Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 23 November 2020
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

We, Them, and It: Dictator Game Offers Depend on Hierarchical Social Status, Artificial Intelligence, and Social Dominance

  • Department of Psychology I: Differential Psychology, Personality Psychology and Psychological Diagnostics, Institute of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany

We investigated the influence of social status on behavior in a modified dictator game (DG). Since the DG contains an inherent dominance gradient, we examined the relationship between dictator decisions and recipient status, which was operationalized by three social identities and an artificial intelligence (AI). Additionally, we examined the predictive value of social dominance orientation (SDO) on the behavior of dictators toward the different social and non-social hierarchical recipients. A multilevel model analysis showed that recipients with the same status as the dictator benefited the most and the artificial intelligence the least. Furthermore, SDO, regardless of social status, predicted behavior toward recipients in such a way that higher dominance was associated with lower dictator offers. In summary, participants treated other persons of higher and lower status equally, those of equal status better and, above all, an algorithm worst. The large proportion of female participants and the limited variance of SDO should be taken into account with regard to the results of individual differences in SDO.

Introduction

In different contexts of life, people repeatedly encounter situations in which they act as either dominant or subordinate, for example, when an employee has to obey his or her superior or when parents order their child to do his or her homework. To address such situations, Sidanius and Pratto (1999) postulated the social dominance theory (SDT) and created a comprehensive model of social relations. In addition to the core aspects of SDT, hierarchy and dominance, several factors are considered important, such as race, gender, age, and religion. Pratto et al. (2006) explained that the basis for group-level hierarchies covers several levels, including individual, group, and institutional behavior.

In the present study, we focus on the influence of the social status of the recipient on the decisions of the dictator in the dictator game (DG; Forsythe et al., 1994). The classic DG is an anonymous, one-shot decision-making task in which one party, the dictator, is provided with an endowment. The dictator is asked to divide this endowment between himself and a partner, the so-called recipient. The DG thus reflects an assessment of altruistic versus selfish behavior in a hierarchical social interaction, since the recipient has no scope for action. Ball et al. (2001) showed in a study on the economic benefits of experimentally manipulated status that members of the high-status group received lucrative offers. Similarly, Glaeser et al. (2000) reported that participants traded favorably in a trust game with individuals of high status. Their study suggested a more trustworthy behavior of the trustee toward participants with high status. In a dictator game study, Rodrigues et al. (2015) were able to show that income as a possible proxy for social status, significantly influenced dictator offers. Low-income recipients received the most generous offers and high-income recipients, the lowest. Finally, Liebe and Tutic (2010) have investigated how status (operationalized with the affiliation to different types of German schools) influences the behavior of dictators and recipients. The authors demonstrated that higher status of the dictators was associated with higher offers and that on the recipient side, higher status was associated with lower offers. Since this experiment was limited to 14- to 18-year-old participants in schools, we wanted to enhance this research by a study in adult life. In addition, the school children were tested in a between-subject design, while in the present experiment, a different approach was applied based on the dictator’s behavior in a within-subject design toward different social hierarchies. Another influential factor in social behavior that should not remain unmentioned is affect. Negative affect increased dictator allocations compared to positive affect, since negative affect led to a stronger external orientation with more concerns about social norms (Forgas and Tan, 2013). Capraro (2019) pointed out in his review on the cognitive basis of social behavior that different emotions influence social decisions in different ways. The consequence is that emotionally driven decisions based on a particular emotion do not necessarily lead to rapid and default processing. Since social hierarchies include differences in status and can also have emotional connotations, social decisions in a hierarchical structure can be very complex for the individual.

As a novel contribution, we also investigated whether social dominance can be perceived exclusively as a social phenomenon. Recently, Melo et al. (2016) had subjects play several social bargaining games and instructed them that they would play against either a human or against a computer. The authors were able to show that human beings would not feel guilty if they exploited machines. De Kleijn et al. (2019) examined the influence of anthropomorphizing various opponents in the ultimatum and dictator game. They were able to show that fairness concerns in the ultimatum game were not influenced by the physical appearance of the opponent, but by individual differences in the anthropomorphization of others. Regarding altruistic behavior in the dictator game, however, an influence of physical appearance was found. A humanoid robot achieved the lowest dictator offers compared to technical-looking robots. For this reason, we have introduced a bot as one of the recipients in the design of our study. We wanted to clarify whether this opponent is treated similarly to certain hierarchy levels in a game framed by social status.

