyebrow more after reuniting with the owner compared to the baseline. No facial expression bias was recorded for positive nonsocial stimuli (such as a dog toy). The authors have concluded that the left eyebrow bias most probably reflects dogs’ attachment to the owner (Nagasawa et al., 2013).

Motor lateralization

Rapidly growing empirical research suggests evidence also for motor lateralization in dogs. Researchers have found a link between the paw and visuospatial preference. Left-pawed dogs were found to consume more food kibbles on the left side of the feeding apparatus and similarly, right-pawed dogs ate more of the food on the right side of the feeding apparatus. Ambidextrous dogs did not show any bias. This evidence indicates an association between motor laterality and visuospatial bias in dogs, similar to the one found in humans (Siniscalchi et al., 2016a).

Interesting findings have been reported from a study investigating the association between motor laterality, emotional state, and cognitive bias in dogs. The emotional state was assessed by a cognitive test assessing dogs’ food-approach latency. The food bowl was positioned in one of three ambiguous positions. According to the study, the distributions of lateralized (57%) and non-lateralized (43%) dogs were roughly equal. It was found that left-pawed dogs tended to be more negative in their cognitive processing than right-pawed or ambidextrous conspecifics. The study proposes that paw preference, as an indicator of hemispheric dominance, can reliably predict cognitive bias in dogs and can therefore represent a quick and useful prevention tool to identify animals that are at risk from negative welfare, thus enabling quick interventions to improve their well-being (Wells et al., 2017).

Research studies are suggesting that bilateral asymmetry of tail-wagging is also related to the valence of emotional stimuli (Quaranta et al., 2007; Artelle et al., 2011; Siniscalchi et al., 2013). Differential amplitudes of lateral asymmetry of tail wagging were found in response to the type of emotion-eliciting visual stimuli the dogs were presented with. Dogs presented with stimuli eliciting approach motivation (seeing dog’s owner) performed higher amplitude of right-side tail wagging. By contrast, stimuli associated with withdrawal motivation (dominant unfamiliar dog presentation) resulted in a higher amplitude of left-side tail wagging (Quaranta et al., 2007).

According to the findings, dogs not only respond with asymmetric tail wagging to qualitatively different emotional stimuli (Quaranta et al., 2007) but are also sensitive to the asymmetric tail wagging displayed by conspecifics (Artelle et al., 2011). Higher cardiac activity and anxiety-indicating behavior were recorded when dogs watched left-side tail wagging compared to right-side tail wagging video recordings of conspecifics (Siniscalchi et al., 2013).

Facial Expression Analysis

A facial expression is nonverbal communication that has both survival and adaptive function (Erickson and Schulkin, 2003). Most nonhuman animals produce an automatic involuntary display of facial expressions in response to specific emotion-eliciting stimuli. Lately, there has been an increase in the number of studies on the facial expression of pain in different animal species (Langford et al., 2010; Sotocina et al., 2011; Keating et al., 2012; Costa et al., 2014; Holden et al., 2014; Guesgen et al., 2016; McLennan et al., 2016; Finka et al., 2019). Further, it has been proposed that facial expression may reliably indicate both negative and positive-emotional states and thus have a substantial potential in animal welfare assessment (Descovich et al., 2017).

One of the methods used, to noninvasively measure human muscle movements is called the Facial Action Coding System—FACS (Ekman and Rosenberg(eds), 2005). This research tool enables the user to manually code almost any anatomically possible facial expression. To identify and code facial movements in nonhuman species, AnimalFACS was developed and adapted from the original human FACS. This coding system facilitates intra-species and inter-species comparisons of facial expressions (Vick et al., 2007; Parr et al., 2010; Waller B. M. et al., 2012; Caeiro et al., 2013, 2017a; Micheletta et al., 2015; Wathan et al., 2015).

Research has documented that domesticated dogs have developed facial muscles, enabling facial expression of emotion, unlike their wolf ancestors (Waller B. M. et al., 2013; Kaminski et al., 2019). DogFACS has been used to study dog facial expressions in response to different emotional stimuli and compare them with human facial expressions (Waller B. et al., 2013). Emotional stimuli eliciting happiness (such as initiation of play with the owner), positive anticipation (related to food and outdoor activity), frustration (inaccessible toy, food, or space), and fear (experience of a thunderstorm or visualization of specific objects) were used. Researchers identified distinctive stimuli-dependent facial expressions in dogs, although these expressions differed from human facial expressions in response to categorically similar emotional stimuli (Caeiro et al., 2017b).

Research groups studying hedonic and aversive reactions toward taste stimuli found homologous inter-species facial expressions. The results revealed similar distinctive facial expressions in response to sweet and bitter taste stimuli, as well as in the intensity of the emotional reaction (Grill and Norgren, 1978; Berridge et al., 1981; Berridge, 2000; Steiner et al., 2001). Thus far, dogs have not been observed to display specific facial expressions in response to palatable food; however, an increase in “ears adductor” has been observed during anticipation of food reward (Bremhorst et al., 2018).

Qualitative Behavioral Assessment

In recent years, qualitative methods to assess animal emotion and welfare have been gaining more and more attention (e.g.,Wemelsfelder et al., 2000; Wemelsfelder and Farish, 2004; Knierim and Winckler, 2009; Walker et al., 2010; Stockman et al., 2011; Rutherford et al., 2012; Fleming et al., 2013; Wemelsfelder and Mullan, 2014; Konok et al., 2015b).

Qualitative Behavioral Assessment (QBA) is a holistic, noninvasive, positive, and dynamic method using fixed lists of descriptors to measure emotional expressivity in different animal species (Wemelsfelder, 2007; Wemelsfelder and Mullan, 2014). In this assessment, the trained observer integrates multiple quantitative behavioral responses to describe the animal’s emotional state qualitatively. QBA has been used to assess the dogs in the shelter and home environments (Walker et al., 2016; Arena et al., 2019). Most recently, a fixed list of qualitative descriptive terms containing both positive (e.g., playful, curious, relaxed, tranquil) and negative (e.g., bored, apathetic, fearful, wary) connotations has been developed as a complementary assessment tool to evaluate the welfare of dogs in a shelter environment (Arena et al., 2019).

Noninvasive Physiological and Endocrine Measures

There exists a close relationship between emotions and physiology, more particularly the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity (Sander and Scherer, 2014). Therefore, biomarkers play an important role and serve as a proxy when assessing emotion and positive well-being in animals (e.g.,von Borell et al., 2007; Schmied et al., 2008, 2010; Reefmann et al., 2009a, b; Coulon et al., 2013, 2015; Briefer et al., 2015; Kowalik et al., 2017). Heart rate (HR) (Csoltova et al., 2017), heart rate variability (HRV) (Bergamasco et al., 2010; Katayama et al., 2016; Travain et al., 2016), surface temperature (Travain et al., 2015, 2016; Csoltova et al., 2017), oxytocin (Odendaal and Meintjes, 2003; Mitsui et al., 2011; Rehn et al., 2014; Nagasawa et al., 2015), vasopressin (Hydbring-Sandberg et al., 2004; MacLean et al., 2017a, b, 2018; Pirrone et al., 2019), cortisol (Hennessy et al., 1998, 2006; Coppola et al., 2006; Bergamasco et al., 2010; Shiverdecker et al., 2013), and alpha-amylase (Contreras-Aguilar et al., 2017; Hong et al., 2019) have been implemented and showed potential usefulness in indirect and noninvasive assessment of positive-emotion in dogs.

Heart Rate

Monitoring HR responses has been utilized as an effective way for assessing the sympathetic branch activity of ANS activation in both animal and human studies (e.g., Beerda et al., 1998; Watkins et al., 1998; Loijens et al., 2002; Cyr et al., 2009; Csoltova et al., 2017). Itis widely accepted that measurements of HR are valid indicators of ANS activity and thus arousal and stress response (Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009). Although HR has been reported as a reliable indicator of arousal during behavioral tests, it is suggested that it cannot be used as an indicator of emotional valence (Beerda et al., 1998; Lensen et al., 2017).

Besides emotions, HR may be affected by different factors, for example, physical activity and temperature (Hales and Dampney, 1975; Maros et al., 2008). In dogs, it has been found that walking increased HR, which decreased during lying, although no differences in HR were found between sitting and standing (Maros et al., 2008).

Positive human-dog tactile contact has been shown to have an attenuating effect on cardiovascular responses of the dog (Anderson and Gantt, 1966; Lynch and McCarthy, 1967; McGreevy et al., 2005; Handlin et al., 2011; Csoltova et al., 2017; McGowan et al., 2018). In addition, the sole presence of the owner during a threatening situation has proved to have a stress-buffering effect on dogs’ HR, thus providing evidence for the “safe haven” effect of the owner on a dog’s well-being during a stressful situation (Gácsi et al., 2013).

Besides, HR monitoring was implemented when testing the hedonic aspects of food in dogs. Eating food was associated with initial increased cardiac activity, with a gradual return to baseline levels. The heart rhythm was found to be affected by the palatability of the food, with the most profound increase and decrease observed in response to the most preferred food (Kostarczyk and Fonberg, 1982). Similarly, a significant increase in HR was also observed when the dogs were offered a tasty treat (Travain et al., 2016).

Heart-Rate Variability

HRV has been shown to be an effective tool to measure the sympathetic and parasympathetic balance of the ANS (van Ravenswaaij-Arts, 1993; Thayer et al., 2010). HRV reflects variance in time intervals in successive heartbeats, indicating the organism’s capacity to regulate internal and external demands (van Ravenswaaij-Arts, 1993; Jarczok et al., 2015; Mccraty and Shaffer, 2015; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017).

There have been three approaches commonly utilized to monitor HRV (van Ravenswaaij-Arts, 1993; Electrophysiology Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017; Young and Benton, 2018; Table 1):

TABLE 1

1. Time-domain measurements (linear analysis) record the variability of the successive heartbeats during the measured time periods. They include parameters such as mean RR, SDRR/SDNN, RMSSD, NN50, and pNN50 (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017; Drury et al., 2020).

2. Frequency-domain (linear analysis) measure the absolute or relative power (the signal energy) distribution to different frequency bands such as ULF, VLF, LF, and HF (Electrophysiology Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017; Drury et al., 2020).

3. Nonlinear measures estimate the unpredictability and irregularity of time series (Stein and Reddy, 2005; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017; Young and Benton, 2018). They include, for example, SD1, SD2, ApEn, SampEn, DFA α1, DFA α2, and D2 parameters.

Assessing canine emotional states by implementing HRV indices has been gaining research popularity in recent years (Bergamasco et al., 2010; Gácsi et al., 2013; Bowman et al., 2015; Katayama et al., 2016; Travain et al., 2016; Zupan et al., 2016; McGowan et al., 2018; Köster et al., 2019). It has been proposed that HRV parameters might be sensitive indicators of emotional valence (Katayama et al., 2016) and tend to be less affected by physical activity compared to HR (Maros et al., 2008).

Positive-emotion-eliciting stimuli, such as food, human-dog interaction, and listening to music, all resulted in changes of HRV parameters in dogs (Bergamasco et al., 2010; Bowman et al., 2015; Travain et al., 2016; Zupan et al., 2016; McGowan et al., 2018).

An increase in RMSSD, pNN50, and HF parameters has been reported as an indicator of a positive-emotional state in dogs after 15 min of physical contact with humans (McGowan et al., 2018).

Exposure to auditory stimuli in the form of classical music resulted, among others, in mean RR, STDRR, RMSSD, pNN50, SD1, SD2 increase, and LF/HF decrease, indicating parasympathetic nervous system dominance and stress-buffering effects of music on dogs in a stressful environment (Bowman et al., 2015; Köster et al., 2019).

Zupan et al. (2016) studied the effects of positive stimuli on cardiac responses in dogs using higher- versus lower-valued food and social reward (familiar and less familiar person). Positive stimuli resulted in an increase of HR and LF/HF ratio, implying sympathetic nervous system activation and a positive arousal state in dogs throughout testing. In contrast to the aforementioned studies, exposure to the positive stimuli resulted in RMSSD and HF decrease compared to the baseline. A similar decrease was recorded for the LF compared with the baseline. A decrease in HF was observed when dogs were offered more preferred food (meatball) compared to less preferred food (commercial kibbles). A decrease in HF and RMSSD was recorded during the reward phase when the dog was allowed to interact with a person or eat the food compared to the anticipation phase when the dogs could only see the rewards. Authors proposed that higher positive-emotional valence in dogs is associated with parasympathetic deactivation. In this regard, lower RMSSD and HF combined are indicators of higher valence in an already positive-emotional state.

Interestingly, a negative experience in the form of isolation in an unfamiliar environment also resulted in RMSSD decrease in dogs. On the other hand, a decrease in SDNN was recorded when the dogs were petted by their owners (Katayama et al., 2016).

In another study, decreased SDNN has been linked to elevated attention, recorded when the dogs focused on their favorite ball (Maros et al., 2008).

When investigating cardiac responses to food treats during appetitive and reward phases, the authors found an increased HR, although no changes in HRV parameters were recorded. A significant increase in SDNN was observed after the positive stimulation occurred, during the post-consumption phase (Travain et al., 2016).

Superficial Temperature

Both positive and negative emotions activate ANS responses, which subsequently lead to physiological changes associated with alterations of blood flow, leading to surface temperature fluctuations (Sinha et al., 1992; Collet et al., 1997; Chotard et al., 2018).

Implementation of infrared thermography (IRT) is an effective way to accurately quantify the smallest superficial temperature changes in response to environmental and psychological stimuli, both in humans (Pavlidis et al., 2000, 2002; Pollina et al., 2006; Goulart et al., 2019; Panasiti et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2019) and animals (Mccafferty, 2007; McManus et al., 2016; Telkanranta et al., 2018). IRT is a noninvasive imaging technique that detects the infrared wavelengths emitted by all objects with a temperature above absolute zero (Vollmer and Möllmann, 2017). Thermographic cameras provide a tool to monitor sudden rises and decreases in superficial temperature by either real-time observations or ultrahigh-speed video or thermal image recordings of studied objects.

In general, areas without the interference of fur are used as regions of interest to detect heat changes. Eye (Stewart et al., 2008; Csoltova et al., 2017) and nose (Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011; Proctor and Carder, 2016) temperatures are used most frequently, but other body parts, such as the ears (Riemer et al., 2016) have also been used as a region of interest when studying emotion-induced heat surface changes in animals.

Due to the recent advances in thermal imaging technology and its noninvasive noncontact nature, the use of portable infrared cameras in animal emotion assessment research has been gaining popularity (e.g., Nakayama et al., 2005; Stewart et al., 2005, 2007, 2008, 2011, 2017; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011; Valera et al., 2012; Bartolomé et al., 2013; Herborn et al., 2015; Proctor and Carder, 2015; Cannas et al., 2018; Chotard et al., 2018; Hussein, 2018; Redaelli et al., 2019).

