Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 December 2021
Sec. Neuropsychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Mechanistic and Clinical Principles of Item-level Scoring Methods Applied to the Category Fluency Test and Other Tests of Semantic Memory View all 8 articles

Emotional Valence Affects Word Retrieval During Verb Fluency Tasks in Alzheimer’s Dementia

  • Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, College of Health Professions, The University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Knoxville, TN, United States

Individuals with amnestic Alzheimer’s disease (AD) often demonstrate preserved emotional processing skills despite the neurodegenerative disease that affects their limbic system. Emotional valence encompasses the encoding and retrieval of memory and it also affects word retrieval in healthy populations, but it remains unclear whether these effects are preserved in individuals with amnestic AD. Previous studies used a variety of encoding procedures and different retrieval methods that resulted in mixed findings. Therefore, the purpose of the current study is to investigate whether emotional enhancement of memory effects is observed in an experimental condition where the memory encoding process is not required, namely verb (action) fluency tasks. Seventeen participants who were cognitively healthy older adults (CHOA) and 15 participants with amnestic AD were asked to complete verb fluency tasks, and the relative degree of emotional valence observed in their responses was compared between the two groups. A neuropsychological test battery was administered to determine the participants’ cognitive and linguistic profiles, and correlational analyses were conducted to delineate relationships between emotional valence, verbal memory, and learning abilities. The results indicated that the participants with amnestic AD produced words with higher emotional valence (i.e., more pleasant words) compared to CHOA during action fluency testing. In addition, the degree of emotional valence in the words was negatively correlated with verbal memory and learning skills, showing that those with poorer memory skills tend to retrieve words with higher emotional valence. The findings are consistent with those previous studies that stressed that individuals with AD have preserved emotional enhancement of memory effects and may benefit from them for retrieval of information, which may offer some insight into the development of novel rehabilitative strategies for this population.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and Alzheimer’s disease-related dementias (ADRDs) represent a worldwide public health crisis that affects the daily lives of those with dementia, presents significant and substantial challenges to their caregivers, and results in a considerable cost of care (Alzheimer’s Association, 2021). Those with AD may exhibit different phenotypes of dementia, although the most commonly observed phenotype involves substantial impairment of memory (McKhann et al., 2011), namely amnestic AD. Individuals with amnestic AD often exhibit breakdowns in communication skills and progressive declines in memory and other cognitive changes, which can lead to decreased social activity and engagement and to increased distress in both patients and caregivers (Kavé and Goral, 2016; Alzheimer’s Association, 2021). Furthermore, the constellation of cognitive and linguistic symptoms may prevent patients with AD from implementing strategies to control or decrease the risk of further cognitive declines, such as active engagement in cognitive, social, and leisure activities (Di Marco et al., 2014; Yates et al., 2016).

Although no definitive cure for AD has been developed, some progress has been made in understanding the cognitive and communication disorders exhibited by individuals with AD and to determine facilitative strategies to maintain or maximize their remaining cognitive and communication skills. For example, whereas episodic and semantic memory breakdowns are observed in individuals with AD from early in the course of the disease, implicit memory is relatively well preserved in earlier stages of the disease (Harrison et al., 2007; De Wit et al., 2021). Accordingly, management and treatment approaches capitalize on these relatively intact cognitive domains that are less affected by the neuropathological changes in AD, such as spaced retrieval training (Oren et al., 2014).

In addition to implicit memory being relatively preserved until later in the disease, the effect of emotion on memory in AD might hold potential in development of intervention and management strategies, particularly because some emotional processing skills also remain relatively intact in AD (Labar et al., 2005; Blessing et al., 2006; Broster et al., 2012; Perrin et al., 2012; Baran et al., 2014; Guzmán-Vélez et al., 2014; Giffard et al., 2015; Gomez-Gallego and Gomez-Garcia, 2017; van Dulmen et al., 2017). For example, individuals with AD often exhibit feelings without memory, anecdotally reporting that they remember how they felt during a conversation, although they do not recall what was discussed, when it happened, or even with whom they spoke. Consistent with anecdotal reports, research literature indicates that emotional processes are actually better preserved and stored than explicit or declarative memory in AD (Guzmán-Vélez et al., 2014). In addition, individuals with AD showed no differences compared to healthy older adults when rating or recognizing emotion in images or words (Labar et al., 2005; Perrin et al., 2012; Baran et al., 2014; Gomez-Gallego and Gomez-Garcia, 2017). Furthermore, emotion in AD seems to affect significantly the patients’ memory formation and behaviors, at least during the earlier stages of disease progression (Wright et al., 2007; Nashiro and Mather, 2011; Klein-Koerkamp et al., 2012; Perrin et al., 2012; Kalenzaga et al., 2015).

