Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 December 2020
Sec. Psychology of Language

Sentence Context Differentially Modulates Contributions of Fundamental Frequency Contours to Word Recognition in Chinese-Speaking Children With and Without Dyslexia

  • 1Beijing Advanced Innovation Center for Language Resources and College of Advanced Chinese Training, Beijing Language and Culture University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Applied Psychology, Beijing Normal University-Hong Kong Baptist University United International College, Zhuhai, China
  • 3International Cultural Exchange School, Shanghai University of Finance and Economics, Shanghai, China
  • 4Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 5Center for Neurobehavioral Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 6National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Previous work has shown that children with dyslexia are impaired in speech recognition in adverse listening conditions. Our study further examined how semantic context and fundamental frequency (F0) contours contribute to word recognition against interfering speech in dyslexic and non-dyslexic children. Thirty-two children with dyslexia and 35 chronological-age-matched control children were tested on the recognition of words in normal sentences versus wordlist sentences with natural versus flat F0 contours against single-talker interference. The dyslexic children had overall poorer recognition performance than non-dyslexic children. Furthermore, semantic context differentially modulated the effect of F0 contours on the recognition performances of the two groups. Specifically, compared with flat F0 contours, natural F0 contours increased the recognition accuracy of dyslexic children less than non-dyslexic children in the wordlist condition. By contrast, natural F0 contours increased the recognition accuracy of both groups to a similar extent in the sentence condition. These results indicate that access to semantic context improves the effect of natural F0 contours on word recognition in adverse listening conditions by dyslexic children who are more impaired in the use of natural F0 contours during isolated and unrelated word recognition. Our findings have practical implications for communication with dyslexic children when listening conditions are unfavorable.

Introduction

Although developmental dyslexia is characterized by difficulties in reading and spelling, speech perception deficits have often been reported in dyslexic individuals, especially under adverse or challenging listening conditions. At the phonemic level, a number of studies on categorical perception have showed that phonetic continua were less categorically perceived in dyslexic children than in age-matched controls, suggesting imprecise representations of phoneme categories in dyslexia (Hazan and Barrett, 2000; Blomert et al., 2004; Bogliotti et al., 2008; Cheung et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2012). At the syllabic level, dyslexic children showed deficits in the identification of naturally produced nonsense disyllables presented in various noisy backgrounds but not in quiet (Ziegler et al., 2009). At the word level, presence of difficulties has also been confirmed in recognition of isolated words in noise by dyslexic adults and children (Brady et al., 1983; Boets et al., 2007; Dole et al., 2012). Taken together, the previous studies indicate that such speech perception difficulties in dyslexia are subtle, which may not be seen in quiet listening conditions partly due to the redundant acoustic cues in speech signals, but tend to emerge in adverse listening conditions with degraded or ambiguous acoustic information.

Previous research has mostly focused on the perception of individual phonemes and isolated words although sentence-level performance would be a better reflection of the linguistic skills for speech communication in natural settings than test results with phonemes and words presented in isolation. Sentence recognition requires extra context-driven semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic processing, which is obviated in the recognition of isolated words. The intelligibility advantage of words in sentences over words in isolation when presented in adverse listening conditions has been confirmed by a number of studies on adults and children without dyslexia (e.g., Dubno et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2017). However, only one study has examined sentence-in-noise recognition by dyslexic children to the best of our knowledge. In Nittrouer et al. (2018), dyslexic children aged 7.8 to 12.7 years were required to repeat short sentences consisting of four words presented with speech-shaped noise background. The recognition scores of dyslexic children were lower than those of non-dyslexic children, indicating that speech-in-noise perception in dyslexia is impaired at the sentence level. The sentence materials in their study, however, were specially designed. Specifically, the sentences have correct syntactic structures but lack strong or predictable semantic contextual cues (e.g., Great shelf needs tape). That is, knowledge of morphological and syntactic structures is the main linguistic influence on recognition beyond sensory information. Therefore, it remains unclear whether dyslexic and non-dyslexic children differ in their use of semantic context to aid sentence recognition in adverse listening conditions. In a previous study that adopted phoneme identification task to assess the bias toward real words rather than nonsense words, dyslexic children have been found to rely more on word knowledge to recognize phonemes than non-dyslexic children, indicating that dyslexic children could use some higher-level processing strategies to compensate for their lower-level perceptual difficulties (Reed, 1989). Therefore, one might suspect that the speech perception difficulties in dyslexic children at the level of isolated words may be attenuated when the children are tested with sentences in which semantic context is available.