In dogs, there has been a growing interest in utilizing IRT to investigate negative emotion-induced surface heat increase associated mainly with stress, fear-based aggression, and separation anxiety, both in clinical and home settings (Travain et al., 2015; Riemer et al., 2016; Csoltova et al., 2017; Rigterink et al., 2018).

The affiliative behavior of the owner during a veterinary examination was found to have an attenuating effect on maximal ocular surface temperature compared to the no-interaction condition, reflecting possible parasympathetic activation (Csoltova et al., 2017).

The reappearance of both familiar and unfamiliar persons after a brief separation was sufficient to increase dogs’ outer ear temperature (Riemer et al., 2016).

The increase of maximal eye temperature was also recorded when dogs were anticipating and eating treats offered by owners (Travain et al., 2016).

From the research carried out so far, it is possible to conclude that IRT is a useful method to assess arousal intensity (Csoltova et al., 2017), but there has been some debate about whether IRT can distinguish emotional valence (Travain et al., 2015, 2016).

Oxytocin

Oxytocin is a neuropeptide and biomarker that is associated with a positive-emotional state in dogs (Mitsui et al., 2011).

Besides, previous studies indicate that in dogs, oxytocin may regulate a dog’s social behavior and attachment toward the owner (Beetz et al., 2012; Romero et al., 2014; MacLean and Hare, 2015; Nagasawa et al., 2015; Buttner, 2016; Kis et al., 2017; MacLean et al., 2017a). It seems that the sole reappearance of a familiar person, following separation, is sufficient to increase dogs’ oxytocin levels, but tactile and verbal contact is required for the oxytocin levels to remain continuously elevated (Rehn et al., 2014). Another study assessing the effect of positive human-dog interaction, found increased concentrations of biomarkers such as b-endorphin, oxytocin, prolactin, β-phenylethylamine, and dopamine in both humans and dogs (Odendaal and Meintjes, 2003). Similarly, other studies have supported the association between positive human-dog physical interaction and an increase in oxytocin in dogs (Handlin et al., 2011; MacLean et al., 2017a; Ogi et al., 2020).

Feeding and food-associated stimuli were equally found to increase oxytocin levels in dogs (Uvnäs−Moberg et al., 1985).

Moreover, the administration of exogenous oxytocin has been shown to have a pronounced effect on dogs’ cognition and behavior. Intranasal oxytocin administration increased the positive expectation of the dogs in the cognitive bias test (Kis et al., 2015), reduced separation anxiety (Thielke and Udell, 2017), and enhanced performance in a cognitive task (Oliva et al., 2015). Likewise, intranasal oxytocin administration increased dogs’ play motivation and intra- and interspecific social play behavior (Romero et al., 2014). Further, intranasally administered oxytocin has been shown to affect cardiac activity by decreasing HR and increasing HRV in dogs (Kis et al., 2014).

Urinary and, most recently, salivary oxytocin sampling has been validated as noninvasive approaches for quantifying oxytocin levels in dogs (Mitsui et al., 2011; MacLean et al., 2018; Powell et al., 2019; Schaebs et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2019).

However, as reported by Rault et al. (2017), it is still too early to consider oxytocin a potential indicator of positive well-being and/or welfare, given the discrepancies in the methodologies used to measure oxytocin in domesticated animals.

Vasopressin

Arginine vasopressin (AVP) is a neuropeptide closely related to oxytocin (Baribeau and Anagnostou, 2015). Both biomarkers play an important role in regulating mammals’ social and affiliative behavior (including pair-bonding and maternal behavior), social cognition, social stress and anxiety, and social aggression (Kendrick et al., 1997; Carter, 1998; Everts and Koolhaas, 1999; Goodson and Bass, 2001; Young and Wang, 2004; Donaldson and Young, 2008; Heinrichs and Domes, 2008; Veenema et al., 2010; Bisagno and Lud Cadet, 2014; Baribeau and Anagnostou, 2015).

In dogs, vasopressin increase has been associated with acute stress responses (Hydbring-Sandberg et al., 2004) and was also reported in dogs with separation anxiety (Pirrone et al., 2019). Further, lower free, but higher total plasma AVP has been found in aggressive dogs (MacLean et al., 2017b).

On the other hand, positive affiliative human-dog interactions, such as physical contact play, licking, lying supine have been associated with oxytocin increases, and AVP decreases (MacLean et al., 2017a).

Apart from blood sampling, salivary AVP has been validated as a potential noninvasive biomarker in dogs (MacLean et al., 2017b, 2018).

Cortisol

Increased cortisol levels are associated with HPA axis activation (Romero and Butler, 2007). Testing salivary cortisol levels in dogs has become a well-established method for measuring stress response and its impact on dogs’ well-being in several settings (Beerda et al., 1996, 1998; Dreschel and Granger, 2005; Haubenhofer and Kirchengast, 2006; Hekman et al., 2012; Glenk et al., 2014; Ng et al., 2014). Differences in the intensity and nature of a given stressor may impact endocrine responses, thus affecting cortisol release. A time frame from 10 to 30 min is required in order to detect a significant rise in cortisol concentrations in saliva after the onset of an acute stressor (Vincent and Michell, 1992; Beerda et al., 1998). To avoid the impact of handling on the measured cortisol concentration, the saliva sample collection time should not exceed 4 min (Kobelt et al., 2003).

There exists evidence suggesting an inverse relationship between cortisol and positive-emotional state in humans (Lindfors and Lundberg, 2002; Lai et al., 2005). Decreased cortisol levels have been implemented as an indicator of the stress-buffering effect of stimuli in several dog welfare studies. Such positive effects have been observed in human-dog interaction and listening to music, as dogs’ cortisol levels showed a decrease when studied in an animal shelter (Hennessy et al., 1998, 2006; Coppola et al., 2006; Bergamasco et al., 2010; Shiverdecker et al., 2013) and veterinary environments (Csoltova et al., 2017).

Alpha-Amylase

As with cortisol, measuring decreased levels of alpha-amylase, a crucial salivary enzyme (Nater and Rohleder, 2009), is another noninvasive approach to test sympathetic nervous system deactivation. Salivary alpha-amylase increase in response to both acute and chronic stressors has been documented in different animal species (Fuentes et al., 2011; Behringer et al., 2012; Fuentes-Rubio et al., 2016; Contreras-Aguilar et al., 2018), including dogs (Contreras-Aguilar et al., 2017; Hong et al., 2019). However, other studies emphasize the involvement of the parasympathetic nervous system branch in alpha-amylase secretion (Asking and Proctor, 1989; Nater and Rohleder, 2009; Bosch et al., 2011).

Noninvasive Neurobiological Methods

Increased tendencies to use neurobiological approaches to investigate the canine brain, social intelligence, and emotion have been observed in recent years. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) have been implemented to noninvasively study dog brain responses to both positive and negative emotion stimuli (e.g., Andics et al., 2014; Cook et al., 2014, 2016, 2018; Gygax et al., 2015; Berns and Cook, 2016).

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

fMRI is a neuroimaging tool used to measure and quantify brain activity indirectly by detecting dynamic changes in brain tissue oxygenation (Chen and Glover, 2015). Research studies mostly use the fMRI blood oxygen level-dependent imaging (BOLD) method, which reflects changes in blood oxygenation, flow, and volume in response to neural activity (Heeger and Ress, 2002; Glover, 2011; Devlin, 2016).

Thus far, studies using fMRI have focused on identifying neural responses to reward preference, temperament, face, odor, and vocal processing in awakened dogs (Andics et al., 2014; Cook et al., 2014, 2016, 2018; Berns et al., 2015; Dilks et al., 2015).

When studying the neural bases of preferences between social and food reward, researchers found that individual differences in mean ventral caudate activation in response to praise versus food predicted the subsequent behavioral choice between interaction with the owner (verbal praise) and food reward (eat a treat). Interestingly, 13 out of 15 tested dogs showed a preference for praise over food (Cook et al., 2016).

In another study, aggressive temperament was found to positively correlate with amygdala activation in dogs, while watching their caregiver give food to a fake dog. The authors interpret this emotion as a complex, jealousy-like phenomenon (Cook et al., 2018).

Another fMRI study showed that dogs are sensitive to human and dog auditory stimuli of different positive and negative emotional valence. Emotion-eliciting sound signals activated similarly located non-primary auditory areas both in dog and human brains. Besides, more pronounced neural activation was recorded to positive sound stimuli in both humans and dogs (Andics et al., 2014).

Olfactory neuroimaging study with dogs provided evidence for scent discrimination and positive associations with different odors, reflected in the maximal activation of the caudate nucleus to the odor of a familiar human in contrast to the odor of a familiar dog (Berns et al., 2015).

Besides, there is evidence suggesting a relationship between certain canine temperament traits and caudate nucleus activation. Twelve dogs were presented with 15 reward and 15 non-reward signals delivered by a familiar human, an unfamiliar human, or screen projected computer-generated signals during fMRI sessions. Results revealed higher caudate activation for reward versus non-reward signals. Dogs scoring lower on the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) aggressivity trait showed higher caudate activation to the reward than non-reward signals when delivered by a familiar human. On the contrary, in dogs scoring higher on the aggressivity trait, higher differential responses to the reward versus non-reward signals presented by the unfamiliar human and computer projected signals were observed (Cook et al., 2014).

Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy

fNIRS is another neuroimaging method and portable equipment used for noninvasive functional mapping of brain activity by using near-infrared detectors to measure cortical concentrations of oxygenation/deoxygenation and hemodynamic changes by absorbing infrared light by hemoglobin (Tamura et al., 1997; Ferrari and Quaresima, 2012).

fNIRS has been used in canine research to assess positive-emotional states during verbal and tactile human interactions. The results revealed that the cortical hemodynamic reaction measured by fNIRS may be a useful indicator of the emotional valence, while measured behavioral responses were shown as useful indicators of arousal level in dogs. The consistency of the hemodynamic frontal cortical reaction throughout the test, in addition to changes in behavioral responses with repetition, indicates that the valence of the stimuli remained the same while the arousal of the dogs decreased as dogs habituated to the repetitions (Gygax et al., 2015).

Possible Future Direction in Dog Positive-Emotion Assessment

Psychophysiology

Historically, psychophysiology has been interested in studying the impact of emotional processing on physiological functions (Davidson, 2003). With the rapid development of new technologies in the pet care industry, implementing complementary psychophysiology biosensors could shed additional light on our understanding of canine positive-emotions.

Pupil Dilation

Pupil dilation is associated with the interactions of sympathetic and parasympathetic innervations of the ANS in the iris muscle. According to findings in human research, pupil response is a sensitive indicator of emotional arousal, irrespective of the hedonic valence of the stimuli. Pupil dilation was observed during emotionally engaging visual (Bradley et al., 2008) and auditory stimuli (Partala and Surakka, 2003), implying increased sympathetic activation during both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli presentation (Bradley et al., 2008).

It would certainly be interesting to investigate the role of the emotional valence of different types of rewarding stimuli (such as visual, gustatory, olfactory, or auditory stimuli) on pupil dilation in dogs.

Galvanic Skin Response

The galvanic skin response (GSR), also known as the skin conductance response or electrodermal response, is based on continuous autonomic variation in the electrical properties of the skin and results from sympathetic activation when either external or internal stimuli occur that are physiologically arousing (Lykken and Venables, 1971). GSR has been closely linked to autonomic emotional and cognitive processing and reflects sympathetic arousal. It is the only autonomic psychophysiological variable not affected by parasympathetic activity. This method can be implemented in an objective assessment of emotional states and attentional processing (Braithwaite et al., 2013).

GSR was sensitive to stimulus valence in humans presented with pleasant and unpleasant food and nonfood pictures. The pleasant pictures were associated with a decrease in skin conductance, while the negative pictures evoked sweating and increased skin conductance. This indicates the potential effectiveness of GSR in a valence assessment of emotional stimuli (Kuoppa et al., 2016).

In addition, a close relationship between GSR and other psychophysiology measures was found. An accuracy rate of 80.2% was observed between HRV and GSR signals in association with emotion assessment (Lee et al., 2005). High covariation between pupillary dilation and GSR was recorded in reaction to positive and negative picture viewing, suggesting sympathetic nervous system activation in humans (Bradley et al., 2008).

Respiration Rate

Autonomic respiration is regulated by metabolic requirements but can shift in response to changes associated with different emotions, such as sadness, happiness, anxiety, or fear (Homma and Masaoka, 2008). Research on emotion-respiration relationships has largely focused on measuring respiration rate, amplitude, volume, and respiratory cycle (Boiten et al., 1994). In human literature, a study suggests that different emotional states can be differentiated according to the type of breathing pattern (Philippot et al., 2002). In farm animals, the respiration rate seems to be a useful indicator of emotional arousal (Briefer et al., 2015), and possibly also of emotional valence (Reefmann et al., 2009a). Therefore, measuring the respiration rate can shed additional light on emotion processing in dogs.

Immunological Indicators

Many studies on humans found a distinct enhancement effect of positive-emotions on the immune system (Dillon et al., 1986; Pressman and Black, 2012; Stellar et al., 2015). In dogs, only the impact of negative stress on the immune reaction has been studied so far. According to the findings, acute stress affects the immune system by inducing overall peripheral leukocytosis (Beerda et al., 1997). Salivary immunoglobulin A (sIgA) has been proposed as another potentially useful bioindicator of acute and chronic stress in dogs (Skandakumar et al., 1995; Kikkawa et al., 2003).

Association between canine immune system enhancement and positive-emotional state is another research area that requires more attention. Salivary immunological indicators could find their potential usefulness, especially in the assessment of dogs’ longer-term emotional states and positive well-being.

Affective Computing in Positive-Emotion Assessment

Both machine learning and deep learning have promising potential in animal behavior, communication, emotion, and welfare research (Valletta et al., 2017; Hantke et al., 2018; Norouzzadeh et al., 2018; Pereira et al., 2019). Machine learning has been used for automatically computing interpretable, quantitative measures and classification of behaviors and activities within complex data sets (Kabra et al., 2013; Dell et al., 2014). Machine learning has been widely applied especially to animal movement and location data analyses (Wang, 2019). In human research, machine learning techniques have already been implemented to recognize emotional states by studying facial expressions (Michel and Kaliouby, 2003), speech (Shami and Verhelst, 2007), body movements and gestures (Castellano et al., 2007), and also physiological responses such as cardiac activity, galvanic skin response, respiration rate, and skin temperature (Shi et al., 2010; Domínguez-Jiménez et al., 2020). Also, infrared thermography data are showing promising usefulness in automatic emotion recognition (Boccanfuso et al., 2016).

Deep learning, a subfield of machine learning, goes further by using a deep neural network to learn from and process raw data (Goodfellow et al., 2015). It has been gaining popularity in human emotion recognition studies (Giannopoulos et al., 2018; Jain et al., 2018; Hassan et al., 2019).