The personality trait that corresponds to the social dominance theory is the so-called social dominance orientation (SDO). Sidanius and Pratto (1999, p. 61) specified this as follows:

Social dominance orientation is defined as a very general individual difference orientation expressing the value that people place on non-egalitarian and hierarchically structured relationships among social groups. It expresses general support for the domination of certain socially structured groups over other[s]… regardless of the manner in which these groups are defined. These groups may be defined on the basis of race, sex, nationality, ethnicity, religion, social class, region, skin color, caste, lineage, tribe, minimal groups, or any other group distinction that the human mind is capable of constructing. Individuals differ in the degree to which they desire group-based inequality and dominance […].

In summary, SDO distinguishes people according to their striving for hierarchy, dominance, and inequality between groups at several levels. Consequently, SDO also reflects the extent to which individuals support the dominance of parent groups over subordinate groups. Other authors (e.g., Duckitt et al., 2002) refer SDO to the personality dimensions of “toughmindedness” and “tendermindedness.” Therefore, individuals who score high on SDO may perceive their world as a competition in which the pursuit of dominance is crucial. In comparison, individuals who score low on SDO believe in cooperation and harmony.

Concerning the competitive world view, several studies have shown that high SDO is associated with certain social attitudes, such as in-group discrimination (Pratto et al., 1994), hostile sexism and racism (Nicol and Rounding, 2013), or generally negative attitudes toward different groups (e.g., Altemeyer, 1998). Focusing on social economic bargaining, the literature review has not identified any relevant studies that have established a link between social dominance orientation and prosocial behavior. Only a recently published study could show, by analyzing questionnaire data based on a Chinese sample, that SDO is negatively related to prosocial behavior (and subjective well-being). This negative correlation was even stronger among women than among men (Yang et al., 2019).

We propose that the DG is a situation where socially dominant personality traits are activated, leading to the exploitation of the dominant position. According to the trait activation theory (Tett and Burnett, 2003), traits are expressed when a particular situation contains trait-relevant cues. Furthermore, it is still unclear in the literature whether social dominance also manifests itself in the face of artificial algorithms or is a personality trait characterized exclusively by social traits.

In summary, the aim of this study is to examine the influence of different hierarchical levels on the distribution of money in the dictator game, as well as the moderating role of the personality trait social dominance orientation. Based on the literature, we assume that artificial intelligence is treated as a non-social interaction partner in a particularly unfairly and dehumanized way and consequently receives the lowest offers regardless of social dominance. Furthermore, we assume that higher social dominance leads to lower offers for socially lower ranked individuals and to higher offers for higher ranked individuals.

Method

Ethics Statement

The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of “Ethical Guidelines, The Association of German Professional Psychologists” (“Berufsethische Richtlinien, Berufsverband Deutscher Psychologinnen und Psychologen”). All participants gave informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki before they participated in the experiment. During the experiment, a cover story was used, but participants were informed about this deception as soon as the task was over, as is common practice in psychological experiments.

Participants

The experiment was created with the online questionnaire platform SoSci Survey (Leiner, 2019) and made available to the participants on www.soscisurvey.de. Of 102 participants who started the experiment, 7 did not complete the entire task, so that the final sample for data analysis included 95 participants (91% female; Mage = 22.21, SDage = 7.09). The participants received either course credit or a monetary compensation of 5€.

Experimental Procedure

First, participants were asked to complete several questionnaires on demography and personality. These included the 16 items of the Social Dominance Orientation Scale (SDO-7; Saldarriaga et al., 2017) for assessing social dominance. For explorative purposes, we also assessed the German version of the International Personality Item Pool (Johnson, 2014). The participants were also asked to indicate whether they were studying or working with the aim of adapting the following experiment to their social situation. Subsequently, the participants played the role of the dictator in a dictator game against four different identities. The participants were asked to make five offers per identity by selecting pi-charts that represented offers of 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 € for an identity that is considered in the social hierarchy as lower, one as equal, one as higher, and finally one identity that is represented by an artificial intelligence (AI). From the perspective of a student, these identities were represented as a high school graduate (lower), a doctoral student (higher), another student (equal), or AI. From the perspective of a worker, these identities were represented as an apprentice (lower), an executive (higher), another worker (equal), or AI. The identities were presented in a within-subject design and in blocks in randomized order.

