Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 26 May 2021
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological Sleep Studies: New Insights to Support and Integrate Clinical Practice Within the Healthcare System View all 14 articles

Dream Activity in Narcoleptic Patients During the COVID-19 Lockdown in Italy

  • 1Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Biomedical and Neuromotor Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
  • 4Department of Humanities, Social Sciences and Cultural Industries, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
  • 5IRCCS Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, Bologna, Italy
  • 6Department of Biomedical, Metabolic and Neural Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
  • 7Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy

Some studies highlighted that patients with narcolepsy type-1 (NT1) experience high lucid dream frequency, and this phenomenon has been associated with a creative personality. Starting from the well-known “pandemic effect” on sleep and dreaming, we presented a picture of dream activity in pharmacologically treated NT1 patients during the Italian lockdown. Forty-three NT1 patients completed a web-survey during Spring 2021 and were compared with 86 matched-controls. Statistical comparisons revealed that: (a) NT1 patients showed greater sleepiness than controls; (b) controls showed higher sleep disturbances than NT1 patients, and this result disappeared when the medication effect in NT1 was controlled; (c) NT1 patients reported higher lucid dream frequency than controls. Focusing on dreaming in NT1 patients, we found that (a) nightmare frequency was correlated with female gender, longer sleep duration, higher intrasleep wakefulness; (b) dream recall, nightmare and lucid dream frequency were positively correlated with sleepiness. Comparisons between low and high NT1 lucid dreamers showed that patients more frequently experiencing lucid dreams reported a greater influence of dreaming during wakefulness, especially concerning problem-solving and creativity. Overall, our results are consistent with previous studies on pandemic dreaming carried out on healthy subjects. Moreover, we confirmed a link between lucidity and creativity in NT1 patients. Considering the small sample size and the cross-sectional design, our findings cannot provide a causal relationship between lucid dreams and the COVID-19 lockdown. Nevertheless, they represent a first contribution to address future studies on this issue, suggesting that some stable characteristics could interact with changes provoked by the pandemic.

Introduction

Narcolepsy type 1 (NT1) is a neurological disorder characterized by excessive daytime sleepiness and sleep abnormalities, mainly dissociated manifestations of Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep. Indeed, rapid access into REM sleep at sleep onset, sleep paralysis, hypnagogic and/or hypnopompic hallucinations, and the pathognomonic symptom cataplexy are key features of this disorder [American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM), 2014].

Narcoleptic patients frequently report unusual and emotional dream experiences (Schredl, 1998; Schiappa et al., 2018). Further, the available literature underlined that NT1 patients often experience a scarce insight of what refers to dream content rather than real experience, leading to “delusional confusion” between dreaming and reality (Hays, 1992; Szucs et al., 2003; Wamsley et al., 2014).

Some studies revealed that narcoleptic patients report a more significant level of awareness during sleep mentation than healthy subjects (Lequerica, 1999; Fosse, 2000). More directly, it has been highlighted that narcolepsy is associated with experiencing lucid dreaming (i.e., the phenomenon of being aware of dreaming during dreaming) (Dodet et al., 2015; Rak et al., 2015). Specifically, Rak et al. (2015) carried out an investigation based on phone-interview and showed that 60 narcoleptic patients had higher dream recall frequency, nightmares, and lucid dreams than controls. This finding was supported by an experimental study, revealing that patients had more lucid dreams and were able to indicate their awareness while they were asleep using a defined eye movement signal (Dodet et al., 2015). Other findings underlined a link between the high metacognition during sleep and creativity personality profile (Zink and Pietrowsky, 2013). Interestingly, in narcoleptic patients lucid dreaming is associated with a higher creative profile, and hypnagogic hallucinations lead to higher creative success and potential, impacting on the creative identity of NT1 patients (Lacaux et al., 2019; D'Anselmo et al., 2020).

The latter findings open new perspectives in the investigation of the lucid dreaming process in narcolepsy (Dodet et al., 2015; Rak et al., 2015), and claim for systematic targeted studies comparing healthy and narcoleptic patients.

Further, dream activity and emotional processing are strictly related (Scarpelli et al., 2019a). In this regard, a recent review highlighted that the neurobiological basis of narcolepsy and patients' mental sleep activity appears to be closely associated (Schiappa et al., 2018). Indeed, neuroimaging studies found that narcolepsy is characterized by limbic dysfunction (Poryazova et al., 2009; Meletti et al., 2015; Vaudano et al., 2019). The amygdala and hypothalamus are also involved in processing emotional memory and fear during wakefulness (Richter-Levin, 2004), which may be responsible for disturbing dreams. In this view, dream experiences could be considered the expression of patient well-being or distress and could be clinically relevant.