Implementation of affective computing in positive-emotion assessment might be particularly useful where sorting and classifying a large data set are required or where multimodal data integration is necessary to further enhance emotion recognition (Caridakis et al., 2007; Yin et al., 2017). Affective computing might provide solutions in accurate automatic detection of the small subtleties related to different emotional states by analyzing data from either a single biosignal input or by a combination of multiple physiological inputs. Affective computing might also provide solutions for the recognition of emotional states with similar biosignal inputs by analyzing data in a time series (Quesnel et al., 2018). Furthermore, affective computing considerably decreases the time of manual data and video analyses.

In canine emotion research, machine learning has thus far shown promising results in individual, context, and emotion (both valence and intensity) recognition of dog barks (Molnár et al., 2008; Hantke et al., 2018).

Overall, both machine and deep learning offer great potential in automatic emotion detection and recognition (Yin et al., 2017).

The Impact of Human-Dog Relationship on Dog Emotion

There is an increasing amount of evidence suggesting that the unique characteristics of the human-dog relationship, owner’s personality, emotional state, or behavior have been shown to affect the behavior, cognition, emotion processing, and expression of pet dogs (Prato-Previde et al., 2008; Handlin et al., 2012; Merola et al., 2012; Gácsi et al., 2013; Konok et al., 2015a; Cimarelli et al., 2016; Schöberl et al., 2016; Huber et al., 2017; Dodman et al., 2018; Gobbo and Zupan, 2020; Salamon et al., 2020).

The relationship between owner and dog resembles that of the parent (primary caretaker) and child (Bowlby, 2005). The dog perceives the owner as a secure base from which to explore the world (Topál et al., 1998). Pet owners differ in their attachment style to dogs, and these differences in behavior and emotion toward dogs impact their behavior and emotion toward their dog, subsequently affecting the dogs’ interaction with the environment. The owner’s attachment and caregiving style have been shown to impact dogs’ responses toward stress-inducing stimuli. The dogs of secure owners showed the most independent and confident behavior when they were exposed to environmental stressors compared with dogs of anxious and avoidant owners (Rehn et al., 2017). Moreover, dogs of owners with insecure attachments had a higher tendency to develop separation anxiety (Konok et al., 2015a).

Some studies have found a link between the owner’s personality and dogs’ behavioral and physiological responses. Owners scoring high in neuroticism and agreeableness had dogs with lower cortisol levels measured in the Strange Situation Test (Schöberl et al., 2016). In addition, dogs pay close attention to owners’ responses and act accordingly when facing novel stimuli (Merola et al., 2012; Salamon et al., 2020).

Further, research studies have shown that dogs tend to synchronize their emotional states with their owner (Sümegi et al., 2014; Turcsán et al., 2015; Sundman et al., 2019). Emotional contagion is an immediate synchronization of the emotional state between the subject and an object (Preston and de Waal, 2002; Nakahashi and Ohtsuki, 2018). Emotional contagion from the owner to the dog was recorded by the assessment of the cardiac responses during the Trier social stress test (TSST). Synchronization of the RMSSD levels between the owner and the dog during a stressful condition was found to positively correlate with the duration of the ownership (Katayama et al., 2019). Oxytocin-level correlation was recorded between the owner and the dog during a short-term interaction (Handlin et al., 2012). Owner-dog synchronization of cortisol levels was confirmed during dog agility competition (Jones and Josephs, 2006; Buttner et al., 2015). Furthermore, a recent study showed evidence of long-term synchronization of stress levels between owners and dogs (Sundman et al., 2019). Research on interspecies human-dog chemo-signaling revealed that dogs’ behavioral and heart responses were affected by human body odor changes, induced by different emotional states such as happiness and fear (D’Aniello et al., 2018).

Conclusion and Further Directions

Our main goal was to review and summarize the current advancement and scattered and dispersed research on positive-emotion in dogs. As previously discussed, prior research has focused on behavioral, cognitive, physiological, endocrine, and neurobiological approaches in dogs’ positive-emotion assessment.

Behavioral indicators have been shown to be contradictory and highly context-dependent. Therefore, environmental conditions need to be taken into consideration when interpreting the results. Another major issue with behavioral indicators is the high intra- and interindividual variability in responses (Beerda et al., 1997; Bell et al., 2009; Biro and Adriaenssens, 2013; Goold and Newberry, 2017). One possible solution to these issues could be the implementation of artificial intelligence in behavior analyses. Machine learning and deep learning algorithms could provide useful solutions in detecting subtle differences in behavioral responses caused by different valence and try to find response patterns to a specific positive-emotion eliciting stimulus. But for the moment, behavioral parameters should be used as complementary indicators of emotional valence and supported by other measurements.

Research findings on cerebral asymmetry have identified diverse lateral biases toward qualitatively different emotion stimuli, suggesting the relevance of this asymmetry to animal well-being and welfare research (Rogers, 2010). However, further research on brain lateralization is necessary to deepen our understanding of positive-emotion and emotional information perception, processing, and response in dogs.

Implementing cognitive bias testing is another promising means to assess the emotional responses and overall well-being of a dog (Burman et al., 2011; Kis et al., 2015; Wells et al., 2017). Therefore, more research studies are required in this area. However, the role of temperament traits as a predictor of cognitive bias remains to be further explored (Barnard et al., 2018).

More and more evidence suggests that QBA is a reliable, dynamic qualitative method for positive-emotion and welfare assessment (Stockman et al., 2011; Fleming et al., 2016; Minero et al., 2016, 2018; Collins et al., 2018; Patel et al., 2019). This tool seems to be particularly beneficial in assessing the overall well-being of dogs, for example, in a shelter environment (Walker et al., 2016; Arena et al., 2019).

Certain physiological (e.g., heart rate and superficial temperature) and neurobiological (e.g., amygdala activation) measures are good indicators of arousal; however, they have also been shown to correlate with more than one type of emotional state (Beebe-Center and Stevens, 1937; Kostarczyk and Fonberg, 1982; Garavan et al., 2001; Hamann et al., 2002; Bonnet et al., 2015; Proctor and Carder, 2016; Travain et al., 2016). Again, applying artificial intelligence in data analysis to find specific patterns of responses or changes in time series might prove useful in the identification of qualitative and quantitative differences in positive-emotion processing and responses.

HRV markers have been found to be sensitive to both valence and intensity of emotion (Katayama et al., 2016; Zupan et al., 2016). These promising findings need to be further explored by implementing additional HRV parameters to measure valence and arousal aspects of positive-emotion-eliciting stimuli under well-controlled conditions.

Some of the noninvasive salivary endocrine measures have been validated only recently, and more research is needed in the area. Nevertheless, the common methodological issues in endocrine measures have been identified as subject-related (intra- and interindividual variability), experiment-related (lack of standardization in methodology, impact of sampling time, owner presence, and data interpretation), and context-related (both the impact of testing and regular living environment). Standardized methodology and well-controlled experiments are required to validate endocrine measures as reliable indicators of positive well-being in animals (Cobb et al., 2016; Rault et al., 2017; Chmelíková et al., 2020).

Research evidence shows that both fMRI and fNIRS might be very useful tools in the systematic exploration of positive-emotion processing in dogs. fNIRS and fMRI are comparative methods sensitive to similar physiologic changes. While implementing neuroscientific research into dogs’ positive assessment can enhance our knowledge, the fMRI is technically demanding and requires pre-trained animals, which limits studies to small sample sizes. On the other hand, fNIRS is portable, with higher tolerances to motion, and cost-effective; and it requires no restraint of animals. The disadvantage of fNIRS over fMRI is a limited spatial resolution (Scarapicchia et al., 2017).

Furthermore, we have proposed additional psychophysiology approaches which could be complementary to the existing methods in the assessment of both short-term emotional responses as well as longer-term emotional states. Moreover, we would like to stress again the great potential and importance of affective computing in animal emotion research, especially in recognizing subtle differences among emotional responses or by detecting specific patterns in time series of single biosignals or by combining multimodal input data (Caridakis et al., 2007; Yin et al., 2017; Quesnel et al., 2018). For example, deep learning might be particularly helpful in developing a newer methodology for fast detection of regions of interest and accurate, reliable data extraction and analyses in infrared imaging (Marzec et al., 2016; Sonkusare et al., 2019).

Overall, it is too early to conclude whether a certain method or technique in positive-emotion research on dogs is more suitable than others. Prevalent methodological issues and the chronic problem of small sample sizes show that there is a huge need for well-controlled studies with larger numbers of participants. Some approaches show more promising results than others, but the great amount of ambiguous and contrasting findings points toward certain issues that need to be addressed before proceeding further:

a. The relevance of dog temperament in emotion perception, regulation, and responses. Research findings indicate the importance of temperament in dogs’ emotion processing and responses (e.g., Cook et al., 2014, 2018; Barnard et al., 2018). Thus, the role of temperament in dog emotion assessment deserves to be investigated further. Implementing validated and reliable dog temperament questionnaires (e.g., PANAS, C-BARQ, MCPQ-R, VIDOPET) (Sheppard and Mills, 2002; Hsu and Serpell, 2003; Ley et al., 2009; Rayment et al., 2016; Turcsán et al., 2018; Savalli et al., 2019) could potentially reduce variability in studies (Manteca and Deag, 1993) and shed additional light on inter-individual differences in dogs’ emotion processing and responses.

b. Establishing the reward value prior to testing. Positive-emotion eliciting stimuli used in previous studies are mostly pleasure-related (e.g., hedonic taste, pleasurable touch). However, not all food is automatically liked by, and thus pleasure-eliciting for all dogs. The liking aspects depend both on individual preferences and levels of hunger, satiation, or satiety. Despite the fact that owner-dog physical interaction is generally regarded as a positive social reward, not all human interaction might present a positive experience. For example, petting dogs in certain areas might be an unpleasant experience for a dog (Kuhne et al., 2014a). Therefore, close attention needs to be paid to the real value of the reward to a dog. Besides, the arousal aspect of the stimulus should be taken into account as well, since it might affect the response strength. The impact of the testing environment might further complicate the issue, since a novel environment might present a stressor to a dog and thus possibly reduce the hedonic valuation of rewards.

c. Dissecting wanting and liking components of reward (Berridge et al., 2009). When measuring responses to pleasure eliciting stimuli, researchers should focus separately on each phase of the pleasure cycle (Figure 1). As we have previously discussed, distinct reward-related motivational, emotional, and cognitive processes are activated, each with separable neurobiological mechanisms during appetitive, consummatory, and satiety phase (Berridge et al., 2009; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2011). Furthermore, reward delay during the anticipatory phase may lead to frustration (Amsel, 1958).

d. Controlling the impact of dogs’ social cognition, attachment, and emotion contagion. The unique psychological characteristics of the owner-dog relationship might significantly impact the measured emotion responses in dogs. Owner-dog relationship or attachment style questionnaires (e.g., MDORS, PAQ) (Dwyer et al., 2006; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2011) could be implemented when dogs are being tested in the presence of the owner, or when owner-dog interaction is used to induce positive-emotion in dogs. Besides, as dogs pay close attention to human emotional signals and cues and act accordingly, this must be taken into consideration during testing conditions as well (Merola et al., 2012; Salamon et al., 2020).

As the research findings show, no single valid method currently exists to reliably assess positive-emotion in dogs. Therefore, great caution is advised when interpreting behavioral, physiological, or endocrine indicators alone (de Vere and Kuczaj, 2016). Multiple noninvasive approaches, combining behavioral, noninvasive physiological, endocrine, and neural measures, need to be implemented to get a rigorous and reliable assessment of qualitative and quantitative differences of emotions in dogs (Paul et al., 2005; Boissy et al., 2007; de Vere and Kuczaj, 2016).

Overall, this review indicates that the research field concerning canine positive-emotion remains largely unexplored, offering researchers opportunities for discoveries that would deepen our knowledge of positive-emotion perception, processing, and recognition, with possible implications for both research and practice to improve the positive well-being and welfare of our companion animals.

Author Contributions

EC reviewed the literature and wrote the manuscript. EM reviewed and approved the final manuscript. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

EC and EM were employed by the company Diana Pet Food.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the reviewers for their constructive comments toward improving our manuscript. We also thank Joshua R. Urban for his editorial assistance.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Even though temperament and personality are often used interchangeably or are not clearly distinguished in animal studies, we adopt the view from human psychology that personality is considered a broader concept encompassing individual characteristics that represent a unique consistent pattern of feeling, thinking, and behaving (Cervone and Pervin, 2015).
  2. ^ For the purposes of this paper, well-being is defined as the animal’s complex subjective internal state affected by the animal’s perception, cognition, emotion, motivation, and the manifestation of its mental and somatic perception and interpretation of the internal and external stimuli (Clark et al., 1997). The term welfare is considered broader, encompassing the overall state of animal’s health and physical, social, and mental well-being (adapted from Halverson, 2001). The exception is when we are discussing the work of others who use a different convention.

References

Adolphs, R., Jansari, A., and Tranel, D. (2001). Hemispheric perception of emotional valence from facial expressions. Neuropsychology 15, 516–524. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.4.516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahern, G. L., and Schwartz, G. E. (1979). Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion. Neuropsychologia 17, 693–698. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90045-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albuquerque, N., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Resende, B., and Mills, D. S. (2018). Mouth-licking by dogs as a response to emotional stimuli. Behav. Processes 146, 42–45. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albuquerque, N., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Savalli, C., Otta, E., and Mills, D. (2016). Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol. Lett. 12:20150883. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and Growth in Personality. New York, NY: Holt, Reinhart & Winston.

Google Scholar

Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychol. Bull. 55, 102–119. doi: 10.1037/h0043125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, S. L., and Gantt, W. H. (1966). The effect of Person on cardiac and motor responsivity to shock in dogs. Cond. Reflex 1, 181–189. doi: 10.1007/BF03001101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andics, A., Gácsi, M., Faragó, T., Kis, A., and Miklósi, Á (2014). Voice-sensitive regions in the dog and human brain are revealed by comparative fMRI. Curr. Biol. 24, 574–578. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arena, L., Wemelsfelder, F., Messori, S., Ferri, N., and Barnard, S. (2019). Development of a fixed list of terms for the qualitative behavioural assessment of shelter dogs. PLoS One 14:e0212652. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Carrión, Ó, and Pöppel, E. (2007). Dopamine, learning, and reward-seeking behavior. Acta Neurobiol. Expe. 67, 481–488.