Speech-in-noise recognition is affected by fundamental frequency (F0) contours irrespective of whether the target language is a tonal or non-tonal language. Specifically, natural F0 contours improve intelligibility of speech presented against interference compared with unnaturally inverted or flattened F0 contours (Laures and Bunton, 2003; Binns and Culling, 2007; Watson and Schlauch, 2008; Miller et al., 2010; Patel et al., 2010). Furthermore, the effect of F0 contours on speech intelligibility in adverse listening conditions can be modulated by semantic context. In our previous study (Wang et al., 2013), intelligibility of normal sentence and wordlist sentence (a sentence composed of pseudorandomly arranged words but syntactically correct) with natural and flat F0 contours was examined in adult listeners without dyslexia. The key difference between normal sentence and wordlist sentence is that normal sentence provides sentential semantic context whereas wordlist sentence strips away that context by arranging random lexical items without coherent semantics. The contrastive manipulation of natural and flat F0 contours is that the former does not make changes to the natural dynamic F0 contours whereas the latter flattens all F0 contours, making all the words in the sentence monotonous. Our findings showed that intelligibility increased to a greater extent for normal sentence than wordlist sentence presented at various signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) when F0 patterns changed from flat contours to natural contours. Previous studies have revealed that non-tonal language speaking children with dyslexia have impaired abilities to accurately use prosodic properties to disambiguate linguistic structures and to identify the stressed word within an utterance (Marshall et al., 2009). Similarly, Chinese-speaking children with dyslexia are impaired in using F0 contours to identify lexical tones (Cheung et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2012). However, whether the detrimental effect of F0 perception deficit on speech recognition in dyslexia is attenuated by semantic context remains unexplored.

The aim of the present study was to explore the effects of semantic context and F0 contours on word recognition against interfering speech by comparing Chinese-speaking dyslexic children with chronological age-matched controls. The experiment followed the design of our previous speech intelligibility studies (Jiang et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2017) by manipulating two factors, namely, semantic context (normal sentence versus wordlist sentence) and F0 contours (utterances with natural versus flat contours). One possible outcome is that semantic context might be similarly used by dyslexic and non-dyslexic children to aid word recognition regardless of the acoustic manipulation. Another possible outcome is that natural F0 contours might be differentially used by dyslexic and non-dyslexic children during word recognition due to potential deficits in the dyslexic individuals to use F0 contours for word segmentation and lexical tone identification. As no previous research has investigated this issue, it remains indeterminate whether semantic context differentially modulates the effect of F0 contours on word recognition between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children, which constituted the primary motivation for this study.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-seven children were recruited for the study, with 32 showing typical sequelae of developmental dyslexia (18 boys and 14 girls) and 35 being typically developing children (21 boys and 14 girls). All children were normal primary and middle school students in Beijing and were free of neurological, psychiatric, or hearing disorders including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) according to parental reports. They all had normal hearing with the threshold of both ears below 25 dB for octave frequencies between 125 and 8000 Hz. The non-dyslexic children were matched with the dyslexics on age and on non-verbal intelligence (according to Raven’s performance IQ). Informed consent was obtained from each of the participants or legal guardians prior to participation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning at Beijing Normal University.