Emotional valence refers to how pleasant a stimulus is and it can be measured at the lexical level to characterize degrees of emotion in a word (Warriner et al., 2013; Kuperman et al., 2014; Kauschke et al., 2019). Emotional valence ratings range from 1 to 9, with a higher value indicating more pleasantness or happiness. For example, the emotional valence of “dance” is 7.27, while that of “kneel” is 4. When information is presented to or retrieved from individuals with AD in the context or with the content of positive or negative emotion, there exist facilitatory effects of emotion on memory formation (retrieval or recognition) and word processing (Labar et al., 2005; Brueckner and Moritz, 2009 Sundstrøm, 2011; Perrin et al., 2012; Giffard et al., 2015; Sava et al., 2015; Bohn et al., 2016; Gomez-Gallego and Gomez-Garcia, 2017; Rodríguez-Ferreiro et al., 2019). For example, individuals with mild AD were better able to recall the gist of memory when the information was provided with positive or negative emotions versus neutral context (Perrin et al., 2012).

To date, however, findings on the effects of emotional valence on memory processes in AD have been somewhat divergent. Whereas some studies report that individuals with AD can benefit from emotional content or context during their encoding, recall, or recognition, others presented contradictory findings demonstrating no such effects (Hamann et al., 2000; Kazui et al., 2000, 2003; Abrisqueta-Gomez et al., 2002; Boller et al., 2002; Kensinger et al., 2002; LaBar et al., 2005; Satler et al., 2007; Brueckner and Moritz, 2009; Perrin et al., 2012; Baran et al., 2014; Chainay et al., 2014; Kalenzaga et al., 2015; Gomez-Gallego and Gomez-Garcia, 2017). This inconsistency may stem from heterogeneity in participant characteristics (e.g., severity of dementia, cognitive profiles such as degree of attention, and deficits in executive functioning) in the current literature. However, more crucial differences between published reports include the nature of experimental conditions and tasks that comprise the study design, as previous studies relied heavily on non-standardized experimental procedures. For example, different types of stimuli were used including written words, recorded dialogues, images of natural or manufactured objects, and photos of living and non-living objects. In addition, a variety of experimental paradigms have been tested, including perceptual priming, immediate or delayed recall, and explicit memory paradigms like free or cued recall.

Demonstrating whether and how experimental paradigms impact emotional valence findings in AD, Sava et al. (2015) reported that individuals with AD may not show enhanced memory effects during a recognition task without deeper and richer encoding, but they show a positivity bias (better recognition of positive memory) when the stimuli were repeated and named before the recognition task for richer and deeper encoding experience. In addition, they found that retrieval cues and supports improved AD patients’ recall of positive images. Similarly, Kalenzaga et al. (2015) found that individuals with AD show no enhancement in recognition of emotional words compared to neutral words. However, their recollection of information, which requires more engagement of episodic memory, was better with emotional words. In this sense, experimental paradigms and conditions such as the encoding procedures, test forms of retrieval, and relevant strategies play a critical role in understanding the effects of emotional valence in language and cognitive processing in AD.

Further investigation is needed to tease apart the effects of experimental paradigms, especially during naming tasks, and to better understand the role that emotional valence can play during word retrieval, an ability that is substantially impaired from early on in AD (Garrard et al., 2005; Balthazar et al., 2008 Kavé and Goral, 2016, 2018). Using an experimental paradigm that does not place a heavy demand on memory encoding process, such as the use of verb fluency tasks, would provide a novel insight into the effects of emotional valence on retrieval of words by people experiencing AD. Verb fluency tasks require participants to name as many words as possible for things that people do in a certain time period, which does not require encoding during the experiment but place heavy emphasis on recall and retrieval of relevant lexical items (Woods et al., 2005; Alegret et al., 2018; Paek and Murray, 2021). Verbal fluency tasks are commonly utilized in both clinical and research settings for populations with AD, given their superior sensitivity in discovering qualitative differences in semantic, lexical, and phonological skills exhibited by an individual. However, it remains unclear whether the lexical items generated during verb fluency tasks would exhibit different levels of emotional valence in individuals with AD compared to cognitively healthy older adults (CHOA), due to the effect of emotional enhancement.

