Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 February 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language

Using Verb Extension to Gauge Children’s Verb Meaning Construals: The Case of Chinese

  • 1School of Human Environmental Sciences, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, United States
  • 2School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
  • 4Department of Linguistics and Cognitive Science, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
  • 5Department of Early Childhood Education and Art Education, School of Education, Brooklyn College, City University of New York, New York, NY, United States
  • 6Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Verb extension is a crucial gauge of the acquisition of verb meaning. In English, studies suggest that young children show conservative extension. An important test of whether an early conservative extension is a general phenomenon or a function of the input language is made possible by Chinese, a language in which verbs are more frequent and acquired earlier. This study tested whether 3-year-old Chinese children extended a group of familiar verbs that specify various ways to carry objects. Shown videos that portrayed typical, mid-typical, or atypical carrying actions (as verified by Chinese adults), children were asked to judge whether they were examples of specific Chinese carry verbs. Children’s verb extensions were mostly limited to typical exemplars, suggesting that an early conservative extension may be universal. Furthermore, extension breadth was related to the onset of verb production: verbs acquired earlier elicited more extension judgments than those acquired later.

Introduction

Verbs label categories of actions and events, thus giving verbs their economy and power (e.g., Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff, 2006). Evidence of verb extension can reveal children’s understanding of the breadth and limits of a verb’s meaning. Consider a verb like carry. In English, carry can be extended to an action regardless of changes in the agent, object (e.g., a man or woman carrying a bag or a baby), or manner (i.e., how an action is carried out; e.g., “carrying” with both arms or on the back). However, in Chinese, there are more than 20 verbs that describe various ways to carry/hold (e.g., Ma et al., 2009). This paper probes how Chinese-speaking children extend carry verbs to a range of appropriate action exemplars. The use of Chinese allows us to test whether the early conservative extension is a general phenomenon or a function of the input language.

While some studies suggest that 1-year-old children can extend their first verbs (Naigles et al., 2009; but see Tomasello and Brandt, 2009), the majority of studies show that young children are conservative in their verb extensions, reluctant to extend both familiar and novel verbs to new instances (e.g., Bates et al., 1979; Gallivan, 1988; Harris et al., 1988; Behrend, 1990; Tomasello, 1992, 2000; Forbes and Poulin-Dubois, 1997; Theakston et al., 2002; Imai et al., 2008; Maguire et al., 2008; Seston et al., 2009; Tomasello and Brandt, 2009). Although recent studies revealed successful learning and extension of novel verbs in 24‐ to 28-month-olds under experimental conditions (e.g., Yuan and Fisher, 2009; Arunachalam and Waxman, 2010, 2011; Scott and Fisher, 2012), successful learning and extension of novel nouns has been documented as occurring a full year earlier (e.g., Ballem and Plunkett, 2005; Yoshida et al., 2009). Furthermore, children were more reluctant to extend verbs to some semantic components (e.g., changes in the manner of the action) than others (e.g., object changes; Behrend, 1990; Forbes and Farrar, 1995). These findings have led to the conservative verb extension hypothesis, which states that children are more conservative in their construals of verb meaning and therefore tend to extend verbs more narrowly than adults do (Seston et al., 2009). This hypothesis is supported by previous research in English-speaking children (Bates et al., 1979; Harris et al., 1988; Tomasello, 1992) but this study examines the generalizability of the conservative extension hypothesis in Mandarin-speaking 3-year-olds.

How do children develop appropriate verb extensions? Manipulating the typicality level of exemplars enables us to explore whether children understand the appropriate range of verb extension. If they do, they should accept typical exemplars more readily than less typical ones. Furthermore, if children’s word extension is limited to typical exemplars, it may suggest that their word knowledge views details as more central than do adults. Indeed, research on the acquisition of words from other form classes shows that children initially employ a prototype framework for word meaning (Barrett, 1986, 1995). For example, young children tend to extend familiar words to prototypical exemplars, and later to less typical exemplars (e.g., Meints et al., 1999). Thus, 18-month-olds linked a familiar noun (e.g., bird) to a less typical exemplar (e.g., ostrich), but 12-month-olds did not (Meints et al., 1999; see also Poulin-Dubois and Sissons, 2002). Research on spatial term learning also shows similar prototype effects in both adults and children (Erreich and Valian, 1979; Hayward and Tarr, 1995; Logan and Sadler, 1996; Meints et al., 2002). For example, 15-month-olds associated spatial terms (e.g., under) with typical (e.g., under the center of the table) rather than atypical exemplars (e.g., under the edge of the table; Meints et al., 2002).

This typicality effect has also been reported in studies on verb understanding with adults and older children. Using adult participants, Ferretti et al. (2001; Experiments 1 and 2) found that verbs presented in isolation primed typical agents (arresting-cop), patients (arresting-criminal), and instruments (stirred-spoon) rather than atypical verbal arguments. Additionally, Meints et al. (2008) showed that the 24-month-olds English-reared toddlers only accepted typical action-patient pairings (e.g., eating-apple) while 3-year-olds and adults also accepted atypical pairings (e.g., eating-houseplants). Relatedly, it was not until 26 months that English-reared toddlers accepted appropriate but atypical manner variations (e.g., kicking a ball with the heel) as instances of familiar verbs (Forbes and Poulin-Dubois, 1997).

