Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2020
Sec. Psychology of Language

Dissimilar Phonemes Create a Contextual Interference Effect During a Nonword Repetition Task

Kimberly M. Meigh
Kimberly M. Meigh*Elisabeth KeeElisabeth Kee
  • Speech Motor Control Lab, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, United States

Purpose: The contextual interference effect is a motor learning phenomenon where conditions that decrease overall learning during practice enhance overall learning with new tasks. In the limb literature, this effect is observed when different practice conditions are used (e.g., blocked vs. random practice schedules). In speech motor learning, contextual interference effects are mixed. The differences observed during speech motor learning may be due to the stimuli used. We hypothesized that dissimilar phonemes might create interference during speech motor learning, such that training accuracy would decrease. However, generalization accuracy would increase compared to practice with nonwords containing similar phonemes.

Method: Thirty young adults with typical speech and hearing participated in a motor learning study using a cross-over design. Participants engaged in nonword repetition training followed by an immediate retention and transfer task with two sets of nonwords: nonwords with similar phonemes and nonwords with dissimilar phonemes. Percent consonants correct were calculated to examine the effects of the two different types of nonwords based on the stage of skill acquisition.

Results: A contextual interference effect was observed in this study using nonwords that varied in phonemic similarity. Nonwords with similar phonemes were produced with greater accuracy during the training stage of skill acquisition, and nonwords with dissimilar phonemes were produced with greater accuracy during the transfer stage.

Conclusion: The proposed hypothesis for this study was met – practicing nonwords with dissimilar phonemes resulted in greater accuracy in the transfer phase of this experiment. Results indicate that phonemic dissimilarity produced contextual interference and influenced speech motor learning. These results indicate that the linguistic properties of stimuli must be factored into speech motor learning. Future research should explore if other linguistic variables interact with variables of motor learning to enhance speech practice and generalization outcomes.

Introduction

The contextual interference effect is a paradoxical phenomenon. Conditions that decrease learning during practice (interference variables) increase overall learning when attempting new tasks (Magill and Hall, 1990; Lee et al., 1992). The most well-researched interference condition investigated in the limb motor learning literature is practice schedule (i.e., blocked vs. random practice). During blocked practice, one motor skill is repetitively practiced before moving onto a second motor skill. In contrast, during random practice, both motor skills are practiced. The order of practice is variable between the two skills. Random practice, when compared to blocked practice, results in better generalization, or transfer, to new movements (Shea and Morgan, 1979; Magill and Hall, 1990). Evaluation of practice schedules during speech motor learning has yielded mixed results (Scheiner et al., 2014; Jones and Croot, 2016). One reason for this may be that speech is a unique motor act influenced by motor and linguistic processing. Thus, the properties of the stimuli used during training may influence speech motor learning.

Empirical evidence of practice schedules creating interference effects in typical speakers during speech motor learning is mixed. Adams and Page (2000) investigated the effect of feedback and practice schedule during repetitions of the phrase “Buy Bobby a puppy.” Practice schedules were varied by temporal duration, with results revealing no difference in absolute error between blocked and random practice schedules at the end of the training. However, lower absolute error rates were reported for the random practice group during a retention task. Wong et al. (2013) conducted a similar experiment in Cantonese to evaluate blocked, random, and mixed practice schedules (e.g., blocked-then-random schedule). No significant differences in utterance duration were observed between the random- and blocked-only practice schedule groups at the end of training or during the retention tasks. Transfer performance was evaluated using a dual-task paradigm and revealed the secondary task less adversely influenced subjects who trained using a mixed practice schedule. Jones and Croot evaluated mixed and singular practice schedules using tongue twisters, e.g., “Bell Pod Pun Boot”(Jones and Croot, 2016, p. 358). They reported no significant differences in accuracy between blocked and random practice schedules by participants following training or retention tests. However, blocked-random and random practice schedules generally facilitated better retention of learning. Additionally, these authors reported differences in errors based on the specific tongue twister used during blocked practice in the training and retention conditions.