Diagnosis of Dyslexia

According to the well-established criteria (Shu et al., 2006; Lei et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2012), three literacy subtests including character recognition, wordlist reading and reading fluency were adopted to identify children with dyslexia. During the character recognition subtest, children were asked to read aloud 150 characters with all learned by Grade 6 primary school children (Shu et al., 2003). During the wordlist reading subtest, children were required to name as quickly as possible 180 words with each of the words composed of two simple and familiar characters. In both subtests, the testing stopped when a child failed 15 items consecutively. Each correctly read character/two-character word was worth one point. During the timed reading fluency subtest that followed the procedure of Landerl et al. (2009), children were asked to read sentences as quickly as possible and judge whether the facts stated in each sentence was correct or not within the time limit of 3 min. There were, in total, 100 sentences with gradually increasing length across the subtest. All the characters in the correctly judged sentences were summed, and each character was worth one point. For inclusion in the dyslexia group, a child would need to have scored one or more standard deviations below the respective age means in all the three subtests. Performance mean scores and standard deviations on the screening subtests are summarized in Table 1 for the two groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics, reading, and cognitive measures (±SD) of dyslexic and non-dyslexic children.

Word Recognition Test

Stimuli

The speech materials were used in our previous studies with native Japanese speakers learning Chinese as a foreign language (Zhang et al., 2016) and Chinese school-age children (Zhou et al., 2017). Cronbach’s alpha values ranged from 0.81 to 0.89 for different types of stimuli in all the studies including the present one, reflecting high internal consistency of each type of stimuli. Specifically, there are four types of stimuli in a 2 × 2 design of variations in semantic context (normal sentence versus wordlist sentence) and F0 contours (natural F0 contours versus flat F0 contours) (Figure 1). The normal sentences consisted of 28 simple declarative Chinese sentences. The wordlist sentences were constructed by pseudorandomly selecting words from the word pool of normal sentences and thus were syntactically correct but lack semantic meanings at the whole sentence level. Normal sentences and wordlist sentences were matched in number of words. Both types of sentences were read by a male native Chinese speaker in a soundproof room with a 44.1-kHz sampling frequency rate and 16-bit digitization. Stimuli with flat F0 contours were implemented using Praat1 to flatten the original F0 contours at the mean F0 value of each sentence while preserving other prosodic features such as amplitude envelope and duration. Consonant-misplaced sentences were used as masker sentences to provide energetic masking because they were lexically not meaningful and syntactically incorrect (Xu et al., 2013). The masker sentences were read by a female native Chinese speaker in order to enable the participants to separate easily the target message from the interfering speech. The target and masker sentences were mixed pairwise at the SNR level of +5 dB.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Acoustic properties of example speech stimuli. Broadband spectrograms (SPG, range: 0–5 kHz), intensity envelopes (INT, range: 50–100 dB) and F0 contours (range: 50–250 Hz; blue: natural; purple, flat) are displayed for (A) normal sentence and the pitch-flattened counterpart, and (B) word list sentence and the pitch-flattened counterpart. Syllables are marked in pinyin (the Chinese phonetic transcription system).

Procedures

The experimental protocol followed our previous studies (Wang et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2017). In a quiet room with ambient noise level no higher than 15 dB sound pressure level, children listened to the stimuli and were required to report orally what they heard. The stimuli were presented through a set of loudspeakers (Edifier R18) with the sound pressure level set at 65 dB calibrated at the listener’s head. Because the sentences with flat F0 contours were created based on those with natural F0 contours, we adopted a counterbalanced design to control for possible stimulus order effect. Specifically, each child was presented with half of the total (14/28) stimuli with flat F0 contours and the other half with natural F0 contours. Therefore, each child listened to 56 trials in total with 14 trials in each of the four types of stimuli. All the stimuli were presented randomly across listeners with each stimulus heard only once. The task was self-paced and practice was carried out before the actual experiment to familiarize the children with all types of stimuli.

A digital voice recorder was used to record children’s spoken responses. The first author of the present study then scored the responses offline according to a strict score standard. Specifically, the response was considered correct only when each segmental and suprasegmental phoneme of a word was correctly identified by the children. An independent linguist who was blind to the aim of the study checked the scoring and differed in scoring for only five words across all the trials and all the subjects. The two raters reexamined and discussed to resolve the scoring discrepancies for the five words.