This does not mean, however, that the 26-month-olds have an appropriate understanding of all verb extensions, since knowledge of verbs continues to develop well beyond the preschool years (Seston et al., 2009). Furthermore, toddlers are most conservative when the manner of the action varied (e.g., Behrend, 1990). The factors influencing children’s verb extension still remain to be elucidated. It is unclear whether conservative extension – especially along the manner dimension – is a general phenomenon of verb learning or a function of the input language. A study of Chinese children’s verb extension may help to address this question.

Chinese differs from English in significant ways that might impact verb acquisition. For example, verbs tend to occupy the salient utterance-final position more frequently in Chinese infant-directed speech (Tardif et al., 1997), and the utterance-final position can facilitate infants’ speech segmentation (Seidl and Johnson, 2006) – a prerequisite for word learning. Furthermore, as a “pro-drop language,” Chinese allows “argument dropping” – a language use phenomenon clearly observable in Chinese infant-directed speech, as well (Tardif et al., 1997; Ma et al., 2019). Thus, the subject, object, or both can be omitted from a sentence and inferred from the context, thereby increasing the frequency and salience of verbs in speech (Tardif et al., 1997). Higher frequency is related to better word comprehension in children (e.g., Rice et al., 1994; Naigles et al., 2009). Additionally, Chinese tends to be pragmatically biased towards verb usage in infant-directed speech (Tardif et al., 1997). For example, in questioning and answering, while English allows nouns as answers, Chinese requires verbs. Thus, to answer the question “Have you eaten your lunch?” one says in Chinese, “Have eaten.” Furthermore, some of the verbs acquired by children-speaking children early in life refer to highly specific meanings. For example, Chinese has more than 20 verbs for “carry/hold,” each labeling a specific way of carrying/holding (Ma et al., 2009). For example, bēi means “to carry on the back,” bào means “to carry in one’s arms in front of the body,” duān means “to carry flat on two hands in front of the body.1” Highly specific verb meanings may facilitate the process of abstracting the commonalities among action exemplars, thus narrowing the semantic scope of a verb. All these factors may enhance early verb acquisition in Chinese children.

Although the majority of research on children’s learning of novel verb showed comparable performance between English‐ and Mandarin-speaking children ranging from 24 months to 5 years of age (Imai et al., 2008; Leddon et al., 2011; read Chan et al., 2011 for counterarguments), the verb-friendliness of Mandarin could facilitate Mandarin-speaking children’s learning and extension of familiar verbs. Indeed, using the Chinese MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (Chinese CDI; Tardif et al., 2008), a parental checklist of young children’s vocabulary, researchers find that Chinese children produce an average of 49 verbs by 19 months. In comparison, English-reared children do not produce this many verbs until 24 months (Fenson et al., 1994). A verb advantage has also been observed in Chinese-speaking children’s spontaneous speech (Tardif et al., 1999).

Do Chinese children know the appropriate semantic scope of familiar verbs as indexed by their understanding of verb extensions? Little research has examined this question. In English, an earlier age of acquisition (AoA) of a word is related to better word processing in a variety of tasks (e.g., Morrison and Ellis, 1995, 2000; Barry et al., 1997, 2001), including visual and auditory lexical decisions (e.g., Turner et al., 1998; Gerhand and Barry, 1999; Morrison and Ellis, 2000). Therefore, an earlier AoA of Chinese verbs presumably offers children more opportunities to assess the range of appropriate verb extensions.

Chinese carry/hold verbs provide multiple advantages for assessing children’s verb extension, not the least among them is that carrying and holding are frequent and familiar events in children’s lives and among the earliest words in Chinese children’s receptive and productive vocabularies (Hao et al., 2008; Tardif et al., 2008). Furthermore, from a methodological standpoint, carry and hold actions are perceptually visible and can be shown dynamically – an important consideration when testing young children. Additionally, compared with verbs with broader meanings, children may be more likely to have a complete understanding of verbs with highly specific meanings and clearly defined semantic boundaries (Maguire et al., 2008; Seston et al., 2009). Thus, this study tests the conservative verb extension hypothesis by investigating whether Chinese children appropriately extend familiar, highly specific, and frequently used carry verbs.

Imai and colleagues examined verb production in Chinese, using carry/hold verbs, for many of the same reasons (Saji et al., 2011). They asked Chinese-speaking 3-, 5-, and 7-year-olds and adults to describe a range of events by using 13 Chinese carry/hold verbs. Results showed that learning the meaning of a verb induced reorganization of the meaning of related verbs. More surprisingly, the 3-year-olds’ verb uses only overlapped with adults’ 17% of the time, suggesting that young children’s verb knowledge significantly differed from adults’. Furthermore, children even seemed to rely on different elements of the verbs’ meaning than adults. For example, the 3-year-olds gave more weight to the salience and the kind of objects used with the carry/hold verbs than adults did, while placing less weight on the manner in selecting the most appropriate verb for a carrying/holding event.