Google Scholar

Artelle, K. A., Dumoulin, L. K., and Reimchen, T. E. (2011). Behavioural responses of dogs to asymmetrical tail wagging of a robotic dog replica. Laterality 16, 129–135. doi: 10.1080/13576500903386700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asking, B., and Proctor, G. B. (1989). Parasympathetic activation of amylase secretion in the intact and sympathetically denervated rat parotid gland. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 74, 45–52. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1989.sp003238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, A. L. A., Randi, D., Müller, C. A., and Huber, L. (2016). The processing of human emotional faces by pet and lab dogs: evidence for lateralization and experience effects. PLoS One 11:e0152393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baribeau, D. A., and Anagnostou, E. (2015). Oxytocin and vasopressin: linking pituitary neuropeptides and their receptors to social neurocircuits. Front. Neurosci. 9:335. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnard, S., Wells, D. L., Milligan, A. D. S., Arnott, G., and Hepper, P. G. (2018). Personality traits affecting judgement bias task performance in dogs (Canis familiaris). Sci. Rep. 8:6660. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25224-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 20–46. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Wager, T. D. (2006). The structure of emotion: evidence from neuroimaging studies. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 79–83. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00411.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolomé, E., Sánchez, M. J., Molina, A., Schaefer, A. L., Cervantes, I., and Valera, M. (2013). Using eye temperature and heart rate for stress assessment in young horses competing in jumping competitions and its possible influence on sport performance. Animal 7, 2044–2053. doi: 10.1017/S1751731113001626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E., and Wright, G. A. (2011). Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr. Biol. 21, 1070–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beebe-Center, J. G., and Stevens, S. S. (1937). Cardiac acceleration in emotional situations. J. Exp. Psychol. 21, 72–87. doi: 10.1037/h0056889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., Bernadina, W., Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., De Vries, H. W., and Mol, J. A. (1999). Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. II. hormonal and immunological responses. Physiol. Behav 66, 243–254. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00290-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., Janssen, N. S. C. R. M., and Mol, J. A. (1996). The use of saliva cortisol, urinary cortisol, and catecholamine measurements for a noninvasive assessment of stress responses in dogs. Horm. Behav. 30, 272–279. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1996.0033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, A. M., and de Vries, H. W. (2000). Behavioural and hormonal indicators of enduring environmental stress in dogs. Anim. Welf. 9, 49–62.

Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., and de Vries, H. W. (1997). Manifestations of chronic and acute stress in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 52, 307–319. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01131-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., and Mol, J. A. (1998). Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 58, 365–381. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00145-147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beetz, A., Uvnäs-Moberg, K., Julius, H., and Kotrschal, K. (2012). Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human-animal interactions: the possible role of oxytocin. Front. Psychol. 3:234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behringer, V., Deschner, T., Möstl, E., Selzer, D., and Hohmann, G. (2012). Stress affects salivary alpha-Amylase activity in bonobos. Physiol. Behav 105, 476–482. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekoff, M. (1974a). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. Integr. Comp. Biol. 14, 323–340. doi: 10.1093/icb/14.1.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekoff, M. (1974b). Social play in coyotes. wolves, and dogs. BioScience 24, 225–230. doi: 10.2307/1296803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekoff, M. (1984). Social play behavior. BioScience 34, 228–233. doi: 10.2307/1309460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, A. M., Hankison, S. J., and Laskowski, K. L. (2009). The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 77, 771–783. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergamasco, L., Osella, M. C., Savarino, P., Larosa, G., Ozella, L., Manassero, M., et al. (2010). Heart rate variability and saliva cortisol assessment in shelter dog: human–animal interaction effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 125, 56–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berns, G. S., and Cook, P. F. (2016). Why did the dog walk into the MRI? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25, 363–369. doi: 10.1177/0963721416665006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berns, G. S., Brooks, A. M., and Spivak, M. (2015). Scent of the familiar: an fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors. Behav. Processes 110, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C. (1996). Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C. (2000). Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 173–198. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00072-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191, 391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology 199, 457–480. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2011). Building a neuroscience of pleasure and well-being. Psychol. Well Being 1:3. doi: 10.1186/2211-1522-1-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 294–303. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 26, 507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., and Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K., Grill, H. J., and Norgren, R. (1981). Relation of consummatory responses and preabsorptive insulin release to palatability and learned taste aversions. J. Compa. Physiol. Psychol. 95, 363–382. doi: 10.1037/h0077782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biro, P. A., and Adriaenssens, B. (2013). Predictability as a personality trait: consistent differences in intraindividual behavioral variation. Am. Nat. 182, 621–629. doi: 10.1086/673213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisagno, V., and Lud Cadet, J. (2014). Stress, gender, and addiction: potential roles of CRF, oxytocin and argininevasopressin. Behav. Pharmacol. 25, 445–457. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boccanfuso, L., Wang, Q., Leite, I., Li, B., Torres, C., Chen, L., et al. (2016). “A thermal emotion classifier for improved human-robot interaction,” in Proceedings of the 2016 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE)718–723. doi: 10.1109/ROMAN.2016.7745198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boissy, A. (1995). Fear and Fearfulness in Animals. Q. Rev Biol. 70, 165–191. doi: 10.1086/418981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boissy, A., Manteuffel, G., Jensen, M. B., Moe, R. O., Spruijt, B., Keeling, L. J., et al. (2007). Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav 92, 375–397. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boiten, F. A., Frijda, N. H., and Wientjes, C. J. (1994). Emotions and respiratory patterns: review and critical analysis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 17, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)90027-90022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnet, L., Comte, A., Tatu, L., Millot, J., Moulin, T., Medeiros, et al. (2015). The role of the amygdala in the perception of positive emotions: an “intensity detector. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:178. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borod, J. C., Cicero, B. A., Obler, L. K., Welkowitz, J., Erhan, H. M., Santschi, C., et al. (1998). Right hemisphere emotional perception: evidence across multiple channels. Neuropsychology 12, 446–458. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch, J. A., Veerman, E. C. I., de Geus, E. J., and Proctor, G. B. (2011). α-Amylase as a reliable and convenient measure of sympathetic activity: don’t start salivating just yet! Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 449–453. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (2005). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory. Milton Parks: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Bowman, A., Scottish, S. P. C. A., Dowell, F. J., and Evans, N. P. (2015). ‘Four Seasons’ in an animal rescue centre; classical music reduces environmental stress in kennelled dogs. Physiol. Behav 143, 70–82. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., and Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45, 602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J. W. S., Pullen, A. J., and Rooney, N. J. (2015). Why do adult dogs ‘play’? Behav. Processes 110, 82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., and Rowe, M. (2013). A Guide for Analysing Electrodermal Activity (EDA) & Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs) for Psychological Experiments. Birmingham: University of Birmingham.

Google Scholar

Bremhorst, A., Bütler, S., Würbel, H., and Riemer, S. (2018). Incentive motivation in pet dogs – preference for constant vs varied food rewards. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28079-28075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremhorst, A., Sutter, N. A., Würbel, H., Mills, D. S., and Riemer, S. (2019). Differences in facial expressions during positive anticipation and frustration in dogs awaiting a reward. Sci. Rep. 9:19312. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55714-55716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briefer, E. F., Tettamanti, F., and McElligott, A. G. (2015). Emotions in goats: mapping physiological, behavioural and vocal profiles. Anim. Behav. 99, 131–143. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, N. J., and Reimchen, T. E. (2020). Right nostril biases to experimental scents in Canis familiaris. Laterality 25:1, 43–52. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2019.1614942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., and Panksepp, J. (2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 173–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burman, O. H. P., and Mendl, M. T. (2018). A novel task to assess mood congruent memory bias in non-human animals. J. Neurosci. Methods 308, 269–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burman, O., McGowan, R., Mendl, M., Norling, Y., Paul, E., Rehn, T., et al. (2011a). Using judgement bias to measure positive affective state in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 132, 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buttner, A. P. (2016). Neurobiological underpinnings of dogs’ human-like social competence: how interactions between stress response systems and oxytocin mediate dogs’ social skills. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 198–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buttner, A. P., Thompson, B., Strasser, R., and Santo, J. (2015). Evidence for a synchronization of hormonal states between humans and dogs during competition. Physiol. Behav. 147, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological Role of Pleasure. Science 173, 1103–1107. doi: 10.1126/science.173.4002.1103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabanac, M. (1979). Sensory Pleasure. Q. Rev Biol. 54, 1–29. doi: 10.1086/410981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caeiro, C. C., Burrows, A. M., and Waller, B. M. (2017a). Development and application of CatFACS: are human cat adopters influenced by cat facial expressions? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 189, 66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caeiro, C. C., Waller, B. M., Zimmermann, E., Burrows, A. M., and Davila-Ross, M. (2013). OrangFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for orangutans. Int. J. Primatol. 34, 115–129. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9652-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caeiro, C., Guo, K., and Mills, D. (2017b). Dogs and humans respond to emotionally competent stimuli by producing different facial actions. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15091-15094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, K. E., Garnham, J., de, Jensen, K., and Bizo, L. A. (2019). Food preference predicts speed of approach on a runway task by dogs. Pet Behav. Sci. 0, 1–10. doi: 10.21071/pbs.v0i8.11179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannas, S., Palestrini, C., Canali, E., Cozzi, B., Ferri, N., Heinzl, E., et al. (2018). Thermography as a noninvasive measure of stress and fear of humans in sheep. Animals 8:146. doi: 10.3390/ani8090146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caridakis, G., Castellano, G., Kessous, L., Raouzaiou, A., Malatesta, L., Asteriadis, S., et al. (2007). “Multimodal emotion recognition from expressive faces, body gestures and speech,” in Artificial Intelligence and Innovations 2007: from Theory to Applications, eds C. Boukis, A. Pnevmatikakis, and L. Polymenakos (Boston, MA: Springer), 375–388. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-74161-1_41

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, R. M., and Tong, Q. (2017). Hunger and satiety gauge reward sensitivity. Front. Endocrinol. 8:104. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellano, G., Villalba, S. D., and Camurri, A. (2007). “Recognising human emotions from body movement and gesture dynamics,” in Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction ACII ’07 (Berlin: Springer-Verlag), 71–82. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-74889-2_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro, D. C., and Berridge, K. C. (2014). Opioid hedonic hotspot in nucleus accumbens shell: Mu, Delta, and Kappa Maps for enhancement of sweetness “Liking” and “Wanting.”. J. Neurosci. 34, 4239–4250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4458-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervone, D., and Pervin, L. A. (2015). Personality: Theory and Research. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Chen, J. E., and Glover, G. H. (2015). Functional magnetic resonance imaging methods. Neuropsychol Rev 25, 289–313. doi: 10.1007/s11065-015-9294-9299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chmelíková, E., Bolechová, P., Chaloupková, H., Svobodová, I., Jovièiæ, M., and Sedmíková, M. (2020). Salivary cortisol as a marker of acute stress in dogs: a review. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 72:106428. doi: 10.1016/j.domaniend.2019.106428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chotard, H., Ioannou, S., and Davila-Ross, M. (2018). Infrared thermal imaging: positive and negative emotions modify the skin temperatures of monkey and ape faces. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22863. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cimarelli, G., Turcsán, B., Bánlaki, Z., Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2016). Dog owners’ interaction styles: their components and associations with reactions of pet dogs to a social threat. Front. Psychol. 7:1979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, J. D., Rager, D. R., and Calpin, J. P. (1997). Animal well-being. I. General considerations. Lab. Anim. Sci. 47, 564–570.

Google Scholar

Clegg, I. L. K. (2018). Cognitive bias in zoo animals: an optimistic outlook for welfare assessment. Animals 8:104. doi: 10.3390/ani8070104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cobb, M. L., Iskandarani, K., Chinchilli, V. M., and Dreschel, N. A. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of salivary cortisol measurement in domestic canines. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 57, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.domaniend.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, K. (2020). ““Comparative perspectives on temperament and personality in human and nonhuman animals,”,” in Adaptive Shyness: Multiple Perspectives on Behavior and Development, eds L. A. Schmidt and K. L. Poole (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 253–277. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collet, C., Vernet-Maury, E., Delhomme, G., and Dittmar, A. (1997). Autonomic nervous system response patterns specificity to basic emotions. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 62, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1838(96)00108-107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, T., Stockman, C. A., Barnes, A. L., Miller, D. W., Wickham, S. L., and Fleming, P. A. (2018). Qualitative behavioural assessment as a method to identify potential stressors during commercial sheep transport. Animals 8:209. doi: 10.3390/ani8110209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Aguilar, M. D., Escribano, D., Martínez-Subiela, S., Martínez-Miró, S., Cerón, J. J., and Tecles, F. (2018). Changes in alpha-amylase activity, concentration and isoforms in pigs after an experimental acute stress model: an exploratory study. BMC Vet. Res. 14:256. doi: 10.1186/s12917-018-1581-1582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, P., Prichard, A., Spivak, M., and Berns, G. S. (2018). Jealousy in dogs? Evidence from brain imaging. Anim. Sentience 3, 1.

Google Scholar

Cook, P. F., Prichard, A., Spivak, M., and Berns, G. S. (2016). Awake canine fMRI predicts dogs’ preference for praise vs food. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1853–1862. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, P. F., Spivak, M., and Berns, G. S. (2014). One pair of hands is not like another: caudate BOLD response in dogs depends on signal source and canine temperament. PeerJ 2:e596. doi: 10.7717/peerj.596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Aguilar, M. D., Tecles, F., Martínez-Subiela, S., Escribano, D., Bernal, L. J., and Cerón, J. J. (2017). Detection and measurement of alpha-amylase in canine saliva and changes after an experimentally induced sympathetic activation. BMC Vet. Res. 13:266. doi: 10.1186/s12917-017-1191-1194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppola, C. L., Grandin, T., and Enns, R. M. (2006). Human interaction and cortisol: can human contact reduce stress for shelter dogs? Physiol. Behav 87, 537–541. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, E. D., Minero, M., Lebelt, D., Stucke, D., Canali, E., and Leach, M. C. (2014). Development of the horse grimace scale (hgs) as a pain assessment tool in horses undergoing routine castration. PLoS One 9:e92281. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulon, M., Nowak, R., Andanson, S., Ravel, C., Marnet, P. G., Boissy, A., et al. (2013). Human–lamb bonding: oxytocin, cortisol and behavioural responses of lambs to human contacts and social separation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulon, M., Nowak, R., Peyrat, J., Chandèze, H., Boissy, A., and Boivin, X. (2015). Do lambs perceive regular human stroking as pleasant? behavior and heart rate variability analyses. PLoS One 10:e0118617. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cozzi, A., Mariti, C., Ogi, A., Sighieri, C., and Gazzano, A. (2016). Behavioral modification in sheltered dogs. Dog Behav. 2, 1–12. doi: 10.4454/db.v2i3.38

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. B. (2005). Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 566–571. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, W. (1917). Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 3, 685–688. doi: 10.1073/pnas.3.12.685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespi, L. P. (1942). Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. Am. J. Psychol. 55, 467–517. doi: 10.2307/1417120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csoltova, E., Martineau, M., Boissy, A., and Gilbert, C. (2017). Behavioral and physiological reactions in dogs to a veterinary examination: owner-dog interactions improve canine well-being. Physiol. Behav. 177, 270–281. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cyr, N. E., Dickens, M. J., and Romero, L. M. (2009). Heart rate and heart−rate variability responses to acute and chronic stress in a wild−caught passerine bird. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 82, 332–344. doi: 10.1086/589839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Aniello, B., Semin, G. R., Alterisio, A., Aria, M., and Scandurra, A. (2018). Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: from humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 21, 67–78. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1139-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (2011). Neural basis of emotions. Scholarpedia 6:1804. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dantzer, R. (2002). Les émotions. 3e éd. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Google Scholar

Daroff, R., and Aminoff, J. M. (eds) (2014). Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences, 2nd Edn. Available at: https://www.elsevier.com/books/encyclopedia-of-the-neurological-sciences/daroff/978-0-12-385157-4 (accessed October 21, 2019).

