Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 February 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Achievement Emotions in University Teaching and Learning, Students’ Stress and Well-being View all 15 articles

Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching as Determinants of Academic Behavioral Confidence and Procrastination in Undergraduate Students

  • 1School of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 3Department of Psychology, Teesside University, Middlesbrough, United Kingdom
  • 4School of Psychology, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 5School of Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 6Unit of Prevention of Stress, University of Sassari, Sassari, Italy
  • 7Universidad Popular Autónoma del Estado de Puebla, Puebla, Mexico

The combination of student Self-Regulation (SR) and the context of Regulatory Teaching (RT), each in varying degree, has recently been demonstrated to have effects on achievement emotions, factors and symptoms of stress, and coping strategies. The aim of the present research study is to verify its possible further effects, on academic behavioral confidence and procrastination. A total of 1193 university students completed validated online questionnaires with regard to specific subjects in their degree program. Using an ex post facto design, multivariate analyses and structural equation modeling (SEM) were carried out in order to test the relationships predicted by the model. SR and RT had a significant joint effect in determining the degree of academic behavioral confidence and of procrastination. Academic behavioral confidence also significantly predicted reasons for procrastinating, and these in turn predicted activities of procrastination. Conclusions are discussed, insisting on the combined weight of the two variables in determining academic behavioral confidence, reasons for procrastinating and activities subject to procrastination, in university students. Implications for guidance and educational support of university students and teachers are analyzed.

Introduction

This study forms part of a series whose aim is to determine the combined effect of student self-regulation and of regulatory teaching on other academic variables. The aim of the present study, therefore, was to establish the combined effect of the student’s level of self-regulation (SR) and the level of regulatory teaching (RT) on students’ degree of academic behavioral confidence, as a precursor to reasons for procrastinating and to activities of procrastination. This study, then, would complete the body of published evidence that consistently indicates a joint effect of the two variables, self-regulation and regulatory teaching, in this Research Topic (de la Fuente et al., 2020a).

The Teaching and Learning Process as Object of Study in Educational Psychology

In formal academic situations, such as the university, it seems reasonable that the variables we study would be jointly determined by learner characteristics as well as by the design and implementation of the teaching process (Cabanach et al., 2007, 2013; Chartier et al., 2011; Alonso-Tapia et al., 2018; Gentsch et al., 2018; Cassady et al., 2019). Previous theoretical models have adopted this idea. Biggs’ 3P model (Biggs, 1989, 1993a, b, 1999a, b; Biggs et al., 2001) has evolved toward a more interactive vision, progressively integrating the teaching process more explicitly (Kember et al., 2020). The Vermunt model (Vermunt, 1995, 1996, 1998; Vermunt and Donche, 2017) has systematically analyzed the role of external regulation as a negative factor for appropriate learning styles (Vermunt, 2005, 2007). The Entwistle model (Entwistle and Ramsden, 1983; Entwistle, 1991) has specifically considered the weight of the context and teaching process in the university environment (Ramsden, 1991; Asikainen et al., 2014; Cano et al., 2020). The Zimmerman model (Zimmerman, 1990, 1998; Zimmerman and Schunk, 2001) has also considered the contextual factor, although in a more implicit way (Kim et al., 2020; Zalazar-Jaime and Medrano, 2020).

The Theory of Self- vs. Externally-Regulated Learning, SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) has attempted to organize the different combinations of student regulation (learning process) and teacher regulation (teaching process) that can occur in a university academic setting, summarizing these in a five-combination heuristic (see Table 1). This heuristic assumes that each study variable should be contextualized within the teaching and learning process, representing a distinct approach to investigation in Educational Psychology. Assuming that the students and the teacher may have varying characteristics (high-medium-low in regulation), different combinations will result, and prove more or less favorable to the teaching and learning process:

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Combinations between model parameters hypothesized by SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente et al., 2017, 2019).

(1) The worst, Very Unfavorable combination (type 1) refers to a classroom combination of a student with low self-regulation (SR) and a teaching process low in external regulation (RT). In this case, the model predicts low academic behavioral confidence and high procrastination.

(2) An Unfavorable combination (type 2) refers to a classroom combination of a student with low SR and a teacher with medium RT, or the inverse. Here, the model predicts medium-low academic behavioral confidence and medium-high procrastination.

(3) A Medium combination (type 3) refers to the combination of a student with medium SR and a teacher with medium RT. The model predicts medium academic behavioral confidence and a medium level of procrastination.

(4) A Favorable combination (type 4) refers to the combination of a student with medium SR and a teacher with high RT, or the inverse. The model predicts medium-high academic behavioral confidence and a medium-low level of procrastination.

(5) The Most Favorable combination (type 5) refers to the combination of a student with high SR and a teacher with high RT. The model predicts high academic behavioral confidence and a low level of procrastination.

Previous research has consistently shown this heuristic to establish significant differences in the factors and symptoms of stress (de la Fuente et al., 2020a), coping strategies (de la Fuente et al., 2020b), achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2019), students’ learning approaches (de la Fuente et al., 2020c) and academic achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2017). All these results are contextualized within the process of university teaching and learning (Gross, 2008, 2014, 2015a, b; Holinka, 2015; Harley et al., 2019; Hirvonena et al., 2019; Kobylińska and Kusev, 2019). Yet to be established, however, is its discriminatory power in determining the level of academic behavioral confidence and procrastination– two behavioral variables of learning that are polar opposites in their association with self-regulated learning at university. Consequently, this will be the aim of the present study.

Academic Behavioral Confidence as Variable of the Teaching and Learning Process

Academic Behavioral Confidence as a Variable of the Learning Process

Although academic behavioral confidence has been defined as an eminently personal and attitudinal construct (Sander and Sanders, 2009; Sander et al., 2013), its self-referring, subjective, perceptual nature suggests that it can be influenced by both personal and contextual factors. Previous research has reported that academic behavioral confidence is associated with and is a positive predictor of a deep learning approach and of academic achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2016). Moreover, it has been positively associated with self-regulation (Nicholson et al., 2013; de la Fuente et al., 2015b), and has a stable nature, associated with academic goals (Putwain et al., 2013). Another research report has shown the predictive value of academic confidence on academic performance (Burr and LeFevre, 2020). Academic confidence has also appeared as a predictor of coping strategies and achievement (Kirikkanat and Kali-Soyer, 2018), as well as predicting confidence in learning (Shoemaker, 2010). More recently, it has been found in association with and a positive predictor of positive achievement emotions, as well as negatively predicting negative emotions (Sander and de la Fuente, 2020).

Earlier research in the development of the academic behavioral confidence scale has shown that the scale meaningfully discriminates between students in different degree programs, such that students in programs that require higher grades at entry, for example Medicine, Speech and Language Therapy, and Nutrition have higher confidence in one or more of the Grades, Studying and Attendance sub-scales (Sander and Sanders, 2009). In a summary article, Sander (2009) presents findings that indicate that dyslexic students studying at universities in the United Kingdom have lower academic confidence on the Grades, Verbalizing and Studying sub-scales but not on the Attendance sub-scale. Furthermore, other data from United Kingdom university students shows that scores on the academic behavioral confidence scale drop during a course of study (Sander, 2009; Putwain and Sander, 2016), a finding that is supported by other research (Beyer, 1999; Zusho et al., 2003; Papinczak et al., 2008).

Academic Behavioral Confidence as a Variable Promoted Through Teaching

Prior research has demonstrated that level of regulatory teaching determined the degree of academic behavioral confidence (de la Fuente et al., 2015b). However, we have not yet seen whether academic behavioral confidence is determined linearly and jointly both by student characteristics and teaching process characteristics (Akbari and Sahibzada, 2020). A pertinent factor to be considered is that the grades and verbalizing components of academic behavioral confidence are under the control of the student only to a lesser degree, whereas the studying and attendance components are largely under the student’s control (Sander, 2009; Sander and Sanders, 2009). A student may choose to study or attend whereas the grades one receives depends partly on the marker, and one’s experience of discussing course materials depends on the person one is talking to. As Putwain and Sander (2016) say, “The dip and return of confidence in studying and attendance may reflect a closer alignment with self-regulative processes determined by control than grades and verbalizing” (p. 393). Finally, show how the expectations that students have of their and their teachers’ responsibility in the teaching and learning process interact with student academic confidence in the prediction of grades.