Saji et al. (2011), however, might have underestimated children’s verb knowledge and sensitivity. Saji et al. (2011) paired a different object with each verb, perhaps biasing children’s attention towards the object (Kersten and Smith, 2002). Additionally, language production requires rapid retrieval and phonological encoding, making the task more difficult for young children than a comprehension or judgment task (e.g., Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff, 1996; Golinkoff et al., 2013). When children do not produce a verb, it cannot be concluded that they do not know it. Furthermore, these factors may have the strongest influence on the youngest age group (3-year-olds). Thus, it is still unclear whether 3-year-old Chinese children extend the carry/hold verbs properly.

This study probed 3-year-old Chinese children’s extensions of six Mandarin Chinese carry/hold verbs (bào: carry with both arms; bēi: carry on the back; kuà: carry with the elbow; līn: carry with bent fingers; : carry with hands; : carry with one arm). All of them refer to specific manners of carrying/holding without requiring certain types of objects. Three-year-olds were tested because they are relatively experienced verb users but their semantic understanding of verbs, especially carry/hold verbs, is still developing, making them an ideal age group to examine the factors that could affect verb meaning acquisition (Seston et al., 2009; Saji et al., 2011). Furthermore, 3-year-olds are familiar with the tested carry/hold verbs based on the Chinese CDI data, thus enabling us to examine children’s extension of familiar verbs. Finally, based on the finding that manner extension is harder than object extension (Behrend, 1990), testing this age group allow us to probe children’s developing sensitivity to manner variation.

Each of the six verbs was portrayed by an actor carrying a bag in three types of events – typical, mid-typical, and atypical – created by changing features of the action (Table 1) and confirmed by asking adults for their judgments. Two puppets each offered a sentence about a carrying event and children were asked to judge which puppet was correct in its use of a particular carry verb. The study addressed whether 3-year-olds’ acceptance of the carrying actions differ among typicality levels. If Chinese children would more readily accept typical than less typical exemplars of familiar verbs, just like their English-speaking counterparts in previous studies (e.g., Meints et al., 1999, 2002, 2008; Poulin-Dubois and Sissons, 2002), this study would support the generalizability of the conservative verb extension.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptions of the typical, mid-typical, and atypical exemplars of the six carry verbs.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Nineteen 3-year-old monolingual Mandarin-speaking children (10 males) at a university preschool (M = 40.58 months; range: 39.63–41.50) in China participated. Five additional children also participated but were excluded due to failure to comply with instructions (n = 2) or to complete the task (n = 3). Children were primarily from middle-class homes with college-educated parents. Before the experiment, parents were asked to indicate whether their children understood and produced the six target verbs. Three of the carry/hold verbs (bào, bēi, ) were produced by all children, consistent with the Chinese CDI data that more than 50% of the normed sample produced these three verbs before 17 months of age (Tardif et al., 2008). For the other three verbs whose Chinese CDI data were unavailable, mothers reported that 17 children (90%) produced , 11 children (58%) produced kuà, and 10 children (53%) produced līn. The rates of children producing each verb suggest that the six verbs fell into two groups: bào, bēi, , were each produced by more than 90% of children, all significantly higher than would be expected by chance based on separate Sign tests (p’s < 0.001), whereas kuà and līn were each produced by 58 and 53% of children, which were not different than chance (p’s > 0.64). Therefore, we divided the six verbs into early-acquired verbs (bào, bēi, , ) and late-acquired verbs (kuà, līn) based on the percentage of children who could produce them. The sample size of child participants (n = 19) was established by conducting a power analysis using G*Power, based on a medium expected effect size (f = 0.25) and a power value of 0.80, and the use of a one-sample repeated measures ANOVA containing one group and six measures (three typicality levels × two AoA sets; Faul et al., 2007). This sample size is also consistent with previous research on children’s comprehension of familiar verbs (e.g., Seston et al., 2009). In addition, thirty Chinese college students (15 males; M = 21.9 years; range: 20–24) from a local university provided typicality and linguistic judgments of the stimuli.

Stimuli

Videos of a human actor carrying a bag were created for six different Mandarin Chinese verbs. For each of the six verbs, three videos varying in the degree of typicality were created; that is, each verb had a typical, mid-typical, and atypical event exemplar. Typicality was manipulated by varying the figure’s manner of motion. This resulted in a total of 18 events, each 8 s in length. The typical events depicted the actions according to their dictionary definitions. For example, for the verb , defined as “carrying with the arm down,” the typical event depicted a human actor carrying a bag with his arm down. In the mid-typical event, the actor carried the bag with his arm up as high as his waist. In the atypical event, the actor carried the bag with his arm extending out horizontally (see Figure 1). Written informed consent was obtained from the individual for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Snapshots of the typical, mid-typical, and atypical action exemplars of (carry in one’s hand with the arm down). Participants were shown looping videos of dynamic events.

Procedure

Adults’ Linguistic and Typicality Judgments

To confirm the appropriateness of the tested carry verbs and the events’ typicality levels, all 18 events were presented to monolingual Mandarin-speaking adults in random orders on a 14-in laptop monitor. Adults were asked to indicate whether an action could be labeled by a particular carry/hold verb. If they answered yes, they were asked to rate whether the event was a good exemplar of the particular verb on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = a poor example, 7 = a great example). Adults who indicated that the action could not be labeled by a carry/hold verb were asked to write the verb they thought could label the action.

