Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 31 March 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Socially Situated? Effects of Social and Cultural Context on Language Processing and Learning View all 16 articles

Effects of Dispositional Affect on the N400: Language Processing and Socially Situated Context

  • 1Department of Psychology and Centre for Neuroscience, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
  • 2Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

We examined whether the N400 Event-Related Potential (ERP) component would be modulated by dispositional affect during sentence processing. In this study, 33 participants read sentences manipulated by direct object type (congruent vs. incongruent) and object determiner type (definite vs. demonstrative). We were particularly interested in sentences of the form: (i) The connoisseur tasted the wine on the tour vs. (ii) The connoisseur tasted the # roof We expected that processing incongruent direct objects (#roof) vs. congruent objects (wine) would elicit N400 effects. Previous ERP language experiments have shown that participants in (induced) positive and negative moods were differentially sensitive to semantic anomaly, resulting in different N400 effects. Presently, we ask whether individual dispositional affect scores (as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; PANAS) would modulate N400 effects as shown previously. Namely, previous results showed larger N400 effects associated with happy moods and attenuated amplitudes associated with sad moods. Results revealed significant N400 effects, driven by the #roof vs. the wine, where larger amplitude differences were found for individuals showing smaller negative affect (NA) scores, thus partially replicating previous findings. We discuss our results in terms of theories of local (lexical) inhibition, such that low NA promotes stronger lexico-semantic links in sentences. Finally, our results support accounts of language processing that include social and biological characteristics of individuals during real-time sentence comprehension.

Introduction

It is well established that individuals differ in their responses to identical stimuli. Whereas one individual perceives, and interprets a glass filled at the 50% mark as half-full, another perceives, and interprets it as half-empty. A common-sense explanation for these differences is that the personality and/or mood of the perceiver plays a role in the above-mentioned interpretive possibilities.

The idea that the emotional mood of the perceiver may influence interpretation is supported by research in cognitive psychology (see, inter alia, Ashby et al., 1999; Gasper and Clore, 2002; Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005; Dale and Arnell, 2010). For example, it has been claimed that positive moods are associated with greater global processing, such that individuals are more sensitive to top-down contextual knowledge. In contrast, negative moods tend to be associated with cognitive styles characterized by item specific processing, where attention is more focused on details (Kimchi and Palmer, 1982; Schwarz, 2002; Rowe et al., 2007). With respect to mood effects on language processing, Chwilla et al. (2011) conducted a series of studies using Event-Related Potential (ERP) methods (Vissers et al., 2010, 2013; see also Federmeier et al., 2001; van Berkum et al., 2013). They used a paradigm where participants were induced into happy or sad emotional moods by watching either happy or sad movies (e.g., short clips of either Happy Feet or Sophie’s Choice). Their studies showed that participants in happy moods exhibited modulation of the language-related ERP effects of interest, whereas sad moods attenuated ERP responses. Relevant to our current study, Chwilla et al. (2011) focused on the N400 component, a negative-going waveform that peaks approximately 400 ms after a word, which does not match previous sentential context in terms of lexico-semantic fit (Kutas and Hillyard, 1980, 1984). In that work, they examined sentences of the form “The pillows are stuffed with feathers…. vs. “The pillows are stuffed with books…” and showed that the latter sentence with the low-cloze fragment showed larger N400 effects for happy vs. sad participants. They interpreted their findings as consistent with literature on mood and cognition (see above, as well as Schwarz and Clore, 2007), where happy individuals were more sensitive to global features of stimuli. That is, global features of sentence interpretation are associated with stereotypical event knowledge, also called script or schemata (cf. Schank and Abelson, 1977; St. George et al., 1994; Chwilla and Kolk, 2005). Finally, they concluded that their ERP results supported the recent shift in cognitive neuroscience that views cognition as “hot” (i.e., not independent of mood) vs. “cold.”

In the present work, we build on the findings above, as well as recent work in our own lab (Selvanayagam et al., 2019) and ask whether the related construct of dispositional affect would modulate sentence interpretation as measured via the N400 component.

In contrast to mood, which can fluctuate according to situation, dispositional affect reflects the stability across time and situation of individuals to view their world with approach-oriented positive affect (PA), or avoidance-oriented negative affect (NA). That is, it is a personality trait, reflective of individual differences (Watson and Clark, 1984; Staw and Ross, 1985; Levin and Stokes, 1989).