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J. (2003). Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: toward a synthesis. Psychophysiology 40, 655–665. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Boyer Des Roches, A., Richard-Yris, M.-A., Henry, S., Ezzaouïa, M., and Hausberger, M. (2008). Laterality and emotions: visual laterality in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) differs with objects’ emotional value. Physiol. Behav. 94, 487–490. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Graaf, C., Blom, W. A. M., Smeets, P. A. M., Stafleu, A., and Hendriks, H. F. J. (2004). Biomarkers of satiation and satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 79, 946–961. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/79.6.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vere, A. J., and Kuczaj, S. A. (2016). Where are we in the study of animal emotions?: where are we in the study of animal emotions? WIREs Cogn. Sci 7, 354–362. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell, A. I., Bender, J. A., Branson, K., Couzin, I. D., de Polavieja, G. G., Noldus, L. P. J. J., et al. (2014). Automated image-based tracking and its application in ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 417–428. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Descovich, K., Wathan, J., Leach, M. C., Buchanan-Smith, H. M., Flecknell, P., Farningham, D., et al. (2017). Facial expression: an under-utilised tool for the assessment of welfare in mammals. ALTEX 34, 409–429. doi: 10.14573/altex.1607161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devlin, H. M. (2016). What is Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)?. Available at: //psychcentral.com/lib/what-is-functional-magnetic-resonance-imaging-fmri/ (accessed May 22, 2020).

Google Scholar

Dilks, D. D., Cook, P., Weiller, S. K., Berns, H. P., Spivak, M., and Berns, G. S. (2015). Awake fMRI reveals a specialized region in dog temporal cortex for face processing. PeerJ 3:e1115. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillon, K. M., Minchoff, B., and Baker, K. H. (1986). Positive Emotional States and Enhancement of the Immune System. Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 15, 13–18. doi: 10.2190/R7FD-URN9-PQ7F-A6J7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodman, N. H., Brown, D. C., and Serpell, J. A. (2018). Associations between owner personality and psychological status and the prevalence of canine behavior problems. PLoS One 13:e0192846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Jiménez, J. A., Campo-Landines, K. C., Martínez-Santos, J. C., Delahoz, E. J., and Contreras-Ortiz, S. H. (2020). A machine learning model for emotion recognition from physiological signals. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 55:101646. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2019.101646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, Z. R., and Young, L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreschel, N. A., and Granger, D. A. (2005). Physiological and behavioral reactivity to stress in thunderstorm-phobic dogs and their caregivers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 95, 153–168. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, R., Ginsberg, J. P., Porges, S. W., and Thayer, J. F. (2020). Heart Rate Variability, Health and Well-being: A Systems Perspective. Lausannel: Frontiers Media SA.

Google Scholar

Duncan, I. J. H. (2005). Science-based assessment of animal welfare: farm animals. Rev. Sci. Tech. 24:483. doi: 10.20506/rst.24.2.1587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, S., and Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: a neurobiological analysis. Cogn. Emot. 21, 1184–1211. doi: 10.1080/02699930701437931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwyer, F., Bennett, P. C., and Coleman, G. J. (2006). Development of the monash dog owner relationship scale (MDORS). Anthrozoös 19, 243–256. doi: 10.2752/089279306785415592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 3, 364–370. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Rosenberg, E. L. (eds) (2005). What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System, 2nd Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., Chan, A., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider, K., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgments of facial expressions of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 712–717. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Electrophysiology Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K., and Schulkin, J. (2003). Facial expressions of emotion: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Brain Cogn. 52, 52–60. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00008-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ettenberg, A. (2009). The runway model of drug self-administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 91, 271–277. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everts, H. G. J., and Koolhaas, J. M. (1999). Differential modulation of lateral septal vasopressin receptor blockade in spatial learning, social recognition, and anxiety-related behaviors in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 99, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00004-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feddersen-Petersen, D. U. (2000). Vocalization of European wolves (Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canis lupus f. fam.). Arch. Anim. Breed, 43, 387–398. doi: 10.5194/aab-43-387-2000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Carriba, S., Loeches, A., Morcillo, A., and Hopkins, W. D. (2002). Functional asymmetry of emotions in primates: new findings in chimpanzees. Brain Res. Bull. 57, 561–564. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00685-682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, M., and Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. NeuroImage 63, 921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finka, L. R., Luna, S. P., Brondani, J. T., Tzimiropoulos, Y., McDonagh, J., Farnworth, M. J., et al. (2019). Geometric morphometrics for the study of facial expressions in non-human animals, using the domestic cat as an exemplar. Sci. Rep. 9:9883. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46330-46335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finlayson, G., King, N., and Blundell, J. E. (2007). Is it possible to dissociate “liking” and “wanting” for foods in humans? A novel experimental procedure. Physiol. Behav. 90, 36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, P. A., Clarke, T., Wickham, S. L., Stockman, C. A., Barnes, A. L., Collins, T., et al. (2016). The contribution of qualitative behavioural assessment to appraisal of livestock welfare. Anim. Prod. Sci. 56, 1569–1578. doi: 10.1071/AN15101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, P. A., Paisley, C. L., Barnes, A. L., and Wemelsfelder, F. (2013). Application of Qualitative Behavioural Assessment to horses during an endurance ride. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 144, 80–88. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, M., Tecles, F., Gutiérrez, A., Otal, J., Martínez-Subiela, S., and Cerón, J. J. (2011). Validation of an automated method for salivary alpha-amylase measurements in pigs (Sus Scrofa Domesticus) and its application as a stress biomarker. J. VET Diagn. Invest. 23, 282–287. doi: 10.1177/104063871102300213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes-Rubio, M., Fuentes, F., Otal, J., Quiles, A., and Hevia, M. L. (2016). Validation of an Assay For quantification of Alpha-Amylase in Saliva of Sheep. Available at: https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/cvma/cjvr/2016/00000080/00000003/art00004 (accessed November 4, 2019).

Google Scholar

Gácsi, M., Maros, K., Sernkvist, S., Faragó, T., and Miklósi, Á (2013). Human analogue safe haven effect of the owner: behavioural and heart rate response to stressful social stimuli in dogs. PLoS One 8:e58475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garavan, H., Pendergrass, J. C., Ross, T. J., Stein, E. A., and Risinger, R. C. (2001). Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Neuroreport 12, 2779–2783. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-200108236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giannopoulos, P., Perikos, I., and Hatzilygeroudis, I. (2018). “Deep learning approaches for facial emotion recognition: a case study on FER-2013,” in Advances in Hybridization of Intelligent Methods, I. Hatzilygeroudis, V. Palade.(Cham: Springer) 1–16. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-66790-4_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenk, L. M., Kothgassner, O. D., Stetina, B. U., Palme, R., Kepplinger, B., and Baran, H. (2014). Salivary cortisol and behavior in therapy dogs during animal-assisted interventions: a pilot study. J. Vet. Behav. 9, 98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, G. H. (2011). Overview of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. 22, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobbo, E., and Zupan, M. (2020). Dogs’ sociability, owners’ neuroticism and attachment style to pets as predictors of dog aggression. Animals 10:315. doi: 10.3390/ani10020315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., and Campos, J. J. (1982). “Toward a Theory of Infant Temperament,” in The Development of Attachment and Affiliative Systems Topics in Developmental Psychobiology, eds R. N. Emde and R. J. Harmon (Boston, MA: Springer), 161–193. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-4076-8_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: what is temperament? four approaches. Child Dev. 58, 505–529. doi: 10.2307/1130527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodfellow, I. G., Bengio, Y., and Courville, A. C. (2015). Deep learning. Nature 521, 436–444. doi: 10.1038/nature14539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., and Bass, A. H. (2001). Social behavior functions and related anatomical characteristics of vasotocin/vasopressin systems in vertebrates. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 246–265. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00043-41

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goold, C., and Newberry, R. C. (2017). Modelling personality, plasticity and predictability in shelter dogs. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 4:170618. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goulart, C., Valadão, C., Delisle-Rodriguez, D., Caldeira, E., and Bastos, T. (2019). Emotion analysis in children through facial emissivity of infrared thermal imaging. PLoS One 14:e0212928. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grill, H. J., and Norgren, R. (1978). The taste reactivity test. I. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in neurologically normal rats. Brain Res. 143, 263–279. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90568-90561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (ed.) (2007). Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Guesgen, M. J., Beausoleil, N. J., Leach, M., Minot, E. O., Stewart, M., and Stafford, K. J. (2016). Coding and quantification of a facial expression for pain in lambs. Behav. Processes 132, 49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, K., Meints, K., Hall, C., Hall, S., and Mills, D. (2009). Left gaze bias in humans, rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs. Anim. Cogn. 12, 409–418. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0199-193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gygax, L., Reefmann, N., Pilheden, T., Scholkmann, F., and Keeling, L. (2015). Dog behavior but not frontal brain reaction changes in repeated positive interactions with a human: a noninvasive pilot study using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Behav. Brain Res. 281, 172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hales, J. R. S., and Dampney, R. A. L. (1975). The redistribution of cardiac output in the dog during heat stress. J. Therm. Biol. 1, 29–34. doi: 10.1016/0306-4565(75)90008-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halverson, M. K. (2001). Farm Animal Health and Well-being. Supplementary Literature Summary and Technical Working Paper for the Minnesota Generic Environmental Impact Statement on Animal Agriculture, Minnesota Planning Agency Environmental Quality Board. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/235954611_FARM_ANIMAL_HEALTH_AND_WELL-BEING_TECHNICAL_WORKING_PAPER_MINNESOTA_GENERIC_ENVIRONMENTAL_IMPACT_STATEMENT_ON_ANIMAL_AGRICULTURE (accessed June 30, 2001).

Google Scholar

Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., and Kilts, C. D. (2002). Ecstasy and agony: activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative emotion. Psychol. Sci. 13, 135–141. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handlin, L., Hydbring-Sandberg, E., Nilsson, A., Ejdebäck, M., Jansson, A., and Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2011). Short-term interaction between dogs and their owners: effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate—an exploratory study. Anthrozoös 24, 301–315. doi: 10.2752/175303711X13045914865385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handlin, L., Nilsson, A., Ejdebäck, M., Hydbring-Sandberg, E., and Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2012). Associations between the psychological characteristics of the human–dog relationship and oxytocin and cortisol levels. Anthrozoös 25, 215–228. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13316289505468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hantke, S., Cummins, N., and Schuller, B. (2018). “What is my dog trying to tell me? The automatic recognition of the context and perceived emotion of dog barks,” in Proceedings of th 2018 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP) (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE) 5134–5138. doi: 10.1109/ICASSP.2018.8461757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S., and Mendl, M. (2004). Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427, 312–312. doi: 10.1038/427312a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haselton, M.G., Nettle, D., and Andrews, P.W. (2015). “The evolution of cognitive bias,” in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, ed. D. M. Buss (Hoboken: Wiley), 724–746.

Google Scholar

Hassan, M. M., Alam, M. D. G. R., Uddin, M. D. Z., Huda, S., Almogren, A., and Fortino, G. (2019). Human emotion recognition using deep belief network architecture. Inf. Fusion 51, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.inffus.2018.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haubenhofer, D. K., and Kirchengast, S. (2006). Physiological arousal for companion dogs working with their owners in animal-assisted activities and animal-assisted therapy. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 9, 165–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0902_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeger, D. J., and Ress, D. (2002). What does fMRI tell us about neuronal activity? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 142–151. doi: 10.1038/nrn730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., and Domes, G. (2008). Neuropeptides and social behaviour: effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Prog. Brain Res. 170, 337–350. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00428-427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hekman, J. P., Karas, A. Z., and Dreschel, N. A. (2012). Salivary cortisol concentrations and behavior in a population of healthy dogs hospitalized for elective procedures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 141, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Held, S. D. E., and Špinka, M. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. Anim. Behav. 81, 891–899. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennessy, M. B., Morris, A., and Linden, F. (2006). Evaluation of the effects of a socialization program in a prison on behavior and pituitary–adrenal hormone levels of shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 99, 157–171. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennessy, M. B., Williams, M. T., Miller, D. D., Douglas, C. W., and Voith, V. L. (1998). Influence of male and female petters on plasma cortisol and behaviour: can human interaction reduce the stress of dogs in a public animal shelter? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 61, 63–77. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00179-178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herborn, K. A., Graves, J. L., Jerem, P., Evans, N. P., Nager, R., McCafferty, D. J., et al. (2015). Skin temperature reveals the intensity of acute stress. Physiol. Behav. 152, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hetts, S., Derrell Clark, J., Calpin, J. P., Arnold, C. E., and Mateo, J. M. (1992). Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 34, 137–155. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80063-80062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hintze, S., Melotti, L., Colosio, S., Bailoo, J. D., Boada-Saña, M., Würbel, H., et al. (2018). A cross-species judgement bias task: integrating active trial initiation into a spatial Go/No-go task. Sci. Rep. 8:5104. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23459-23453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C.-Y., and Berridge, K. C. (2013). An orexin hotspot in ventral pallidum amplifies hedonic ‘Liking’ for sweetness. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1655–1664. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, K. K. L., Moody, G. B., Peng, C. K., Mietus, J. E., Larson, M. G., Levy, D., et al. (1997). Predicting survival in heart failure case and control subjects by use of fully automated methods for deriving nonlinear and conventional indices of heart rate dynamics. Circulation 96, 842–848. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.96.3.842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holden, E., Calvo, G., Collins, M., Bell, A., Reid, J., Scott, E. M., et al. (2014). Evaluation of facial expression in acute pain in cats. J. Small Anim. Pract. 55, 615–621. doi: 10.1111/jsap.12283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Homma, I., and Masaoka, Y. (2008). Breathing rhythms and emotions. Exp. Physiol. 93, 1011–1021. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2008.042424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, H.-R., Oh, Y.-I., Kim, Y. J., and Seo, K.-W. (2019). Salivary alpha-amylase as a stress biomarker in diseased dogs. J. Vet. Sci. 20:e46. doi: 10.4142/jvs.2019.20.e46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horváth, Z., Dóka, A., and Miklósi, Á (2008). Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directions. Horm. Behav. 54, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, Y., and Serpell, J. A. (2003). Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 223, 1293–1300. doi: 10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, A., Barber, A. L. A., Faragó, T., Müller, C. A., and Huber, L. (2017). Investigating emotional contagion in dogs (Canis familiaris) to emotional sounds of humans and conspecifics. Anim. Cogn. 20, 703–715. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1092-1098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, C. L. (1934). The rat’s speed-of-locomotion gradient in the approach to food. J. Comp. Psychol. 17, 393–422. doi: 10.1037/h0071299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussein, N. J. (2018). Using eye and nasal temperatures to measure positive emotions in free-range hamdani sheep. Basrah J. Agric. Sci. 31, 24–30. doi: 10.37077/25200860.2018.96

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hydbring-Sandberg, E., von Walter, L., Hoglund, K., Svartberg, K., Swenson, L., and Forkman, B. (2004). Physiological reactions to fear provocation in dogs. J. Endocrinol. 180, 439–448. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1800439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychol. Bull. 115, 288–299. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 260–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., and Zajonc, R. B. (1984). Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior. Cambridge: CUP Archive.