Therefore, the current study aimed to examine whether individuals with amnestic AD show increased emotional valence in their verb fluency responses compared to CHOA, since words with increased emotional valence may be easier to retrieve (Brueckner and Moritz, 2009 Perrin et al., 2012; Sava et al., 2015; Bohn et al., 2016). We predicted that emotional valence analysis would demonstrate a positivity bias given the findings of better recall or recognition of positive stimuli from some previous AD studies. Additionally, we hypothesized that the degree of emotional valence in words would correlate with the severity of verbal memory symptoms caused by dementia. Words with higher emotional valence may be more resilient to progressive cognitive declines, more resistant to neural deterioration, and therefore more easily accessed and retrieved in people with AD. This would be similar to the phenomenon that words with certain psycholinguistic properties (e.g., higher word frequency and lower age of acquisition) are more easily retrieved than others (Forbes-McKay et al., 2005; Vita et al., 2014; Rofes et al., 2019; Paek et al., 2020).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria

A total of 32 participants were enrolled in the current study, with 15 individuals who were diagnosed with mild to moderate amnestic AD and 17 individuals who were CHOA. All participants in this study were native speakers of English who were right-handed and who passed hearing and vision screening (either corrected or uncorrected). Hearing screening was administered using the speech discrimination subtest of the Arizona Battery for Communication Disorders of Dementia (ABCD; Bayles and Tomoeda, 1993), and vision was screened using a picture matching task. To be included in the study, all participants were required to score over 70% accuracy on the hearing screening test and 75% accuracy on the vision screening test. To meet the inclusionary criteria of the AD group, participants were required to: (a) be diagnosed by their neurologists with ADRDs and (b) demonstrate amnestic profiles according to the consensus criteria described by McKhann et al. (2011). Thus, the participants with AD exhibited the following clinical symptoms: (a) insidious onset of symptoms, (b) progressive deficits or worsening reported by caregivers or observation, (c) prominent memory deficits with at least one other domain of cognition being impaired, including attention or executive function, and (d) no other diagnosed neurological or neurodegenerative disorder that could explain these deficits, such as Lewy body dementia or strokes. Participants in the control group were required to demonstrate: (a) no history of or existing neurological or neurodegenerative diseases such as traumatic brain injury, strokes, or dementia, (b) no developmental language disorders such as dyslexia, Asperger’s syndrome, or specific language impairments, (c) no psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia or clinical depression, and (d) no history of substance or alcohol abuse. Participants in the control group were also required to perform above the age- and education-matched cutoff scores on the Mini-Mental State Exam-2 (MMSE-2; Folstein et al., 2010) as suggested by Crum et al. (1993).

The two participant groups did not show any statistically significant differences in age (t=−1.99, df=30, p=0.055) or level of education (t=1.41, df=30, p=0.169). However, their MMSE-2 scores differed significantly (t=3.75, df=15.56, p=0.002; unequal variance assumed), with lower scores for individuals with AD than for the control participants (see Table 1). The AD group was comprised of 5 males and 10 females, while there were seven males and 10 females in the control group. The chi-square test indicated that there is no significant difference between groups in gender distribution, X2 (1, N=32)=0.209, p=0.647. The present study was reviewed and approved by The University of Tennessee Institutional Review Board. All participants were provided verbal and written information about the study and compensated monetarily for their participation. Either the participants or their legal representatives provided informed consent after reviewing the study procedures, potential risks, and possible benefits.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information and verb fluency results.

Procedures

Verb Fluency Task

All participants performed a verb fluency task for a 30s period (Woods et al., 2005 Fernaeus et al., 2008; Alegret et al., 2018; Paek and Murray, 2021). The participants were instructed to name as many things as possible that people do, such as eat and smell. The verbal responses were audio-recorded and transcribed after the experiment, then scored as correct or incorrect. Correct responses included verbs that are relevant to what people do, while incorrect responses included repetition of previously spoken verbs or irrelevant words or utterances (e.g., uh, I do not know, hmm). These incorrect responses were excluded from the analysis of emotional valence of words.

Neuropsychological Test Battery

A battery of neuropsychological tests was administered to all participants to delineate their cognitive-linguistic profiles. The battery included: (a) Scales of Cognitive and Communicative Ability for Neurorehabilitation (SCCAN; Milman and Holland, 2003) to test orientation, attention, problem solving, reading, writing, and speech and reading comprehension, (b) Test of Memory and Learning – Senior Edition (TOMAL-SE; Reynolds and Voress, 2012) to measure immediate and delayed verbal and nonverbal memory and learning, (c) Calibrated Ideational Fluency Assessment (CIFA; Schretlen and Vannorsdall, 2010) to delineate verbal and nonverbal fluency and executive function skills, (d) Digit and Visual Memory Span subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) to examine visual and verbal short-term and working memory, (e) Pictorial Analogies and Geometric Categories subtests of the Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (CTONI-2; Hammill et al., 2009) to quantify nonverbal reasoning skills, and (f) the Expressive and Receptive versions of the Vocabulary Assessment Scales (Gerhardstein-Nader, 2013) to determine vocabulary size and naming skills. See Table 2 for the results. Among the 32 participants, two participants with dementia were unable to complete this battery due to scheduling conflicts.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of neuropsychological test battery.