Procrastination as a Variable of the Teaching and Learning Process

Procrastination as a Variable of the Learning Process

Procrastination has been studied and described for general matters of daily life as well as for specific areas, such as the contexts of health and academics. Procrastination is understood to be a failure in motivation that creates a gap between intention and action, with negative consequences for the individual (Steel, 2007; Steel and Klingsieck, 2015), and has been established as the polar opposite of self-regulation. It has thus been considered a dysregulatory behavior (de la Fuente, 2017), being negatively predicted by self-regulation (Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018). High levels of procrastination have also been related to anxiety problems, general stress, and physical and mental health issues (Stead et al., 2010; Sirois and Tosti, 2012; Kim and Seo, 2015). In general, research studies on procrastination can be classified as focusing either on the reasons that lead to procrastinating, or on the activities or frequency of procrastination behaviors. The motives that define the volitional basis leading to procrastination differ in valence (positive vs. negative emotionality) and direction (approach vs. avoidance), while frequency describes the intensity of procrastination in different activities.

The study of the motives or reasons for procrastinating has established certain commonalities, such as attraction/uncertainty about the task, fear of failure or fear of evaluation (Zarick and Stonebraker, 2009) and perfectionism (Sudler, 2013). Among university students, inadequate time management, test anxiety, and laziness are the principal triggers for procrastinating (Gil et al., 2019).

Examples of procrastination in activities of daily life may involve paying a bill or taking one’s medication; in the academic context, preparing for a test or doing an assignment. General procrastination behaviors have been on the rise in recent decades. In the 1970s, figures for recurring procrastinators fell between 4–5% of the adult population, while this incidence has recently been estimated at 15–20% (Steel and Ferrari, 2012). Specifically, academic procrastination appears with greater frequency than general procrastination. Certain studies indicate that students often put off starting to prepare for exams (30–40%) or writing papers (46%) (Rothblum et al., 1986; Beswick et al., 1988).

At the same time, the intensity of academic procrastination shows differences between certain population subgroups. For example, gender has been described as having an indirect effect on procrastination and academic performance, with lower levels of procrastination and greater achievement in women; age also has an effect, where procrastination is positively predicted in younger people (Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018). Similarly, there is evidence to indicate that procrastination varies according to the student’s degree program (Clariana, 2013); that there is a greater tendency to procrastinate in the transition from high school to university; and that procrastination is associated with plagiarism or dishonest academic behavior (Clariana et al., 2012).

Different studies have confirmed that procrastination is inversely associated with academic achievement: the greater the procrastination, the lower the achievement (Kim and Seo, 2015). Procrastination has more predictive value for achievement than do variables like class attendance or university admissions scores (Steel, 2007; Rozental and Carlbring, 2014). Procrastination has also been associated with other important academic variables. When students perceive tasks as difficult, unattractive, ambiguous and requiring more effort, they tend to present higher rates of procrastination (Ferrari et al., 2006). Accordingly, those who present more confidence in their academic skills (high levels of self-efficacy) tend to procrastinate less, and procrastination has less impact on their academic achievement (Klassen et al., 2008).

The psychological mechanism by which procrastination originates in the student seems to be low expectation of achievement; this affects motivation to start the task and to follow through, possibly leading to avoidance behavior and procrastination (Rozental and Carlbring, 2014). Self-efficacy seems to mediate the effect of achievement expectations; students with low perceived self-efficacy are more vulnerable to being caught in a vicious cycle of procrastination (Wäschle et al., 2014). By contrast, high levels of self-efficacy are related to the use of planning tools and starting tasks at the right time (Wolters, 2003).

Procrastination as a Variable Promoted Through the Teaching Process

Given that the mechanism behind this behavioral phenomenon is a lack of motivation or expectations, it is reasonable that most research has focused on procrastination as it relates to student characteristics, looking for internal explanatory mechanisms. However, it is also possible that procrastination can be triggered externally, by characteristics of the teaching process (Codina et al., 2020; Yang, 2020). Insufficient attention has been given to this perspective. Adopting the perspective of SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) allows us to take this two-fold approach.

Situational and contextual factors –social factors included– play an important role in explaining the types of procrastination. Parents’ and teachers’ negative attitudes toward procrastination, for example, have been found to trigger a kind of procrastination as rebellion (Klingsieck et al., 2013). There is evidence that students’ perception of autonomy-supportive teaching, or effective or regulatory teaching, is positively associated with feeling competent, and negatively associated with procrastination behaviors (Codina et al., 2020). Procrastination increases when the teacher lowers demands, is willing to negotiate academic deadlines, and tends to be more flexible in grading (Schraw et al., 2007). Consequently, task characteristics and teacher characteristics, as powerful contextual factors, are important in triggering or increasing the likelihood of procrastination in students (Steel and Klingsieck, 2016).

Aims and Hypotheses

Based on the models and previous empirical data, the following research objectives were set: (1) to establish whether the combination levels defined in SRL vs. ERL Theory (Table 1) determine the level of academic behavioral confidence, as well as reasons for and activities of procrastination; (2) to determine the predictive value of both self-regulation and regulatory teaching in academic behavioral confidence, and the latter’s predictive value in reasons for and activities of procrastination.

From these objectives, the following hypotheses were stated. (1) A graded increase in level of regulation (internal and external) would give rise to an increase in academic behavioral confidence, and a proportionate decrease in reasons for and activities of procrastination. By contrast, a graded decrease in level of regulation (internal and external) would give rise to a decrease in academic behavioral confidence, and a proportionate increase in reasons for and activities of procrastination. (2) Regulation factors in students and in the teaching would be positive, significant predictors of academic behavioral confidence; the latter would in turn negatively predict reasons for and activities of procrastination.

Materials and Methods

Participants

To establish interdependence relations between low-medium-high levels of Self-Regulation (SR) and Regulatory Teaching (RT), we used a total sample of 1193 undergraduate students from two public universities of Spain, taken through convenience sampling. The sample contained students majoring in Psychology, Primary Education, and Educational Psychology; 85.5% were women and 14.5% were men. The age range was 19 to 25 years, and mean age was 21.33 (σ = 2.26) years.

Instruments

Self-Regulation (Meta-Behavioral Variable)

The Short Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SSRQ) (Miller and Brown, 1991) was used to measure this variable. The Spanish version has been validated in Spanish samples (Pichardo et al., 2014, 2018), showing acceptable validity and reliability values, comparable to the English version. The Spanish Short SRQ comprises four factors (goal setting-planning, perseverance, decision making and learning from mistakes) and contains 17 items (all with saturations greater than 0.40). This questionnaire has a Likert format, with possible responses ranging from 1 (“not true of me at all”) to 5 (“very true of me). It has the advantage of significantly reducing completion time with respect to the original 63-item scale. The confirmatory factor structure is consistent (Chi-Square = 250.83, df = 112, CFI = 0.90, GFI = 0.92, AGFI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.05). Internal consistency was acceptable for the questionnaire total (α = 0.86) and for all factors: goal setting-planning (α = 0.79), decision making (α = 0.72), learning from mistakes (α = 0.72), and perseverance (α = 0.73). Correlations were obtained for the following: (1) between each item and its factor total, (2) between the factors, and (3) between each factor and the questionnaire total. The results were good in all cases, except for decision making, which had a lower correlation with other factors (0.41 to 0.58). The correlations between the original long SRQ and the long Spanish version, and between the English short SRQ and the Spanish short version are better for the short version (original SSRQ: r = 0.85 and Spanish SSRQ: r = 0.94; p < 0.01) than for the original, long SRQ (r = 0.79; p < 0.01).