We have to underline that recent studies highlighted that, during the COVID-19 pandemic, people reported sleep alterations and, in parallel, high dream recall and nightmare frequency (e.g., Iorio et al., 2020; MacKay and DeCicco, 2020; Gorgoni et al., 2021; Scarpelli et al., 2021a). During the pandemic, people's dreams showed a greater negative emotional charge (e.g., Iorio et al., 2020; Schredl and Bulkeley, 2020; Gorgoni et al., 2021; Scarpelli et al., 2021a) and pandemic-related contents (e.g., Iorio et al., 2020; Schredl and Bulkeley, 2020). Moreover, fragmented and lighter sleep has proven to be linked to dreaming during lockdown in healthy subjects (Scarpelli et al., 2021a), consistently to the “arousal-retrieval hypothesis” pointed out that dream recall may be promoted by awakenings during sleep (Koulack and Goodenough, 1976). Further, a gender difference was observed since females revealed a larger dream recall rate according to the previous literature (e.g., Barrett, 2020; Iorio et al., 2020; Scarpelli et al., 2021a). In addition, aging was associated with a drop in dream recall frequency also during lockdown (Scarpelli et al., 2021a).

Here, for the first time, we present a picture of dream activity in pharmacologically treated narcoleptic patients during the pandemic. We intend to provide a better understanding of dreaming in narcolepsy, reporting the results from a cross-sectional study in which we have compared NT1 and healthy subjects. In light of the current background, we expected that NT1 patients had a greater dream activity (dreams, nightmares and lucid dreams) than controls. We also hypothesized that (1) poor sleep quality and high arousal (i.e., frequent awakenings) are associated with dream frequency in NT1 patients; (2) gender and age are related to dream activity during the pandemic in NT1 patients; (3) lucid dream frequency and creativity are linked in NT1 patients, as highlighted by previous literature.

Materials and Methods

Study Design, and Participants

Patients who met diagnostic criteria for narcolepsy type 1 (NT1) and followed at the Outpatients Clinical for Narcolepsy at the IRCCS—Institute of the Neurological Sciences of Bologna were requested to fill out a web-survey on the Microsoft Azure platform. Specifically, NT1 subjects had to meet the international criteria for NT1 [American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM), 2014].

Also, the web-survey was promoted by means of university communication systems and virtual learning environments to recruit healthy subjects. The survey lasted about 30 min and was addressed to over-18 exclusively.

All questionnaires were collected during spring 2020. Specifically, the survey was available online from March 10, 2020, to May 4, 2020.

Firstly, subjects filled out a self-administered questionnaire on socio-demographic information. In addition, psychological measures, sleep measures and dream variables were collected by specific self-reported questionnaires.

All subjects signed an electronic informed consent before accessing the questionnaires. The subjects also explicitly agreed to provide an email contact and created an identification code to anonymize it. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local ethic committee (Comitato Etico Indipendente di Area Vasta Emilia Centro—CE-AVEC).

Overall, 60 NT1 patients received the link to fill out the web-survey. A total of 43 NT1 patients completed the survey (response rate: 71.67%). In keeping with many studies including patients with rare conditions (e.g., Fortuyn et al., 2010) we chose to compare NT1 patients and healthy controls with a ratio case/matched controls of 1:2 to increase statistical power. Hence, 86 healthy controls were extracted from respondents to the web-survey (Franceschini et al., 2020b) and were matched for age, gender, Italian area, and days elapsed since the beginning of lockdown.

All NT1 patients were on pharmacological treatment: 28 patients on pharmacotherapy with Sodium oxybate; 15 patients on pharmacotherapy with other medications (e.g., Pitolisant; Modafinil). Data from healthy subjects reported in the current study have been presented elsewhere (Franceschini et al., 2020b; Scarpelli et al., 2021a) and were part of a wider project, “Resilience and the COVID-19: how to react to perceived stress. Effects on sleep quality and diurnal behavior/thoughts,” with different objectives concerning the impact of lockdown in the Italian population.

Measures

The webform was composed of 4 sections:

Socio-Demographic and COVID-19-Related Information

This section allowed us to collect the following variables: age, gender, marital status, presence/absence of children, education level, Italian area, occupation, forced quarantine, having COVID-19-infected friends or relatives.

Psychological Measures

Psychological measures were evaluated by the Italian version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) short form (Bottesi et al., 2015), a self-administered questionnaire in which subjects rate the frequency and severity of depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. The 21 Items refer to the previous week and each item is scored on a 4-point scale (0 = “Did not apply to me at all,” to 3 = “Applied to me very much, or most of the time”). Anxiety, depression and stress scores result from the sum of the responses to the items from each subscale multiplied by 2 to suit the original 42 items. The cut-offs for severe rating of depression, anxiety and stress are ≥21, ≥15, and ≥26, respectively (Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995).