Practice schedules have also been evaluated using nonword stimuli. Scheiner et al. (2014) constructed four nonwords that varied by the number of syllables and phonemes. Following training, the blocked practice group produced these nonwords with significantly higher accuracy, shorter duration, and lower variability compared to the random practice group. The random practice group was significantly more accurate in producing the nonwords during the retention task. However, no significant difference in nonword duration or variability between the practice groups was reported. Post hoc item analyses following training and retention revealed specific nonwords were produced more accurately, quickly, and with less overall variation during each phase of the experiment compared to the other trained nonwords. Kaipa et al. (2017) used nonword phrases to evaluate practice variability and practice schedule in younger and older adults. Only the retention phase of motor learning was evaluated with results indicating significantly better spatial accuracy (as measured by percent phonemes correct, PPC) for older subjects using the random practice schedule. No significant difference in PPC scores was reported for younger subjects regardless of practice schedule. Temporal accuracy, as measured by mean phi correlation, revealed younger participants were able to learn the nonword phrases significantly better than older adults regardless of practice schedule.

The varying degrees of contextual interference reported in the speech motor learning literature may stem from the different interpretations regarding the fundamental programming unit, i.e., speech motor programs, studied during speech motor learning. Currently, there is no consensus on the size of the programming unit for speech production. Linguistic and speech production models have postulated syllables as a programming unit (e.g., Levelt et al., 1999; Cholin et al., 2006). However, there is also significant evidence to suggest smaller programming units, e.g., phonemes, may influence speech production more overtly (e.g., Meigh, 2017; Liu et al., 2018; Meigh et al., 2018). These different interpretations influence the construction of stimuli and the variables controlled across stimuli sets in experimental studies of speech motor learning. Of the reviewed studies, two explicitly noted the influence of specific phoneme factors on the outcomes of their study. Scheiner et al. (2014, p. 35) observed certain levels of complexity in their stimuli were not controlled, e.g., phoneme markedness. Similarly, Jones and Croot (2016) controlled the initial phoneme pairs in their tongue twisters for sequence and articulation positions. They also reported an imbalance in their stimuli based on place of articulation, which may have influenced the difficulty of the stimuli.

These potential influences suggest that selecting specific phonemes may alter the amount of interference present during learning. This effect is reported in the perceptual learning literature involving second language acquisition using high variability pronunciation training (HVPT). During HVPT, minimal pairs containing nonnative phonemes are trained until speakers can discriminate between similar sounds (for a review, see Barriuso and Hayes-Harb, 2018; Thomson, 2018). Nishi and Kewley-Port (2007) reported better generalization to new English vowels following HVPT training with a full set of vowels than a subset of three vowels in native Japanese speakers. These generalization results were in contrast to observing better acquisition during training with the smaller subset of vowels. A follow-up study a year later replicated these findings in Korean speakers indicating that training with larger stimuli sets provided greater overall generalization and learning compared to smaller sets (Nishi and Kewley-Port, 2008). By increasing the variability of conditions, such as the number of contrasting phonemes, Nishi and Kewley-Port (2007, 2008) observed a contextual interference effect in their perceptual learning studies.

This study aimed to investigate whether varying the number of phoneme contrasts would produce a contextual interference effect when practice schedules were held constant during training. Using a cross-over design, participants engaged in two speech motor learning tasks with different nonword stimuli that varied in phoneme similarity. In this study, phoneme similarity was defined in two ways: (1) by the number of repeated phonemes within a nonword, and (2) by psycholinguistic variables evaluated in previous studies (e.g., biphone probability, articulatory features). Following both motor learning tasks, we evaluated participants’ accuracy based on the type of stimuli (similar and dissimilar) and motor learning phase (training and generalization). Our first hypothesis was that participants would demonstrate learning of similar and dissimilar nonword stimuli as the result of practice during the training phase of the experiment. Our second hypothesis, presented in two parts, predicted the contextual interference effect. Participants practicing similar nonwords during training would have greater accuracy at the end of training than when practicing dissimilar nonwords, with the reverse pattern observed during generalization.