Verb Extension Task

A trust in testimony method was used (Koenig et al., 2004). Children were tested individually at their preschool by two native Mandarin speakers. One experimenter (A) manipulated two puppets (a bear and a panda), using a distinct voice for each, to elicit children’s responses: The bear had a low-pitched voice and the panda spoke with a soft, high-pitched voice. Children were told that the puppets needed help with learning some new words. Their responses were recorded by the second experimenter (B) who sat behind the child. All visual stimuli were presented to children on a 14-in laptop computer, while all auditory stimuli were presented in Mandarin child-directed speech by Experimenter A.

Familiarization Trials

An experiment started with two familiarization trials, in which static images were shown. Experimenter A showed children a picture of a car on the computer and said, “Look, this is a car.” Then the bear puppet said, “Yes, it is a car,” and the panda puppet followed with, “No, it is not a car.” Experimenter A held the two puppets still and asked children, “Which puppet is correct? Can you point to it for me?” If the child did not respond, the experimenter repeated the question once more. The next familiarization trial was conducted in the same way with a picture of an airplane.

The same experimental method was used throughout the entire experimental session. Each puppet was “correct” once and the order of the two trials was counterbalanced. However, children received no feedback on their choices and therefore did not know which puppet the experimenter thought was “correct.” All children succeeded in the two familiarization trials.

Training Trials

Next, children participated in four training trials (8 s each) with animated and familiar actions (i.e., flying, swimming, drinking, and sweeping) presented in counterbalanced order. In one training trial, for example, children were shown an animated bird flying. As in the familiarization trials, the experimenter described the action for children using a familiar verb, “Look! The bird is flying.” Each puppet then responded to the experimenter’s description with either, “Yes, the bird is flying,” or “No, the bird is not flying.” The experimenter held the two puppets still and asked the children, “Which puppet is correct? Can you point to it for me?” If children did not respond, the experimenter repeated the question once more. To be included in the final sample, children had to be correct on at least three of the four training trials. Only two children failed to meet this criterion and were excluded.

Test Trials

The test trials examined children’s acceptance of the carrying/holding events. The procedure was the same as in the training trials. In each test trial, children were shown a single video clip depicting a typical, mid-typical, or atypical exemplar of a verb. The experimenter described the action for children using a familiar verb (e.g., “Look! He is X-ing a bag.”). Then, each puppet responded to the experimenter’s description with either, “Yes, he is X-ing a bag,” or “No, he is not X-ing a bag.” The experimenter held the two puppets still and asked the children to point to the puppet who was correct. The videos were presented in a pseudorandom order, with the constraints that atypical actions or events labeled by the same verb were never presented consecutively. Across children, test trials were presented in two counterbalanced blocks each containing nine videos of three carry verbs: Block A showed bào, tí, kuà; Block B showed ná, bēi, līn. Children were given a 5-min break between Blocks A and B.

Filler Trials

To keep children engaged, a filler trial was inserted after every two test trials and prior to Block B, resulting in a total of eight filler trials involving eight familiar actions: fishing, jumping, climbing, canoeing, playing basketball, kicking a football, licking a lollipop, and pushing a cart. As in training, children were asked to choose the puppet that correctly described the familiar action.

Thus, an experiment consisted of two familiarization trials, four training trials, and two test blocks each containing nine test trials and four filler trials. An experiment lasted about 35 min. Three additional factors were counterbalanced: (1) the hand on which the puppets appeared; (2) the order in which the puppets spoke; and (3) the number of “yes” and “no” responses produced by each puppet.

Coding

Each trial received a score of 1 when a child accepted the action as an exemplar of a certain verb or a score of 0 when the child rejected it. Then, at each typicality level, each child had a score of acceptance rate, calculated as the proportion of trials accepted, for the early-acquired (n = 4), late-acquired (n = 2), and all the verbs (overall), respectively. For example, if a child accepted the typical exemplars of four verbs (e.g., bào, bēi, kuà, and ; bào, bēi, and are three of the four early-acquired verbs, whereas kuà is one of the two late-acquired verbs), her overall acceptance rate for the typical exemplars would be 0.66 (4/6), and her acceptance rates for the early-acquired and late-acquired verbs were 0.75 (3/4) and 0.50 (1/2), respectively. Only 5% of the trials (19 trials from 11 children) failed to elicit a response, as the children enjoyed the task and were happy to comply. These trials were coded as no-responses in the analyses.

Results

Adults’ Linguistic and Typicality Judgments

Preliminary analyses revealed that for all but one action, adults’ acceptance levels were above 90% for the verbs across the typicality levels. That is, in most instances, adults responded to the question of whether an action could be labeled by a particular carry verb with a yes. The atypical event was dropped from further analyses as 90% of the adults rejected it as an exemplar of . Across the six verbs, adults accepted 100% of the typical events, 97% of the mid-typical events, and 95% of the atypical events as exemplifying target carry verbs. The differences in acceptance rates among the typicality levels were not significant as assessed by a one-way ANOVA (F < 1), suggesting that adults accepted the action exemplars regardless of the typicality levels. However, by comparing adults’ typicality judgments as assessed via the Likert scale, we found that the action exemplars were rated differently even though they were accepted as examples. Paired-sample t-tests with Bonferroni adjustment revealed that adults’ typicality judgments significantly decreased from typical (M = 5.98, SD = 0.47) to mid-typical (M = 4.59, SD = 0.42), and from mid-typical to atypical events (M = 3.43, SD = 0.57; t’s > 11.85; p’s < 0.001; Cohen’s d’s > 2.65). Thus, the typicality levels to which the stimulus events had been assigned were validated by Chinese adults.