Regulatory Teaching (Meta-Instructional Variable)

The Assessment of the Teaching-Learning Process, ATLP, student version (de la Fuente et al., 2012) was used to evaluate students’ perception of the teaching process. The Regulatory Teaching scale constitutes Dimension 1 of the confirmatory model. The ATLP-D1 contains 29 items with a five-factor structure: Specific regulatory teaching, regulatory assessment, preparation for learning, satisfaction with the teaching, and general regulatory teaching. Having been previously validated in university students (de la Fuente et al., 2012, 2020c), the scale shows a factor structure with adequate fit indices (Chi-Square = 590.626; df = 48, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.938, TLI = 0.939, NFI = 0.950, NNFI = 0.967; RMSEA = 0.058). Internal consistency is also adequate (ATLP D1: α = 0.83; specific regulatory teaching, α = 0.897; regulatory assessment, α = 0.883; preparation for learning, α = 0.849; satisfaction with the teaching, α = 0.883 and general regulatory teaching, α = 0.883). The ATLP is a self-report instrument that collects data from students and teachers and is available in Spanish and English. External validity results are also consistent, since there are several interdependent relationships between the reported perceptions of variables in an academic setting.

Academic Behavioral Confidence (Attitudinal Variable)

This was measured by the Academic Behavioral Confidence Scale (Sanders and Sander, 2003; Sander and Sanders, 2006, 2009) in a validated Spanish version (Sander et al., 2011). Developed from the established constructs of self-concept and self-efficacy, the ABC scale assesses specific aspects in undergraduate students. This psychometric scale, designed for students from Spain and the United Kingdom, asks them to report their anticipated study-related behaviors within their degree program (assumed to consist primarily of lecture-based courses). Crucially distinct aspects of students’ academic behavior are represented in four subscales: Grades, Studying, Verbalizing and Attendance (Sander, 2009). Students are required to respond to a question stem (‘How confident are you that you will be able to…’) for items such as ‘…manage your workload to meet coursework deadlines’ and ‘…write in an appropriate academic style.’ Responses fall along a five-point scale (1 = ‘not at all confident,’ 5 = ‘very confident’). A higher score therefore indicates greater confidence in one’s efficacy in study skills or behaviors. A four-factor model (confidence in attaining grades, studying, attending classes and discussing course material) has shown adequate reliability and validity in prior studies (Sander and Sanders, 2009). The confirmatory model showed good fit [Chi-square = 693.405; Degrees of freedom (152–54) = 98; p ≤ 0.001; NFI = 0.916; RFI = 0.904; IFI = 0.927; TLI = 0.909, CFI = 0.927; RMSEA = 0.062; HOELTER = 276 (p < 0.05) and 302 (p < 0.01)]. There is also good internal consistency for the total scale [α = 0.952; Part 1 = 0.932, Part 2 = 0.872; Spearman-Brown = 0.961; Guttman = 0.935].

Procrastination (Motivational Variable)

Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students, in its Spanish version (Garzón and Gil, 2017). This scale was originally constructed by Solomon and Rothblum (1984) and has been often used in the study of academic procrastination internationally. Its 44 items describe the frequency of academic procrastination activities (18 items) and the underlying reasons for doing them (26 items). Thirteen possible reasons for procrastinating are incorporated, including such options as: evaluation anxiety, perfectionism, difficulty making decisions, dependency and help seeking, aversiveness of the task, lack of self-confidence and laziness. Response options are presented on a Likert scale with values from 1 to 5, where 1 means “does not reflect my motives at all,” 3 means “it reflects them to a certain degree” and 5 means “it reflects them perfectly.”

For the present study, we considered procrastination frequency in the academic activities addressed by the PASS: writing a term paper, studying for an exam, keeping up with weekly assigned reading, performing administrative tasks, attendance. Each activity also included the question: To what degree is procrastination in this area a problem for you? and, To what degree would you like to decrease your procrastination in this area? For this section, the test uses a five-point Likert scale: 1 (Never), 2 (Almost never), 3 (Sometimes), 4 (Almost always) and 5 (Always). Reasons for procrastinating were grouped into five factors: arousal seeking, low self-control, perfectionism, test anxiety and low self-confidence.

Procedure

Participants voluntarily completed the scales using an online platform (de la Fuente et al., 2015a). The assessments covered a total of five specific teaching-learning processes of different university subjects over a period of two academic years. All the questionnaires were answered in their Spanish versions, previously translated and validated, using the online platform1. This research platform allows teachers and students to register online and give their informed consent. Each questionnaire is completed independently; students then have access to their scores for the total construct and for its factors. Additionally, the student can access self-help feedback, based on their scores, to work on aspects of their learning process. This platform is presently available in Spanish and English, but the number of available languages for questionnaire completion is currently being expanded, following validation of each tool in each language. Self-regulation and Academic Behavioral Confidence were evaluated in October-November of 2018 and 2019; Procrastination Behavior and Regulatory Teaching in March-April 2018 and 2019.

Students signed their informed consent and received a certificate of Project participation for completing the inventories outside of regular class hours. The procedure was approved by the respective Ethics Committees of the two universities, in the context of an R&D Project (see Funding).

Data Analysis

Research Design

In line with the method of sample selection, an ex post facto design was used, collecting the data and manipulating it by selection.

Inferential effects of regulation levels

Through cluster analysis, continuous independent variables were transformed into discrete dependent variables, with three levels (low-medium-high). Preliminary analyses were carried out to determine the distribution of the variables, and so be able to perform analyses of variance [SR (M = 3.48, SD = 0.60); Kolmogoroff-Smirnoff = 0.25, p < 0.200; RT (M = 3.37, SD = 0.59); Kolmogoroff-Smirnoff = 0.37, p < 0.3501]. ANOVAs and MANOVAs were conducted, with Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching as independent Variables (IV), while Academic Behavioral Confidence and Procrastination were the dependent Variables. In all cases, error variance differences were confirmed to be non-significant (Box’s M test as a multivariate statistical test used to check the equality of multiple variance-covariance matrices. The test is commonly used to test the assumption of homogeneity of variances and covariances in MANOVA and linear discriminant analysis), p > 0.05). The multivariate analyses (MANOVAs) showed a statistically significant main effect of the five interaction types on low-medium-high levels of the dependent variables (see Table 1):

Combination 1 represents a statistically significant low level of SR and low level of RT (1 and 1). The average regulation level is 1.0, and its regulation rank is 1. The regulation trend is low SR and low RT; this is associated with a high level of dysregulation. The effects would be a low level of academic behavioral confidence and a high level of procrastination reasons and activities.

Combination 2 represents a statistically significant low level in SR and medium level in RT, or vice versa (1 and 2, or 2 and 1). The average regulation level is 1.5, and its regulation rank is 2. The regulation trend is low SR and medium RT, or vice versa; associated in turn with a medium-low level of dysregulation. The effects, then, would be a low-medium level of academic behavioral confidence and a medium-high level of procrastination reasons and activities.

Combination 3 represents a statistically significant medium level of SR and medium level of RT (2 and 2). The average regulation level is 2.0, and its regulation rank is 3. The regulation trend is medium SR and medium RT; this is associated with a medium level of dysregulation. The effects, then, would be a medium level of academic behavioral confidence and a medium level of procrastination reasons and activities.

Combination 4 represents a statistically significant medium level in SR and high level in RT, or vice versa (2 and 3, or 3 and 2). The average regulation level is 2.5, and its regulation rank is 4. The regulation trend is high SR and medium RT, or medium SR and high RT; this is associated with a good level of regulation. The effects, then, would be a medium-high level of academic behavioral confidence and a medium-low level of procrastination reasons and activities.