Results

Speech recognition accuracy Table 2 was computed based on a keyword-correct count. Specifically, the number of keywords (i.e., content words) identified correctly by each child was counted and converted to a percentage of the total number of words (Scott et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2013). A 2 × 2 × 2 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with group (dyslexic children versus non-dyslexic controls) as the between-subject factor and semantic context (normal sentence versus wordlist sentence) and F0 contours (natural F0 contours versus flat F0 contours) as the within-subject factors. Results demonstrated that all the three main effects were highly significant: semantic context, F(1,65) = 189.567, p = 6.12e−21, partial η2 = 0.745; F0 contours, F(1,65) = 277.133, p = 3.948e−25, partial η2 = 0.81; group, F(1,65) = 22.554, p = 1.167e−5, partial η2 = 0.258. These results indicate that both natural F0 contours and sentential semantic context contribute to better speech recognition by children with and without dyslexia. More importantly, compared with non-dyslexic peers, dyslexic children are impaired in word recognition against interfering speech.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean accuracy (±SD) of each condition for the two groups.

ANOVA tests further revealed no significance for all the two-way interaction effects [semantic context × F0 contours, F(1,65) = 0.067, p = 0.796, partial η2 = 0.0001; semantic context × group, F(1,65) = 0.374, p = 0.543, partial η2 = 0.006; F0 contours × group, F(1,65) = 1.739, p = 0.192, partial η2 = 0.026]. However, there was a significant three-way interaction effect between group, semantic context, and F0 contours [F(1,65) = 4.586, p = 0.036, partial η2 = 0.066]. Further analyses, therefore, focused on the significant three-way interaction. Specifically, we decomposed this interaction effect step by step (Maxwell and Delaney, 2004). Firstly, separate two-way ANOVAs were conducted to examine the simple interaction effects. Significant interaction effect between group and F0 contours was revealed in the wordlist sentence condition [F(1,65) = 6.871, p = 0.011, partial η2 = 0.096], but absent in the normal sentence condition [F(1,65) = 0.107, p = 0.745, partial η2 = 0.002], indicating that semantic context modulates the effect of F0 contours on word recognition by dyslexic versus non-dyslexic children (Figure 2). Second, follow-up analyses showed that for both wordlist sentence and normal sentence, stimuli with natural F0 contours were recognized better than those with flat F0 contours, and moreover, non-dyslexic children performed better than their peers on the recognition of stimuli with natural and flat F0 contours, consistent with the significant main effects of F0 contours and group revealed by the original three-way ANOVA.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Word-report accuracies of the simple interaction effects carried out on the significant three-way interaction between group, semantic context, and F0 contours. Error bars represent standard deviation.

Discussion

The current study investigated word recognition against single-talker interfering speech by Chinese-speaking children with dyslexia compared with chronological-age-matched controls. Our results revealed clear evidence for impairment in word recognition in the dyslexic children, and furthermore, semantic context differentially modulated the effect of F0 contours on word recognition between the two groups.

In the present study, dyslexic children showed much lower performance on the recognition of normal sentence. Specifically, the recognition rates of normal sentence with natural and flat F0 contours were 74 and 54% in the dyslexic children in contrast to 91 and 72% in the non-dyslexic children. Together with findings of previous studies on the perception of various smaller linguistic units including phonemes, nonsense syllables, and isolated words (Brady et al., 1983; Hazan and Barrett, 2000; Blomert et al., 2004; Boets et al., 2007; Bogliotti et al., 2008; Ziegler et al., 2009; Dole et al., 2012), our results confirmed that in addition to difficulties in reading and spelling, the overall abilities in speech perception is impaired in dyslexia under adverse listening conditions. The beneficial role of semantic context in word recognition by non-dyslexic children has been well established in previous research. For example, children of 4 years old detected mispronounced words in high-predictability sentences better than in low-predictability sentences (Cole and Perfetti, 1980), and children of 7 to 11 years old repeated target words in semantically appropriate contexts more rapidly than in semantically anomalous sentences (Liu et al., 1997). In the present study, the highly significant main effect of semantic context together with the absence of interaction between group and semantic context revealed that dyslexic children used sentential context information to aid word recognition to a similar extent as non-dyslexic children did, thus confirming our prediction.