Sleep Measures

Sleep measures were assessed by the Italian adaptation of the Medical Outcomes Study- sleep scale (MOS-SS) (Palagini and Manni, 2016), a self-reported questionnaire including 12 items to assess sleep quality and quantity within a 4-week period. Ten of the 12 MOS-SS items are scored on a 6-point categorical scale ranging from “1 = all of the time” to “6 = none of the time.” The question about the time required to fall asleep uses a 5-point categorical response scale ranging from “0 to 15 min,” to “more than 60 min.” “Sleep duration” is reported by subjects as the average number of hours they sleep each night. All domains except sleep duration are converted from 0 to 100, and item 2 is recorded as the average number of hours slept per night (0–24 h). This instrument provides six measures for sleep quality: sleep disturbance, snoring, awakening short of breath or with headache, sleep adequacy, somnolence and sleep duration/optimal sleep. Finally, MOS-SS has a global index to assess quality of sleep defined Sleep Index II or Sleep problem index, an aggregate measure of responses to nine of the questions about the four sleep domains (sleep disturbance, awakening short of breath or with headache, sleep adequacy and sleepiness). Scores for the MOS-SS Sleep Index II range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating greater sleep problems (cut-off = 25.8; Hays et al., 2005). In keeping with previous work (Scarpelli et al., 2021a), we have taken into consideration also self-reported evaluation of the intrasleep wakefulness (item 8), dichotomized as follows: “high intrasleep wakefulness” (answer “3,” “4,” “5”) and “low intrasleep wakefulness” (answer “1,” “2”).

Dream Variables

Dreaming was assessed by the Italian adaptation of the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire (MADRE) (Settineri et al., 2019), a self-reported questionnaire with 20 items. Dream recall frequency (item 1) was rated by a 7-point scale format (0 = never and 6 = almost every morning). Nightmare frequency (item 4) and lucid dream frequency (item 10) was rated by an 8-point scale (0 = never and 8 = several times a week). The dream and nightmare frequency were asked with reference to the last month. Also, emotional intensity, emotional tone and nightmare distress were evaluated (item 2, 3, 5). Individuals' attitudes toward dreams (i.e., the personal meaning of one's own dreams and the impression that dreams provide impulses or pointers for waking life) were assessed by item 12, consisting of eight sentences with a 5-point format (0 = Not at all/4 = Totally). The impact of dream on daily life (the frequency of dream sharing, the recording of dreams, the dreams affecting day-time mood, the creative. dreams, and the problem-solving dreams, from item 13 to 17). Dream variables eliciting utilizations of dreams (i.e., frequency of dream sharing, recording of dreams, dreams affecting day-time mood, creative dreams, problem-solving dreams) were in 8-point scale with 0 = never and 8 = several times a week.

Further, the questionnaire required information on presence/absence and percentage of recurring nightmares (item 6, 7) and on and deja-vu experiences based on dream (item 18). Additionally, the questionnaire includes specific items on age of first lucid dream (item 11); reading about dreams (item 19, rated using a 3-points scale) and helpful dream literature (item 20, rated using a 5-points scale).

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive analyses were conducted to outline the sociodemographic characteristics of the sample, considering the following features: age, gender, marital status, presence/absence of children. education level, occupation, forced quarantine, having COVID-19-infected friends or relatives.

Firstly, the Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare differences between the NT1 and control group concerning the psychological measures extracted from DASS (anxiety, depression and stress) and sleep variables extracted from MOS-SS (sleep disturbances, snoring, awakening short of breath or with headache, sleep adequacy, sleepiness, and sleep problem index) and the oneiric measures regarding dream frequency (dreams, nightmares, lucid dreams) and qualitative emotional features (emotional intensity, emotional tone and nightmares distress).

We performed non-parametric Spearman's correlations (rank bivariate or rank biserial, depending on the variable type) to assess the relationship between NT1 dreaming (dream recall frequency, nightmare frequency, lucid dream frequency) and (1) demographic factors (age, gender) previously associated to oneiric activity (Scarpelli et al., 2021a) and (2) sleep measures (sleep duration and intrasleep awakenings extracted from MOS-SS, sleep disturbances, snoring, sleepiness) to test the arousal-retrieval hypothesis.

In order to better understand the relationship between lucid dreaming and the waking-correlates of oneiric activity in narcolepsy, we carried out statistical comparisons between high and low lucid dreamers (Mann-Whitney U test), considering the following independent variables: attitude toward dreams (items 12—averaged score), and items regarding utilization of dreaming during wakefulness (item 13-frequency of dream sharing, item 14-recording of dreams, item 15- dreams affecting day-time mood, item 16- creative dreams, item 17- problem-solving dreams). Item 10 about lucid dreaming was dichotomized, as follows: “low lucid dreamers” (answer from 0-never to 5-about 2/3 times a month; N = 27) and “high lucid dreamers” (answer from 6-about once a week to 7-several times a week; N = 16).

The statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25.0 and Matlab R2019a. P-values of < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Characteristics of Samples

The characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1. In short, 37.2% of the samples were young subjects (age 18–25). Female gender was represented by 58.1% of the samples. Subjects were mainly single, both in NT1 (37.2%) and HC group (36%). Further, most of the participants received high school education in NT1 (55.8%) and HC group (33.7%). A high percentage of respondents were employed both in NT1 (55.8%) and HC group (58.1%). Further, 58.1% came from the North of Italy in both groups. Also, more than 70% of the individuals of each sample did not have children. The majority of the respondents from both groups did not experience a forced quarantine (97.7 %). Among healthy individuals, 14% had COVID-19-infected friends or relatives, while in the NT1 group only a subject had a significant other infected.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic and COVID-19-related characteristics.

Comparisons Between Narcoleptics and Controls

Table 2 summarized the statistical comparisons between the NT1 and control group, considering psychological, sleep and dream measures. The results revealed no difference in psychological measures. Conversely, concerning sleep variables, NT1 patients reported significantly higher sleepiness than controls (p < 0.001). Also, healthy individuals reported higher sleep complaints than NT1 patients (p < 0.001). Considering that the NT1 group was on pharmacological treatment, we performed a control analysis to check whether the pharmacotherapy could impact self-reported sleep disturbances among patients (please, see the Supplementary Material). This supplementary analysis showed that the difference in sleep disturbances between controls and NT1 subjects is maintained only when patients were treated with Sodium oxybate, a pharmacological treatment promoting slow-wave sleep and sleep quality (Franceschini et al., 2020a). Conversely, the difference in sleep disturbances disappears comparing controls with NT1 patients treated with other types of medications.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean rank of psychological, sleep and dream measures and results of statistical comparisons (Mann-Whitney U Test) between controls and narcoleptic patients.

Crucially, patients showed higher lucid dreams than controls (p = 0.014). No other significant difference was found regarding dream frequency and dream emotionality.

Demographic and Sleep Features Related to Dream Frequency

Spearman's correlation analyses (see Table 3) showed that (a) nightmare frequency in the NT1 group was related to female gender (p = 0.030), longer sleep duration (p = 0.043), higher intrasleep wakefulness (p = 0.006); (b) dream recall frequency, nightmare frequency and lucid dream frequency were positively correlated with sleepiness (p = 0.003).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Features related to dream frequency in narcoleptic patients: Spearman's correlation coefficients.

Comparisons Between High and Low Lucid Dreamers

Table 4 summarized the statistical comparisons between low and high NT1 lucid dreamers revealing that high lucid dreamers, as compared to low lucid dreamers: (a) had higher mean score in the factor “attitude toward dream” (p = 0.010); (b) more frequently recorded their dreams (p = 0.036); (c) more frequently had dreams influencing their creativity during wakefulness (p = 0.011); (d) more frequently had dreams influencing their problem-solving during wakefulness (p = 0.001).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean rank of attitude toward dreams (averaged score), items regarding utilization of dream during wakefulness and results of statistical comparisons (Mann-Whitney U Test) between narcoleptic patients with low and high lucid dreamers.

Discussion

For the first time, the current study aimed to investigate pandemic's dream activity in NT1 patients. Overall, we revealed that NT1 patients showed greater sleepiness than healthy subjects, consistently to their diagnosis [American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM), 2014]. In contrast, the healthy group reported higher sleep disturbances than NT1 patients as measured by MOS-SS. This result could appear surprising, nevertheless, we showed that the difference disappeared when matched-controls were compared with NT1 patients treated with medications different from Sodium oxybate. In other words, the observed difference considering the whole NT1 group can be ascribed to effective treatment with Sodium oxybate on nocturnal sleep, which reduced disturbed sleep across the night (Franceschini et al., 2020a). In parallel, it should be noted that our data were collected during Spring 2020. In this regard, several studies demonstrated that the pandemic is provoking remarkable sleep alterations in healthy subjects (Blume et al., 2020; Casagrande et al., 2020; Cellini et al., 2020; Franceschini et al., 2020b; Wright et al., 2020), especially causing insomnia symptoms (i.e., COVID-Somnia; Gupta and Pandi-Perumal, 2020).