Google Scholar

Izard, C., Stark, K., Trentacosta, C., and Schultz, D. (2008). Beyond emotion regulation. Child. Dev. Perspect. 2, 156–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00058.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jain, N., Kumar, S., Kumar, A., Shamsolmoali, P., and Zareapoor, M. (2018). Hybrid deep neural networks for face emotion recognition. Pattern Recognit. Lett. 115, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.patrec.2018.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarczok, M. N., Kleber, M. E., Koenig, J., Loerbroks, A., Herr, R. M., Hoffmann, K., et al. (2015). Investigating the associations of self-rated health: heart rate variability is more strongly associated than inflammatory and other frequently used biomarkers in a cross sectional occupational sample. PLoS One 10:e0117196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, A. C., and Josephs, R. A. (2006). Interspecies hormonal interactions between man and the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Horm. Behav. 50, 393–400. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabra, M., Robie, A. A., Rivera-Alba, M., Branson, S., and Branson, K. (2013). JAABA: interactive machine learning for automatic annotation of animal behavior. Nat. Methods 10, 64–67. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaminski, J., Waller, B. M., Hartstone-Rose, A., and Burrows, A. M. (2019). Evolution of Facial Muscle Anatomy in Dogs | PNAS. Available at: https://www.pnas.org/content/116/29/14677?fbclid=IwAR3lu8dKSqFFvcuWQIK5KEzgiwQvH-ZfzvV4aIsCE_yIUSMqya5BiNFxxj0 (accessed June 19, 2020).

Google Scholar

Katayama, M., Kubo, T., Mogi, K., Ikeda, K., Nagasawa, M., and Kikusui, T. (2016). Heart rate variability predicts the emotional state in dogs. Behav. Processes 128, 108–112. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katayama, M., Kubo, T., Yamakawa, T., Fujiwara, K., Nomoto, K., Ikeda, K., et al. (2019). Emotional contagion from humans to dogs is facilitated by duration of ownership. Front. Psychol. 10:1678. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keating, S. C. J., Thomas, A. A., Flecknell, P. A., and Leach, M. C. (2012). Evaluation of EMLA cream for preventing pain during tattooing of rabbits: changes in physiological, behavioural and facial expression responses. PLoS One 7:e44437. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendrick, K. M., Da Costa, A. P. C., Broad, K. D., Ohkura, S., Guevara, R., Lévy, F., et al. (1997). Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 383–395. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00218-219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikkawa, A., Uchida, Y., Nakade, T., and Taguchi, K. (2003). Salivary secretory IgA concentrations in beagle dogs. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 65, 689–693. doi: 10.1292/jvms.65.689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kis, A., Ciobica, A., and Topál, J. (2017). The effect of oxytocin on human-directed social behaviour in dogs (Canis familiaris). Horm. Behav. 94, 40–52. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kis, A., Hernádi, A., Kanizsár, O., Gácsi, M., and Topál, J. (2015). Oxytocin induces positive expectations about ambivalent stimuli (cognitive bias) in dogs. Horm. Behav. 69, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kis, A., Kanizsár, O., Gácsi, M., and Topál, J. (2014). Intranasally administered oxytocin decreases heart rate and increases heart rate variability in dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 9:e15. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knierim, U., and Winckler, C. (2009). On-farm welfare assessment in cattle: validity, reliability and feasibility issues and future perspectives with special regard to the welfare Quality® approach. Anim. Welf. 18, 451–458.

Google Scholar

Kobelt, A. J., Hemsworth, P. H., Barnett, J. L., and Butler, K. L. (2003). Sources of sampling variation in saliva cortisol in dogs. Res. Vet. Sci. 75, 157–161. doi: 10.1016/S0034-5288(03)00080-88

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konok, V., Kosztolányi, A., Rainer, W., Mutschler, B., Halsband, U., and Miklósi, Á (2015a). Influence of Owners’ attachment style and personality on their dogs’ (Canis familiaris) separation-related disorder. PLoS One 10:e0118375. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konok, V., Nagy, K., and Miklósi, Á (2015b). How do humans represent the emotions of dogs? The resemblance between the human representation of the canine and the human affective space. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 162, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostarczyk, E., and Fonberg, E. (1982). Characteristics of the heart rate in relation to the palatability of food in dogs. Appetite 3, 321–328. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80049-80044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köster, L. S., Sithole, F., Gilbert, G. E., and Artemiou, E. (2019). The potential beneficial effect of classical music on heart rate variability in dogs used in veterinary training. J. Vet. Behav. 30, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kowalik, S., Janczarek, I., Kędzierski, W., Stachurska, A., and Wilk, I. (2017). The effect of relaxing massage on heart rate and heart rate variability in purebred Arabian racehorses. Anim. Sci. J. 88, 669–677. doi: 10.1111/asj.12671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krach, S., Paulus, F. M., Bodden, M., and Kircher, T. (2010). The rewarding nature of social interactions. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:22. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremer, L., Klein Holkenborg, S. E. J., Reimert, I., Bolhuis, J. E., and Webb, L. E. (2020). The nuts and bolts of animal emotion. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 273–286. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M. L., and Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhne, F., Hößler, J. C., and Struwe, R. (2014a). Behavioral and cardiac responses by dogs to physical human–dog contact. J. Vet. Behav. 9, 93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhne, F., Hößler, J. C., and Struwe, R. (2014b). Emotions in dogs being petted by a familiar or unfamiliar person: validating behavioural indicators of emotional states using heart rate variability. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 161, 113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.09.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuoppa, P., Pulkkinen, K., Tarvainen, M. P., Lankinen, M., Lapveteläinen, A., Sinikallio, S., et al. (2016). Psychophysiological responses to positive and negative food and nonfood visual stimuli. J.Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 9, 78–88. doi: 10.1037/npe0000053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuraoka, K., and Nakamura, K. (2011). The use of nasal skin temperature measurements in studying emotion in macaque monkeys. Physiol. Behav. 102, 347–355. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, J. C. L., Evans, P. D., Ng, S. H., Chong, A. M. L., Siu, O. T., Chan, C. L. W., et al. (2005). Optimism, positive affectivity, and salivary cortisol. Br. J. Health Psychol. 10, 467–484. doi: 10.1348/135910705X26083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langford, D. J., Bailey, A. L., Chanda, M. L., Clarke, S. E., Drummond, T. E., Echols, S., et al. (2010). Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat Methods 7, 447–449. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, A. B., Vigors, B., and Sandøe, P. (2019). What is so positive about positive animal welfare?—A critical review of the literature. Animals 9:783. doi: 10.3390/ani9100783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (1998). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional Brain. Neuron 73, 653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C. K., Yoo, S. K., Park, K., Kim, N., Jeong, K., and Lee, B. (2005). “Using neural network to recognize human emotions from heart rate variability and skin resistance,” in Proceedings of the 2005 IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology 27th Annual Conference (Shanghai: IEEE), 5523–5525. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2005.1615734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leliveld, L. M. C., Langbein, J., and Puppe, B. (2013). The emergence of emotional lateralization: evidence in non-human vertebrates and implications for farm animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 145, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lensen, R. C. M. M., Betremieux, C., Bavegems, V., Sys, S. U., Moons, C. P. H., and Diederich, C. (2017). Validity and reliability of cardiac measures during behavioural tests in pet dogs at home. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 186, 56–63. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ley, J. M., McGreevy, P., and Bennett, P. C. (2009). Inter-rater and test–retest reliability of the Monash canine personality questionnaire-revised (MCPQ-R). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 119, 85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindell, A. K. (2013). Continuities in emotion lateralization in human and non-human primates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:464. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindfors, P., and Lundberg, U. (2002). Is low cortisol release an indicator of positive health? Stress Health 18, 153–160. doi: 10.1002/smi.942

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Aragona, B. J., Young, K. A., Dietz, D. M., Kabbaj, M., Mazei-Robison, M., et al. (2010). Nucleus accumbens dopamine mediates amphetamine-induced impairment of social bonding in a monogamous rodent species. PNAS 107, 1217–1222. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911998107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loijens, L. W. S., Janssens, C. J. J. G., Schouten, W. G. P., and Wiegant, V. M. (2002). Opioid activity in behavioral and heart rate responses of tethered pigs to acute stress. Physiol. Behav. 75, 621–626. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00650-653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lykken, D. T., and Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology 8, 656–672. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1971.tb00501.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, J. J., and McCarthy, J. F. (1967). The effect of petting on a classically conditioned emotional response. Behav. Res. Ther. 5, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(67)90056-90053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, E. L., and Hare, B. (2015). Dogs hijack the human bonding pathway. Science 348, 280–281. doi: 10.1126/science.aab1200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, E. L., Gesquiere, L. R., Gee, N. R., Levy, K., Martin, W. L., and Carter, C. S. (2017a). Effects of affiliative human–animal interaction on dog salivary and plasma oxytocin and vasopressin. Front. Psychol. 8:1606. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, E. L., Gesquiere, L. R., Gee, N., Levy, K., Martin, W. L., and Carter, C. S. (2018). Validation of salivary oxytocin and vasopressin as biomarkers in domestic dogs. J. Neurosci. Methods 293, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, E. L., Gesquiere, L. R., Gruen, M. E., Sherman, B. L., Martin, W. L., and Carter, C. S. (2017b). Endogenous oxytocin, vasopressin, and aggression in domestic dogs. Front. Psychol. 8:1613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahler, S. V., Smith, K. S., and Berridge, K. C. (2007). Endocannabinoid hedonic hotspot for sensory pleasure: anandamide in nucleus accumbens shell enhances ‘Liking’ of a sweet reward. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 2267–2278. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manteca, X., and Deag, J. M. (1993). Individual differences in temperament of domestic animals: a review of methodology. Anim. Welf. 2, 247–268.

Google Scholar

Mariti, C., Raspanti, E., Zilocchi, M., Carlone, B., and Gazzano, A. (2015). The assessment of dog welfare in the waiting room of a veterinary clinic. Anim. Welf. 24, 299–305. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.3.299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maros, K., Dóka, A., and Miklósi, Á (2008). Behavioural correlation of heart rate changes in family dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 329–341. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzec, M., Lamża, A., Wróbel, Z., and Dziech, A. (2016). Fast eye localization from thermal images using neural networks. Multimed. Tools Appl. 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s11042-016-4094-4097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massen, J. J. M., Behrens, F., Martin, J. S., Stocker, M., and Brosnan, S. F. (2019). A comparative approach to affect and cooperation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 370–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mccafferty, D. J. (2007). The value of infrared thermography for research on mammals: previous applications and future directions. Mamm. Rev. 37, 207–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2907.2007.00111.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mccraty, R., and Shaffer, F. (2015). Heart rate variability: new perspectives on physiological mechanisms, assessment of self-regulatory capacity, and health risk. Glob. Adv. Health Med. 4, 46–61. doi: 10.7453/gahmj.2014.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGowan, R. T. S., Bolte, C., Barnett, H. R., Perez-Camargo, G., and Martin, F. (2018). Can you spare 15min? The measurable positive impact of a 15-min petting session on shelter dog well-being. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 203, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGowan, R. T. S., Rehn, T., Norling, Y., and Keeling, L. J. (2014). Positive affect and learning: exploring the “Eureka Effect” in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 17, 577–587. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0688-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGreevy, P. D., Righetti, J., and Thomson, P. C. (2005). The reinforcing value of physical contact and the effect on canine heart rate of grooming in different anatomical areas. Anthrozoös 18, 236–244. doi: 10.2752/089279305785594045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLennan, K. M., Rebelo, C. J. B., Corke, M. J., Holmes, M. A., Leach, M. C., and Constantino-Casas, F. (2016). Development of a facial expression scale using footrot and mastitis as models of pain in sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 176, 19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McManus, C., Tanure, C. B., Peripolli, V., Seixas, L., Fischer, V., Gabbi, A. M., et al. (2016). Infrared thermography in animal production: an overview. Comput. Electron. Agric 123, 10–16. doi: 10.1016/j.compag.2016.01.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellor, D., and Beausoleil, N. (2015). Extending the “Five Domains” model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 24, 241–253. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.3.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mench, J. A. (1998). Thirty years after Brambell: whither animal welfare science? J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 1, 91–102. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0102_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendl, M., Burman, O. H. P., and Paul, E. S. (2010). An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2895–2904. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendl, M., Burman, O. H. P., Parker, R. M. A., and Paul, E. S. (2009). Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 118, 161–181. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012). Dogs’ social referencing towards owners and strangers. PLoS One 7:e47653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, P., and Kaliouby, R. E. (2003). “Real time facial expression recognition in video using support vector machines,” in Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (ICMI). (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE).