In tonal languages like Chinese, lexical tones are suprasegmental features, which are phonologically as important as segmental phonemes. As the primary acoustic correlates of Chinese lexical tones, natural F0 contours play a very important role in word recognition, especially when speech is presented in suboptimal listening conditions or without contextual information (Patel et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2017). Our previous study revealed that dyslexic children aged 9–11 years were able to use F0 contours to identify Chinese lexical tones of isolated syllables presented in quiet although their recognition rates were significantly lower than those of age-matched children without dyslexia (Zhang et al., 2012). In the present study, the highly significant main effect of F0 contours together with the absence of interaction effect between group and F0 contours revealed that even in a suboptimal listening condition, dyslexic children showed sensitivity to dynamic F0 contours to a similar extent as non-dyslexic children did. More importantly, the significant three-way interaction further revealed that semantic context differentially modulated the effect of F0 contours on word recognition between the two groups. Specifically, in the wordlist condition, dyslexic children showed a smaller recognition advantage of natural F0 contours over flat F0 contours compared with non-dyslexic controls, but in the normal sentence condition, both groups showed a similar recognition advantage of natural F0 contours. Thus, these results extended our previous findings by indicating that semantic context helps dyslexic children compensate to some extent for the perceptual deficit in F0 contours of isolated syllables and unrelated words. This means that dyslexic children benefited more from semantic context than non-dyslexic children in the use of natural F0 contours to assist word recognition, reflecting the interactive contributions of top-down semantic processing and bottom-up phonetic processing to word recognition by dyslexic children at the SNR level (i.e., +5 dB) adopted in the present study.

The current results are limited in several aspects. Firstly, no linguistic subskills (e.g., phonological awareness and vocabulary) or cognitive competences (e.g., short-term memory and working memory) were measured. Therefore, it is unclear whether the overall difficulties in word recognition against single-talker interference simply reflects one component of the broadly based language deficits in dyslexia or the deficits in speech perception and visual forms of language arise from a single factor (e.g., auditory dysfunction versus phonological awareness as suggested by many previous studies on dyslexia) (Tallal, 1980; Snowling, 2000; Ramus et al., 2003; Vellutino et al., 2004; Nittrouer et al., 2011). Our results also did not address how various interrelated linguistic and cognitive skills benefit, separately or interactively with semantic context, the use of natural F0 contours during word recognition by dyslexic children. Secondly, the interfering speech used in the present study are consonant-misplaced sentences that are syntactically incorrect and semantically anomalous at the sentence level. One concern might arise with the masking effects of this specific type of stimuli on the current results. Different kinds of maskers (e.g., white noise, speech-shaped noise, one- and multi-talker babbles and time-reversed speech) have different effects on speech recognition because they differ to a great extent in distracting listeners and masking targets (Garcia Lecumberri and Cooke, 2006; Dole et al., 2012). Furthermore, male and female voices were designated to be the target and masker stimuli, respectively, to make the instructions easy to follow for our participants. Further research using different kinds of interfering sounds at various SNRs and speech of the same and different genders as target and masker are necessary to clarify how the effects of semantic and F0 contours on word recognition by dyslexic children is affected by characteristics of targets and maskers. Thirdly, reading experience may likely be reduced in dyslexics relative to controls, which would conceivably contribute to the experimental outcomes. A further complication is that the current findings might be affected to some extent by comorbidity of dyslexia and ADHD in some of the children as we only relied on parental reports without administering a strict diagnostic test to exclude children with ADHD. Previous research has indicated that comorbidity of dyslexia and ADHD can lead to deficits in language processes such as phonological awareness, listening comprehension, and verbal working-memory (Tannock et al., 2000; McInnes et al., 2003; Tiffin-Richards et al., 2008). How semantic context and natural F0 contours may affect word recognition differently in dyslexic children with and without ADHD warrants further investigation. Finally, caution is needed in interpreting the significant three-way interaction effect due to the small effect size. Future studies with a larger sample size and more specific inclusion and exclusion criteria are needed to verify the different effects of semantic context and F0 contours on word recognition in children with dyslexia.