However, studies that investigated nocturnal sleep quality in narcoleptic patients during lockdown reported mixed results. Changes in sleep quality resulted related to changes in work habits in adult NT1 patients in the study of Postiglione et al. (2021). They found no significant alterations in patients working or studying at home and increased sleep disturbances in patients that do not change their working schedule (Postiglione et al., 2021). A study on NT1 and NT2 patients reported an increase in sleep fragmentation in 43.4% and not change in 35.5% of the sample (Rodrigues Aguilar et al., 2020). Finally, recent data on pharmacologically treated NT1 children and adolescents reported no differences in sleep quality during lockdown compared to an earlier period (Filardi et al., submitted).

Concerning the primary objective of our investigation, we revealed that only lucid dreams frequency significantly differed between groups. In line with previous literature (Dodet et al., 2015; Rak et al., 2015), patients with narcolepsy had higher lucid dreams frequency than healthy subjects. Surprisingly, no difference was found in emotional dream features.

Consistently to the findings on healthy subjects during lockdown (e.g., Scarpelli et al., 2021a), we found that nightmares in NT1 patients were more frequent in women. This result is also in line with the evidence that females reported higher rates of negative emotions in their sleep mentation during pandemic (Barrett, 2020). Barrett suggests that this gender difference may be explained in light of the “continuity-hypothesis,” considering women more vulnerable to anxiety and mood alteration than men during lockdown (Barrett, 2020). The impact of gender on dream activity is well-established (Schredl and Reinhard, 2008) and may be independent by the COVID-19 health emergency.

It should be noted that the cross-sectional protocol makes it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the link between pandemic and the differences highlighted between NT1 patients and control subjects. Moreover, the small sample size of NT1 patients and a response rate of around 70% could represent a serious weakness of our study. On the one hand, the limited number of patients involving in the study is not surprising since NT1 is a rare condition affecting 0.02–0.06% of adults, as highlighted from epidemiological studies in the United States and Europe (e.g., Mignot et al., 1997; Longstreth et al., 2007; Kornum et al., 2017). On the other hand, we have to mention that the small NT1 sample size did not allow us to study directly the impact of adverse events (forced quarantine, COVID-19-infected relatives or friends) on emotional dream features since the pandemic-related conditions were reported in ~2% of our sample. In fact, all patients and healthy controls spent that period in conditions similar to quarantine due to the lockdown from March 10, 2020, to May 4, 2020, that forced people to stay at home in confinement. For all these reasons, our considerations should be taken with caution. Actually, we can only describe a picture of dream activity in narcolepsy during Spring 2020 without disentangling whether we observed a stable phenomenon in NT1 or a condition influenced by the COVID-19 emergency (i.e., stay-at-home in confinement).

Although we showed that sleep disturbances were attenuated by pharmacotherapy in the NT1 group, we revealed that sleepiness was correlated with dream recall, nightmare, and lucid dream frequency. In keeping with the activation-hypothesis (D'Atri et al., 2019; Scarpelli et al., 2021b) and the so-called arousal-retrieval model (Koulack and Goodenough, 1976), this result may be interpreted as an index of lighter nocturnal sleep. More directly, a greater amount of intrasleep wakefulness was positively associated with nightmare frequency, as highlighted by previous evidence (Scarpelli et al., 2021a). Awakenings during sleep would guarantee the elaboration of dream material and its transfer from short-term memory storage to long-term storage (Koulack and Goodenough, 1976; Scarpelli et al., 2021b).

Interestingly, NT1 patients, as well as control subjects (see Scarpelli et al., 2021a) reported an association between frightening dreams and higher sleep duration. This finding is only apparently in conflict with the activation/arousal-retrieval model. In fact, the phenomenon of sleep extension during lockdown has been described by Bottary et al. (2020) highlighting that even if people spontaneously prolonged their sleep duration, the rest could be featured by poor quality and fragmentation, and confirmed by recent data (Alfonsi et al., 2021).

The comparisons between high and low lucid dreamers confirmed that the increased awareness of their own mental sleep activity was linked to the perception that the oneiric dimension has a remarkable impact on daily-life. Specifically, the greater problem-solving and creative dreams in high lucid dreamers are consistent with the studies suggesting that lucid dreaming and dissociated REM sleep manifestations are associated with creativity in narcoleptic patients (Lacaux et al., 2019; D'Anselmo et al., 2020). Additionally, it is worth noting that a greater awareness of dreams may have a role in emotional regulation (Scarpelli et al., 2019a). Again, it should be mentioned that our results cannot provide a direct association between pandemic, creativity and lucid dreams in NT1 patients. However, in keeping with the available literature (Dresler et al., 2014; Rak et al., 2015; Schiappa et al., 2018), we can speculate that narcoleptic patients with more frequent experiences of lucid dreams have developed a coping strategy in their dream experience to face the negative emotional contents linked to the COVID-19 emergency. In this view, narcoleptic patients reporting high lucid dreams felt that lucidity provides relief for their nightmares (Rak et al., 2015). We have also to consider that high lucid dreamers in our sample reported a greater attitude toward oneiric experience, being more prone to record their dreams. In other words, these subjects reported high dream salience (Cohen, 1979) that may be considered an intraindividual stable and COVID-unrelated feature affecting dream recall frequency (Schredl and Göritz, 2015).