Participants

Thirty adults (23 female, seven male) between 18 and 31 years of age (M = 21, SD = 3) participated in this study. All participants were native speakers of English, and the primary investigators screened their speech and hearing skills. Participants’ conversational speech was evaluated for articulation and fluency errors. The Test of Minimal Articulation sentence and reading screening subtests (Secord, 1981), as well as an oral motor examination, were used to rule out speech disorders. Hearing acuity was screened using pure tone thresholds at 35 dB HL at 500, 1,000, 2,000, and 4,000 Hz in at least one ear (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 1990). Speech discrimination abilities were screened using the Northwestern University Auditory Test No. 6 word list (Tillman and Carhart, 1966). All participants were required to identify 98% of all words correctly. Working memory capacity and phonological processing abilities were documented using the Digit Span and Nonword Repetition Subtests from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Second Edition (CTOPP; Wagner et al., 2013). These working memory measures were not used to rule out participants but to classify the participants’ overall memory processing capabilities: Digit Span percentile (M = 50.39, SD = 19.68), Nonword Repetition percentile (M = 42.09, SD = 23.42).

All participants signed informed consent documents approved by the West Virginia Institutional Review Board (IRB) before initiating the screening procedures outlined above. Participants who were eligible for the study based on the above screening procedures were compensated for their participation in this study. All procedures outlined (screening and experimental) were approved by the West Virginia University (WVU) IRB and are in accordance with all guidelines and regulations related to behavioral experiments with human subjects.

Stimuli

The stimuli used in this study consisted of 40 nonwords divided into two sets of 20 based on phoneme repetition (Kendall et al., 2005; Meigh, 2017). The set of nonwords that had the most phoneme repetitions was considered the “similar” set. The other set was considered the “dissimilar” set. Each set of 20 nonwords was then randomly split into two sets of 10 nonwords to create a “training” and “transfer” set. All nonwords consisted of three syllables (CV|CV|CVC) comprised of novel combinations of phonemes that followed English phonotactic rules. Syllable stress for all nonwords was on the first or second syllable. Table 1 details the full list of stimuli.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Stimuli categorized by type (similar and dissimilar) and motor learning phase (training and transfer).

Several parameters were used to distinguish similar and dissimilar sets. The previous review of the literature suggested that multiple phonemic factors may influence speech motor learning (Table 2). The average number of different phonemes within a set was calculated based on a single occurrence of a phoneme within a nonword (i.e., repetitions of phonemes were not included). Average phonotactic probabilities were calculated using the University of Kansas’ phonotactic probability online calculator (Vitevitch and Luce, 2004). Position-specific probabilities relate to the frequency of a given phoneme to appear in a specific position in all words of the English language. In contrast, biphone probabilities describe the probability of two adjacent phonemes occurring within a word together (Vitevitch and Luce, 2004). Intraword similarity values were calculated by counting the number of shared consonants or vowels within a nonword and then averaging all values within stimuli sets. A feature-based analysis was also conducted for each set of stimuli based on consonant voice, place and manner, and vowel height and advancement to provide further evidence of similarity/dissimilarity between sets (Rogers and Storkel, 1998; Bailey and Hahn, 2005). These percentages included all repetitions of a phoneme within a word.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Stimuli set characteristics.

Study Design

A randomized, cross-over design was used to evaluate the effects of phoneme similarity on speech motor learning. Participants were randomly assigned to start training with either similar or dissimilar nonwords. Once the training and generalization phases were complete with the first set of nonwords, a second nonword repetition task with the other nonwords was initiated following a 5–10-min break. As noted previously, nonwords were randomized within blocks during training for each participant. Randomization was used to enhance overall motor learning during training (Maas et al., 2008). Two blocks of nonwords were created for each type of stimuli (similar, dissimilar) and generalization task (retention, transfer). These blocks were counterbalanced across participants. Figure 1 depicts a schematic example of the cross-over design of this study, the random assignment of nonwords (similar or dissimilar), and the counterbalancing used during the generalization tasks.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic depiction of the nonword repetition task across participants using a cross-over design.