As such, we can consider dispositional affect of an individual as a socially relevant and arguably biological characteristic of an individual during language comprehension. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) indexes trait and state features of affect and mood. Given that it has been shown that there are individual differences in temperaments that can be more or less susceptible to mood induction (Larsen and Ketelaar, 1989, 1991; Brief et al., 1995), we ask whether dispositional affect, as measured by the PANAS, modulated N400 responses to sentences. In a preliminary investigation in our lab (Selvanayagam et al., 2019), we showed that the P300 effect was indeed influenced by affect. Results were consistent with the view that less positive individuals, measured via the PANAS, were less sensitive to global heuristic cues of meaning.

In the current experiment, we focus on the N400 ERP component. We examined neural responses to words that were either congruent or incongruent with sentential context. That is, we contrasted sentences of the form The connoisseur tasted the wine during the tour vs. The connoisseur tasted the #roof1 during the tour. The direct object in the latter sentence clearly violates our real-world expectations of what possible objects of tasting might be. As such, the critical word roof should elicit an N400 effect vs. its control wine. Our predictions regarding dispositional affect, prima facie, would be to replicate the findings of the induced mood participants in Chwilla et al. (2011). That is, N400 effects would be larger for individuals exhibiting higher PA scores, given findings of larger N400 effects for happy participants. Furthermore, N400 effects would be smaller for individuals with higher NA scores, mirroring previous findings of attenuated N400 effects for sad participants. These findings are expected on the cognitive view that positive participants should be more sensitive to lexico-semantic cues regarding global sentence meaning (also called heuristics, Townsend and Bever, 2001; Ferreira, 2003; Dwivedi, 2013) vs. negative participants, who are not. When a word does not match its lexico-semantic context, the greater sensitivity of positive (vs. negative) participants to this mismatch should therefore elicit larger N400 effects.

As such, correlational analyses will be conducted between N400 amplitudes and positive and negative affect scores. We predict (i) a significant positive correlation between N400 amplitude and positive affect scores as well as (ii) a significant negative correlation between the size of the N400 effect and negative affect scores of our participants.

Next, we note here that a separate condition was included in this experiment to ask a question independent of dispositional affect and is discussed in detail elsewhere (Dwivedi and Selvanayagam, 2020a,b). Briefly, we wanted to know whether neural responses to lexico-semantic incongruency differed from those elicited via discourse semantic violations. We did so by examining “double violations” (Hagoort, 2003). As such, the other independent variable we examined was determiner type at object position, such as The connoisseur tasted *that2 #roof (without previous context, use of that results in presuppositional violation). In terms of our present study, the double violation condition might show the largest N400 effect, and if so, it too would correlate with positive and negative affect scores as above. See Table 1 for list of conditions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Critical conditions with example stimuli.

Methods

Participants

Thirty seven Brock University undergraduates were recruited and either paid for their participation or received partial course credit. All participants were native, monolingual speakers of English, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were right-handed, as assessed by the Handedness Inventory. No participants reported any neurological impairment, history of neurological trauma, or use of neuroleptics.

Four participants with comprehension question accuracy for filler items (discussed below) at less than 85% were excluded from analysis leaving 33 eligible participants (25 females; mean age = 19.6; ranging from 18 to 25).

This study received ethics approval from the Brock University Bioscience Research Ethics Board (BREB) prior to the commencement of the experiment (REB 13-282). Written, informed consent was received from all participants prior to their participation in the experiment.

Materials

160 critical items (adapted from Dwivedi and Gibson, 2017) were presented in four conditions (see Table 1) counterbalanced across four lists. All sentences in this experiment were simple active sentences, using SUBJECT VERB OBJECT word order, followed by a prepositional phrase. Sentences varied according to two factors: object type (congruent vs. incongruent object) and determiner type (definite vs. demonstrative). All subjects were animate (e.g., connoisseur, kid) and preceded by the definite determiner the. An active, past-tense verb followed the subject (e.g., tasted, climbed). All direct objects were inanimate (e.g., wine, roof) and were either congruent with sentence context in the control conditions (e.g., connoisseur – tasted – wine, kid – climbed – tree) or incongruent with sentential context (e.g., connoisseur – tasted – roof, kid – climbed – jade). Direct objects were not repeated and were matched for word length item by item (e.g., wine vs. #roof). Next, word frequency for direct objects in congruent vs. incongruent conditions was controlled for, where log word frequencies (SUBTLEX-US database; see Brysbaert and New, 2009) indicated no significant difference, t(159) = 0.63, p = 0.533. Also, the direct object was never the final word in the sentence. Sentences ended with prepositional phrases that (crucially) did not alter the interpretation of the direct object. Instead, these phrases served to modify the event by referring to time (e.g., in the morning), manner (e.g., with difficulty), and instrument (e.g., with a pen). Comprehension questions did not follow presentation of critical trials (stimuli available upon request).