Conclusions

The main finding of the current study is the presence of higher lucid dreams frequency in NT1 patients than healthy subjects during pandemic. In line with previous studies, we highlighted that sleep measures influenced oneiric activity (Bottary et al., 2020; Gorgoni et al., 2021; Scarpelli et al., 2021a) and female gender was linked with higher nightmare frequency (Barrett, 2020; Scarpelli et al., 2021a). In addition, NT1 high lucid dreamers had more dream salience and their oneiric activity appeared to significantly affect the wakefulness (Schiappa et al., 2018). Interestingly, we confirmed a link between lucid dreaming and the creative dimension in NT1 patients.

Although we reported for the first time a picture of dreaming and sleep pattern in NT1 patients during pandemic, we have to emphasize that our findings cannot provide a causal relationship between lucid dreams and the COVID-19 lockdown. Only a longitudinal design could clarify whether the changes detected are linked to the pandemic or whether they are stable features in the investigated groups. Indeed, using retrospective questionnaires represents an important limitation of our study. In this regard, the literature points to that perspective diaries and collecting dream reports immediately after awakening are more reliable methods to obtain information on dreaming (Scarpelli et al., 2019b). Similarly, an intrinsic limitation of the study concerning the use of a self-reported questionnaire to assess sleep patterns without the support of any objective measures (e.g., actigraphy).

Finally, we believe that the present study offers a first contribution to address future research on COVID-19 outbreak and individual's well-being, suggesting that some trait-like characteristics such as individual attitude toward dreams, gender and also underlying sleep disorders could interact with changes provoked by the pandemic.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the study protocol was approved by the local ethics committee (Comitato Etico Indipendente di Area Vasta Emilia Centro—CE-AVEC). Electronic informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to starting the investigation. Participants could withdraw from the survey at any moment without providing any justification. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SS and VA provided substantial contributions to the conception of the work, deep analysis of the literature, study design, development, and final approval of the manuscript. CF and LD contributed in the design of the study and participated in the development and revision of the work and agreement for final approval of the manuscript. AD'A and MG contributed to data analysis and agreement for final approval of the manuscript. AM and GP contributed to the revision of the work and agreement for final approval of the manuscript. GP, LD, and CF contributed to deep revision of the work, with literature analysis and agreement for final approval of the manuscript contributed to deep revision of the work, with literature analysis and agreement for final approval of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

GP is a consultant and participated in advisory board for UCB Pharma, Jazz pharmaceuticals, Bioprojet, Takeda, and Idorsia outside the submitted work.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681569/full#supplementary-material

References

Alfonsi, V., Gorgoni, M., Scarpelli, S., Zivi, P., Sdoia, S., Mari, E., et al. (2021). COVID-19 lockdown and poor sleep quality: not the whole story. J. Sleep Res. e13368. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) (2014). International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 3rd Edn. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (2020). Dreams about COVID-19 versus normative dreams: trends by gender. Dreaming 30, 216–221. doi: 10.1037/drm0000149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blume, C., Schmidt, M. H., and Cajochen, C. (2020). Effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on human sleep and rest-activity rhythms. Curr. Biol. 30, 795–797. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottary, R., Simonelli, G., Cunningham, T. J., Kensinger, E. A., and Mantua, J. (2020). Sleep extension: an explanation for increased pandemic dream recall? Sleep 43:zsaa131. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottesi, G., Ghisi, M., Altoè, G., Conforti, E., Melli, G., and Sica, C. (2015). The Italian version of the depression anxiety stress scales-21: factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr. Psychiatry 60, 170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casagrande, M., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., and Forte, G. (2020). The enemy who sealed the world: effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Med. 75, 12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cellini, N., Canale, N., Mioni, G., and Costa, S. (2020). Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Sleep Res. 29:e13074. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, D. B. (1979). “Remembering and forgetting dreaming,” in Functional Disorders of Memory, eds J. F. Kihlstrom, and F. J. Evans (New York, NY: Wiley), 239–274.