Effects of Typicality and Age of Acquisition on Children’s Verb Extension

To examine how typicality and AoA affected children’s verb extension, a 3 (typicality level) × 2 (AoA: early‐ vs. late-acquired) one-sample repeated-measures ANOVA analyzed children’s acceptance rates of the early‐ and late-acquired verbs. Main effects emerged for typicality [F(2,36) = 59.86, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.77] and AoA [F(1,18) = 4.67, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.21]. These two main effects were further examined.

The Effect of Typicality

Paired-sample t-tests showed that children’s overall acceptance rates of the typical carry events (M = 0.83, SD = 0.18) were significantly higher than for the mid-typical events [M = 0.54, SD = 0.21; t(18) = 6.90, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.58], which were in turn significantly higher than for the atypical events [M = 0.32, SD = 0.26; t(18) = 5.26, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.21]. Children’s acceptance rates were also compared to chance (0.50) in separate one-sample t-tests. Rates that were significantly higher than chance level would suggest children’s acceptance of the exemplars; rates that were significantly lower than chance would suggest children’s rejection of the exemplars. Results showed that children accepted the typical events [t(18) = 8.27, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 3.79], and rejected the atypical events [t(18) = −3.08, p = 0.006, Cohen’s d = 1.41]. Their acceptance of the mid-typical events did not differ from chance [t(18) = 0.89, p = 0.38]. This pattern held when we examined the early‐ and late-acquired words separately in all the participants. Children accepted typical exemplars of early-acquired [M = 0.89, SD = 0.17; t(18) = 9.94, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 4.56] and late-acquired [M = 0.71, SD = 0.35; t(18) = 2.65, p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 1.22] verbs, and rejected atypical exemplars of early-acquired [M = 0.35, SD = 0.36; t(18) = −1.81, p = 0.09, Cohen’s d = 0.83] and late-acquired [M = 0.26, SD = 0.31; t(18) = −3.38, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 1.55] verbs. Children’s acceptance rates of mid-typical exemplars did not differ from chance for either early-acquired [M = 0.58, SD = 0.26; t(18) = 1.30, p = 0.21] and late-acquired [M = 0.47, SD = 0.35; t(18) = −0.33, p = 0.75] verbs (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The acceptance rates of the carry events. *p < 0.05 when compared to chance level (0.5).

The Effect of AoA

The significant main effect of AoA showed that exemplars of early-acquired verbs were accepted at a higher rate than exemplars of late-acquired verbs. The lack of interaction with typicality indicates that this was true at each typicality level (F < 1).

The Relation Between Children’s Production of the Late-Acquired Verbs and Acceptance of Verb Extensions

Given that children differed in whether they produced the two late-acquired verbs, for each late-acquired verb, we divided children into producers or non-producers. Producers were children whose parents reported that they produced a late acquired verb; non-producers did not yet use it. For kuà, all 11 producers and 3 out of 8 non-producers accepted the typical exemplar. A chi-square test revealed that producers of kuà were more likely to accept the typical exemplars of kuà than the non-producers (χ2 = 9.33, df 1, p = 0.002). For līn, 9 out of 10 producers and 4 out of 9 non-producers accepted the typical exemplar. A chi-square test revealed that producers of līn were more likely to accept the typical exemplars of līn than the non-producers (χ2 = 4.55, df 1, p = 0.03). For the mid-typical and atypical exemplars, the differences did not reach statistical significance.

Discussion

This study evaluated whether Mandarin-speaking 3-year-olds extended carry/hold verbs in the way that adults do, or whether children’s extension was more restricted. We presented children with typical, mid-typical, and atypical examples of carrying events accompanied by verbal descriptions and asked them to judge which of two puppets appropriately described these events using carry verbs. Chinese-speaking 3-year-olds, as a group, accepted typical exemplars, were uncertain about mid-typical exemplars, and reliably rejected the atypical exemplars, showing that they were unwilling to extend these familiar verbs to mid-typical or atypical exemplars that vary a figure’s manner of action. Thus, although Chinese children learn verbs earlier and have more verbs in their early vocabularies, they appear to resemble their English-reared counterparts in their similarly conservative verb extensions.

One concern about the task should be mentioned. Children were asked to make metalinguistic judgments – choosing the puppet that correctly described the action – a potentially challenging task. However, children’s ceiling performance in the familiarization, training, and filler trials suggested that they had little difficulty navigating the task. Furthermore, children’s acceptance rates differed systematically across the typicality levels, suggesting that they did not respond randomly. Thus, we believe that this task was valid in gauging children’s verb extensions. What do these results tell us about the acquisition of verb meaning?