Procedures

The experiment consisted of two nonword repetition tasks, each consisting of two parts: the “training” phase and the “generalization” phase. the experiment occurred in a quiet room where participants were seated in a chair at a table with a dynamic headset with a unidirectional microphone (SURE WH20XLR) placed approximately one inch from the participant’s mouth. The microphone connected to a digital voice recorder (Olympus DM-901), which recorded each nonword repetition task. All stimuli played through a stereo speaker (Bose Companion 2 Series 3) centered 15 in infront of the participant. The speaker was connected to a 64-bit Dell Latitude 3340 laptop with Windows 7 operating system, which ran Eprime (Schneider et al., 2002).

Training

Eprime randomly presented all training stimuli within a block. A total of 10 blocks of training were completed (i.e., 100 repetitions). All stimuli during training were of the same type (either similar or dissimilar). During a single trial, participants listened to an auditory presentation of a nonword and repeated the nonword into the microphone. The examiner, a graduate student in the WVU speech-language pathology program, perceptually rated the pronunciation of the participant’s nonword production and noted all incorrect productions by pushing a button on the laptop. At the end of 10 trials (or a single training block), summary feedback was provided by Eprime to enhance overall motor learning (Maas et al., 2008). Misarticulated nonwords were replayed through the speaker, and the participant was instructed to listen carefully to the repeated nonwords. Following summary feedback, training continued for another block. This procedure continued until all 10 blocks were completed.

Generalization

Following training, two generalization tasks using the same type of nonwords (similar or dissimilar) were administered: a retention task and a transfer task. For each task, 10 nonwords were presented via Eprime using the same nonword repetition procedure as used in training. No summary feedback was provided to participants following each task. The only difference between generalization tasks was the nonwords used. Trained stimuli were used in the retention task, and new nonwords (i.e., transfer nonwords) were used in the transfer task.

Measurements

All nonword responses from training blocks 1 and 10, and all retention and transfer blocks, were individually scored for phoneme accuracy and coded as dichotomous variables (correct or incorrect; Shriberg and Kwiatkowski, 1982). Two blinded raters, trained in phonetic transcription, independently listened to the audio recordings of the participant’s nonword production and determined phoneme-by-phoneme accuracy by perceptual judgment. A third, blinded rater resolved any discrepancies in accuracy ratings. This point-by-point system forced 100% interscorer reliability.

Each phoneme was scored relative to the model production (total of seven phonemes per nonword). All phoneme distortions, substitutions, omissions, and insertions were considered incorrect. A percent phonemes correct (PPC) was calculated for each nonword by dividing the total number of correctly produced phonemes by the total number of phonemes. An average PPC score was calculated for each participant per nonword type (similar, dissimilar) and block (training blocks 1, 10, retention, transfer).

Statistical Analyses

Two mixed-design analyses of variance (SPSS version 26) were used to evaluate the order of task administration (similar vs. dissimilar protocols) in relation to within-subject performance between different motor performance time points using similar and dissimilar nonwords. The first analysis was conducted to evaluate PPC score differences between different sets of stimuli during motor learning (blocks 1 and 10). The second analysis was conducted to evaluate PPC score differences between stimuli sets to determine if a contextual interference effect was observed (comparison of retention vs. transfer blocks). Outliers were observed with both analyses; however, all data were included in the analyses. Assumptions of normality were not met for all conditions in each analysis (p < 0.05). No correction was made as ANOVA statistics are typically robust to these violations. Assumptions of homogeneity of variance, homogeneity of covariances, and sphericity were met for both analyses. A significance level of 0.05 was used for hypothesis testing. Planned comparisons employed a Bonferroni correction, which adjusted the alpha level for multiple comparisons (0.05/4) for each analysis. All reported comparison significant values have already been adjusted using a Bonferroni correction and should be compared to a significance level of 0.05.

Results

Data from 23 participants were included in the analyses. Subject attrition was due to failing one or more screening measures (N = 4) and an inability to finish the experimental protocol secondary to equipment failure (N = 3). Following attrition, 12 participants initiated training with similar nonwords, and 11 participants initiated training with dissimilar nonwords.