Google Scholar

Micheletta, J., Whitehouse, J., Parr, L. A., and Waller, B. M. (2015). Facial expression recognition in crested macaques (Macaca nigra). Anim. Cogn. 18, 985–990. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0867-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minero, M., Dalla Costa, E., Dai, F., Canali, E., Barbieri, S., Zanella, A., et al. (2018). Using qualitative behaviour assessment (QBA) to explore the emotional state of horses and its association with human-animal relationship. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 204, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minero, M., Dalla Costa, E., Dai, F., Murray, L. A. M., Canali, E., and Wemelsfelder, F. (2016). Use of qualitative behaviour assessment as an indicator of welfare in donkeys. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 174, 147–153. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitsui, S., Yamamoto, M., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., Kikusui, T., Ohtani, N., et al. (2011). Urinary oxytocin as a noninvasive biomarker of positive emotion in dogs. Horm. Behav. 60, 239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molnár, C., Kaplan, F., Roy, P., Pachet, F., Pongrácz, P., Dóka, A., et al. (2008). Classification of dog barks: a machine learning approach. Anim. Cogn. 11, 389–400. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0129-129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, C. A., Schmitt, K., Barber, A. L. A., and Huber, L. (2015). Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Biol. 25, 601–605. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasawa, M., Kawai, E., Mogi, K., and Kikusui, T. (2013). Dogs show left facial lateralization upon reunion with their owners. Behav. Processes 98, 112–116. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., et al. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348, 333–336. doi: 10.1126/science.1261022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasawa, M., Murai, K., Mogi, K., and Kikusui, T. (2011). Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions. Anim. Cogn. 14, 525–533. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0386-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakahashi, W., and Ohtsuki, H. (2018). Evolution of emotional contagion in group-living animals. J. Theor. Biol. 440, 12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2017.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakayama, K., Goto, S., Kuraoka, K., and Nakamura, K. (2005). Decrease in nasal temperature of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in negative emotional state. Physiol. Behav 84, 783–790. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nater, U. M., and Rohleder, N. (2009). Salivary alpha-amylase as a noninvasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 486–496. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newquist, G., and Gardner, R. A. (2015). Reconsidering food reward, brain stimulation, and dopamine: incentives act forward. Am. J. Psychol. 128, 431. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.4.0431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, Z. Y., Pierce, B. J., Otto, C. M., Buechner-Maxwell, V. A., Siracusa, C., and Werre, S. R. (2014). The effect of dog–human interaction on cortisol and behavior in registered animal-assisted activity dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 159, 69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norouzzadeh, M. S., Nguyen, A., Kosmala, M., Swanson, A., Palmer, M. S., Packer, C., et al. (2018). Automatically identifying, counting, and describing wild animals in camera-trap images with deep learning. PNAS 115, E5716–E5725. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1719367115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odendaal, J. S. J., and Meintjes, R. A. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet. J. 165, 296–301. doi: 10.1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogi, A., Mariti, C., Baragli, P., Sergi, V., and Gazzano, A. (2020). Effects of stroking on salivary oxytocin and cortisol in guide dogs: preliminary results. Animals 10:708. doi: 10.3390/ani10040708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okon-Singer, H., Hendler, T., Pessoa, L., and Shackman, A. J. (2015). The neurobiology of emotion–cognition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for future research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:58. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliva, J. L., Rault, J.-L., Appleton, B., and Lill, A. (2015). Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task. Anim. Cogn. 18, 767–775. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0843-847

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palagi, E., Nicotra, V., and Cordoni, G. (2015). Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, 150505. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panasiti, M. S., Ponsi, G., Monachesi, B., Lorenzini, L., Panasiti, V., and Aglioti, S. M. (2019). Cognitive load and emotional processing in psoriasis: a thermal imaging study. Exp. Brain Res. 237, 211–222. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5416-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1982). The pleasure in brain substrates of foraging. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 71–72. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00010566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 30–80. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: do animals have affective lives? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1791–1804. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Burrows, A. M., Gothard, K. M., and Vick, S. J. (2010). Brief communication: MaqFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 143, 625–630. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Part, C. E., Kiddie, J. L., Hayes, W. A., Mills, D. S., Neville, R. F., Morton, D. B., et al. (2014). Physiological, physical and behavioural changes in dogs (Canis familiaris) when kennelled: testing the validity of stress parameters. Physiol. Behav 133, 260–271. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partala, T., and Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. Int. J Hum. Comput. Stud. 59, 185–198. doi: 10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00017-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, F., Wemelsfelder, F., and Ward, S. J. (2019). Using qualitative behaviour assessment to investigate human-animal relationships in zoo-housed giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis). Animals 9:381. doi: 10.3390/ani9060381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, E. S., Harding, E. J., and Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 469–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, E. S., Sher, S., Tamietto, M., Winkielman, P., and Mendl, M. T. (2020). Towards a comparative science of emotion: affect and consciousness in humans and animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108, 749–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlidis, I., Eberhardt, N. L., and Levine, J. A. (2002). Seeing through the face of deception. Nature 415, 35–35. doi: 10.1038/415035a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlidis, I., Levine, J., and Baukol, P. (2000). “Thermal imaging for anxiety detection,” in Proceedings IEEE Workshop on Computer Vision Beyond the Visible Spectrum: Methods and Applications (Cat. No.PR00640) (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE) 104–109. doi: 10.1109/CVBVS.2000.855255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peciña, S., and Berridge, K. C. (2005). Hedonic hot spot in nucleus accumbens shell: where do μ-opioids cause increased hedonic impact of sweetness? J. Neurosci. 25, 11777–11786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2329-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, C.-K., Havlin, S., Stanley, H. E., and Goldberger, A. L. (1995). Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. Chaos 5, 82–87. doi: 10.1063/1.166141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, T. D., Aldarondo, D. E., Willmore, L., Kislin, M., Wang, S. S.-H., Murthy, M., et al. (2019). Fast animal pose estimation using deep neural networks. Nat. Methods 16, 117–125. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0234-235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philippot, P., Chapelle, G., and Blairy, S. (2002). Respiratory feedback in the generation of emotion. Cogn. Emot. 16, 605–627. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pincus, S. M., Gladstone, I. M., and Ehrenkranz, R. A. (1991). A regularity statistic for medical data analysis. J. Clin. Monitor. Comput. 7, 335–345. doi: 10.1007/BF01619355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirrone, F., Pierantoni, L., Bossetti, A., Uccheddu, S., and Albertini, M. (2019). Salivary vasopressin as a potential non–invasive biomarker of anxiety in dogs diagnosed with separation–related problems. Animals 9:1033. doi: 10.3390/ani9121033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollina, D. A., Dollins, A. B., Senter, S. M., Brown, T. E., Pavlidis, I., Levine, J. A., et al. (2006). facial skin surface temperature changes during a “Concealed Information” test. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 34, 1182–1189. doi: 10.1007/s10439-006-9143-9143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pongrácz, P., Lenkei, R., Marx, A., and Faragó, T. (2017). Should I whine or should I bark? Qualitative and quantitative differences between the vocalizations of dogs with and without separation-related symptoms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 196, 61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., and Miklósi, Á (2006). Acoustic parameters of dog barks carry emotional information for humans. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 100, 228–240. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., and Miklósi, Á (2010). Barking in family dogs: an ethological approach. Vet. J. 183, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2008.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., Miklósi, A., and Csányi, V. (2005). Human listeners are able to classify dog (Canis familiaris) barks recorded in different situations. J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 136–144. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L., Edwards, K. M., Bauman, A., Guastella, A. J., Drayton, B., Stamatakis, E., et al. (2019). Canine endogenous oxytocin responses to dog-walking and affiliative human–dog interactions. Animals 9;51. doi: 10.3390/ani9020051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prato-Previde, E., Marshall-Pescini, S., and Valsecchi, P. (2008). Is your choice my choice? The owners’ effect on pet dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) performance in a food choice task. Anim. Cogn. 11, 167–174. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0102-107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pressman, S. D., and Black, L. L. (2012). “Positive emotions and immunity,” in The Oxford Handbook of Psychoneuroimmunology, ed. S. C. Segerstrom (New York: Oxford University Press), 92–104. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195394399.013.0006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1–20. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X02000018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proctor, H. S., and Carder, G. (2015). Nasal temperatures in dairy cows are influenced by positive emotional state. Physiol. Behav 138, 340–344. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proctor, H., and Carder, G. (2016). Can changes in nasal temperature be used as an indicator of emotional state in cows? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 184, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quaranta, A., Siniscalchi, M., and Vallortigara, G. (2007). Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 17, R199–R201. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quesnel, D., DiPaola, S., and Riecke, B. E. (2018). “Deep learning for classification of peak emotions within virtual reality systems,” in Proceedings of the International SERIES on Information Systems and Management in Creative eMedia (CreMedia: San Diego, CA), 6–11.

Google Scholar

Racca, A., Amadei, E., Ligout, S., Guo, K., Meints, K., and Mills, D. (2010). Discrimination of human and dog faces and inversion responses in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 13, 525–533. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0303-303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racca, A., Guo, K., Meints, K., and Mills, D. S. (2012). Reading faces: differential lateral gaze bias in processing canine and human facial expressions in dogs and 4-Year-old children. PLoS One 7:e36076. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rault, J.-L., van den Munkhof, M., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2017). Oxytocin as an Indicator of psychological and social well-being in domesticated animals: a critical review. Front. Psychol. 8:1521. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayment, D. J., Peters, R. A., Marston, L. C., and Groef, B. D. (2016). Investigating canine personality structure using owner questionnaires measuring pet dog behaviour and personality. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 180, 100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redaelli, V., Luzi, F., Mazzola, S., Bariffi, G. D., Zappaterra, M., Nanni Costa, L., et al. (2019). The use of infrared thermography (irt) as stress indicator in horses trained for endurance: a pilot study. Animals 9:84. doi: 10.3390/ani9030084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reefmann, N., Bütikofer Kaszàs, F., Wechsler, B., and Gygax, L. (2009a). Physiological expression of emotional reactions in sheep. Physiol. Behav. 98, 235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reefmann, N., Wechsler, B., and Gygax, L. (2009b). Behavioural and physiological assessment of positive and negative emotion in sheep. Animal Behaviour 78, 651–659. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehn, T., and Keeling, L. J. (2011). The effect of time left alone at home on dog welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 129, 129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehn, T., Beetz, A., and Keeling, L. J. (2017). Links between an Owner’s adult attachment style and the support-seeking behavior of their dog. Front. Psychol. 8:2059. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehn, T., Handlin, L., Uvnäs-Moberg, K., and Keeling, L. J. (2014). Dogs’ endocrine and behavioural responses at reunion are affected by how the human initiates contact. Physiol. Behav. 124, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richman, J. S., and Moorman, J. R. (2000). Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 278, H2039–H2049. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.6.H2039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riemer, S., Assis, L., Pike, T. W., and Mills, D. S. (2016). Dynamic changes in ear temperature in relation to separation distress in dogs. Physiol. Behav. 167, 86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riemer, S., Ellis, S. L. H., Thompson, H., and Burman, O. H. P. (2018). Reinforcer effectiveness in dogs—The influence of quantity and quality. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 206, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.05.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Righetti, D. J. (2005). “Barking problems solved: examples of successful management of barking problems in the urban environment,” in Proceedings of the National Urban Animal Management Conference, Australian Istitute of Animal Management Inc., Canberra, 125–126.

Google Scholar

Rigterink, A., Moore, G. E., and Ogata, N. (2018). Pilot study evaluating surface temperature in dogs with or without fear-based aggression. J. Vet. Behav. 28, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S., Sandstrom, S. M., Denenberg, V. H., and Palmiter, R. D. (2005). Distinguishing whether dopamine regulates liking, wanting, and/or learning about rewards. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 5–15. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, S., Boleij, H., Nordquist, R. E., and van der Staay, F. J. (2016). Making decisions under ambiguity: judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:2059. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (2010). Relevance of brain and behavioural lateralization to animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 127, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J., Vallortigara, G., and Andrew, R. J. (2013). Divided brains: The biology and Behaviour of brain Asymmetries. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511793899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2000). On The brain and emotion. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 219–228. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00512424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2005). The Functions of Emotion: Reward, Punishment, and Emotion in Brain Design. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Rømer Thomsen, K., Whybrow, P. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Reconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:49. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero, M. L., and Butler, L. K. (2007). Endocrinology of stress. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 20, 89–95.

Google Scholar

Romero, T., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., Hasegawa, T., and Kikusui, T. (2014). Oxytocin promotes social bonding in dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 9085–9090. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322868111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rooney, N. J., Gaines, S. A., and Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2007). Behavioural and glucocorticoid responses of dogs (Canis familiaris) to kennelling: investigating mitigation of stress by prior habituation. Physiol. Behav. 92, 847–854. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royet, J.-P. (2004). Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chem. Senses 29, 731–745. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 110, 145–172. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A., and Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 805–819. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, K. M. D., Donald, R. D., Lawrence, A. B., and Wemelsfelder, F. (2012). Qualitative behavioural assessment of emotionality in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 139, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamon, A., Száraz, J., and Miklósi, Á, and Gácsi, M. (2020). Movement and vocal intonation together evoke social referencing in companion dogs when confronted with a suspicious stranger. Anim. Cogn. 23, 913–924. doi: 10.1007/s10071-020-01401-1403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, D., and Scherer, K. (2014). Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences. Oxford: OUP Oxford.

Google Scholar

Savalli, C., Albuquerque, N., Vasconcellos, A. S., Ramos, D., de Mello, F. T., and Mills, D. S. (2019). Assessment of emotional predisposition in dogs using PANAS (Positive and Negative Activation Scale) and associated relationships in a sample of dogs from Brazil. Sci. Rep. 9:18386. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54645-54646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarapicchia, V., Brown, C., Mayo, C., and Gawryluk, J. R. (2017). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and functional near-infrared spectroscopy: insights from combined recording studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaebs, F. S., Marshall-Pescini, S., Range, F., and Deschner, T. (2019). Analytical validation of an Enzyme Immunoassay for the measurement of urinary oxytocin in dogs and wolves. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 281, 73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2019.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmied, C., Boivin, X., Scala, S., and Waiblinger, S. (2010). Effect of previous stroking on reactions to a veterinary procedure: behaviour and heart rate of dairy cows. Interact. Stud. 11, 467–481. doi: 10.1075/is.11.3.08sch

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmied, C., Waiblinger, S., Scharl, T., Leisch, F., and Boivin, X. (2008). Stroking of different body regions by a human: effects on behaviour and heart rate of dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöberl, I., Beetz, A., Solomon, J., Wedl, M., Gee, N., and Kotrschal, K. (2016). Social factors influencing cortisol modulation in dogs during a strange situation procedure. J. Vet. Behav. 11, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiol. Rev. 95, 853–951. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00023.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., and Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shami, M., and Verhelst, W. (2007). An evaluation of the robustness of existing supervised machine learning approaches to the classification of emotions in speech. Speech Commun. 49, 201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2007.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheppard, G., and Mills, D. S. (2002). The development of a psychometric scale for the evaluation of the emotional predispositions of pet dogs. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 15, 201–222.

Google Scholar

Sherrington, C. (1907). The integrative action of the nervous system. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 34, 801–802.

Google Scholar

Shi, Y., Nguyen, M. H., Blitz, P., French, B., Fisk, S., De la Torre, F., et al. (2010). “Personalized stress detection from physiological measurements,” in International Symposium on Quality of Life Technology, Las Vegas: USA, 28–29.