Conclusion

The current results demonstrate the important roles of semantic context and natural F0 contours in word recognition by dyslexic children. Our findings imply that one effective means of enhancing speech intelligibility under adverse listening conditions during communication with dyslexic children is to introduce modification of the talker’s speech. Specifically, intelligibility of speech directed at dyslexic children may be enhanced through changes in the speaking style with an emphasis on the use of dynamic F0 contours and the sentential semantic context with immediate beneficial effects.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning at Beijing Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

LZ, YL, and HZ: conceptualization, data collection, software, analysis, and manuscript preparation. HS and YZ: conceptualization and manuscript preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Social Science Fund of Beijing (17YYA004), the National Social Science Fund of China (20BYY092), and the Discipline Team Support Program of Beijing Language and Culture University (JC201901) to LZ, and the Humanities and Social Sciences Fund of Ministry of Education of China (20YJCZH079) to YL.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/

References

Binns, C., and Culling, J. F. (2007). The role of fundamental frequency contours in the perception of speech against interfering speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122, 1765–1776. doi: 10.1121/1.2751394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomert, L., Mitterer, H., and Paffen, C. (2004). In search of the auditory, phonetic, and/or phonological problems in dyslexia: context effects in speech perception. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 1030–1047. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/077)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boets, B., Ghesquiere, P., vanWieringen, A., and Wouters, J. (2007). Speech perception in preschoolers at family risk for dyslexia: relations with low-level auditory processing and phonological ability. Brain Lang. 101, 19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., and Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 137–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, S., Shankweiler, D., and Mann, V. A. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 35, 345–367. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(83)90087-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, H., Chung, K. K., Wong, S. W., McBride-Chang, C., Penney, T. B., and Ho, C. S. (2009). Perception of tone and aspiration contrasts in Chinese children with dyslexia. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 726–733. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02001.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, R. A., and Perfetti, C. A. (1980). Listening for mispronunciations in a children’s story: the use of context by children and adults. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 19, 297–315. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90239-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dole, M., Hoen, M., and Meunier, F. (2012). Speech-in-noise perception deficit in adults with dyslexia: effects of background type and listening configuration. Neuropsychologia 50, 1543–1552. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubno, J. R., Ahlstrom, J. B., and Horwitz, A. R. (2000). Use of context by young and aged adults with normal hearing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107, 538–546. doi: 10.1121/1.428322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia Lecumberri, M. L., and Cooke, M. (2006). Effect of masker type on native and non-native consonant perception in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119, 2445–2454. doi: 10.1121/1.2180210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, V., and Barrett, S. (2000). The development of phonemic categorization in children aged 6–12. J. Phon. 28, 377–396. doi: 10.1006/jpho.2000.0121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, W., Li, Y., Shu, H., Zhang, L., and Zhang, Y. (2017). Use of semantic context and F0 contours by older listeners during Mandarin speech recognition in quiet and single-talker interference conditions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141, EL338–EL344. doi: 10.1121/1.4979565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., and Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 309–324. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laures, J. S., and Bunton, K. (2003). Perceptual effects of a flattened fundamental frequency at the sentence level under different listening conditions. J. Commun. Disord. 36, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00032-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, L., Pan, J., Liu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Li, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2011). Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 212–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02311.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Bates, E., Powell, T., and Wulfeck, B. (1997). Single-word shadowing and the study of lexical access. Appl. Psycholinguist. 18, 157–180. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400009954

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, C. R., Harcourt-Brown, S., Ramus, F., and van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). The link between prosody and language skills in children with SLI and/or dyslexia. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 44, 466–488. doi: 10.1080/13682820802591643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, S. E., and Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective, 2nd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