Data Analysis

The correct responses during verb fluency tasks were analyzed for each participant with respect to emotional valence. The value of emotional valence was determined using norms provided by Warriner et al. (2013), where the emotional valence of 13,915 words were rated by 1,827 adult participants residing in the US. As these participants indicated how they felt after reading each stimulus word on a scale from 1 to 9, the values of emotional valence used in our analysis indicate affective valence (emotional response) than semantic valence (factual knowledge; see Itkes and Kron, 2019). The emotional valence values of each response were averaged for each individual; then, SPSS 25 statistical analysis software was used to conduct independent sample t tests to investigate whether the two groups differed in their verbal fluency responses in terms of emotional valence.

The two groups were also compared in terms of their cognitive and linguistic skills by comparing the raw scores of each test from the neuropsychological test battery. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to measure relationships between the emotional valence in their verb fluency responses and neuropsychological test scores related to verbal memory and learning. This was performed to determine whether the impairment level of these cognitive domains correlated with the degree of emotion observed in their word production. The test scores used for these correlations were Memory for Stories and Word List Learning of TOMAL-SE, as verbal episodic memory and learning are one of the cognitive domains that are most impaired and that demonstrate the earliest declines in the amnestic phenotype of AD (McKhann et al., 2011).

Results

Verb Fluency Results

The participants with AD produced a lower number of correct responses during the 30s verb fluency task compared to CHOA (see Table 1). The analysis of emotional valence revealed that individuals with AD produced words with higher emotional valence than CHOA. CHOA produced 11.65 words with the average emotional valence of 6.01, while individuals with AD produced 8.27 words with the average emotional valence 6.27. Statistical significance between the two groups (p<0.05) was confirmed.

Cognitive-Linguistic Profiles

The participants with AD showed poorer performances compared to CHOA on a number of cognitive and linguistic subtests, as shown in Table 2. The participants with dementia performed worse than CHOA during short term and working memory tests (Digit Span Backward and Digit Span Total of WMS-R; Visual Memory Span Forward and Backward, and Visual Memory Span Total) and a receptive vocabulary test (VAS-R; total scores). The participants with AD demonstrated lower scores in Oral Expression, Memory, Speech Comprehension, Writing, Attention, Problem Solving of SCCAN compared to CHOA. They also exhibited poorer memory skills compared to CHOA during TOMAL-SE testing, including Facial Memory, Memory for Stories, Word List Learning, Visual Sequential Memory, Object Recall, Memory for Location, Facial Memory Delayed, Memory for Stories Delayed, and Object Recall Delayed. In the CIFA test, the total number of acceptable designs and percent unacceptable designs were smaller in the AD group than observed in the CHOA group.

Correlation Results

The relationships among verbal learning and memory and the degree of emotional valence in verb fluency responses were determined using correlation analyses. The Pearson correlation coefficients indicated that there existed a significant negative relationship between verbal episodic memory (TOMAL-SE Memory for Stories) and emotional valence (r=−0.403, p=0.027). Higher emotional valence found in the verb fluency responses of the study participants were associated with lower verbal episodic memory skills in all participants. That is, the fewer components of a story that a participant could recall and reproduce, the higher emotional valence that was found in their verb fluency responses. Similarly, there was a negative correlation between the TOMAL-SE Word List Learning and emotional valence (r=−0.378, p=0.039), indicating that the fewer words a participant could learn from a list and recall, the higher emotional valence that was observed in their verb fluency responses.

Discussion

The current study examined the degree of emotional valence in verb fluency responses in individuals with AD and CHOA to delineate the effect of emotional enhancement of memory in AD, independent of the experimental encoding procedures (Werheid et al., 2011; Klein-Koerkamp et al., 2012; Kalenzaga et al., 2015; Sava et al., 2015). The participants with AD produced a smaller number of verbs during verb fluency tasks and their verbs exhibited higher emotional valence than those produced by CHOA. In addition, the degree of emotional valence negatively correlated with verbal episodic memory skills in the participants, suggesting that the more severe one’s memory symptoms, the higher the emotional valence in their verb fluency responses.

It has been debated whether the emotional enhancement of memory effects are observed in individuals with AD or whether they can benefit from such effects for retrieval of information. Although AD results in limbic damages from the earlier stages of the disease and the amygdala plays a very important role in emotional processing, the results of many previous research studies suggested that emotional enhancement of memory effects is relatively well preserved in individuals with AD when using intentional encoding tasks (Hamann et al., 2000; Kazui et al., 2000, 2003; Mizuno et al., 2000; Boller et al., 2002; LaBar et al., 2005; Horínek et al., 2007; Satler et al., 2007; Brueckner and Moritz, 2009; Schultz et al., 2009; Sundstrøm, 2011; Werheid et al., 2011; Perrin et al., 2012; Guzmán-Vélez et al., 2014; Kalenzaga et al., 2015; Bohn et al., 2016; Gomez-Gallego and Gomez-Garcia, 2017). The current study strengthens the evidence that emotional enhancement of memory is preserved in individuals with AD, since our participants retrieved words during verb fluency tasks that exhibited higher emotional valence than did that of the CHOA. In contrast to previous studies, our experimental paradigm did not require intentional (or implicit) encoding of stimuli during the experiment since it employed a verb fluency task. As this task does not involve encoding or learning new words or stimuli, there was little to no experimental influence on testing the emotional enhancement of memory effects. Instead, the participants were tested solely on retrieval of lexical items, i.e., searching through their mental lexicon to select and retrieve verbs that are relevant to the task instructions (i.e., name as many words as possible for things that people do). Accordingly, our findings indicated that the processes of search and retrieval for words as well as that of encoding in the memory system are affected by emotional factors in AD.