Combination 5 represents statistically significant high levels of SR and RT (3 and 3). The average regulation level is 3.0, and its regulation rank is 5. The regulation trend is high SR and high RT; this is associated with a high level of regulation. The effects, then, would be a high level of academic behavioral confidence and a low level of procrastination reasons and activities.

Predictive structural effects

For analysis of SEM model fit, the comparative adjustment index (CFI) and the mean square approximation error (RMSEA) were used. CFI values equal to or greater than 0.90 and 0.95, respectively, were taken to indicate acceptable and close fit to the data (McDonald and Marsh, 1990). RMSEA values equal to or less than 0.08 and 0.05 were also taken to indicate acceptable and close levels of fit (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993). IBM-AMOS statistical program (v. 22) was used.

Results

Interdependent Complex Effects Between Levels of Self-Regulation (SR) and Levels of Regulatory Teaching (RT)

Effect on Total Academic Behavioral Confidence and Its Factors

There was a statistically significant main effect of SR levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) on total Academic Behavioral Confidence (1 < 2 < 3, p < 0.001). Complementarily, there was a statistically significant main effect of RT (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) on total Academic Behavioral Confidence (1 < 2 < 3, p < 0.001). A statistically significant effect of SR levels and RT levels was noted in all factors of Academic Behavioral Confidence. There was no statistically significant SR × RT interaction effect. The most powerful effect of SR was produced on the factors of Grades and Verbalization, while the most powerful effect of RT was on the factors of Grades, Verbalization, and Attendance. See Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Interdependent complex effects (3 × 3) of low-medium-high levels of Self-Regulation (SR) and low-medium-high levels of Regulatory Teaching (RT) with academic behavioral confidence and procrastination (n = 986).

Effect on Total Reasons for Procrastination and Its Factors

A statistically significant main effect of SR levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) was noted on total Reasons for Procrastination (1 > 2 > 3, p < 0.001). Complementarily, a statistically significant main effect of RT levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) was noted on total Reasons for Procrastination (1,2 > 3, p < 0.001). Complementary, a statistically significant main effect of SR levels was noted on the factors of Reasons for Procrastination. The main partial effects of SR appeared in the procrastination reasons of low self-control, perfectionism and test anxiety (1 > 2 > 3, p < 0.001), while the main partial effects of RT appeared in the reasons low self-control and test anxiety (1 > 2 > 3, p < 0.001). See Table 3 and Figure 1.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effects of combination types on academic behavioral confidence, procrastination reasons and activities (n = 1026).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the effect of combination types (1–5) on Academic Behavioral Confidence.

Effect on Total Procrastination Behavior and Its Factors

A statistically significant main effect of SR levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) was noted on total Procrastination Activities (1 > 2 > 3, p < 0.001). Complementarily, a statistically significant main effect of RT was noted on total Procrastination Activities (1,2 > 3, p < 0.001). Also, a statistically significant main effect of SR levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) was noted on the factors of Procrastination Activities. The main partial effects of SR levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) appeared in the procrastination activities of writing a term paper, studying for an exam, and keeping up with weekly reading (1 > 2 > 3, p < 0.001), while the RT levels (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) variable did not carry sufficient statistical strength to determine differences in any specific procrastination activity. See Table 2 and Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Graphic representation of the effect of Self-Regulation level (GRUPSR) and Regulatory Teaching level (GRUPRT) on Reasons and Activities of Procrastination. PROCRF1, term papers; PROCRATF2, study for exams; PROCRATF3, assigned reading; PROCRATF4, admin. tasks; PROCRASTF5, attendance; PROCRATF6, activities in general. Reasons for procrastination: PROCRATOT, TOTAL; PROCRAR1, AROUSAL SEEKING; PROCRR2, LOW CONTROL; PROCRATR3, PERFECTIONISM.

Combination Effects in Academic Behavioral Confidence and Procrastination

Preliminary Analysis

The MANOVA that was carried out showed statistically significant differences, in all levels of the SR and RT variables, among the five groups. SR and RT are adequately configured as established in Table 3.

Academic Behavioral Confidence

A statistically significant main effect of the five combination of SR levels and RT levels (see regulatory rank in Table 1) was noted on total Academic Behavioral Confidence (5,4 > 3,2 > 1; p < 0.001). Complementarily, a significant main effect of the five combinations of SR levels and RT levels was noted on the factors of Academic Behavioral Confidence (with variations of 5,4 > 3,2 > 1; p < 0.001). See Table 3 and Figure 1.

Reasons for and Activities of Procrastination

A statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of SR levels and RT levels was noted on total Reasons for Procrastination (1 > 2,3 > 4,5, p < 0.001). Regarding the factors of Reasons for Procrastination, a statistically significant main effect of the five combination of SR and RT was noted in all (1 > 2,3 > 4,5, p < 0.001).

For total Activities of Procrastination, a statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of SR and RT levels was observed (1,2 > 3 > 4,5, p < 0.001). For all factors of Activities of Procrastination, a statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of SR and RT levels was noted, with particular statistical strength in Writing term papers, Studying for exams and Keeping up with weekly reading (1,2 > 3 > 4,5, p < 0.001). See Table 3 and Figures 3, 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Graphical representation of the effect of combination types (1–5) on reasons to procrastinate. Reasons to procrastinate: RF1. AROUSAL SEEKING; RF2. LOW CONTROL; RF3. PERFECTIONISM; RF4. TEST ANXIETY; RF5. LOW CONFIDENCE.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Graphical representation of the effect of combination types (1–5) on procrastination activities. Procrastination activities: F1. TERM PAPERS; F2. STUDY FOR EXAMS; F3. WEEKLY READING; F4. ADMINIST. TASKS; F5. ATTENDANCE; F6. ACTIV. IN GENERAL.

Structural Prediction Model

Pathway analysis (SEM) revealed an acceptable model of the relationships between variables. The relationship parameters of the two models are presented below. Both models were tested. In model 1 the relationships Combination-> Academic confidence -> Activities of procrastination were tested, while in model 2 the relationships Combination-> Academic confidence -> Reasons to procrastinate-> Activities of Procrastination The second model produced more consistent results and was taken as definitive. See Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Models of structural linear results of the variables.

Standardized Direct Effects

Of particular interest was the differential weight of SR (B = 0.62) and RT (B = 0.33) on the latent variable COMBINATION. The Model reflected that the combination of regulation factors (COMB) was a significant, positive predictor of academic behavioral confidence (CONFIDENCE) (B = 0.93). CONFIDENCE was also a significant, negative predictor of procrastination reasons (RAZPROCRAST) (B = −0.46) and procrastination activities (FACTPROCRAST) (B = −0.25). Finally, reasons for procrastinating appeared as a significant, positive predictor of procrastination activities (B = 0.32). See Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardized direct effects (default model).

Standardized Indirect Effects

The combination of SR and RT (COMBINATION) had statistically significant effects on the totals for procrastination reasons and procrastination activities and on their factors. Academic behavioral confidence (CONFIDENCE) also had an indirect negative, predictive effect on each of the factors and total of procrastination activities. See Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Standardized indirect effects (default model).

A graphic representation of the final structural model is seen in Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. SEM of relations between academic behavioral confidence, reasons for procrastination and procrastination activities. COMBINAT, SR and RT GROUPS: ACADCONFIDENCE, Academic Behavioral Confidence; REASONPROCRAT, Reasons to procrastinate: RR1. AROUSAL SEEKING; RR2. LOW CONTROL; RR3. PERFECTIONISM; RR4. TEST ANXIETY; RR5. LOW CONFIDENCE. PROCRACTIVITIES, Procrastination activities (Factors): F1. TERM PAPERS; F2. STUDY FOR EXAMS; F3. WEEKLY READING; F4. ADMINIST. TASKS; F5. ATTENDANCE; F6. ACTIV. IN GENERAL.