Statistical Analysis

We used R software (R Core Team, 2019) and the nlme package (Pinheiro et al., 2020), with the procedure “lme” and the psych package (Revelle, 2020), to statistically analyze our data by two-level hierarchical linear modeling. We were particularly interested in the offered amount of money (level-1 outcome variable), the opponent for whom the offer was made (level-1 predictor variable; factor with four levels: equal status, higher status, lower status, and AI), and the individual score on social dominance orientation (level-2 predictor variable; grand mean centered). We included both predictors and the cross level interaction of opponent and social dominance orientation into our model and random slopes for the factor opponent. The model fit was determined using the Akaike information criterion (AIC), as displayed in Table 1. To investigate the difference between the social and non-social opponents in detail, we performed Helmert contrasts in the mixed model, resulting in the following contrasts: human (equal, higher, and lower status) versus AI, equal status versus non-equal status (higher and lower status) and higher versus lower status. To fully examine the influence of social status, we used Bonferroni adjusted post hoc comparisons.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) for linear mixed-effects models and information loss difference.

Results

The regression coefficient relating the dictator offers to AI versus human interaction partners was negative and statistically significant (β = −2.48, p < 0.001), as shown in Table 2. The comparison of equal to unequal status was also statistically significant (β = 0.52, p < 0.001), whereas high and low status did not differ (β = 0.11, p = 0.439).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the hierarchical linear model analysis.

The main effect of social dominance was also significant (β = −0.45, p < 0.001), indicating a decrease in dictator offers with an increase in social dominance orientation. The cross−level interaction between the opponent and social dominance was not significant for all contrasts (AI vs. human: β = 0.29, p = 0.162; equal vs. high and low: β = −0.06, p = 0.603; high vs. low: β = −0.08, p = 0.648).

The post hoc comparisons of the four opponents indicated that all of the three social identities received significantly more money compared to the AI identity (all values of p < 0.011; Mequal = 4.16, Mhigher = 3.58, Mlower = 3.69; MAI = 1.32). Furthermore, opponents of both lower and higher status received less money compared to the equally framed opponent (p < 0.001). Finally, lower- and higher-framed opponents did not differ in their received money (p = 0.438), as illustrated in Figure 1. To test for the consistency of the dictator decisions toward the four opponents, we calculated an intraclass coefficient (ICC). The ICC (k = 95 raters) was based on a two-way mixed-effects model and revealed a measure ICC of 0.56. Hence, we found a moderate degree of consistency for the dictator decisions across the different recipients. As illustrated in Table 3, the social opponents correlated highly with each other (all values of r ≥ 0.742), but not with AI (all values of r ≤ 0.196). Moreover, we compared the correlation coefficients (Eid et al., 2011) between SDO and each of the four opponents. The correlation coefficients between SDO and equal status compared to SDO and AI differed significantly (p = 0.018). By trend, the coefficients between SDO and equal status compared to SDO and lower status (p = 0.061) and the coefficients between SDO and higher status compared to SDO and AI (p = 0.08) were also different. The other comparisons did not yield significance (all values of p ≥ 0.12). For exploratory reasons, we performed correlations of the subscale altruism of the International Personality Item Pool with SDO, the four identities and the dictator offers averaged across all trials (see Table 3). While dominance and altruism did not correlate significantly (r = −0.114, p = 0.273), higher scores on altruism led to higher average dictator offers (r = 0.215, p = 0.036). Especially, the identity of lower status received more money from altruists (r = 0.243, p = 0.018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effect of the different opponents on dictator offers. The error bars indicate the standard error of the mean, ∗∗∗p ≤ 0.001.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Confirmatory and exploratory correlations for the average amount of dictator offers across all trials, the offers toward the four identities, social dominance orientation (SDO), and trait altruism.