To reduce predictability, 170 filler sentences were included, of varying syntactic and semantic structure.3 These sentences were 6–10 words in length and a subset of these (125 items, 38% of all trials) were followed by superficial Yes/No or True/False comprehension questions.

Offline Plausibility Ratings

We evaluated the plausibility of our critical materials by conducting a norming study using Qualtrics software, Version (March 2020) of the Qualtrics Research Suite (Qualtrics, 2020). Critical and filler sentences were rated in this web-based study according to plausibility on a scale from 0 (very implausible) to 5 (neutral) to 10 (very plausible), in steps of 0.1. The 160 critical items were presented in eight pseudorandomized, counterbalanced lists such that half of the critical items were presented in each list and each participant only saw each item once. 80 filler items were presented in all lists, for a total 160 items in each list. 34 participants completed the study, of which 30 met the eligibility criteria described above (as outlined in Section “Participants”). 10 participants were excluded for having a mean plausibility rating lower than seven on filler items (all of which were perfectly plausible). Data from the remaining 20 participants (20 females; mean age = 18.65; ranging from 18 to 25) were used to calculate plausibility ratings. A few trials had to be excluded due to software error (<0.3% of trials). Mean plausibility ratings for the critical conditions were: congruent definite (the wine; M = 8.43, SD = 0.85), congruent demonstrative (that wine; M = 7.99, SD = 0.99), incongruent definite (the roof; M = 2.25, SD = 2.10); incongruent demonstrative (that roof; M = 2.05, SD = 2.07). An ANOVA was conducted on the mean plausibility ratings with the independent variables of object type (congruent vs. incongruent), determiner type (definite vs. demonstrative). Significant main effects of object type, F(1, 19) = 220.7, MSE = 3.33, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.920, and determiner type, F(1, 19) = 6.00, MSE = 0.34, p = 0.024, ƞp2 = 0.240 were observed. However, no significant interaction of object and determiner type was observed, F(1, 19) = 1.60, MSE = 0.17, p = 0.221, ƞp2 = 0.078. Overall, these results confirm the intended readings regarding congruent vs. incongruent sentences.

Electrophysiological Measures

Electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings were made using a 64-channel Active Two BioSemi system (BioSemi, Amsterdam). Data were sampled at a rate of 512 Hz and digitized with a 24-bit analog-to-digital converter. EEG data were preprocessed offline using EMSE v5.5.1 software (Cortech Solutions, 2013). Two infinite impulse response filters were applied at 12 db/octave: a bandpass filter from 0.1 to 100 Hz used to remove high and low frequency noise and a bandstop filter from 59 to 61 Hz used to remove 60 Hz noise. All electrodes were re-referenced to the averaged mastoids for analysis. Prior to segmentation, eye movements artifacts and blinks were filtered from the data using a spatial ocular artifact correction algorithm (Pflieger, 2001). Due to equipment malfunction, data from electrode Fp1 was lost in some participants. A spatial interpolation filter (Cortech Solutions, 2013) was applied for this electrode, for all participants. Manual artifact rejection was applied. Epochs were created from an interval 200 ms prior to stimulus onset to 1,200 ms after stimulus onset.