In languages such as English and Chinese, there are hundreds of manner verbs (e.g., Talmy, 1985; Slobin, 2003). In English, for example, running, jogging, sprinting, and dashing all name similar actions that vary only in the speed with which they are performed. In Chinese, related but distinctive manner verbs such as the carry/hold verbs studied here are highly productive and appear early in children expressive vocabulary. Children immersed in a manner language may realize that slight differences in a manner can mean that another verb is required to describe the action. This conjecture about manner languages is supported by the research that asked when children begin to construe a novel verb as adults do (Maguire et al., 2010). English-speaking 3-year-olds reliably assumed that a new verb labeled the manner of a novel action, while their Spanish-speaking counterparts more often mapped the new verb to the path of the action. Given a large number of manner verbs in Chinese, Chinese children may be sorting out how much change in a figure’s manner of motion is acceptable before this variation warrants a new verb label. Thus, the under-extension of manner verbs minimizes promiscuous extensions that are likely to be wrong – especially when the class of verbs under consideration has highly specific meanings.

Why do Chinese children, like their English-reared counterparts (e.g., Meints et al., 2008), also limit their verb extensions to mostly typical cases? Typical events may fall more squarely into the category a verb labels than less typical events. Consider the verb push, for example. A typical pushing event involves the hands propelling an object forward. A less typical pushing event might involve a bulldozer pushing dirt. Should this exemplar also be described by the verb “pushing”? Children might wait to hear odd events labeled to decide the boundary of the lexical category. Furthermore, typical exemplars might occur more frequently in the world than less typical exemplars. The action of hammering, for example, occurs more often with a hammer than with a shoe, probably causing children to hear a verb applied more frequently to typical than to less typical exemplars. Finally, parents may be biased to label typical rather than less typical exemplars, even when both event types occur, just as they use more basic level (e.g., doggie) than subordinate (e.g., poodle) nouns with young children (e.g., Rosch and Mervis, 1975). The latter two explanations suggest that typicality is closely related to familiarity (Barsalou, 1985). Here all the exemplars were novel, although the typical ones may have been more similar to carry exemplars children had seen before. Thus, the influence of familiarity vs. prototypicality cannot be disentangled in this study.

Children’s reluctance to extend verbs to mid-typical and atypical events suggests that they actually understood the core semantic elements of the tested verbs. However, their verb construals may include more details relative to those of adults’. For example, for the verb , children only extended to new exemplars when: (1) the bag was carried in the hand; and (2) the arm was straight down. In contrast, Chinese-speaking adults apparently only considered the first element to be the defining feature of . While evidence suggests that children are aware of the features that may contribute to verb meaning (e.g., Behrend, 1990), perhaps they remain conservative in their verb extensions until they discern which features are criterial. These findings are analogous to how infants initially store the phonological forms of words. Rather than abstracting away the details, infants begin by storing features such as speaker gender and the speaker’s emotional tone along with the acoustic features of a word (Houston and Jusczyk, 2000; Newman, 2008).

Eventually, children do extend verbs as adults do, suggesting that verb meaning may undergo a “characteristic-to-defining shift” just as noun meaning does (Keil and Batterman, 1984). Children begin, for instance, by claiming that the definition of the noun “island” is “a place with palm trees” (i.e., a characteristic), not yet cognizant of the fact that “surrounded on all sides by water” is the only necessary defining feature. The same shift may be seen with verbs: Characteristic features may yield to defining features over time. Multiple exposures to a variety of verb-action combinations may help children come to understand the parameters of verb application (Forbes and Farrar, 1995; Gentner, 2006; Childers and Paik, 2009; Childers, 2011).

Although all children in our sample comprehended the test verbs, not all children produced the late-acquired verbs and this difference was reflected in children’s verb extension. Children who produced a late-acquired verb appeared to be more likely to extend that verb than children who did not yet produce it. One possibility is that the demands of verb production may cause children to notice those event features that influence the use of that verb. However, it is also possible that a better understanding of a verb’s meaning increases the likelihood that it is produced. Future research can determine the direction of causality in this relationship and the changes in the role of typicality over developmental time.

Conclusion

Despite the putative verb-friendliness of Chinese and the fact that Chinese children have a relative verb advantage compared with their English-speaking counterparts, Chinese-speaking children were reluctant to extend familiar verbs beyond typical exemplars. Thus, it is unlikely that the properties of the input language per se are responsible for children’s conservative verb extensions. Children seem to use typical exemplars as their starting point and gradually expand the meaning of verbs to include less typical instances. Learning the meanings of verbs is a prolonged and demanding process (e.g., Bates et al., 1979; Gallivan, 1988; Harris et al., 1988; Tomasello, 1992; Barrett, 1995; Theakston et al., 2002; Seston et al., 2009).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Human subject research committee, School of Education, University of Delaware. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

WM and RG conceived and designed the study. WM collected data. WM, RG, and LS analyzed the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

RG and KH-P were funded by joint grants (NSF: SBR9615391; SBR-990-5832; 0642632; NIH: R01-HD15964; RO1HD050199; NICHHD: R01-HD19568; IES: R305A100215; R324A160241).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. The strike above a denotes the lexical tone of the Chinese character. There are four basic tones in Mandarin Chinese. Tones are distinctive features in Chinese. For example, with a high, level tone means mother; with a rising tone means numb; with a dipping tone means horse; and with a falling tone means to curse. Furthermore, there are additional instances in which multiple early-acquired Chinese verbs correspond to one verb in English. For example, chuān means ‘to wear something that the body goes through it (e.g., a jacket, pants); daì means “to wear something on the surface of the body (e.g., a hat, a badge, a necklace). In addition, tăng means ‘to lie on the back or side’, and pā means ‘to lie on the stomach’; tī means ‘to kick’, and dēng means ‘to kick with the bottom of the foot.