Motor Learning

There was not a statistically significant interaction between the order of training (similar vs. dissimilar training) and PPC scores during blocks 1 and 10 regardless of stimuli type (similar or dissimilar nonwords), F(3,63) = 0.076, p = 0.972, partial η 2 = 0.004. No main effect was observed for order of training, F(1,21) < 0.001, p = 0.989, partial η 2 < 0.001. However, a main effect of training block showed that PPC scores were significantly different between blocks 1 and 10 when different stimuli were practiced, F(3,63) = 45.977, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.686.

Planned comparisons related to motor learning revealed PPC scores were significantly higher from blocks 1 to 10 when participants were practicing similar nonwords [p < 0.001, CI (−5.108, −1.279), η 2 = 0.497] and dissimilar nonwords [p < 0.001, CI (−9.015, −3.402), η 2 = 0.635]. At the end of the first block of training, participants were significantly more accurate in producing similar nonwords than dissimilar nonwords, p < 0.001, CI (5.699, 12.021), η 2 = 0.736. This difference in accuracy persisted through the end of training (block 10), where participants’ similar nonword productions were more accurately produced compared to dissimilar nonwords, p < 0.001, CI (2.979, 8.771), η 2 = 0.586. Figure 2 depicts average PPC scores and 95% confidence intervals for all stimuli.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean phoneme accuracy for similar and dissimilar nonwords during training (blocks 1 and 10).

Contextual Interference Effect

There was not a statistically significant interaction between the order of training (similar vs. dissimilar training) and PPC scores during retention and transfer blocks regardless of stimuli type (similar or dissimilar nonwords), F(3,63) = 0.092, p = 0.964, partial η 2 = 0.004. No main effect was observed for order of training, F(1,21) = 0.786, p = 0.385, partial η 2 = 0.036. A main effect of generalization showed that PPC scores were significantly different between retention and transfer following practice with different stimuli, F(3,63) = 20.213, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.49.

As noted in Figure 3, planned comparisons evaluating generalization of learning revealed a statistically significant decrease in PPC scores from the retention to transfer blocks when participants repeated similar nonwords [p < 0.001, CI (4.657, 12.118), η 2 = 0.642]. No significant difference in PPC scores was observed between retention and transfer blocks when participants repeated dissimilar nonwords [p = 0.056, CI (−4.995, 0.742), η 2 = 0.163].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean phoneme accuracy for similar and dissimilar nonwords retention and transfer blocks.

Planned comparisons also revealed a contextual interference effect. Mean PPC scores were increased during the retention block when participants repeated similar nonwords compared to dissimilar nonwords, p < 0.001, CI (2.865, 8.054), η 2 = 0.611. Despite this gain, PPC scores were significantly lower when participants repeated new similar nonwords compared to new dissimilar nonwords during the transfer block, p < 0.001, CI (−8.363,-1.745), η 2 = 0.453.

Discussion

This study investigated the contextual interference effect in speech motor learning by evaluating participants’ production accuracy during two nonword learning tasks. We had two predictions regarding participants’ speech motor learning. First, we predicted participants’ accuracy would increase with training (i.e., from blocks 1 to 10) regardless of stimuli type based on large amounts of random practice (Maas et al., 2008). This prediction was met. However, these effects were more significant when participants practiced similar nonwords compared to dissimilar nonwords. These results align with previous work where repeated, or identical phoneme sequences resulted in fewer speech production errors (Damian and Dumay, 2007; Mailend et al., 2019). Similarly, during speech perception training, practicing with fewer phoneme contrasts resulted in better acquisition during training (Nishi and Kewley-Port, 2007, 2008).