As emotion heavily influences word processing in healthy populations, for example, resulting in faster or more accurate recognition or retrieval of words with more emotional stimuli or contexts, our investigation of emotional valence of words produced by individuals with AD may provide insights not only into the cognitive changes due to disease progression, but also whether this factor can be leveraged for rehabilitation of word retrieval deficits in AD (Kuperman et al., 2014; Kauschke et al., 2019). Supporting our first hypothesis and consistent with the previous studies that showed positivity biases in memory recall in AD (e.g., Hamann et al., 2000; Brueckner and Moritz, 2009 Gallo et al., 2010; Perrin et al., 2012; Kalenzaga et al., 2015; Sava et al., 2015; Bohn et al., 2016), our participants with AD produced verbs with more pleasantness (i.e., higher emotional valence) during verb fluency tasks than did the CHOA. Our second hypothesis was also supported, with the data suggesting that higher emotional valence in verb fluency responses is associated with poorer immediate verbal memory skills. These findings may indicate that those words with higher emotional valence are easier to retrieve among the competing lexical items. Alternatively, it is also plausible that those words are more resilient to the effects of the cognitive and linguistic declines that are associated with AD and thus are preserved in the mental lexicon better or longer. Indeed, previous studies suggest that individuals with dementia can maintain or retrieve certain words for a longer period of time over the course of their disease progression, including those words with stronger or more salient psycholinguistic properties (e.g., word frequency, concreteness, imageability, and neighborhood density; Forbes-McKay et al., 2005; Vita et al., 2014; Rofes et al., 2019; Paek et al., 2020). However, further research is warranted to delineate whether this effect is attributable to stronger representations or easier access. That is, are words with higher emotional valence better preserved as they have stronger representations and connections in the mental lexicon, or among competing lexical items, are they more easily accessible during search and retrieval because of their psycholinguistic salience?

Most of the previous studies investigated the effects of emotion on retrieval using the category of objects or nouns (e.g., Brueckner and Moritz, 2009 Giffard et al., 2015; Rodríguez-Ferreiro et al., 2019). However, the current study suggests that these effects may be observed across different parts of speech, including verbs. As individuals with AD exhibits difficulties with production and comprehension of both nouns and verbs (Kim and Thompson, 2004; De Almeida et al., 2021), the findings in the current study might provide insights into development of treatment methods or to determine which words should be prioritized for treatment and maintenance. Future research should also examine whether this finding can be replicated across different types of verbal fluency tasks such as category fluency (e.g., supermarket items, animals) and letter fluency (e.g., words that start with the letter H). The verb fluency task requires retrieval of human actions only, which may be more pertinent to memories about self (self-concept and self-continuity; Rasmussen and Berntsen, 2009) or autobiographical memory. Comparison among traditional category and letter fluency tasks and verb fluency tasks may provide additional insights into the cognitive changes in AD.

The limitations and future directions of the current study include the following. Since only those individuals with mild to moderate dementia were tested in the study, testing the hypotheses with individuals demonstrating different severity levels would offer a more comprehensive examination of the effects. It has been suggested that retrieval of emotional information in AD is enhanced by amygdala and medial temporal lobe structures engaging in the process, even when these regions are impaired (Klein-Koerkamp et al., 2012). However, as the functional impairments caused by neuropathological changes in limbic regions will only be worsened in those experiencing more severe AD, it is plausible that emotional enhancement of memory effects may not be observed or may be attenuated in patients exhibiting symptoms of more severe AD. Thus, future studies will be needed to examine whether the effects of emotional valence differ across the continuum of disease, from individuals with mild cognitive impairments to those experiencing more severe dementia.

Since we used a 30-s verb fluency task to probe emotional valence in words produced by our participants (Fernaeus et al., 2008), it is possible that the psycholinguistic properties of words might differ if the participants were required to perform the task for a 1-min period, because the psycholinguistic quality of words in verbal fluency performance changes over time (Vonk et al., 2019). That is, different levels of emotional valence might have been observed if the participants were to have put forth effort beyond the 30-s period we used. Future studies will need to examine whether there exist differences in the emotional valence during the initial time bins (e.g., 0–30s) versus later ones (e.g., 30–60s) and to investigate what might provide more sensitive information about the cognitive status of individuals with AD.