Google Scholar

Shiverdecker, M. D., Schiml, P. A., and Hennessy, M. B. (2013). Human interaction moderates plasma cortisol and behavioral responses of dogs to shelter housing. Physiol. Behav. 109, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinha, R., Lovallo, W. R., and Parsons, O. A. (1992). Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosom. Med. 54, 422–435. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199207000-199207005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., d’Ingeo, S., Fornelli, S., and Quaranta, A. (2016a). Relationship between visuospatial attention and paw preference in dogs. Sci. Rep. 6:31682. doi: 10.1038/srep31682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., d’Ingeo, S., and Quaranta, A. (2016b). The dog nose “KNOWS” fear: asymmetric nostril use during sniffing at canine and human emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 304, 34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., d’Ingeo, S., and Quaranta, A. (2018). Orienting asymmetries and physiological reactivity in dogs’ response to human emotional faces. Learn. Behav. 46, 574–585. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0325-322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., Laddago, S., and Quaranta, A. (2016c). Auditory lateralization of conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations in cats. Laterality 21, 215–227. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2015.1116541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., Lusito, R., Vallortigara, G., and Quaranta, A. (2013). Seeing left- or right-asymmetric tail wagging produces different emotional responses in dogs. Curr. Biol. 23, 2279–2282. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., Quaranta, A., and Rogers, L. J. (2008). Hemispheric specialization in dogs for processing different acoustic stimuli. PLoS One 3:e3349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., Sasso, R., Pepe, A. M., Dimatteo, S., Vallortigara, G., and Quaranta, A. (2011). Sniffing with the right nostril: lateralization of response to odour stimuli by dogs. Anim. Behav. 82, 399–404. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniscalchi, M., Sasso, R., Pepe, A. M., Vallortigara, G., and Quaranta, A. (2010). Dogs turn left to emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 208, 516–521. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skandakumar, S., Stodulski, G., and Hau, J. (1995). Salivary IgA: a Possible Stress Marker In Dogs. Available at: https://www.ingentaconnect.com/contentone/ufaw/aw/1995/00000004/00000004/art00007 (accessed November 25, 2019).

Google Scholar

Smith, K. S., and Berridge, K. C. (2005). The ventral pallidum and hedonic reward: neurochemical maps of sucrose “Liking” and food intake. J. Neurosci. 25, 8637–8649. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1902-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommerville, R., O’Connor, E. A., and Asher, L. (2017). Why do dogs play? Function and welfare implications of play in the domestic dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 197, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Hänninen, L., Krause, C. M., and Vainio, O. (2014). How dogs scan familiar and inverted faces: an eye movement study. Anim. Cogn. 17, 793–803. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0713-710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Kujala, M. V., Hänninen, L., Krause, C. M., and Vainio, O. (2016). Dogs evaluate threatening facial expressions by their biological validity – evidence from gazing patterns. PLoS One 11:e0143047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonkusare, S., Ahmedt-Aristizabal, D., Aburn, M. J., Nguyen, V. T., Pang, T., Frydman, S., et al. (2019). Detecting changes in facial temperature induced by a sudden auditory stimulus based on deep learning-assisted face tracking. Sci. Rep. 9:4729. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41172-41177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sotocina, S. G., Sorge, R. E., Zaloum, A., Tuttle, A. H., Martin, L. J., Wieskopf, J. S., et al. (2011). The rat grimace scale: a partially automated method for quantifying pain in the laboratory rat via facial expressions. Mol. Pain 7:55. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-7-55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, P. K., and Reddy, A. (2005). Non-linear heart rate variability and risk stratification in cardiovascular disease. Indian Pacing Electrophysiol. J 5, 210–220.

Google Scholar

Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stellar, J. E., John-Henderson, N., Anderson, C. L., Gordon, A. M., McNeil, G. D., and Keltner, D. (2015). Positive affect and markers of inflammation: discrete positive emotions predict lower levels of inflammatory cytokines. Emotion 15, 129–133. doi: 10.1037/emo0000033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Stafford, K. J., Dowling, S. K., Schaefer, A. L., and Webster, J. R. (2008). Eye temperature and heart rate variability of calves disbudded with or without local anaesthetic. Physiol. Behav. 93, 789–797. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Stratton, R. B., Beausoleil, N. J., Stafford, K. J., Worth, G. M., and Waran, N. K. (2011). Assessment of positive emotions in horses: implications for welfare and performance. J. Vet. Behav. 5:296. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2011.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Webster, J. R., Verkerk, G. A., Schaefer, A. L., Colyn, J. J., and Stafford, K. J. (2007). Non-invasive measurement of stress in dairy cows using infrared thermography. Physiol. Behav. 92, 520–525. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Webster, J., Schaefer, A., Cook, N., and Scott, S. (2005). Infrared thermography as a noninvasive tool to study animal welfare. Anim. Welf. 14, 319–325.

Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Wilson, M. T., Schaefer, A. L., Huddart, F., and Sutherland, M. A. (2017). The use of infrared thermography and accelerometers for remote monitoring of dairy cow health and welfare. J. Dairy Sci. 100, 3893–3901. doi: 10.3168/jds.2016-12055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stockman, C. A., Collins, T., Barnes, A. L., Miller, D., Wickham, S. L., Beatty, D. T., et al. (2011). Qualitative behavioural assessment and quantitative physiological measurement of cattle naïve and habituated to road transport. Anim. Prod. Sci. 51, 240–249. doi: 10.1071/AN10122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stracke, J., Bert, B., Fink, H., and Böhner, J. (2011). Assessment of stress in laboratory beagle dogs constrained by a pavlov sling. ALTEX 28, 317–325. doi: 10.14573/altex.2011.4.317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sümegi, Z., Oláh, K., and Topál, J. (2014). Emotional contagion in dogs as measured by change in cognitive task performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 160, 106–115. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundman, A.-S., Poucke, E. V., Holm, A.-C. S., Faresjö, Å, Theodorsson, E., Jensen, P., et al. (2019). Long-term stress levels are synchronized in dogs and their owners. Sci. Rep. 9:7391. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43851-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, H. (2011). Animal Emotion Display System and Method. Available at: https://patents.google.com/patent/US20110018717A1/en (accessed September 27, 2019).

Google Scholar

Tamura, M., Hoshi, Y., and Okada, F. (1997). Localized near-infrared spectroscopy and functional optical imaging of brain activity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, Biol. Sci. 352, 737–742. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Telkanranta, H., Paul, E., and Mendl, M. (2018). Measuring Animal Emotions with Infrared Thermography: How to Realise the Potential and Avoid the Pitfalls. Available at: https://researchportal.helsinki.fi/en/publications/measuring-animal-emotions-with-infrared-thermography-how-to-reali (accessed October 22, 2019).

Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Yamamoto, S. S., and Brosschot, J. F. (2010). The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Int. J. Cardiol. 141, 122–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thielke, L. E., and Udell, M. A. R. (2017). The role of oxytocin in relationships between dogs and humans and potential applications for the treatment of separation anxiety in dogs. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 92, 378–388. doi: 10.1111/brv.12235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tod, E., Brander, D., and Waran, N. (2005). Efficacy of dog appeasing pheromone in reducing stress and fear related behaviour in shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 93, 295–308. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect Imagery Consciousness: Volume I: The Positive Affects. Berlin: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Topál, J., Miklósi, Á, Csányi, V., and Dóka, A. (1998). Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test. J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 219–229. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travain, T., Colombo, E. S., Grandi, L. C., Heinzl, E., Pelosi, A., Prato Previde, E., et al. (2016). How good is this food? A study on dogs’ emotional responses to a potentially pleasant event using infrared thermography. Physiol. Behav. 159, 80–87. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travain, T., Colombo, E. S., Heinzl, E., Bellucci, D., Prato Previde, E., and Valsecchi, P. (2015). Hot dogs: thermography in the assessment of stress in dogs (Canis familiaris)—A pilot study. J. Vet. Behav. 10, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulppo, M. P., Makikallio, T. H., Takala, T. E., Seppanen, T., and Huikuri, H. V. (1996). Quantitative beat-to-beat analysis of heart rate dynamics during exercise. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 271, H244–H252. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1996.271.1.H244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turcsán, B., Szánthó, F., and Miklósi, Á, and Kubinyi, E. (2015). Fetching what the owner prefers? Dogs recognize disgust and happiness in human behaviour. Anim. Cogn. 18, 83–94. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0779-773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turcsán, B., Wallis, L., Virányi, Z., Range, F., Müller, C. A., Huber, L., et al. (2018). Personality traits in companion dogs—Results from the VIDOPET. PLoS One 13:e0195448. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., and Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 397–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn2647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uvnäs−Moberg, K., Stock, S., Eriksson, M., Lindén, A., Einarsson, S., and Kunavongkrit, A. (1985). Plasma levels of oxytocin increase in response to suckling and feeding in dogs and sows. Acta Physiol. Scand. 124, 391–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1985.tb07674.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valera, M., Bartolomé, E., Sánchez, M. J., Molina, A., Cook, N., and Schaefer, A. (2012). Changes in eye temperature and stress assessment in horses during show jumping competitions. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 32, 827–830. doi: 10.1016/j.jevs.2012.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valletta, J. J., Torney, C., Kings, M., Thornton, A., and Madden, J. (2017). Applications of machine learning in animal behaviour studies. Anim. Behav. 124, 203–220. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ravenswaaij-Arts, C. M. A. (1993). Heart rate variability. Ann. Intern. Med. 118, 436–447. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-118-6-199303150-199303158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veenema, A. H., Beiderbeck, D. I., Lukas, M., and Neumann, I. D. (2010). Distinct correlations of vasopressin release within the lateral septum and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis with the display of intermale aggression. Horm. Behav. 58, 273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vick, S.-J., Waller, B. M., Parr, L. A., Pasqualini, M. C. S., and Bard, K. A. (2007). A Cross-species comparison of facial morphology and movement in humans and chimpanzees using the facial action coding system (FACS). J. Nonverbal. Behav. 31, 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s10919-006-0017-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, I. C., and Michell, A. R. (1992). Comparison of cortisol concentrations in saliva and plasma of dogs. Res. Vet. Sci. 53, 342–345. doi: 10.1016/0034-5288(92)90137-Q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voigt, J.-P., and Fink, H. (2015). Serotonin controlling feeding and satiety. Behav. Brain Res. 277, 14–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollmer, M., and Möllmann, K.-P. (2017). Infrared Thermal Imaging: Fundamentals, Research and Applications.Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

von Borell, E., Langbein, J., Després, G., Hansen, S., Leterrier, C., Marchant-Forde, J., et al. (2007). Heart rate variability as a measure of autonomic regulation of cardiac activity for assessing stress and welfare in farm animals — A review. Physiol. Behav. 92, 293–316. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, J., Dale, A., Waran, N., Clarke, N., Farnworth, M., and Wemelsfelder, F. (2010). The assessment of emotional expression in dogs using a free choice profiling methodology. Anim. Welf. 19, 75–84.

Google Scholar

Walker, J. K., Dale, A. R., D’Eath, R. B., and Wemelsfelder, F. (2016). Qualitative behaviour assessment of dogs in the shelter and home environment and relationship with quantitative behaviour assessment and physiological responses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 184, 97–108. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, B. M., Lembeck, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Burrows, A. M., and Liebal, K. (2012). GibbonFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. Int. J. Primatol. 33, 809–821. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9611-9616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, B., Caeiro, C. C., Peirce, K., Burrows, A., and Kaminski, J. (2013). DogFACS: the dog facial action coding system. Available at: http://dogfacs.com/manual.html (accessed October 22, 2019).

Google Scholar

Waller, B. M., Peirce, K., Caeiro, C. C., Scheider, L., Burrows, A. M., McCune, S., et al. (2013). Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. PLoS One 8:e82686. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallez, C., and Vauclair, J. (2011). Right hemisphere dominance for emotion processing in baboons. Brain Cogn. 75, 164–169. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G. (2019). Machine learning for inferring animal behavior from location and movement data. Ecol. Inform. 49, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoinf.2018.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Marti, D. W., and Anderson, R. E. (2019). Development and validation of a simple LC-MS method for the quantification of oxytocin in dog saliva. Molecules 24:3079. doi: 10.3390/molecules24173079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., and Aragona, B. J. (2004). Neurochemical regulation of pair bonding in male prairie voles. Physiol. Behav. 83, 319–328. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wathan, J., Burrows, A. M., Waller, B. M., and McComb, K. (2015). EquiFACS: the equine facial action coding system. PLoS One 10:e0131738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, L. L., Grossman, P., Krishnan, R., and Sherwood, A. (1998). Anxiety and vagal control of heart rate. Psychosom. Med. 60, 498–502. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199807000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wedding, D., and Stalans, L. (1985). Hemispheric differences in the perception of positive and negative faces. Int. J. Neurosci. 27, 277–281. doi: 10.3109/00207458509149773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, D. L., Hepper, P. G., Milligan, A. D. S., and Barnard, S. (2017). Cognitive bias and paw preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 131, 317–325. doi: 10.1037/com0000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wemelsfelder, F. (2007). How animals communicate quality of life: the qualitative assessment of behaviour. Anim. Welf. 16, 25–31.

Google Scholar

Wemelsfelder, F., and Farish, M. (2004). Qualitative categories for the interpretation of sheep welfare: a review. Anim. Welf. 13, 261–268.

Google Scholar

Wemelsfelder, F., and Mullan, S. (2014). Applying ethological and health indicators to practical animal welfare assessment. Rev. Sci. Tech. 33, 111–120. doi: 10.20506/rst.33.1.2259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wemelsfelder, F., Hunter, E. A., Mendl, M. T., and Lawrence, A. B. (2000). The spontaneous qualitative assessment of behavioural expressions in pigs: first explorations of a novel methodology for integrative animal welfare measurement. 67, 193–215. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00093-93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerback, T., (2011). Dogs’ Behavioural Responses to Repeated Positive Events. Second Cycle, A1E. SLU, Department of Animal Environment and Health, Uppsala. Available online at: https://stud.epsilon.slu.se/3653/

Google Scholar

Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 483–494. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeates, J. W., and Main, D. C. J. (2008). Assessment of positive welfare: a review. Vet. J. 175, 293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, S. (2002). A new perspective on barking in dogs (Canis familiaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 116, 189–193. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, S., and McCowan, B. (2004). Barking in domestic dogs: context specificity and individual identification. Anim. Behav. 68, 343–355. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.07.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, Z., Zhao, M., Wang, Y., Yang, J., and Zhang, J. (2017). Recognition of emotions using multimodal physiological signals and an ensemble deep learning model. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 140, 93–110. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2016.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, H. A., and Benton, D. (2018). Heart-rate variability: a biomarker to study the influence of nutrition on physiological and psychological health? Behav Pharmacol 29, 140–151. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, L. J., and Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. Am. Psychol. 35, 151–175. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Ihme, K., and Drewitz, U. (2019). Discriminating drivers’ emotions through the dimension of power: evidence from facial infrared thermography and peripheral physiological measurements. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 63, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2019.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilcha-Mano, S., Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2011). An attachment perspective on human–pet relationships: conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment orientations. J. Res. Pers. 45, 345–357. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zupan, M., Buskas, J., Altimiras, J., and Keeling, L. J. (2016). Assessing positive emotional states in dogs using heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiol. Behav. 155, 102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: behavior, dog, multiple measurement, physiology, positive-emotion

Citation: Csoltova E and Mehinagic E (2020) Where Do We Stand in the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris) Positive-Emotion Assessment: A State-of-the-Art Review and Future Directions. Front. Psychol. 11:2131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02131

Received: 10 January 2020; Accepted: 30 July 2020;
Published: 08 September 2020.

Edited by:

David A. Leavens, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Natalia Albuquerque, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Sonja Elena Koski, University of Helsinki, Finland

Copyright © 2020 Csoltova and Mehinagic. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Erika Csoltova, ecsoltova@diana-petfood.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.