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the influence of social status and its relationship with social dominance orientation on behavior in a modified dictator game. Therefore, we let the participants play the role of the dictator against three socially hierarchical identities and one identity represented by artificial intelligence. We could show that with regard to social status, AI received less money compared to all social interaction partners, and partners of socially equal status profited the most. Moreover, a stronger social dominance orientation generally led to lower dictator offers and did not have a significant effect on the social and non-social levels.

Contrary to our hypotheses and the findings of Rodrigues et al. (2015), for example, dictators did not distribute money profitably for higher hierarchical levels and were not deficient for lower ones. A possible explanation for the differences is that in our study, status is translated with attributes that are also relevant at the educational and career level. Given that, for example, Rodrigues et al. (2015) used income as a proxy for social status, the different meanings of social status (monetary versus educational) might be the reason why we did not find the expected effects on dictators’ offers to hierarchical recipients. However, the exploratory correlation of altruism and the identity of lower status has shown that only those individuals who supposedly need more support benefit from higher dictator offers. This coincides with the findings of Rodrigues et al. (2015) and supports the validity of the paradigm presented here. The fact that in our paradigm the largest amount of money was given to recipients of the same status as the dictator is consistent with findings on ingroup favoritism, according to which in cooperative tasks members of one’s own group are treated more favorably than outgroup members or strangers (e.g., Balliet et al., 2014). In terms of social status independent of SDO, there is a perception of AI that differs from the social identities, which is manifested in the low dictator offers. However, from a rational point of view, it makes no sense at all to give any money to AI. Therefore, the mean offer of 1.32€ is surprisingly high. One explanation would be that in a student sample of psychology students, there might be a response bias, especially in relation to the effect of AI. Our finding contradicts, at least in part, the findings of Melo et al. (2016) who found that humans have no sense of guilt when they exploit machines. Several studies have already shown that there are behavioral differences in interactions with machines compared to humans in tasks requiring social decision making (e.g., Gallagher et al., 2002). At the neural level, Gallagher et al. (2002) could show that when interacting with machines compared to humans in a rock–paper–scissors game, the medial prefrontal cortex was not particularly activated, an area that is associated with mentalizing (assumptions of a counterpart’s intentions and beliefs). In order to break down these differences in the way people handle machines more precisely, future studies could take a closer look at individual differences that might shed light on supposedly irrational generous behavior toward machines.

In our sample, SDO was associated with lower monetary offers to all hierarchical levels (i.e., AI, equal, higher, and lower). In the context of this study, this would mean that individuals who consider themselves more socially dominant generally want to gain a financial advantage over other human individuals. Thus, one could speak of ubiquitous dominance, which applies regardless of the social status of the interaction partner. This result contrasts with previous findings, which have shown that higher dominance is associated with a devaluation of individuals with lower status (Umphress et al., 2008). In addition, it was found that people with a highly developed SDO prefer to surround themselves with members of groups with socially high status, i.e., they favor high social status (Umphress et al., 2007). Possible missing interaction effects could also be due to our sample size, since social dominance at least tends to interact with the contrast human versus AI. The post hoc comparison of correlation coefficients supports this trend, since above all the correlations between SDO and the social identities are substantially different from the correlation between SDO and monetary offers to AI.

Also, one might expect that there is still a difference between the constructs of social dominance and egoism/altruism. As indicated by the exploratory correlations, altruism and dominance indicate opposing dictator decisions, as dominance led to less and altruism to more generous offers. This coincides with the finding that SDO is negatively correlated with cooperation and positively correlated with the tendency to behave selfishly with resources in economic games (e.g., Halali et al., 2018). While social dominance may be linked to low altruism, the findings of Rodrigues et al. (2015) were derived with extreme group selection, while in the case of the present study, only an ad hoc sampling was done. Therefore, it is still questionable whether this sampling has led to a similar distribution of the construct to actually compare the findings.