Secondly, we predicted that a contextual interference effect would be observed during the generalization task. The contextual interference effect is a phenomenon where performance is diminished during training when conditions create interference; however, this interference results in overall better generalization (Magill and Hall, 1990; Lee et al., 1992). The most well-researched variables of this effect include random vs. blocked practice (Shea and Morgan, 1979; Magill and Hall, 1990). We hypothesized for speech motor learning that similar and dissimilar phonemes might also produce this effect based on the mixed empirical results evaluating practice schedule during speech motor learning (Scheiner et al., 2014; Jones and Croot, 2016) and perceptual learning (Nishi and Kewley-Port, 2007, 2008). Specifically, we predicted that participants would have significantly higher phonemic accuracy repeating trained similar nonwords than dissimilar nonwords following training. However, this pattern would reverse when participants repeated new dissimilar transfer nonwords during the generalization task. This prediction was met. Participants were more accurate producing new dissimilar nonwords during the transfer task despite difficulties practicing with dissimilar nonwords during the training task.

Our results suggest the processing of repeated and similar phonemes within nonwords may impede speech motor learning. Practicing nonwords with similar phonemes may have resulted in the learner not discriminating unique features between stimuli during training. This lack of discrimination may have resulted in memory encoding of speech representations that lacked distinctive attributes. During the transfer block, retrieving memory representations from long-term memory resulted in decreased accuracy due to difficulty discriminating between similar memory representations (Lee, 1988). In contrast, participants practicing nonwords with different phonemes may have encoded distinctive memory representations. These distinctive features would allow for more efficient and accurate memory retrieval during the transfer stage of motor learning (Lin et al., 2018).

These results contrast to a language processing effect termed the “phoneme similarity effect,” where an increase in production errors was observed with repeated and similar phonemes (Yaniv et al., 1990; Rogers and Storkel, 1998; Wilshire, 1999). We did not anticipate the phoneme similarity effect to influence the motor learning outcomes in this study. However, the contrasting effect of repeated or similar phonemes on language vs. motor processing is interesting. During phonetic encoding, linguistic code transforms into a motor code executed by the speech system (Levelt et al., 1999). Nevertheless, the handoff between language and motor processing is not well understood (Laganaro, 2019). During this process, variables that inhibit language processing become facilitative during speech production. Further investigation is warranted to address the contexts and variables that may be altered by phonetic encoding. Moreover, there is also other evidence to suggest that language processing may enhance speech production.

Vowels are hyper articulated (i.e., an enlarged vowel space) when target words reside in dense lexical neighborhoods compared to sparse neighborhoods (Munson and Solomon, 2004; Wright, 2004; Watson and Munson, 2007). Similar vowel hyper articulation is observed if the target word comprises minimal pairs of phonemes, e.g., /b/ and /p/ (Peramunage et al., 2011). In these conditions, target words share similar phonemes. However, the phoneme similarity effect is not observed, and the overall articulation of the target word is enhanced. Fewer speech errors have also been reported when target words are from dense neighborhoods (Vitevitch, 2002; Vitevitch and Luce, 2004). Paradoxically, these effects reverse in broader contexts, such as conversation, where vowels became centralized and shortened in dense lexical neighborhoods (Gahl et al., 2012) or with frequently produced words (Fox et al., 2015). Thus, further research is essential in identifying the contexts and processing demands that shift linguistic influences from facilitating to inhibiting speech production.

There were limitations to our study. We used a carry-over design to evaluate the effect of phoneme similarity on speech motor learning. Our design limited the amount of time between each motor learning task, which may have created a potential carry-over effect between treatments. Although we attempted to minimize these effects with multiple sets of stimuli used for retention and transfer conditions, future studies should allow longer “wash out” periods between training sets. Moreover, this study investigated the immediate retention and transfer effects of training on two stimulus sets. This study provided a point of investigation for future studies evaluating lasting motor changes, where at least a day between training and generalizations tasks would be observed (Kantak and Winstein, 2012).

The use of pre-constructed stimuli, used in prior speech motor learning paradigms, ensured that participants with intact speech abilities would learn a novel speech-like task during this experiment. However, this limited the amount of control over how similar phonemes were based on multiple indexes of similarity. The findings from this study provide a preliminary definition of “similarity” that may be manipulated in future studies, further exploring the contextual interference effect in speech motor control. Other factors may include changes in construction related to consonant age of acquisition, manner and place of articulation, or voicing features of phonemes (Bailey and Hahn, 2005). Further evaluation of similarity indices in constructing speech stimuli is needed to understand better how phonemic properties may influence speech motor learning.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board at West Virginia University. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Both the authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was supported by the second author’s Wirt C. and Mae S. Belcher Graduate Education Award from West Virginia University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the West Virginia Speech Motor Control lab members who aided in data collection and analysis, as well as Katelyn Bond, who provided additional research and analysis into different similarity indices for our stimuli.