Procedure

Participants were tested individually in one session of approximately 3 h. In each session, participants completed a short questionnaire regarding reading habits, a handedness inventory (Briggs and Nebes, 1975), and the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988) before the application of the electrodes. PANAS consists of 20 items (10 positive items, e.g., interested, excited, and 10 negative items, e.g., distressed, upset) for which the participants provided a response on a five-point Likert scale indicating the extent to which they felt this way (1 = “Very slightly or not at all,” 5 = “Extremely”; see Watson et al., 1988 for further details). Following a practice session of eight trials, each participant completed the experimental trials in six blocks of 55 trials, with rest periods between each block. Each participant saw one of four pseudorandomized, counterbalanced lists consisting of 330 items. The pseudorandomized lists were created using the Mix utility (van Casteren and Davis, 2006) such that the first three items and last two items of each block were always filler sentences; no more than two critical items were presented sequentially and items from the same condition were never presented sequentially. Using E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA), sentences were presented in the center of the computer monitor (screen size 50.8 cm) in light gray, 18-point Courier New font on a black background at a viewing distance of approximately 70 cm. See Figure 1 for a sample trial procedure (Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 2.0], 2012).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Condensed sample trial for the current paradigm. Time values above the screen represent the duration of stimulus presentation on screen whereas time values below the screen represent the inter-stimulus interval (ISI). The “Ready?” slide requires participant input to proceed and is sometimes preceded by a comprehension question.

Each trial sentence began with the participant being prompted to press a button on the response pad, then the word “Blink” was presented for 1,000 ms, followed by a fixation cross (+) for 500 ms. After a variable inter-trial interval lasting between 200 and 400 ms, sentences were presented word-by-word in serial visual presentation mode with a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 600 ms and an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 200 ms. 125 filler items were followed by comprehension questions after the last word of the sentence, to which participants were asked to press a “1” or “2” key corresponding to answers on the screen using the response pad. Response time and accuracy was recorded for each response. The next trial began following another inter-trial interval lasting between 500 and 1,000 ms.

Results

Behavioral Analyses

Filler Comprehension Questions

Comprehension rates for questions at filler conditions were at 95.20% (SD = 2.75%), indicating that participants were indeed paying attention to sentence materials.

Electrophysiological Analyses

Figure 2 shows a topographic map for 33 participants, in the typical N400 time range (300–500 ms) after critical word (wine/#roof) onset. It shows a large, broadly distributed N400 effect with slight right lateralization in the definite condition, and a smaller N400 effect, constrained to centroparietal sites, also with slight right lateralization, in the demonstrative condition. Given this broad (and typical, see Federmeier and Kutas, 1999) distribution, we focus analyses at midline sites (see grand average ERPs, Figure 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Topographic plots of mean amplitude (μV) 300–500 ms after stimulus onset at CW for definite (top) and demonstrative (bottom) conditions. Black dots indicate scalp position of electrodes. Bolded black dots indicate midline electrode positions, used for ANOVA (see Section “N400 at the critical word”); white circles indicate the region of interest (ROI) used in correlational analyses (see Section “Correlational analyses”).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Grand average event-related potential (ERP) waveforms for object type condition: congruent (black) and incongruent (red) separately for definite (A) and demonstrative (B) conditions at five midline electrodes (Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz) time-locked to the onset of the critical word (wine/#roof). Asterisks * indicate significant amplitude differences (see Section “N400 at the critical word”).

N400 at the Critical Word

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for midline electrode sites (Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz) at the critical word (wine vs. #roof) on mean voltage in the traditional N400 time window (300–500 ms) using SPSS (IBM Corp, 2018) statistical software (see Table 2). We employed the Greenhouse-Geisser (Greenhouse and Geisser, 1959) non-sphericity correction for effects with more than one degree of freedom in the numerator. Following convention, unadjusted degrees of freedom are reported, along with the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon value (ε) and adjusted p value. Mean square error values reported are those corresponding to the Greenhouse-Geisser correction. Partial eta squared (ηp 2) is reported as a measure of effect size and post hoc comparisons are Bonferroni corrected.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. F-values for midline analyses of Object and Determiner type at critical word for time window 300–500 ms.

Here, we observed the incongruent condition was significantly more negative than its control in the definite condition at all midline electrode sites (the wine/#roof; Δ = 1.76–2.65 μV, all values of p < 0.001; see Figure 3A), whereas the incongruent condition was significantly more negative than its control in the demonstrative condition only at electrode site CPz (that wine/#roof; Δ = 1.42 μV, p = 0.006; all other values of p > 0.05; see Figure 3B). Thus, a large, broadly distributed N400 effect was observed for a semantic anomaly preceded by the definite determiner the, whereas a heavily attenuated N400 effect restricted to one site was observed for a semantic anomaly preceded by the demonstrative determiner that.