We briefly want to discuss several limitations of the present study. First, the present results are based primarily on female participants. Previous studies suggest that women act more altruistically compared to men in the dictator game (Rand et al., 2016; Brañas-Garza et al., 2018). Thus, the fact that socially higher and lower ranked recipients receive the same amount and AI receives anything at all could also be a mere gender effect. This is also reflected in the limited variance and distribution of SDO in the present sample (M = 2.62, SD = 0.83). Furthermore, we have a rather small sample size with regard to the investigation of individual differences. As debated, for example, by Schönbrodt and Perugini (2013); Gelman and Carlin (2014), and Patil et al. (2016), too small samples are problematic when it comes to the reproducibility of empirical findings. With regard to the individual differences in SDO, both aspects are the reason why this study has the character of a pilot study, which requires a larger and more sophisticated sample for a detailed evaluation. Additionally, there are empirical findings showing that there is no general statistically significant difference between hypothetical and real rewards, although individuals with certain traits (e.g., agreeableness and extraversion) behave differently when receiving real compared to hypothetical rewards (Ben-Ner et al., 2008). Other studies indicate differences between hypothetical and real rewards based on the mechanics of the game. While the first round of a repeated ultimatum game did not show any difference, from the second round on, players offered less hypothetical money (Gillis and Hettler, 2007). When playing Andreoni’s Public Goods Game, however, the authors could not find a substantial difference in behavior between hypothetical and real rewards. Thus, future studies might also examine the influence of hypothetical versus real monetary offers on the behavior of socially dominant individuals. Finally, future studies might counterbalance the order of the SDO measure and the dictator game. Filling in the SDO questionnaire first might have made salient social hierarchy to the participants.

In summary, the presented study showed that bargaining with an artificial intelligence seems to be conceptually different from the interaction with social identities, which opens new perspectives on human–machine interactions for future studies. Social dominance orientation predicted a more selfish behavior in a socially dominant position, regardless of the social rank of the interaction partner. In line with the trait activation theory (Tett and Burnett, 2003), the DG provided the necessary trait-relevant dominance gradient to evoke dominant behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MW designed and performed the experiments. MW, JR, and MP analyzed the data. MW wrote the manuscript in consultation with JR, MP, and JH. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by the European Union through the project “Individualisierung Digital” (Fonds 823881) in the Europäischer Fonds für regionale Entwicklung (EFRE). Moreover, this publication was supported by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Würzburg.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality. Adv. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 30, 47–92. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60382-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, S., Eckel, C., Grossman, P. J., and Zame, W. (2001). Status in markets. Q. J. Econ. 116, 161–188. doi: 10.1162/003355301556374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balliet, D., Wu, J., and De Dreu, C. K. (2014). Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 140:1556. doi: 10.1037/a0037737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Ner, A., Kramer, A., and Levy, O. (2008). Economic and hypothetical dictator game experiments: Incentive effects at the individual level. J. Soc. Econ. 37, 1775–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2007.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brañas-Garza, P., Capraro, V., and Rascon-Ramirez, E. (2018). Gender differences in altruism on mechanical turk: expectations and actual behaviour. Econ. Lett. 170, 19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2018.05.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capraro, V. (2019). The Dual-Process Approach to Human Sociality: A Review. Available online at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3409146 (accessed June 24, 2019).

Google Scholar

De Kleijn, R., van Es, L., Kachergis, G., and Hommel, B. J. (2019). Anthropomorphization of artificial agents leads to fair and strategic, but not altruistic behavior. Intern. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 122, 168–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2018.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckitt, J., Wagner, C., Du Plessis, I., and Birum, I. (2002). The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing a dual process model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 75–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.75

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eid, M., Gollwitzer, M., and Schmitt, M. (2011). Statistik und Forschungsmethoden. Weinheim: Beltz.

Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P., and Tan, H. B. (2013). To give or to keep? Affective influences on selfishness and fairness in computer-mediated interactions in the dictator game and the ultimatum game. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., and Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games Econ. Behav. 6, 347–369. doi: 10.1006/game.1994.1021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, H. L., Jack, A. I., Roepstorff, A., and Frith, C. D. (2002). Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game. Neuroimage 16(3 Pt 1), 814–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Carlin, J. (2014). Beyond power calculations: assessing type S (sign) and type M (magnitude) errors. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 641–651. doi: 10.1177/1745691614551642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillis, M. T., and Hettler, P. L. (2007). Hypothetical and real incentives in the ultimatum game and Andreoni’s public goods game: an experimental study. East. Econ. J. 33, 491–510. doi: 10.1057/eej.2007.37

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaeser, E. L., Laibson, D. I., Scheinkman, J. A., and Soutter, C. L. (2000). Measuring trust. Q. J. Econ. 115, 811–846. doi: 10.1162/003355300554926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halali, E., Dorfman, A., Jun, S., and Halevy, N. (2018). More for us or more for me? Social dominance as parochial egoism. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 9, 254–262. doi: 10.1177/1948550617732819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. A. (2014). Measuring thirty facets of the five factor model with a 120-item public domain inventory: development of the IPIP-NEO-120. J. Res. Pers. 51, 78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leiner, D. (2019). SoSci Survey (Version 3.2.03-i) [Computer Software]. Available online at: http://www.soscisurvey.de (accessed January 7, 2019).

Google Scholar

Liebe, U., and Tutic, A. (2010). Status groups and altruistic behaviour in dictator games. Ration. Soc. 22, 353–380. doi: 10.1177/1043463110366232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melo, C. D., Marsella, S., and Gratch, J. (2016). People do not feel guilty about exploiting machines. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 23, 1–17. doi: 10.1145/2890495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicol, A. A., and Rounding, K. (2013). Alienation and empathy as mediators of the relation between social dominance orientation, right-wing authoritarianism and expressions of racism and sexism. Pers. Individ. Differ. 55, 294–299. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patil, P., Peng, R. D., and Leek, J. T. (2016). What should researchers expect when they replicate studies? A statistical view of replicability in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 539–544. doi: 10.1177/1745691616646366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., and Sarkar, D. R Core Team (2020). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed February 4, 2020).

Google Scholar

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., and Levin, S. (2006). Social dominance theory and the dynamics of intergroup relations: Taking stock and looking forward. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 17, 271–320. doi: 10.1080/10463280601055772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., and Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: a personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67:741. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A., Capraro, V., and Barcelo, H. (2016). Social heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. J. Exper. Psychol. Gen. 145, 389–396. doi: 10.1037/xge0000154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revelle, W. (2020). Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed February 4, 2020).

Google Scholar

Rodrigues, J., Ulrich, N., and Hewig, J. (2015). A neural signature of fairness in altruism: a game of theta? Soc. Neurosci. 10, 192–205. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.977401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saldarriaga, L., Boege, R., Rees, J., Koller, C., Carvacho, H., and Bohner, G. (2017). SDO7 Scale: Translation and Validation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the FGSP, Ulm.

Google Scholar

Schönbrodt, F. D., and Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? J. Res. Pers. 47, 609–612. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sidanius, J., and Pratto, F. (1999). Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Tett, R. P., and Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 88:500. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umphress, E. E., Simmons, A. L., Boswell, W. R., and Triana, M. D. C. (2008). Managing discrimination in selection: the influence of directives from an authority and social dominance orientation. J. Appl. Psychol. 93:982. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umphress, E. E., Smith-Crowe, K., Brief, A. P., Dietz, J., and Watkins, M. B. (2007). When birds of a feather flock together and when they do not: status composition, social dominance orientation, and organizational attractiveness. J. Appl. Psychol. 92:396. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Li, W., Sheldon, K. M., and Kou, Y. (2019). Chinese adolescents with higher social dominance orientation are less prosocial and less happy: a value-environment fit analysis. Intern. J. Psychol. 54, 325–332. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: decision-making, dictator game, personality, social dominance, social status

Citation: Weiß M, Rodrigues J, Paelecke M and Hewig J (2020) We, Them, and It: Dictator Game Offers Depend on Hierarchical Social Status, Artificial Intelligence, and Social Dominance. Front. Psychol. 11:541756. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.541756

Received: 10 March 2020; Accepted: 13 October 2020;
Published: 23 November 2020.

Edited by:

Gerald Matthews, University of Central Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Valerio Capraro, Middlesex University, United Kingdom
Janina Steinmetz, City University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Weiß, Rodrigues, Paelecke and Hewig. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Martin Weiß, martin.weiss@uni-wuerzburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.