References

Adams, S. G., and Page, A. D. (2000). Effects of selected practice sand feedback variables on speech motor learning. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 8, 215–220. doi: 10.23641/asha.5116324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (1990). Guidelines for screening hearing impairment and middle-ear disorders. ASHA 32, 17–24.

Google Scholar

Bailey, T. M., and Hahn, U. (2005). Phoneme similarity and confusability. J. Mem. Lang. 52, 339–362. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barriuso, T. A., and Hayes-Harb, R. (2018). High variability phonetic training as a bridge from research to practice. CATESOL J. 30, 177–194.

Google Scholar

Cholin, J., Levelt, W. J. M., and Schiller, N. O. (2006). Effects of syllable frequency in speech production. Cognition 99, 205–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F., and Dumay, N. (2007). Time pressure and phonological advance planning in spoken production. J. Mem. Lang. 57, 195–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, N. P., Reilly, M., and Blumstein, S. E. (2015). Phonological neighborhood competition affects spoken word production irrespective of sentential context. J. Mem. Lang. 83, 97–117. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gahl, S., Yao, Y., and Johnson, K. (2012). Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech. J. Mem. Lang. 66, 789–806. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K., and Croot, K. (2016). The effect of blocked, random and mixed practice schedules on speech motor learning of tongue twisters in unimpaired speakers. Mot. Control. 20, 350–379. doi: 10.1123/mc.2013-0102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaipa, R., Robb, M., and Jones, R. (2017). The effectiveness of constant, variable, random, and blocked practice in speech-motor learning. J. Mot. Learn. Dev. 5, 103–125. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2015-0044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantak, S. S., and Winstein, C. J. (2012). Learning–performance distinction and memory processes for motor skills: a focused review and perspective. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 219–231. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendall, D., McNeil, M. R., Shaiman, S., and Pratt, S. (2005). Phonetic encoding of infrequent articulatory phonetic transitions. Aphasiology 19, 39–52. doi: 10.1080/02687030444000606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laganaro, M. (2019). Phonetic encoding in utterance production: a review of open issues from 1989 to 2018. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 1193–1201. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1599128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. D. (1988). “Transfer-appropriate processing: a framework for conceptualizing practice effects” in Complex movement behaviour: The motor-action controversy. eds. O. G. Meijer and K. Roth (North Holland, Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers BV), 201–215.