Correlational Analyses

To investigate the relationship between N400 amplitude and dispositional affect, we computed difference scores by subtracting the mean amplitude at incongruent conditions from congruent conditions in the definite condition, which had elicited a robust N400 effect (e.g., the wine – the #roof). Based on visual inspection of the N400 effect (see Figure 2), we chose a region of interest (ROI) consisting of right lateralized centroparietal electrode sites (Cz, C2, CPz, and CP2) where the effect was maximal, a region consistent with and typical of N400 scalp topography (Federmeier and Kutas, 1999; Kutas and Federmeier, 2011). We computed the difference in the mean amplitude at the average of these electrode sites at 300–500 ms following stimulus onset. These difference scores were correlated with PA (M = 27.61; SD = 6.89; ranging from 14 to 35) and Negative Affect (NA; M = 15.73; SD = 4.71; ranging from 10 to 28) scores.

We did not observe a significant correlation between PA scores and N400 amplitudes (see Figure 4A), r(31) = −0.15, p = 0.419.4 We observed a moderate negative correlation between NA scores and N400 amplitude (see Figure 4B), r(31) = −0.36, p = 0.041. That is, participants with larger NA scores had smaller N400 amplitudes (conversely, participants with smaller NA scores had larger N400 amplitudes).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatterplot of mean N400 amplitude difference in the definite condition (the winethe #roof) with (A) Positive Affect (PA) and (B) Negative Affect (NA) at the right lateralized centroparietal ROI (Cz, C2, CPz, and CP2).

Discussion

In the present study, we examined whether N400 responses to incongruent objects in sentences would be modulated by dispositional affect scores. Based on previous work on mood and language processing, we predicted (i) a significant positive correlation between N400 amplitude and positive affect scores, in addition to (ii) a negative correlation for this ERP component and negative affect scores. Our second prediction was borne out in this study. That is, N400 effects at incongruent #the roof vs. the wine, were modulated, such that more negative participants had smaller N400 amplitudes, whereas no correlation was found with positive affect. In a separate question, we also examined how neural responses to sentences containing “double violations,” as in *that #roof might differ from those with single violations. Results revealed an attenuated N400 response. This question and results are addressed in separate work (Dwivedi and Selvanayagam, 2020a,b) and we do not further discuss that question, to not detract from the issue at hand.

Below, we address the cognitive significance of our findings regarding dispositional affect and sentence perception. Specifically, we do so relying on previous work in emotion literature (Schwarz and Clore, 2007), which indicated that positive affect promotes global processing (focusing on the forest vs. the trees) vs. negative affect, which promoted more local processing (focusing on the trees vs. the forest).

Dispositional Affect and Conceptual Event Semantic Processing

To reiterate, we found no correlation with positive affect and N400 amplitude, in contrast to a significant negative correlation with negative affect. Previously, Chwilla et al. (2011) found that participants induced into happy moods produced large N400 effects, whereas those in sad moods produced attenuated effects. A corollary of the latter finding is that low negative affect individuals produce larger N400 effects – exactly what we found. We interpret the cognitive significance of our result to mean that low negative affect can facilitate contextual or heuristic processing, which we discuss in further detail below.

Real-time sentence comprehension necessarily involves global context/event knowledge for interpretation (Barton and Sanford, 1993; Zwaan and Radvansky, 1998; Ferretti et al., 2007; Dwivedi et al., 2018). We claim that low negative affect promotes local associations between words in sentences and in this way facilitates sensitivity to contextual knowledge (Chwilla et al., 1998). Here, we draw on work by Huntsinger et al. (2010, p. 725) who indicate that “a global focus should result either from having an accessible global orientation empowered by positive affect, or a local orientation inhibited by negative affect.” We draw on this logic and reason that if negative affect inhibits a local orientation, then lower negative affect would lessen that inhibition. Less inhibition between words would allow for more spreading activation and build stronger lexical expectations between words during sentence comprehension (Kamide et al., 2003; Madden and Zwaan, 2003; McRae et al., 2005). A natural outcome of this increased spreading activation between words would be stronger lexical expectations during sentence comprehension (Collins and Loftus, 1975; Metusalem et al., 2012). In this way, larger N400 amplitudes are elicited for low negative affect individuals when lexical expectations are violated. In contrast, a high negative affect individual would strongly inhibit local lexical meaning, yielding weaker links between words in a sentence and therefore exhibit less sensitivity to incongruent meaning – thereby producing attenuated N400 effects.