References

Arunachalam, S., and Waxman, S. R. (2010). Meaning from syntax: evidence from 2-year-olds. Cognition 114, 442–446. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arunachalam, S., and Waxman, S. R. (2011). Grammatical form and semantic context in verb learning. Lang. Learn. Dev. 7, 169–184. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2011.573760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballem, K., and Plunkett, K. (2005). Phonological specificity in children at 1;2. J. Child Lang. 32, 159–173. doi: 10.1017/s0305000904006567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, M. D. (1986). “Early semantic representations and early word usage” in The development of word meaning. eds. S. A. Kuczaj and M. D. Barrett (New York: Springer), 39–67.

Google Scholar

Barrett, M. D. (1995). “Early lexical development” in The handbook of child language. eds. P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (Oxford, UK: Blackwell), 362–292.

Google Scholar

Barry, C., Hirsh, K. W., Johnston, R. A., and Williams, C. L. (2001). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the locus of repetition priming of picture naming. J. Mem. Lang. 44, 350–375. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, C., Morrison, C. M., and Ellis, A. W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: effects of age of acquisition, frequency and name agreement. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 50A, 560–585. doi: 10.1080/783663595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 11, 629–654. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.11.1-4.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, I., and Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Behrend, D. A. (1990). The development of verb concepts: children’s use of verbs to label familiar and novel events. Child Dev. 61, 681–696.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chan, C. C. Y., Tardif, T., Chen, J., Pulverman, R. B., Zhu, L., and Meng, X. (2011). English‐ and Chinese-learning infants map novel labels to objects and actions differently. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1459–1471. doi: 10.1037/a0024049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childers, J. B. (2011). Attention to multiple events helps 2 1/2-year-olds extend new verbs. First Lang. 31, 3–22. doi: 10.1177/0142723710361825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childers, J. B., and Paik, J. H. (2009). Korean‐ and English-speaking children use cross-situational information to learn novel predicate terms. J. Child Lang. 36, 201–224. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erreich, A., and Valian, V. (1979). Children’s internal organization of locative categories. Child Dev. 50, 1071–1077.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., and Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59, 1–173.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., and Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. J. Mem. Lang. 44, 516–547. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, J. N., and Farrar, M. J. (1995). Learning to represent word meaning: what initial training events reveal about children’s developing action verb concepts. Cogn. Dev. 10, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Forbes, J. N., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (1997). Representational change in young children’s understanding of familiar verb meaning. J. Child Lang. 24, 389–406. doi: 10.1017/s0305000997003127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, J. (1988). Motion verb acquisition: development of definitions. Percept. Mot. Skills 66, 979–986. doi: 10.2466/pms.1988.66.3.979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D. (2006). “Why verbs are hard to learn” in Action meets word: How children learn verbs. eds. K. Hirsh-Pasek and R. Golinkoff (New York: Oxford University Press), 544–564.

Google Scholar

Gerhand, S., and Barry, C. (1999). Age of acquisition, word frequency and the role of phonology in the lexical decision task. Mem. Cogn. 27, 592–602. doi: 10.3758/bf03211553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golinkoff, R. M., Ma, W., Song, L., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013). Twenty-five years using the intermodal preferential looking paradigm to study language acquisition: what have we learned? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 316–339. doi: 10.1177/1745691613484936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, M., Shu, H., Xing, A., and Li, P. (2008). Early vocabulary inventory for mandarin Chinese. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 728–733. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M., Barrett, M., Jones, D., and Brookes, S. (1988). Linguistic input and early word meaning. J. Child Lang. 15, 77–94. doi: 10.1017/s030500090001206x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, W. G., and Tarr, M. J. (1995). Spatial language and spatial representation. Cognition 55, 39–84. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00643-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsh-Pasek, K., and Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). “The preferential looking paradigm reveals emerging language comprehension” in Methods for assessing children’s syntax. eds. D. McDaniel, C. McKee, and H. Cairns (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 105–124.

Google Scholar

Hirsh-Pasek, K., and Golinkoff, R. M. (eds.) (2006). Action meets word: How children learn verbs. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Houston, D. M., and Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1570–1582. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.5.1570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., et al. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. Child Dev. 79, 979–1000. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keil, F. C., and Batterman, N. (1984). A characteristic-to-defining shift in the development of word meaning. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 23, 221–236.

Google Scholar

Kersten, A. W., and Smith, L. B. (2002). Attention to novel objects during verb learning. Child Dev. 73, 93–109. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, M., Clement, F., and Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychol. Sci. 15, 694–698. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leddon, E. M., Arunachalam, S., Waxman, S. R., Fu, X., Gong, H., and Wang, L. (2011). “Noun and verb learning in Mandarin-acquiring 24-month-olds” in Online Proceedings Supplement of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.

Google Scholar

Logan, G. D., and Sadler, D. D. (1996). “A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations” in Language and space. eds. P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, and M. F. Garrett (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 493–529.