In addition, the sample size of the current study was relatively small, which might be associated with reduced statistical power and difficulties with generalization of findings to the population of interest. It is also possible that there are disparities between different race and ethnicity groups in terms of what is considered positive or negative emotional valence (i.e., the ratings of the emotional valence) stemming from the cultural differences. The distribution of race and ethnicity in our sample was mostly homogeneous, including only one black participant with AD and all the others being white. However, the normative study by Warriner et al. (2013) that we used to measure emotional valence of generated words did not present the race and ethnicity data of their sample. Moreover, race and ethnicity as well as socioeconomic status may be a factor that influences linguistic performance during word retrieval and verbal fluency (Johnson-Selfridge et al., 1998; Gladsjo et al., 1999). Thus, future studies will need to carefully consider these demographic characteristics of the participant sample as well as the normative sample.

In summary, the current study provides insights into the effects of emotion on word retrieval processes in AD, as we found that word retrieval may be easier or more challenging depending on the emotional aspect of the lexical item. It also supports the hypothesis that lexical items with higher emotional valence may be more resilient to the cognitive declines and may be a good candidate for treatment or maintenance. That is, in selecting words for rehabilitation or in choosing which words should be practiced for maintenance in AD, the emotional valence of words may play an important role in leveraging the outcomes.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Tennessee Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

EP designed and performed the experiments, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded in part by Alzheimer’s Association Greater Indiana Chapter.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank all our participants and their care partners for participating in the study, and Adult Language and Brain Lab members who assisted with data collection and analysis, including Christina Oakley.

References

Abrisqueta-Gomez, J., Bueno, O., Oliveira, M., and Bertolucci, P. (2002). Recognition memory for emotional pictures in Alzheimer’s patients. Acta Neurol. Scand. 105, 51–54. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.00035.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alegret, M., Peretó, M., Pérez, A., Valero, S., Espinosa, A., Ortega, G., et al. (2018). The role of verb fluency in the detection of early cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 62, 611–619. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association (2021). Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 17, 1–104.

Google Scholar

Balthazar, M. L. F., Cendes, F. G., and Damasceno, B. P. (2008). Semantic error patterns on the Boston Naming Test in normal aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and mild Alzheimer’s disease: Is there semantic disruption? Neuropsychology 22, 703–709.

Google Scholar

Baran, Z., Cangöz, B., and Ozel-Kizil, E. T. (2014). The impact of aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional enhancement of memory. Eur. Neurol. 72, 30–37. doi: 10.1159/000359924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayles, K., and Tomoeda, C. (1993). Arizona Battery for Communication Disorders of Dementia. Tucson, AZ: Canyonlands.

Google Scholar

Blessing, A., Keil, A., Linden, D. E. J., Heim, S., and Ray, W. J. (2006). Acquisition of affective dispositions in dementia patients. Neuropsychologia 44, 2366–2373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohn, L., Kwong See, S. T., and Fung, H. H. (2016). Time perspective and positivity effects in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Aging 31, 574–582. doi: 10.1037/pag0000084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boller, F., El Massioui, F., Devouche, E., Traykov, L., Pomati, S., and Starkstein, S. E. (2002). Processing emotional information in Alzheimer’s disease: effects on memory performance and neurophysiological correlates. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 14, 104–112. doi: 10.1159/000064932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broster, L. S., Blonder, L. X., and Jiang, Y. (2012). Does emotional memory enhancement assist the memory-impaired? Front. Aging Neurosci. 4:2. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brueckner, K., and Moritz, S. (2009). Emotional valence and semantic relatedness differentially influence false recognition in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy elderly. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 268–276. doi: 10.1017/S135561770909047X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chainay, H., Sava, A., Michael, G. A., Landre, L., Versace, R., and Krolak-Salmon, P. (2014). Impaired emotional memory enhancement on recognition of pictorial stimuli in Alzheimer’s disease: no influence of the nature of encoding. Cortex 50, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, R. M., Anthony, J. C., Bassett, S. S., and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Population-based norms for the mini-mental state examination by age and educational level. JAMA 269, 2386–2391. doi: 10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Almeida, R. G., Mobayyen, F., Antal, C., Kehayia, E., Nair, V. P., and Schwartz, G. (2021). Category-specific verb-semantic deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from static and dynamic action naming. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 38, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2020.1858772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Wit, L., Marsiske, M., O’Shea, D., Kessels, R. P. C., Kurasz, A. M., DeFeis, B., et al. (2021). Procedural learning in individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 31, 103–114. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09449-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Marco, L. Y., Marzo, A., Muñoz-Ruiz, M., Ikram, M. A., Kivipelto, M., Ruefenacht, D., et al. (2014). Modifiable lifestyle factors in dementia: a systematic review of longitudinal observational cohort studies. J. Alzheimers Dis. 42, 119–135. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernaeus, S. E., Östberg, P., Hellström, Å., and Wahlund, L. O. (2008). Cut the coda: early fluency intervals predict diagnoses. Cortex 44, 161–169. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2006.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., McHugh, P. R., and Fanjiang, G. (2010). Mini-Mental State Examination: MMSE-2. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Forbes-McKay, K. E., Ellis, A. W., Shanks, M. F., and Venneri, A. (2005). The age of acquisition of words produced in a semantic fluency task can reliably differentiate normal from pathological age related cognitive decline. Neuropsychologia 43, 1625–1632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallo, D. A., Foster, K. T., Wong, J. T., and Bennett, D. A. (2010). False recollection of emotional pictures in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 48, 3614–3618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrard, P., Maloney, L. M., Hodges, J. R., and Patterson, K. (2005). The effects of very early Alzheimer’s disease on the characteristics of writing by a renowned author. Brain 128, 250–260. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerhardstein-Nader, R. (2013). Vocabulary Assessment Scales. Psychological Assessment Resources. Lutz, FL: PAR.