We speculate here that the account above could explain the lack of an effect found with positive affect in our experiment, vs. what was found previously in Chwilla et al. (2011). We claim that differences in experimental stimuli design between our work and Chwilla et al. (2011) resulted in different processing strategies, which would be differentially sensitive to affect. Recall that sentence stimuli of Chwilla et al. (2011) consisted of high-cloze vs. low-cloze word manipulations (e.g., stuffing pillows with feathers vs. stuffing pillows with #books). This manipulation meant that sentences with high-cloze words yielded meanings that were (by definition) consistent with event knowledge, that is, these were stereotypical events. In contrast, sentences inconsistent with real-world experience, (i.e., those with low-cloze words) yielded improbable or unlikely events. As such, conceptual event semantics was an important cue to access and attend to in order to compute meaning coherence.5 In contrast, meanings associated with our sentence stimuli with incongruent words consisted of impossible, not improbable events (to paraphrase above example, e.g., stuffing pillows with #clouds). As a result, participants did not need to access top-down information regarding stereotypical events in the same way – word by word association and integration was enough to construct a coherent mental representation. As such, the design of our materials did not necessitate a processing strategy associated with accessing global event knowledge – a strategy linked with positive affect (Ashby et al., 1999; Isen and Reeve, 2005; Talarico et al., 2009; Gable and Harmon-Jones, 2010; Selvanayagam et al., 2019). In fact, preliminary evidence suggests that we are on the right track. Given that sentence plausibility is a good measure of conceptual event-based expectations (Matsuki et al., 2011), we note that the off-line plausibility ratings reported above show comparable correlations with negative (r = −0.438) and positive (r = 0.069) affect. This result supports our preliminary explanation.

Conclusion

Our work showed a significant N400 effect for sentences such as The connoisseur tasted the wine/#roof on the tour, which was modulated by dispositional negative affect. This is the first study to show that dispositional affect can modulate N400 magnitude, furthering our understanding of individual differences associated with this component (Tanner et al., 2013; Grey et al., 2017). Furthermore, the findings are consistent with studies in affect and other domains of cognition that suggest a link between global and local perception of stimuli and positive and negative affect. Finally, our work supports views espoused by Münster and Knoeferle (2018), and others that relevant “contextual cues” regarding real-time language processing includes characteristics of the very human being perceiving sentences in context.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Brock University Bioscience Research Ethics Board (BREB) prior to the commencement of the experiment (REB 13-282). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

VDD conceived and designed the experiment. JS and VDD conducted the experiment, analyzed the data, and wrote the paper. Both the authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the Canada Foundation for Innovation (grant number 13831), and Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council grants (430-2016-00740 and 435-2019-1308) awarded to VDD. JS was supported by Match of Minds summer funds from Brock University. Open Access publication fees were supported by the Brock Library Open Access Publishing Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Alanna Kozak, Harmonie Chan, Brent Dryczewycz, Anushka Lalwani, and Azaan Adnan for help in running participants. In addition, we thank Louis Schmidt for comments and discussion, as well as audiences at Maastricht University, McGill University, and the Cognitive Neuroscience Society 2019, where versions of this work were presented. Finally, we also like to thank Aaron Ayub for additional help in manuscript preparation.

Footnotes

1. Note that the ‘#’ symbol (borrowed from semantic theory, cf., Roberts, 1989) indicates that the word does not match its context.

2. The * symbol is borrowed from syntactic theory, indicating ungrammaticality, as in *He likes Sarah, where the pronoun he lacks an antecedent. We note that using a pronoun or referential noun phrase (e.g., that wine) without an explicit antecedent consists of a violation of presupposition and could be thus be equally characterized as a semantic anomaly (as in #). However, for ease of exposition, we use a separate symbol, * for this condition.

3. 20 sentences began with a prepositional phrase (e.g., After thirty minutes…). 60 sentences began with a quantifier: (e.g., Many, Most). 20 sentences had direct objects as irregular plural nouns, half of which were plural (e.g., teeth, women) and half were singular, (e.g., fireman, mouse). The remaining 70 sentences were not controlled for any specific sentence construction. These filler items were used in other experiments in our lab.

4. While the N400 effect was attenuated in the demonstrative condition, as it was still present at electrode CPz, we conducted the correlational analyses here using same ROI. No significant correlations were observed here for either PA, r(31) = -0.02, p = 0.905, or NA, r(31) = -0.01, p = 0.967.