Google Scholar

D'Anselmo, A., Agnoli, S., Filardi, M., Pizza, F., Mastria, S., Corazza, G. E., et al. (2020). Creativity in narcolepsy type 1: the role of dissociated REM sleep manifestations. Nat. Sci. Sleep 12, 1191–1200. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S277647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Atri, A., Scarpelli, S., Schiappa, C., Pizza, F., Vandi, S., Ferrara, M., et al. (2019). Cortical activation during sleep predicts dream experience in narcolepsy. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 6, 445–455. doi: 10.1002/acn3.718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodet, P., Chavez, M., Leu-Semenescu, S., Golmard, J. L., and Arnulf, I. (2015). Lucid dreaming in narcolepsy. Sleep 38, 487–497. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dresler, M., Eibl, L., Fischer, C. F., Spoormaker, V. I., Steiger, A., Czisch, M., et al. (2014). Volitional components of consciousness vary across wakefulness, dreaming and lucid dreaming. Front. Psychol. 4:987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortuyn, H. D., Lappenschaar, M. A., Furer, J. W., Hodiamont, P. P., Cees, A. T., Renier, W. O., et al. (2010). Anxiety and mood disorders in narcolepsy: a case–control study. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 32, 49–956. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fosse, R. (2000). REM mentation in narcoleptics and normals: an empirical test of two neurocognitive theories. Conscious Cogn. 9, 488–509. doi: 10.1006/ccog.2000.0466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, C., Musetti, A., Zenesini, C., Palagini, L., Scarpelli, S., Quattropani, M. C., et al. (2020b). Poor sleep quality and its consequences on mental health during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. Front. Psychol. 11:574475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, C., Pizza, F., Antelmi, E., Folli, M. C., and Plazzi, G. (2020a). Narcolepsy treatment: pharmacological and behavioral strategies in adults and children. Sleep Breath. 24, 615–627. doi: 10.1007/s11325-019-01894-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgoni, M., Scarpelli, S., Alfonsi, V., Annarumma, L., Cordone, S., Stravolo, S., et al. (2021). Pandemic dreams: quantitative and qualitative features of the oneiric activity during the lockdown due to COVID-19 in Italy. Sleep Med. 81, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, R., and Pandi-Perumal, S. R. (2020). COVID-Somnia: how the pandemic affects sleep/wake regulation and how to deal with it? Sleep Vigil. 3, 1–3. doi: 10.1007/s41782-020-00118-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hays, P. (1992). False but sincere accusations of sexual assault made by narcoleptic [correction of narcotic] patients. Med. Leg. J. 60, 265–271. doi: 10.1177/002581729206000405

CrossRef Full Text

Hays, R. D., Martin, S. A., Sesti, A. M., and Spritzer, K. L. (2005). Psychometric properties of the 552 medical outcomes study sleep measure. Sleep Med. 6, 41–44. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2004.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iorio, I., Sommantico, M., and Parrello, S. (2020). Dreaming in the time of COVID-19: a quali-quantitative Italian study. Dreaming 30, 199–215. doi: 10.1037/drm0000142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornum, B., Knudsen, S., Ollila, H., Pizza, F., Jennum, P., Dauvilliers, Y., et al. (2017). Narcolepsy. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 3, 1–19. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2016.100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koulack, D., and Goodenough, D. R. (1976). Dream recall and dream recall failure: an arousal-retrieval model. Psychol. Bull. 83, 975–984. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacaux, C., Izabelle, C., Santantonio, G., De Villèle, L., Frain, J., Lubart, T., et al. (2019). Increased creative thinking in narcolepsy. Brain 142, 1988–1999. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lequerica, A. (1999). Dream cognition and rapid eye movement sleep in the narcolepsy syndrome. J. Ment. Image. 23, 85–98.

Longstreth, W. T. Jr, Koepsell, T. D., Ton, T. G., Hendrickson, A. F., and Van Belle, G. (2007). The epidemiology of narcolepsy. Sleep 30, 13–26. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.1.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, C., and DeCicco, T. L. (2020). Pandemic dreaming: the effect of COVID-19 on dream imagery, a pilot study. Dreaming 30, 222–234. doi: 10.1037/drm0000148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meletti, S., Vaudano, A. E., Pizza, F., Ruggieri, A., Vandi, S., Teggi, A., et al. (2015). The brain correlates of laugh and cataplexy in childhood narcolepsy. J Neurosci. 35, 11583–11594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0840-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mignot, E., Kimura, A., Lattermann, A., Lin, X., Yasunaga, S., Mueller-Eckhardt, G., et al. (1997). Extensive HLA class II studies in 58 non-DRB1* 15 (DR2) narcoleptic patients with cataplexy. Tissue Antigens 49, 329–341. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0039.1997.tb02761.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palagini, L., and Manni, R. (2016). Misurare il sonno: repertorio delle scale di valutazione dei disturbi del sonno. Torino, ON: Minerva medica, 3–4.