Google Scholar

Giffard, B., Laisney, M., Desgranges, B., and Eustache, F. (2015). An exploration of the semantic network in Alzheimer’s disease: influence of emotion and concreteness of concepts. Cortex 69, 201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gladsjo, J. A., Schuman, C. C., Evans, J. D., Peavy, G. M., Miller, S. W., and Heaton, R. K. (1999). Norms for letter and category fluency: demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment 6, 147–178. doi: 10.1177/107319119900600204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Gallego, M., and Gomez-Garcia, J. (2017). Negative bias in the perception and memory of emotional information in Alzheimer disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 30, 131–139. doi: 10.1177/0891988716686833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzmán-Vélez, E., Feinstein, J. S., and Tranel, D. (2014). Feelings without memory in Alzheimer disease. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 27, 117–129. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, S. B., Monarch, E. S., and Goldstein, F. C. (2000). Memory enhancement for emotional stimuli is impaired in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 14, 82–92. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.14.1.82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammill, D. D., Pearson, N. A., and Weiderholt, J. L. (2009). Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence. 2nd Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

Harrison, B. E., Son, G. R., Kim, J., and Whall, A. L. (2007). Preserved implicit memory in dementia: a potential model for care. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 22, 286–293. doi: 10.1177/1533317507303761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horínek, D., Varjassyová, A., and Hort, J. (2007). Magnetic resonance analysis of amygdalar volume in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20, 273–277. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ebb613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itkes, O., and Kron, A. (2019). Affective and semantic representations of valence: a conceptual framework. Emot. Rev. 11, 283–293. doi: 10.1177/1754073919868759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson-Selfridge, M. T., Zalewski, C., and Aboudarham, J. F. (1998). The relationship between ethnicity and word fluency. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 13, 319–325. doi: 10.1093/arclin/13.3.319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalenzaga, S., Piolino, P., and Clarys, D. (2015). The emotional memory effect in Alzheimer’s disease: Emotional words enhance recollective experience similarly in patients and control participants. Cogn. Emot. 29, 342–350.

Google Scholar

Kauschke, C., Bahn, D., Vesker, M., and Schwarzer, G. (2019). The role of emotional valence for the processing of facial and verbal stimuli positivity or negativity bias? Front. Psychol. 10:1654. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavé, G., and Goral, M. (2016). Word retrieval in picture descriptions produced by individuals with Alzheimer’s disease. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 38, 958–966. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1179266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavé, G., and Goral, M. (2018). Word retrieval in connected speech in Alzheimer’s disease: a review with meta-analyses. Aphasiology 32, 4–26. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2017.1338663

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazui, H., Mori, E., Hashimoto, M., and Hirono, N. (2003). Enhancement of declarative memory by emotional arousal and visual memory function in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neuropsychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 15, 221–226. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.2.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazui, H., Mori, E., Hashimoto, M., Hirono, N., Imamura, T., Tanimukai, S., et al. (2000). Impact of emotion on memory: controlled study of the influence of emotionally charged material on declarative memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Br. J. Psychiatry 177, 343–347. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.4.343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., and Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion 2, 118–134. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., and Thompson, C. K. (2004). Verb deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and agrammatism: implications for lexical organization. Brain Lang. 88, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00147-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein-Koerkamp, Y., Baciu, M., and Hot, P. (2012). Preserved and impaired emotional memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Psychol. 3:331. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperman, V., Estes, Z., Brysbaert, M., and Warriner, A. B. (2014). Emotion and language: valence and arousal affect word recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 1065–1081. doi: 10.1037/a0035669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaBar, K. S., Torpey, D. C., Cook, C. A., Johnson, S. R., Warren, L. H., Burke, J. R., et al. (2005). Emotional enhancement of perceptual priming is preserved in aging and early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 43, 1824–1837. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milman, L., and Holland, A. (2003). Scales of Cognitive and Communicative Ability for Neurorehabilitation. Pro-Ed. Tucson, USA: The University of Arizona.