5. Accessibility of this cue was further strengthened since half of all trials contained pseudo-homophones, yielding sentences with no propositional meaning.

References

Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., and Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol. Rev. 106, 529–550. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, S. B., and Sanford, A. J. (1993). A case study of anomaly detection: shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment. Mem. Cogn. 21, 477–487. doi: 10.3758/BF03197179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., and Roberson, L. (1995). Cookies, disposition, and job attitudes: the effects of positive mood-inducing events and negative affectivity on job satisfaction in a field experiment. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 62, 55–62. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1995.1030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, G. G., and Nebes, R. D. (1975). Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex 11, 230–238. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(75)80005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., and New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: a critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 977–990. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chwilla, D. J., Hagoort, P., and Brown, C. M. (1998). The mechanism underlying backward priming in a lexical decision task: spreading activation versus semantic matching. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 531–560. doi: 10.1080/713755773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chwilla, D. J., and Kolk, H. H. J. (2005). Accessing world knowledge: evidence from N400 and reaction time priming. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 589–606. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chwilla, D. J., Virgillito, D., and Vissers, C. T. W. M. (2011). The relationship of language and emotion: N400 support for an embodied view of language comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2400–2414. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, A., and Loftus, E. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol. Rev. 82, 407–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortech Solutions. (2013). EMSE suite (version 5.5.1). Cortech Solutions.

Google Scholar

Dale, G., and Arnell, K. M. (2010). Individual differences in dispositional focus of attention predict attentional blink magnitude. Atten. Percept. Psychophysiol. 72, 602–606. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.3.602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwivedi, V. D. (2013). Interpreting quantifier scope ambiguity: evidence of heuristic first, algorithmic second processing. PLoS One 8:e81461. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwivedi, V. D., and Gibson, R. M. (2017). An ERP investigation of quantifier scope ambiguous sentences: evidence for number in events. J. Neurolinguistics 42, 63–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwivedi, V. D., Goertz, K. E., and Selvanayagam, J. (2018). Heuristics in language comprehension. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 430–446. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2018.87027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwivedi, V. D., and Selvanayagam, J. (2020a). An electrophysiological investigation of conceptual vs. compositional semantic anomaly. Ms. under review.

Google Scholar

Dwivedi, V. D., and Selvanayagam, J. (2020b). “Distinct neurocognitive effects of compositional vs. conceptual semantic violations.” in Poster presentation for the 12th annual meeting of the Society for Neurobiology of Language. Philadelphia, USA [virtual].

Google Scholar

Federmeier, K. D., Kirson, D. A., Moreno, E. M., and andand Kutas, M. (2001). Effects of transient, mild mood states on semantic memory organization and use: an event-related potential investigation in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 305, 149–152. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01843-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federmeier, K. D., and Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and sentence processing. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 469–495. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2660

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cogn. Psychol. 47, 164–203. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00005-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferretti, T. R., Kutas, M., and McRae, K. (2007). Verb aspect and the activation of event knowledge. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 182–196. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L., and Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognit. Emot. 19, 313–332. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognit. Emot. 24, 322–337. doi: 10.1080/02699930903378305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasper, K., and Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychol. Sci. 13, 34–40. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhouse, S. W., and Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 24, 95–112. doi: 10.1007/BF02289823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grey, S., Tanner, D., and Van Hell, J. G. (2017). How right is left? Handedness modulates neural responses during morphosyntactic processing. Brain Res. 1669, 27–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagoort, P. (2003). Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 883–899. doi: 10.1162/089892903322370807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., and Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion 10, 722–726. doi: 10.1037/a0019356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2018). SPSS (Version 27.0) (No. 27). IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Isen, A. M., and Reeve, J. (2005). The influence of positive affect on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: facilitating enjoyment of play, responsible work behavior, and self-control. Motiv. Emot. 29, 297–325. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9019-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., and Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: evidence from anticipatory eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 49, 133–156. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00023-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimchi, R., and Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 8, 521–535. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.8.4.521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 137, 161–163.