Discussion and Conclusion

Importance of the Level of Regulation Promoted Both Internally and Externally

Self- vs. Externally-Regulated Learning Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) had predicted that university students’ academic confidence and procrastination could be determined, jointly, by the students’ degree of self-regulation and by the level of contextual, external regulation from the teaching process. Furthermore, this type of interaction could be understood as the combination of the low-medium-high level of the two factors, and is supported by prior evidence in this direction, in reference to achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2015b), to coping strategies used (de la Fuente et al., 2019) and to factors and symptoms of stress (de la Fuente et al., 2020c). In this study, in line with the hypotheses posed, the results contribute evidence that a graded increase in level of regulation (internal and external) gave rise to an increase in academic behavioral confidence, and a proportionate decrease in reasons for and activities of procrastination. By contrast, a graded decrease in level of regulation (internal and external) would lead to a decrease in academic behavioral confidence, and a proportionate increase in reasons for and activities of procrastination (Putwain et al., 2015; Putwain, 2018; Putwain and Pescod, 2018). We may consider that Hypothesis 1 was validated in almost every case. Both individually and in combination, levels of self-regulation (SR) and of regulatory teaching (RT) have produced an increase in academic behavioral confidence, as well as a decrease in procrastination reasons and activities. These results further our conceptualization of academic behavioral confidence, by showing that it depends not only on the university student’s level of regulation (de la Fuente et al., 2015b), but is also influenced by the level of regulation established in the teaching process. Specifically, the five-combination model (de la Fuente et al., 2019) is the most predictive model of variability in academic behavioral confidence (Sanders and Sander, 2003; Rusk et al., 2011; Saklofske et al., 2012).

Hypothesis 2 was also confirmed, establishing that regulation in students and regulation in teaching were both positive, significant predictors of academic behavioral confidence. Academic behavioral confidence, in turn, negatively predicted reasons for and activities of procrastination. Our linear predictive model revealed the same relationship in a structural format. It has been clearly shown that the combination of SR and RT predicts academic behavioral confidence, and that the latter directly and indirectly affects reasons for procrastinating and procrastination activites. Certain prior research studies have reported similar results, showing the predictive value of confidence with respect to procrastination in Secondary Education (Saputra et al., 2020). Klassen et al. (2008) showed that those who present more confidence in their academic skills (high levels of self-efficacy) procrastinate less. Given the results of the present study, there is evidence that academic behavioral confidence is determined not only by the student’s personal factors; and that academic behavioral confidence affects not only the intensity but also the types of procrastination (Brando-Garrido et al., 2020).

Conclusion, Limitations and Future Research

Once again, consistent with the evidence reported in prior studies (de la Fuente et al., 2015b, 2017, 2019, 2020c, d), it has been confirmed that both the level of SR (in greater measure) and the level of RT produce effects on academic behavioral confidence, and on procrastination reasons and activities, and that the former is predictive of the latter. In a complementary way, it is possible to consider academic confidence as a protective factor against procrastination during university learning, since it minimizes the reasons and behaviors of procrastination (Batool, 2020).

One limitation of this study is the exclusive use of questionnaires for collecting data; obtaining another type of evidence from other data sources would make it possible to triangulate the information (Aguilar and Barroso, 2015), as well as corroborate and/or examine in more depth the findings presented here. A second limitation is the sample composition, which is predominantly female. For this reason, the sampling of participants may affect generalizability of the findings.

Future studies could address questions like the connection to previously reported variables with similar effects (health, flourishing, academic outcomes, etc.) in a model that integrates the cumulative evidence. In addition, further study could be made of the critical components of student self-regulation and of regulatory teaching, components that account for the important differences between the groups compared in this study. A clear understanding of these practices, habits and competencies would make it possible to develop guidance programs or classroom interventions that offer specific training in personal self-regulation and teaching regulation, and would promote application of these principles in educational contexts of university (Martín et al., 2003; Linnenbrink-Garcia and Pekrun, 2011; Lüftenegger et al., 2016; Shaw et al., 2017; Tada, 2017; Lekwa et al., 2018; Loderer et al., 2018; Mainhard et al., 2018; Shannon et al., 2019; McGee, 2020).

Interventions that seek to increase self-regulation or to decrease procrastination describe three types of action strategies: therapeutic treatment, therapeutic prevention and teacher/counselor intervention (Zacks and Hen, 2018). Along these lines, it is possible to develop non-therapeutic strategies in the academic context, for example, teacher- or counselor-led interventions to increase academic behavioral confidence, or interventions to improve the teacher’s external regulation skills. This type of strategy makes it possible to reach a larger student population, using a preventive approach (Freire et al., 2016, 2018; Frenzel et al., 2018).

Implications for the Practice of Educational Psychology at University

These results once again confirm the importance of prior student variables (SR) in students’ academic behavioral confidence, and in their reasons for procrastination and procrastination activities. Hence the importance of understanding individual characteristics (Park and Adler, 2003; Moffa et al., 2016; Murayama et al., 2017; Pidgeon and Pittner, 2017) for preventing academic failure, and for carrying out counseling and educational guidance processes with university students (Parpala and Lindblom-Ylänne, 2012; Bhullar et al., 2014; Eckerlein et al., 2020).

One may also infer the need to intervene with teaching processes, offering training and guidance to help teachers design and develop more regulatory teaching processes, and reduce teaching processes that are non-regulatory or dysregulatory (Asikainen et al., 2014). Some meta-analytical studies (Schneider and Preckel, 2017) have indicated the importance of teacher-student interactions in academic achievement. After analyzing the effect of 105 variables on academic achievement, they found that the variable of teacher “availability and help” occupied the eleventh position, and “being friendly and respectful” with students occupied position 30. However, the present study shows that specific regulatory practices of teachers would have a positive impact on academic behavioral confidence, on reducing procrastination and on increasing students’ academic achievement, and can guide educational practice (Vermunt, 1989; Willcoxson et al., 2011; Villasana et al., 2016; Utriainen et al., 2018).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by http://www.estres.investigacion-psicopedagogica.org/lib/pdf/CERTIFICADO_COMITE_DE_ETICA_UNAV.pdf. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JF and AG-U contributed to conceptualization, design and analysis of data and contributed to first writing. PS wrote the final and revised the article. MV-M, SF, and MG contributed to data collection. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

R & D Project ref. PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra, Pamplona, and Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, Spain; R & D Project UAL18-SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER, University of Almería, Spain, and the European Social Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.inetas.net

References

Aguilar, S., and Barroso, J. M. (2015). La triangulación de datos como estrategia en investigación educativa [Data triangulation as a strategy in educational research]. Píxel-Bit. Rev. Medios Educ. 47, 73–88. doi: 10.12795/pixelbit.2015.i47.05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akbari, O., and Sahibzada, J. (2020). Students’ self-confidence and its impacts on their learning process. Am. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Res. 5, 1–15. doi: 10.46281/aijssr.v5i1.462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alonso-Tapia, J., Rodríguez-Rey, R., Garrido-Hernansaiz, H., Ruiz, M., and Nieto, C. (2018). Coping assessment from the perspective of the person-situation interaction: development and validation of the Situated Coping Questionnaire for Adults (SCQA). Psicothema 28, 479–486. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asikainen, H., Parpala, A., Lindblom-Ylänne, S., Vanthournout, G., and Coertjens, L. (2014). The development of approaches to learning and perceptions of the teaching-learning environment during bachelor level studies and their relation to study success. High. Educ. Stud. 4, 24–36. doi: 10.5539/hes.v4n4p24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batool, S. S. (2020). Academic achievement: interplay of positive parenting, self-esteem, and academic procrastination. Aust. J. Psychol. 72, 174–187. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beswick, G., Rothblum, E., and Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents of student procrastination. Aust. Psychol. 23, 207–217. doi: 10.1080/00050068808255605