Google Scholar

Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., McDonough, C., and Tardif, T. (2009). Imageability predicts the age of acquisition of verbs in Chinese children. J. Child Lang. 36, 405–423. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908009008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, W., Zhou, P., Golinkoff, R. M., Lee, J., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Syntactic cues to the noun and verb distinction in mandarin child-directed speech. First Lang. 39, 433–461. doi: 10.1177/0142723719845175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., and Brandone, A. (2008). Focusing on the relation: fewer exemplars facilitate children’s initial verb learning and extension. Dev. Sci. 11, 628–634. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00707.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Imai, M., Haryu, E., Vengas, S., et al. (2010). A developmental shift from similar to language-specific strategies in verb acquisition: a comparison of English, Spanish, and Japanese. Cognition 114, 299–319. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meints, K., Plunkett, K., and Harris, P. L. (1999). When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Dev. Psychol. 35, 1072–1078. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.4.1072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meints, K., Plunkett, K., and Harris, P. L. (2008). Eating apples or houseplants? The role of typicality in early comprehension of verbs. Lang. Cogn. Process. 23, 434–463. doi: 10.1080/01690960701726232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meints, K., Plunkett, K., Harris, P. L., and Dimmock, D. K. (2002). What is ‘on’ and ‘under’ to 15-, 18‐ and 24-month-olds? Typicality effects in early comprehension of spatial prepositions. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 20, 113–130. doi: 10.1348/026151002166352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, C. M., and Ellis, A. W. (1995). Roles of word frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and lexical decision. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 116–133. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, C. M., and Ellis, A. W. (2000). Real age of acquisition effects in word naming and lexical decision. Br. J. Psychol. 91, 167–180. doi: 10.1348/000712600161763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naigles, L., Hoff, E., and Vear, D. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: evidence from a multiple-n diary study. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 74, vii–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00513.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, R. S. (2008). The level of detail in infants’ word learning. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 229–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00580.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulin-Dubois, D., and Sissons, M. E. (2002). Is this still called a dog? 18-month-olds’ generalization of familiar object labels to unusual objects. Infant Child Dev. 11, 57–67. doi: 10.1002/icd.256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, M., Oetting, J., Marquis, J., Bode, J., and Pae, S. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Hear. Res. 59, 106–122. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3701.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 573–605.

Google Scholar

Saji, N., Imai, M., Saalbach, H., Zhang, Y., Shu, H., and Okada, H. (2011). Word learning does not end at fast-mapping: evolution of verb meanings through reorganization of an entire semantic domain. Cognition 118, 45–61. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, R. M., and Fisher, C. (2012). 2.5-year-olds use cross-situational consistency to learn verbs under referential uncertainty. Cognition 122, 163–180. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidl, A., and Johnson, E. K. (2006). Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Dev. Sci. 9, 565–573. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00534.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seston, R., Golinkoff, R. M., Ma, W., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2009). Vacuuming with my mouth?: Children’s ability to comprehend novel extensions of familiar verbs. Cogn. Dev. 24, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. (2003). “Language and thought online: cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity” in Language in mind: Advances in the investigation of language and thought. eds. D. Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 157–191.

Google Scholar

Talmy, L. (1985). “Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms” in Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. 3. ed. T. Shopen (New York: Cambridge University Press), 57–149.

Google Scholar

Tardif, T., Fletcher, P., Zhang, Z. X., Liang, W. L., and Zuo, Q. H. (2008). The Chinese communicative development inventory (Putonghua and Cantonese versions): Manual, forms, and norms. Beijing: Peking University Medical Press.

Google Scholar

Tardif, T., Gelman, S. A., and Xu, F. (1999). Putting the "noun bias" in context: a comparison of English and mandarin. Child Dev. 70, 620–635. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tardif, T., Shatz, M., and Naigles, L. R. (1997). Caregiver speech and children’s use of nouns versus verbs: a comparison of English, Italian, and mandarin. J. Child Lang. 24, 535–565. doi: 10.1017/s030500099700319x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., and Rowland, C. F. (2002). Going, going, gone: the acquisition of the verb ‘go’. J. Child Lang. 29, 783–811. doi: 10.1017/s030500090200538x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early lexical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–253. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00069-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., and Brandt, S. (2009). Flexibility in the semantics and syntax of children’s early verb use. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 74, 113–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00523.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, J. E., Valentine, T., and Ellis, A. W. (1998). Contrasting effects of age of acquisition and word frequency on auditory and visual lexical decision. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1282–1291. doi: 10.3758/bf03201200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, K., Fennell, C., Swingley, D., and Werker, J. F. (2009). Fourteen-month-olds learn similar-sounding words. Dev. Sci. 12, 412–418. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00789.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, S., and Fisher, C. (2009). “Really? She blicked the baby?”: two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychol. Sci. 20, 619–626. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: verb extension, age of acquisition, typicality, verb, Chinese

Citation: Ma W, Golinkoff RM, Song L and Hirsh-Pasek K (2021) Using Verb Extension to Gauge Children’s Verb Meaning Construals: The Case of Chinese. Front. Psychol. 11:572198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572198

Received: 13 June 2020; Accepted: 28 December 2020;
Published: 04 February 2021.

Edited by:

Gabriella Vigliocco, University College London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Laura Gonnerman, McGill University, Canada
LouAnn Gerken, University of Arizona, United States

Copyright © 2021 Ma, Golinkoff, Song and Hirsh-Pasek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weiyi Ma, weiyima@uark.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.