Google Scholar

Larsen, R. J., and Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures. Personal. Individ. Differ. 10, 1221–1228. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(89)90233-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, R. J., and Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 132–140. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, I., and Stokes, J. P. (1989). Dispositional approach to job satisfaction: role of negative affectivity. J. Appl. Psychol. 74, 752–758. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.5.752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, C. J., and Zwaan, R. A. (2003). How does verb aspect constrain event representations? Mem. Cogn. 31, 663–672. doi: 10.3758/BF03196106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuki, K., Chow, T., Hare, M., Elman, J. L., Scheepers, C., and McRae, K. (2011). Event-based plausibility immediately influences on-line language comprehension. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 37, 913–934. doi: 10.1037/a0022964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Hare, M., Elman, J. L., and Ferretti, T. R. (2005). A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Mem. Cogn. 33, 1174–1184. doi: 10.3758/BF03193221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metusalem, R., Kutas, M., Urbach, T. P., Hare, M., McRae, K., and Elman, J. L. (2012). Generalized event knowledge activation during online sentence comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 66, 545–567. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Münster, K., and Knoeferle, P. (2018). Extending situated language comprehension (accounts) with speaker and comprehender characteristics: toward socially situated interpretation. Front. Psychol. 8:2267. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pflieger, M. E. (2001). “Theory of a spatial filter for removing ocular artifacts with preservation of EEG.” in EMSE Workshop. September 2001. 7–8.

Google Scholar

Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 2.0] (2012). Available at: https://www.pstnet.com

Google Scholar

Qualtrics (2020). Qualtrics (version March 2020). Qualtrics: Provo, Utah, USA.

Google Scholar

Roberts, C. (1989). Modal subordination and pronominal anaphora in discourse. Linguist. Philos. 12, 683–721. doi: 10.1007/BF00632602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., and Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 383–388. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605198104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schank, R., and Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Schwarz, N. (2002). “Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: cognitive tuning” in The wisdom in feelings: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence. eds. L. Feldman Barrett and P. Salovey (New York: Guilford Press), 144–166.

Google Scholar

Schwarz, N., and Clore, G. L. (2007). “Feeling and phenomenal experiences” in Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. 2nd Edn. eds. A. W. Kruglanski and E. T. Higgins (New York: The Guilford Press), 385–407.

Google Scholar

Selvanayagam, J., Witte, V., Schmidt, L. A., and Dwivedi, V. D. (2019). A preliminary investigation of dispositional affect, the P300, and sentence processing. Brain Res. 1721. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staw, B. M., and Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of change. A dispositional approach to job attitudes. J. Appl. Psychol. 70, 469–480. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.70.3.469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. George, M., Mannes, S., and Hoffman, J. E. (1994). Global semantic expectancy and language comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 70–83. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.70

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognit. Emot. 23, 380–398. doi: 10.1080/02699930801993999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanner, D., McLaughlin, J., Herschensohn, J., and Osterhout, L. (2013). Individual differences reveal stages of L2 grammatical acquisition: ERP evidence. Biling. Lang. Congn. 16, 367–382. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, D. J., and Bever, T. G. (2001). Sentence comprehension: The integration of rules and habits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

van Berkum, J. J. A., De Goede, D., van Alphen, P. M., Mulder, E. R., and Kerstholt, J. H. (2013). How robust is the language architecture? The case of mood. Front. Psychol. 4:505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Casteren, M., and Davis, M. H. (2006). Mix, a program for pseudorandomization. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 584–589. doi: 10.3758/BF03193889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vissers, C. T. W. M., Chwilla, U. G., Egger, J. I. M., and Chwilla, D. J. (2013). The interplay between mood and language comprehension: evidence from P600 to semantic reversal anomalies. Neuropsychologia 51, 1027–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vissers, C. T. W. M., Virgillito, D., Fitzgerald, D. A., Speckens, A. E. M., Tendolkar, I., van Oostrom, I., et al. (2010). The influence of mood on the processing of syntactic anomalies: evidence from P600. Neuropsychologia 48, 3521–3531. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychol. Bull. 96, 465–490.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A., and Radvansky, G. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol. Bull. 123, 162–185. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: N400, dispositional affect, conceptual semantics, sentence comprehension, mood/emotion, global-local

Citation: Dwivedi VD and Selvanayagam J (2021) Effects of Dispositional Affect on the N400: Language Processing and Socially Situated Context. Front. Psychol. 12:566894. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.566894

Received: 28 May 2020; Accepted: 10 March 2021;
Published: 31 March 2021.

Edited by:

Pia Knoeferle, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Viviana Masia, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Marijn Struiksma, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2021 Dwivedi and Selvanayagam. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Veena D. Dwivedi, vdwivedi@brocku.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.