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beyer, J. M. (1999). Taming and promoting charisma to change organizations. Leadersh. Q. 10, 307–330. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(99)00019-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhullar, N., Hine, D. W., and Phillips, W. J. (2014). Profiles of psychological well-being in a sample of Australian university students. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 288–294. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1993b). What do inventories of students’ learning really measure? A theoretical review and clarification. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 63, 1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1993.tb01038.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1999b). What the student does: teaching for enhanced learning. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 18, 57–75. doi: 10.1080/07294360.2012.642839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1999a). Teaching for Quality Learning at University. Buckingham: Open UP and SRHE.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1989). Approaches to the enhancement of tertiary teaching. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 8, 7–25. doi: 10.1080/0729436890080102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1993a). From theory to practice: a cognitive systems approach. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 12, 73–85. doi: 10.1080/0729436930120107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B., Kember, D., and Leung, D. Y. P. (2001). The revised two factory study process questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 133–149. doi: 10.1348/000709901158433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brando-Garrido, C., Montes-Hidalgo, J., Limonero, J. T., Gómez-Romero, M. J., and Tomás-Sábado, J. (2020). Relationship of academic procrastination with perceived competence, coping, self-esteem and self-efficacy in nursing students.. Enfermería Clín. 30, 398–403. doi: 10.1016/j.enfcle.2019.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burr, S. M. D. L., and LeFevre, J. A. (2020). Confidence is key: unlocking the relations between ADHD symptoms and math performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 77:101808. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabanach, R. G., Fariña, F., Freire, C., González, P., and Ferradás, M. M. (2013). Diferencias en el afrontamiento del estrés en estudiantes universitarios hombres y mujeres [Differences in coping between male and female university students]. Eur. J. Educ.Psychol. 6, 19–32. doi: 10.30552/ejep.v6i1.89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabanach, R. G., Rodríguez, S., Valle, A., Piñeiro, I., and Millán, P. G. (2007). Metas académicas y vulnerabilidad al estrés en contextos académicos [Academic goals and vulnerability to stress in academic contexts]. Aula Abierta 36, 3–16.

Google Scholar

Cano, F., Pichardo, M. C., Berbén, A. B. G., and Fernández-Cabezas, M. (2020). An integrated test of multidimensionality, convergent, discriminant and criterion validity of the course experience questionnaire: an exploratory structural equation modelling. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 1–13. doi: 10.1080/02602938.2020.1771278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassady, J. C., Pierson, E. E., and Starling, J. M. (2019). Predicting student depression with measures of general and academic anxieties. Front. Educ. 4:11. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2019.00011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chartier, I. S., Gaudreau, P., and Fecteau, M. (2011). From dispositional affect to academic goal attainment: the mediating role of coping. Anxiety Stress Coping 24, 43–58. doi: 10.1080/10615801003725360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clariana, M. (2013). Personalidad, Procrastinación y conducta deshonesta en alumnado de distintos grados universitarios. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 11, 451–472. doi: 10.14204/ejrep.30.13030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clariana, M., Gotzens, C., Badia, M., and Cladellas, R. (2012). Procrastinación y engaño académico desde la Secundaria hasta la Universidad. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 737–754.

Google Scholar

Codina, N., Castillo, I., Pestana, J. V., and Balaguer, I. (2020). Preventing procrastination behaviours: teaching styles and competence in university students. Sustainability 12:2448. doi: 10.3390/su12062448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2017). Theory of Self- vs. externally- regulated learning: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Kauffman, D., and Yılmaz-Soylu, M. (2020a). Achievement Emotions in University Teaching and Learning, Students’ Stress and Well-being. Reseach Topic. Front. Psychol. 11.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Lahortiga-Ramos, F., Laspra-Solís, C., Maestro-Martín, C., Alustiza, I., Aubá, E., et al. (2020b). A structural equation model of achievement emotions, coping strategies and engagement-burnout in undergraduate students: a possible underlying mechanism in facets of perfectionism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2106. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17062106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., López, M., Zapata, L., Sollinas, G., and Fadda, S. (2015a). “Improving mental Health through an online self-assessment and self-help e-utility in university students,” in Progress in Education, Vol. 33, ed. R. V. Nata (New York, NY: Nova Publisher), 63–74.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., Vera, M. M., and Paoloni, P. (2019). Applying the SRL vs ERL theory to the knowledge of achievement emotions in undergraduate university students. Front. Psychol. 10:2070. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Martínez- Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Zapata, L. (2020c). Effects of self-regulation and regulatory teaching on the factors and symptoms of academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:1773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Kauffman, D., and Yilmaz-Soylu, M. (2020d). Differential effects of self- vs. external-regulation on learning approaches, academic achievement, and satisfaction in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:1773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Vera, M. M., Garzón, A., and Fadda, S. (2017). Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, on meta-motivational, and on academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Cardelle-Elawar, M., Sander, P., Justicia, F., et al. (2012). Regulatory teaching and self-regulated learning in college students: confirmatory validation study of the IATLP Scales. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 839–866.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., and Cardelle-Elawar, M. (2015b). The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: a structural model. Front. Psychol. 6:399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckerlein, N., Dresel, M., Steuer, G., Foerst, N., Ziegler, A., Schmitz, B., et al. (2020). “Modelling, assessing, and promoting competences for self-regulated learning in higher education,” in Student Learning in German Higher Education, eds C. Lautenbach, H. A. Pant, M. Toepper, and O. Zlatkin-Troitschanskaia (Wiesbaden: Springer VS), 165–179. doi: 10.1007/978-3-658-27886-1_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entwistle, N. (1991). Approaches to learning and perceptions of the learning environment. High. Educ. 22, 201–204.

Google Scholar

Entwistle, N., and Ramsden, P. (1983). Understanding Student Learning. London: Croom Helm.

Google Scholar

Ferrari, J., Mason, C., and Hammer, C. (2006). Procrastination as a predictor of task perceptions: examining delayed and non-delayed tasks across varied deadlines. Individ. Differ. Res. 4, 28–36.

Google Scholar

Freire, C., Ferradás, M. M., Núñez, J. C., Valle, A., and Vallejo, G. (2018). Eudaimonic well-being and coping with stress in university students: the mediating/moderating role of self-efficacy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:48. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16010048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freire, C., Ferradás, M. M., Valle, A., Núñez, J. C., and Vallejo, G. (2016). Profiles of psychological well-being and coping strategies among university students. Front. Psychol. 7:1554. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Becker-Kurz, B., Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., and Lüdtke, O. (2018). Emotion transmission in the classroom revisited: a reciprocal effects model of teacher and student enjoyment. J. Educ. Psychol. 110, 628–639. doi: 10.1037/edu0000228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón, A. Y., and Gil, J. (2017). Propiedades psicométricas de la versión en español de la prueba PASS. [Psychometric properties of the Spanish versión of PASS.]. RIDEP 1, 149–163. doi: 10.21865/RIDEP43_149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., and Acelas-Russi, L. (2019). “Características y dimensiones de la procrastinación académica en estudiantes universitarios colombianos,” in Diálogos sobre investigación (Bogota: Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz).

Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Amate, J., Paoloni, P. V., Fadda, S., and Pérez, J. F. (2018). A linear empirical model of self-regulation on flourishing, health, procrastination, and achievement, among university students. Front. Psychol. 9:536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentsch, K., Loderer, K., Soriano, C., Fontaine, J. R., Eid, M., Pekrun, R., et al. (2018). Effects of achievement contexts on the meaning structure of emotion words. Cogn. Emot. 32, 379–388. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1287668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, J., De Besa, M. R., and Garzón-Umerenkova, A. (2019). ¿Por qué procrastina el alumnado universitario? Análisis de motivos y caracterización del alumnado con diferentes tipos de motivaciones. [Why do university students procrastinate? An analysis of the reasons and characterization of students with different reasons for procrastination]. Rev. Invest. Educ. 38, 183–200. doi: 10.6018/rie.344781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2008). Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2014). Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015a). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inquiry 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015b). The extended process model of emotion regulation: elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychol. Inquiry 26, 130–137. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harley, J. M., Pekrun, R., Taxer, J. L., and Gross, J. J. (2019). Emotion regulation in achievement situations: an integrated model. Educ. Psychol. 54, 106–126. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2019.1587297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirvonena, R., Yli-Kivistöa, L., Putwain, D. W., Ahonena, T., and Kiuru, N. (2019). School-related stress among sixth-grade students – Associations with academic buoyancy and temperament. Learn. Individ. Differ. 70, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holinka, C. (2015). Stress, emotional intelligence, and life satisfaction in college students. Coll. Stud. J. 49, 300–311.