Google Scholar

Poryazova, R., Schnepf, B., Werth, E., Khatami, R., Dydak, U., and Meier, D. (2009). Evidence for metabolic hypothalamo- amygdala dysfunction in narcolepsy. Sleep 32, 607–613. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.5.607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Postiglione, E., Pizza, F., Ingravallo, F., Vignatelli, L., Filardi, M., Mangiaruga, A., et al. (2021). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdown on narcolepsy type 1 management. Brain Behav. 11:e01955. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rak, M., Beitinger, P., Steiger, A., Schredl, M., and Dresler, M. (2015). Increased lucid dreaming frequency in narcolepsy. Sleep 38, 787–792. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter-Levin, G. (2004). The amygdala, the hippocampus, and emotional modulation of memory. Neuroscientist 10, 31–39. doi: 10.1177/1073858403259955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues Aguilar, A. C., Frange, C., Huebra, L., Dias Gomes, A. C., Tufik, S., and Santos Coelho, F. M. (2020). The effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on patients with narcolepsy. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 17, 621–627. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.8952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpelli, S., Alfonsi, V., Gorgoni, M., Giannini, A. M., and De Gennaro, L. (2021b). Investigation on neurobiological mechanisms of dreaming in the new decade. Brain Sci. 11:220. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpelli, S., Alfonsi, V., Mangiaruga, A., Musetti, A., Quattropani, M. C., Lenzo, V., et al. (2021a). Pandemic nightmares: effects on dream activity of the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Sleep Res. e13300. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpelli, S., Bartolacci, C., D'Atri, A., Gorgoni, M., and De Gennaro, L. (2019a). The functional role of dreaming in emotional processes. Front. Psychol. 10:459. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpelli, S., Gorgoni, M., D'Atri, A., Ferrara, M., and De Gennaro, L. (2019b). Structural and functional differences in brain mechanisms of dream recall. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 30, 269–281. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-813743-7.00018-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiappa, C., Scarpelli, S., D'Atri, A., Gorgoni, M., and De Gennaro, L. (2018). Narcolepsy and emotional experience: a review of the literature. Behav. Brain Funct. 14, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12993-018-0151-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schredl, M. (1998). Dream content in patients with narcolepsy: preliminary findings. Dreaming 8, 103–107. doi: 10.1023/B:DREM.0000005900.63728.a7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schredl, M., and Bulkeley, K. (2020). Dreaming and the COVID-19 pandemic: a survey in a US sample. Dreaming 30, 189–198. doi: 10.1037/drm0000146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schredl, M., and Göritz, A. S. (2015). Changes in dream recall frequency, nightmare frequency, and lucid dream frequency over a 3-year period. Dreaming 25, 81–87. doi: 10.1037/a0039165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schredl, M., and Reinhard, I. (2008). Gender differences in dream recall: a meta-analysis. J. Sleep Res. 17, 125–131. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00626.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Settineri, S., Frisone, F., Alibrandi, A., and Merlo, E. M. (2019). Italian adaptation of the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire (MADRE): age, gender and dream recall effects. Int. J. Dream Res. 12, 119–129. doi: 10.11588/ijodr.2019.1.59328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szucs, A., Janszky, J., Holl,ó, A., Migléczi, G., and Halász, P. (2003). Misleading hallucinations in unrecognized narcolepsy. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 108, 314–316. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00114.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaudano, A. E., Pizza, F., Talami, F., Plazzi, G., and Meletti, S. (2019). The neuronal network of laughing in young patients with untreated narcolepsy. Neurology 92, e504–e514. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wamsley, E., Donjacour, C. E., Scammell, T. E., Lammers, G. J., and Stickgold, R. (2014). Delusional confusion of dreaming and reality in narcolepsy. Sleep 37, 419–422. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, K. P., Linton, S. K., Withrow, D., Casiraghi, L., Lanza, S. M., de la Iglesia, H., et al. (2020). Sleep in university students prior to and during COVID-19 stay-at-home orders. Curr. Biol. 30, 797–798. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zink, N., and Pietrowsky, R. (2013). Relationship between lucid dreaming, creativity and dream characteristics. Int J Dream Res. 6, 98–103. doi: 10.11588/ijodr.2013.2.10640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19 pandemic, creativity, lucid dreaming, nightmares, dream recall, sleep, narcolepsy

Citation: Scarpelli S, Alfonsi V, D'Anselmo A, Gorgoni M, Musetti A, Plazzi G, De Gennaro L and Franceschini C (2021) Dream Activity in Narcoleptic Patients During the COVID-19 Lockdown in Italy. Front. Psychol. 12:681569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681569

Received: 16 March 2021; Accepted: 29 April 2021;
Published: 26 May 2021.

Edited by:

Federica Scarpina, Istituto Auxologico Italiano (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Armando D'Agostino, University of Milan, Italy
Franca Crippa, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Scarpelli, Alfonsi, D'Anselmo, Gorgoni, Musetti, Plazzi, De Gennaro and Franceschini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Serena Scarpelli, serena.scarpelli@uniroma1.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.