McInnes, A., Humphries, T., Hogg-Johnson, S., and Tannock, R. (2003). Listening comprehension and working memory are impaired in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder irrespective of language impairment. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 31, 427–443. doi: 10.1023/a:1023895602957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S., Schlauch, R., and Watson, P. (2010). The effects of fundamental frequency contour manipulations on speech intelligibility in background noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 128, 435–343. doi: 10.1121/1.3397384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nittrouer, S., Krieg, L. M., and Lowenstein, J. H. (2018). Speech recognition in noise by children with and without dyslexia: how is it related to reading? Res. Dev. Disabil. 77, 98–113. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nittrouer, S., Shune, S., and Lowenstein, J. H. (2011). What is the deficit in phonological processing deficits: auditory sensitivity, masking, or category formation? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 762–785. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., Xu, Y., and Wang, B. (2010). “The role of F0 variation in the intelligibility of Mandarin sentences,” in Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010, Chicago, IL.

Google Scholar

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 126(Pt 4), 841–865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, M. A. (1989). Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in reading disabled children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 48, 270–292. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(89)90006-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. K., Rosen, S., Wickham, L., and Wise, R. J. (2004). A positron emission tomography study of the neural basis of informational and energetic masking effects in speech perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 813–821. doi: 10.1121/1.1639336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., and Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: implications for learning to read. Child Dev. 74, 27–47. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., and Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 122–133. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain Lang. 9, 182–198. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(80)90139-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tannock, R., Martinussen, R., and Frijters, J. (2000). Naming speed performance and stimulant effects indicate effortful, semantic processing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28, 237–252. doi: 10.1023/a:1005192220001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiffin-Richards, M., Hasselhorn, M., Woerner, W., Rothenberger, A., and Banaschewski, T. (2008). Phonological short-term memory and central executive processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with/without dyslexia—evidence of cognitive overlap. J. Neural. Transm. 115, 227–234. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0816-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., and Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 2–40. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Shu, H., Zhang, L., Liu, Z., and Zhang, Y. (2013). The roles of fundamental frequency contours and sentence context in Mandarin Chinese speech intelligibility. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134, EL91–EL97. doi: 10.1121/1.4811159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, P., and Schlauch, R. (2008). The effect of fundamental frequency on the intelligibility of speech with flattened intonation contours. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 17, 348–355. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/07-0048)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, G., Zhang, L., Shu, H., Wang, X., and Li, P. (2013). Access to lexical meaning in pitch-flattened Chinese sentences: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 51, 550–556. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Li, Y., Wu, H., Li, X., Shu, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2016). Effects of semantic context and fundamental frequency contours on Mandarin speech recognition by second language learners. Front. Psychol. 7:908. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Zhang, L., Shu, H., Xi, J., Wu, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2012). Universality of categorical perception deficit in developmental dyslexia: an investigation of Mandarin Chinese tones. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 874–882. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02528.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, H., Li, Y., Liang, M., Guan, C., Zhang, L., Shu, H., et al. (2017). Mandarin-speaking children’s speech recognition: developmental changes in the influences of semantic context and F0 Contours. Front. Psychol. 8:1090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., and Lorenzi, C. (2009). Speech-perception-in-noise deficits in dyslexia. Dev. Sci. 12, 732–745. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00817.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dyslexia, word recognition, semantic context, F0 contours, noise

Citation: Zhang L, Li Y, Zhou H, Zhang Y and Shu H (2020) Sentence Context Differentially Modulates Contributions of Fundamental Frequency Contours to Word Recognition in Chinese-Speaking Children With and Without Dyslexia. Front. Psychol. 11:598658. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.598658

Received: 25 August 2020; Accepted: 05 November 2020;
Published: 03 December 2020.

Edited by:

David Saldaña, University of Seville, Spain

Reviewed by:

Yang Yang, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Anthony James Krafnick, Dominican University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Zhang, Li, Zhou, Zhang and Shu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hong Zhou, hellozhouhong@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.