Google Scholar

Jöreskog, K. G., and Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the SIMPLIS Command Language. Scientific Software International.

Google Scholar

Kember, D., Webster, B. J., and Chan, W. S. (2020). Refocusing the 3P model to incorporate a learning and teaching environment and graduate attributes. Educ. Psychol. 40, 592–607. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2020.1732304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. R., and Seo, E. H. (2015). The relationship between procrastination and academic performance: a meta-analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 82, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. E., Brady, A. C., and Wolters, C. A. (2020). College students’ regulation of cognition, motivation, behavior, and context: distinct or overlapping processes? Learn. Individ. Differ. 80:101872. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2020.101872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirikkanat, B., and Kali-Soyer, M. (2018). A path analysis model pertinent to undergraduates’ academic success: examining academic confidence, psychological capital and academic coping factors. Eur. J. Educ. Res. 7, 133–150. doi: 10.12973/eu-jer.7.1.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R., Krawchuk, L., and Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of undergraduates: low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procrastination. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33, 915–931. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klingsieck, K. B., Grund, A., Schmid, S., and Fries, S. (2013). Why students procrastinate: a qualitative approach. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 54, 397–412. doi: 10.1353/csd.2013.0060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobylińska, D., and Kusev, P. (2019). Flexible emotion regulation: how situational demands and individual differences influence the effectiveness of regulatorystrategies. Front. Psychol. 10:72. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lekwa, A. J., Reddy, L. A., and Shernoff, E. S. (2018). Measuring teacher practices and student academic engagement: a convergent validity study. Sch. Psychol. Q. 34, 109–118. doi: 10.1037/spq0000268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., and Pekrun, R. (eds) (2011). Students’ emotions and academic engagement [Special issue]. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loderer, K., Pekrun, R., and Lester, J. C. (2018). Beyond cold technology: a meta-analytic review of research on emotions in technology-based learning environments. Learn. Instruct. 70:101162. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüftenegger, M., Klug, J., Harrer, K., Langer, M., Spiel, C., and Schober, B. (2016). Students’ achievement goals, learning-related emotions and academic achievement. Front. Psychol. 7:603. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mainhard, T., Oudman, S., Hornstra, L., Bosker, R. J., and Goetz, T. (2018). Student emotions in class: the relative importance of teachers and their interpersonal relations with students. Learn. Instruct. 53, 109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín, M. O., Trujillo, F. D., and Moreno, N. (2003). Estudio y evaluación del estrés académico en estudiantes de Grado de ingenierías industrieales en la Universidad de Málaga [assessment of academic stress in industrial engineering students at the University of Malaga]. Rev. Educ. Hekademos 13, 55–63.

Google Scholar

McDonald, R. P., and Marsh, H. W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: noncentrality and goodness of fit. Psychol. Bull. 107, 245–255. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGee, G. E. (2020). Process Goals Raise Academic Confidence and Performance of First-Generation College Students. Rock Hill, SC: Winthrop University.

Google Scholar

Miller, W. R., and Brown, J. M. (1991). “Self-regulation as a conceptual basis for the prevention and treatment of addictive behaviours,” in Self-Control and the Addictive Behaviours, eds N. Heather, W. R. Miller, and J. Greeley (Sydney: Maxwell Macmillan Publishing Australia), 3–79.

Google Scholar

Moffa, K., Dowdy, E., and Furlong, M. J. (2016). Exploring the contributions of school belonging to complete mental health screening. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 33, 16–32. doi: 10.1017/edp.2016.8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murayama, K., Goetz, T., Malmberg, L.-E., Pekrun, R., Tanaka, A., and Martin, A. J. (2017). “Within-person analysis in educational psychology: importance and illustrations,” in British Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph Series II, Vol. 12, eds D. W. Putwain and K. Smart (Oxford: Wiley), 71–87.

Google Scholar

Nicholson, L., Putwain, D., Connors, L., and Hornby-Atkinson, P. (2013). The key to successful achievement as an undergraduate student: confidence and realistic expectations? Stud. High. Educ. 38, 285–298. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2011.585710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papinczak, T., Young, L., Groves, M., and Haynes, M. (2008). Effects of a metacognitive intervention on students’ approaches to learning and self-efficacy in a first-year medical course. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 13, 213–232. doi: 10.1007/s10459-006-9036-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., and Adler, N. E. (2003). Coping style as a predictor of health and well-being across the first year of medical school. Health Psychol. 22, 627–631. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parpala, A., and Lindblom-Ylänne, S. (2012). Using a research instrument for developing quality at the university. Q. High. Educ. 18, 313–328. doi: 10.1080/13538322.2012.733493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichardo, M. C., Cano, F., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., and Amate-Romera, J. (2018). Self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) in Spanish adolescents: factor structure and rasch analysis. Front. Psychol. 9:1370. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichardo, M. C., Justicia, F., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2014). Factor structure of the self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish universities. Span. J. Psychol. 17:e62. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pidgeon, A. M., and Pittner, L. (2017). Examining the differences between university students’ levels of resilience on mindfulness, psychological distress and coping strategies. Eur. Sci. J. 13, 103–113.

Google Scholar

Putwain, D., Sander, P., and Larkin, D. (2013). Academic self-efficacy in study-related skills and behaviours: relations with learning-related emotions and academic success. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 83, 633–650. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02084.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W. (2018). An examination of the self-referent executive processing model of test anxiety: control, emotional regulation, self-handicapping, and examination performance. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 34, 341–358. doi: 10.1007/s10212-018-0383-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W., Daly, A. L., Chamberlain, S., and Sadreddini, S. H. (2015). ‘Sink or swim’: buoyancy and coping in the cognitive test anxiety – academic performance Relationship. Educ. Psychol. Int. J. Exp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 1807–1825. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2015.1066493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W., and Pescod, M. (2018). Is reducing uncertain control the key to successful test anxiety intervention for secondary school students? Findings from a randomized control trial. Sch. Psychol. Q. 33, 283–292. doi: 10.1037/spq0000228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W., and Sander, P. (2016). Does the confidence of first-year undergraduate students change over time according to achievement goal profile? Stud. High. Educ. 41, 381–398. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2014.934803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsden, P. (1991). A performance indicator of teaching quality in higher education: the course experience questionnaire. Stud. High. Educ. 16, 129–150. doi: 10.1080/03075079112331382944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothblum, E., Solomon, L., and Murakami, J. (1986). Affective, cognitive, and behavioral differences between high and low procrastinators. J. Couns. Psychol. 33, 387–394. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.33.4.387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozental, A., and Carlbring, P. (2014). Understanding and treating procrastination: a review of a common self-regulatory failure. Psychology 5, 1488–1502. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.513160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusk, R., Tamir, M., and Rothbaum, F. (2011). Performance and learning goals for emotional regulation. Motiv. Emot. 35, 444–460. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9229-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Mastoras, S. M., Beaton, L., and Osborne, S. E. (2012). Relationships of personality, affect, emotional intelligence and coping with student stress and academic success: different patterns of association for stress and success. Learn. Individ. Differ. 22, 251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P. (2009). Current developments in measuring academic behavioural confidence. Psychol. Teach. Rev. 15, 32–44.