Google Scholar

Lee, T. D., Wulf, G., and Schmidt, R. A. (1992). Contextual interference in motor learning: dissociated effects due to the nature of task variations. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 627–644. doi: 10.1080/14640749208401303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., and Meyers, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 1–38. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. -H., Yang, H. -C., Knowlton, B. J., Wu, A. D., Iacoboni, M., Ye, Y. -L., et al. (2018). Contextual interference enhances motor learning through increased resting brain connectivity during memory consolidation. NeuroImage 181, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q., Kawamoto, A. H., Payne, K. K., and Dorsey, G. N. (2018). Anticipatory coarticulation and the minimal planning unit of speech. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44, 139–153. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maas, E., Robin, D. A., Austermann Hula, S. N., Freedman, S. E., Wulf, G., Ballard, K. J., et al. (2008). Principles of motor learning in treatment of motor speech disorders. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 17, 277–298. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/025)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magill, R. A., and Hall, K. G. (1990). A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Hum. Mov. Sci. 9, 241–289. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(90)90005-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailend, M. -L., Maas, E., Beeson, P. M., Story, B. H., and Forster, K. I. (2019). Speech motor planning in the context of phonetically similar words: evidence from apraxia of speech and aphasia. Neuropsychologia 127, 171–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meigh, K. M. (2017). A novel investigation of GMP theory: syllable stress as a motor class variable. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 1685–1694. doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meigh, K. M., Shaiman, S., Tompkins, C. A., Katherine, V. A., and Nokes-Malach, T. (2018). What memory representation is acquired during nonword speech production learning? The influence of stimulus features and training modality on nonword encoding. Cogent Psychol. 5:1493714. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2018.1493714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munson, B., and Solomon, N. P. (2004). The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 1048–1058. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/078)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishi, K., and Kewley-Port, D. (2007). Training Japanese listeners to perceive American English vowels: influence of training sets. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 1496–1509. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/103)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishi, K., and Kewley-Port, D. (2008). Nonnative speech perception training using vowel subsets: effects of vowels in sets and order of training. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 51, 1480–1493. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0109)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peramunage, D., Blumstein, S. E., Myers, E. B., Goldrick, M., and Baese-Berk, M. (2011). Phonological neighborhood effects in spoken word production: an FMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 593–603. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, M. A., and Storkel, H. L. (1998). Reprogramming phonologically similar utterances: the role of phonetic features in pre-motor encoding. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41, 258–274. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4102.258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheiner, L. R., Sadagopan, N., and Sherwood, D. E. (2014). Effects of blocked versus random practice on speech motor skill acquisition and retention. J. Mot. Learn. Devel. 2, 29–36. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2013-0028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., and Zuccolotto, A. (2002). Eprime user’s guide (version 2.0). Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.

Google Scholar

Secord, W. (1981). Test of minimal articulation competence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Shea, J. B., and Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 5, 179–187. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.5.2.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shriberg, L. D., and Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders III: a procedure for assessing severity of involvement. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 47, 256–270. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4703.256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, R. I. (2018). High variability [pronunciation] training (HVPT): a proven technique about which every language teacher and learner ought to know. J. Second Lang. Pronun. 4, 208–231. doi: 10.1075/jslp.17038.tho

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillman, T. W., and Carhart, R. (1966). An expanded test for speech discrimination utilizing CNC monosyllabic words: Northwestern University auditory test no. 6. DTIC document. Tech. Rep. SAM-TR, 1–12. doi: 10.21236/ad0639638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitevitch, M. S. (2002). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 28, 735–747. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitevitch, M. S., and Luce, P. A. (2004). A web-based Interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 481–487. doi: 10.3758/bf03195594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., and Pearson, N. A. (2013). CTOPP-2: Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

Watson, P. J., and Munson, B. (2007). “A comparison of vowel acoustics between older and younger adults” in Proceedings of the 16th international congress of phonetic sciences. 561–564.

Google Scholar

Wilshire, C. E. (1999). The ‘tongue twister’ paradigm as a technique for studying phonological encoding. Lang. Speech 42, 57–82. doi: 10.1177/00238309990420010301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, A. W. -K., Whitehill, T. L., Ma, E. P. -M., and Masters, R. (2013). Effects of practice schedules on speech motor learning. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 15, 511–523. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2012.761282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, R. (2004). “Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation” in Papers in laboratory phonology VI. eds. J. Local, R. Ogden, and R. Temple (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 75–87.

Google Scholar

Yaniv, I., Meyer, D. E., Gordon, P. C., Huff, C. A., and Sevald, C. A. (1990). Vowel similarity, connectionist models, and syllable structure in motor programming of speech. J. Mem. Lang. 29, 1–26. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(90)90007-m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: speech, motor learning, contextual interference, phoneme, phonemic complexity

Citation: Meigh KM and Kee E (2020) Dissimilar Phonemes Create a Contextual Interference Effect During a Nonword Repetition Task. Front. Psychol. 11:585745. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.585745

Received: 21 July 2020; Accepted: 12 October 2020;
Published: 10 November 2020.

Edited by:

Pol Ghesquière, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

LouAnn Gerken, University of Arizona, United States
F. Sayako Earle, University of Delaware, United States

Copyright © 2020 Meigh and Kee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kimberly M. Meigh, kmmeigh@mail.wvu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.