Google Scholar

Sander, P., and de la Fuente, J. (2020). Modelling students’ academic confidence, personality and academic emotions. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00957-0 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P., de la Fuente, J., Stevenson, K., and Jones, T. (2011). The validation of the academic behavioural confidence scale with spanish psychology students. Psychol. Learn. Teach. 10, 11–24. doi: 10.2304/plat.2011.10.1.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P., Putwain, D., and de la Fuente, J. (2013). “Using structural equation modelling to understand predictors of undergraduate students’ academic performance,” in Theory and Method in Higher Education Research (International Perspectives on Higher Education Research, ed. M. Tight (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 9. doi: 10.1108/S1479-362820130000009015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P., and Sanders, L. (2006). Understanding academic confidence. Psychol. Teach. Rev. 12, 29–39.

Google Scholar

Sander, P., and Sanders, L. (2009). Measuring academic behavioural confidence: the ABC scale revisited. Stud. High. Educ. 34, 19–35. doi: 10.1080/03075070802457058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, L., and Sander, P. (2003). Academic behavioral confidence: a comparison of medical and psychology students. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 13, 633–650.

Google Scholar

Saputra, H. R., Alizamar, A., and Afdal, A. (2020). Relationship of confidence with academic procrastination of middle school students and its implications in guidance and counseling. J. Neo Konseling 2, 1–10. doi: 10.24036/00249kons2020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M., and Preckel, F. (2017). Variables associated with achievement in higher education: a systematic review of meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 143:565. doi: 10.1037/bul0000098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schraw, G., Wadkins, T., and Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: a grounded theory of academic procrastination. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 12–25. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shannon, M., Suldo, S. M., Storey, L. D., O’Brennan, L. M., Shaunessy-Dedrick, E., Ferron, J. M., et al. (2019). Identifying high school freshmen with signs of emotional or academic risk: screening methods appropriate for students in accelerated courses. Sch. Ment. Health 11, 210–227. doi: 10.1007/s12310-018-9297-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, M. P., Peart, D. J., and William, O. J. (2017). Perceived stress in university students studying in a further education college. Res. Post-Compulsory Educ. 22, 442–452. doi: 10.1080/13596748.2017.1362534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoemaker, C. A. (2010). Student confidence as a measure of learning in an undergraduate principles of horticultural science course. Hortechology 20, 683–688. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.20.4.683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois, F., and Tosti, N. (2012). Lost in the moment? An investigation of procrastination, mindfulness, and well-being. J. Rational Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 30, 237–248. doi: 10.1007/s10942-012-0151-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, L., and Rothblum, E. (1984). Academic procrastination: frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. J. Couns. Psychol. 31, 503–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stead, R., Shanahan, M., and Neufeld, R. (2010). “I’ll go to therapy, eventually”: procrastination, stress and mental health. Personal. Individ. Differ. 49, 175–180. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychol. Bull. 133, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, P., and Ferrari, J. (2012). Sex, education and procrastination: an epidemiological study of procrastinators’ characteristics from a global sample. Eur. J. Personal. 27, 51–58. doi: 10.1002/per.1851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, P., and Klingsieck, K. (2015). “Procrastination,” in The International Encyclopedia of The Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn, Vol. 19, ed. J. D. Wright (Oxford: Elsevier), 73–78.

Google Scholar

Steel, P., and Klingsieck, K. B. (2016). Academic procrastination: psychological antecedents revisited. Austr. Psychol. 51, 36–46. doi: 10.1111/ap.12173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudler, E. (2013). Academic Procrastination as Mediated By Executive Functioning, Perfectionism, and Frustration Intolerance in College Students. St. John’s University, New York, NY. doctoral dissertation.

Google Scholar

Tada, A. (2017). The associations among psychological distress, coping style, and health habits in Japanese nursing students: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14:1434. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14111434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Utriainen, J., Tynjälä, P., Kallio, E., and Marttunen, M. (2018). Validation of a modified version of the experiences of teaching and learning questionnaire. Stud. Educ. Eval. 56, 133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.stueduc.2017.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, D. H. M. (1989). “The interplay between internal and external regulation of learning, and the design of process-oriented instruction,” in Paper presented at the Conference of the European Association of Research on Learning and Instruction (Madrid).

Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (1995). Process-oriented instruction in learning and thinking strategies. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 10, 325–349. doi: 10.1007/bf03172925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (1996). Metacognitive, cognitive and affective aspects of learning styles and strategies: a phenomenographic analysis. High. Educ. 31, 25–50. doi: 10.1007/bf00129106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (1998). The regulation of constructive learning processes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 68, 149–171. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1998.tb01281.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (2005). Relations between student learning patterns and personal and contextual factors and academic performance. High. Educ. 49, 205–234. doi: 10.1007/s10734-004-6664-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (2007). The power of teaching-learning environments to influence student learning. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. Monogr. Ser. 4, 73–90. doi: 10.1348/000709906X162406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D., and Donche, V. (2017). A learning patterns perspective on student learning in higher education: state of the art and moving forward. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 29, 269–299. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9414-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villasana, M., Alonso-Tapia, J., and Ruiz, M. (2016). A model for assessing coping and its relation to resilience in adolescence from the perspective of “person-situation interaction”. Personal. Individ. Differ. 98, 250–256. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wäschle, K., Allgaier, A., Lachner, A., Fink, S., and Nückles, M. (2014). Procrastination and self-efficacy: tracing vicious and virtuous circles in self-regulated learning. Learn. Instruct. 29, 103–114. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcoxson, L., Cotter, J., and Joy, S. (2011). Beyond the first-year experience: the impact on attrition of student experiences throughout undergraduate degree studies in six diverse universities. Stud. High. Educ. 36, 331–352. doi: 10.1080/03075070903581533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolters, C. (2003). Understanding procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 179–187. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y. (2020). Research on the influence of interactive teaching method on the learning effect of college english. J. Contemp. Educ. Res. 4, 65–69. doi: 10.26689/jcer.v4i1.974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zacks, S., and Hen, M. (2018). Academic interventions for academic procrastination: a review of the literature. J. Prev. Interv. Community 46, 117–130. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalazar-Jaime, M. F., and Medrano, L. A. (2020). An integrative model of self-regulated learning for university students: the contributions of social cognitive theory of carriers. J. Educ. doi: 10.1177/0022057420904375 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarick, L., and Stonebraker, R. (2009). I’ll do it tomorrow: the logic of procrastination. Coll. Teach. 57, 211–215. doi: 10.1080/87567550903218687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: an overview. Educ. Psychol. 25, 3–17. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2501_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). “Developing self-fulfilling cycles of academic regulation: An analysis of exemplary instructional models,” in Self-Regulated Learning: From Teaching to Self-Reflective Practice, eds D. H. Schunk and B. J. Zimmerman (Guilford Publications), 1–19.

Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J., and Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theoretical Perspectives. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Zusho, A., Pintrich, P. R., and Coppola, B. (2003). Skill and will: the role of motivation and cognition in the learning of college chemistry. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 25, 1081–1094. doi: 10.1080/0950069032000052207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: theory of self-regulated learning vs. externally-regulated learning, academic behavioral confidence, procrastination, university, structural equation modeling

Citation: de la Fuente J, Sander P, Garzón-Umerenkova A, Vera-Martínez MM, Fadda S and Gaetha ML (2021) Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching as Determinants of Academic Behavioral Confidence and Procrastination in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 12:602904. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.602904

Received: 04 September 2020; Accepted: 14 January 2021;
Published: 10 February 2021.

Edited by:

Lawrence Jun Zhang, University of Auckland, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Yu Huang, Wuhan University, China
Juan Luis Castejon, University of Alicante, Spain

Copyright © 2021 de la Fuente, Sander, Garzón-Umerenkova, Vera-Martínez, Fadda and Gaetha. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús de la Fuente, jdlfuente@unav.es; jfuente@ual.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.