Google Scholar

Mizuno, K., Wakai, M., Takeda, A., and Sobue, G. (2000). Medial temporal atrophy and memory impairment in early stage of Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI volumetric and memory assessment study. J. Neurol. Sci. 173, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00289-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nashiro, K., and Mather, M. (2011). The effect of emotional arousal on memory binding in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Psychol. 124, 301–312. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.124.3.0301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oren, S., Willerton, C., and Small, J. (2014). Effects of spaced retrieval training on semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 57, 247–270. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0352)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paek, E. J., and Murray, L. L. (2021). Quantitative and qualitative analysis of verb fluency performance in individuals with probable Alzheimer’s disease and healthy older adults. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 30, 481–490. doi: 10.1044/2019_AJSLP-19-00052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paek, E. J., Murray, L. L., and Newman, S. D. (2020). Neural correlates of verb fluency performance in cognitively healthy older adults and individuals with dementia: a pilot fMRI study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:73. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrin, M., Henaff, M. A., Padovan, C., Faillenot, I., Merville, A., and Krolak-Salmon, P. (2012). Influence of emotional content and context on memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 29, 817–826. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-111490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, A. S., and Berntsen, D. (2009). Emotional valence and the functions. Mem. Cogn. 37, 477–492. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.4.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, C. R., and Voress, J. K. (2012). Test of Memory and Learning–Senior Edition Examiner’s Manual. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Inc.

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Martínez, C., and Cuetos, F. (2019). Differential effects of negative and positive emotional content over veridical and false recognition in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurolinguistics 49, 109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rofes, A., de Aguiar, V., Ficek, B., Wendt, H., Webster, K., and Tsapkini, K. (2019). The role of word properties in performance on fluency tasks in people with primary progressive aphasia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 68, 1521–1534. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satler, C., Garrido, L. M., Sarmiento, E. P., Leme, S., Conde, C., and Tomaz, C. (2007). Emotional arousal enhances declarative memory in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 116, 355–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2007.00897.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sava, A. A., Paquet, C., Krolak-Salmon, P., Dumurgier, J., Hugon, J., and Chainay, H. (2015). Emotional memory enhancement in respect of positive visual stimuli in Alzheimer’s disease emerges after rich and deep encoding. Cortex 65, 89–101. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schretlen, D. J., and Vannorsdall, T. D. (2010). Calibrated Ideational Fluency Assessment (CIFA) Professional Manual. Lutz, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Schultz, R. R., De Castro, C. C., and Bertolucci, P. H. F. (2009). Memory with emotional content, brain amygdala and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 120, 101–110. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2008.01132.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundstrøm, M. (2011). Modeling recall memory for emotional objects in Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 18, 396–413. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.567324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dulmen, S., Smits, L., and Eide, H. (2017). Filling in memory gaps through emotional communication: promising pathways in caring for persons with dementia. Patient Educ. Couns. 100, 2121–2124. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2017.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vita, M. G., Marra, C., Spinelli, P., Caprara, A., Scaricamazza, E., Castelli, D., et al. (2014). Typicality of words produced on a semantic fluency task in amnesic mild cognitive impairment: linguistic analysis and risk of conversion to dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 42, 1171–1178. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vonk, J. M. J., Flores, R. J., Rosado, D., Qian, C., Cabo, R., Habegger, J., et al. (2019). Semantic network function captured by word frequency in nondemented APOE ε4 carriers. Neuropsychology 33, 256–262. doi: 10.1037/neu0000508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., and Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behav. Res. Methods 45, 1191–1207. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, TXPsychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Werheid, K., McDonald, R. S., Simmons-Stern, N., Ally, B. A., and Budson, A. E. (2011). Familiar smiling faces in Alzheimer’s disease: understanding the positivity-related recognition bias. Neuropsychologia 49, 2935–2940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, S. P., Scott, J. C., Sires, D. A., Grant, I., Heaton, R. K., Tröster, A. I., et al. (2005). Action (verb) fluency: test–retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 408–415. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, C. I., Dickerson, B. C., Feczko, E., Negeira, A., and Williams, D. (2007). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala responses to human faces in aging and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1388–1395. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yates, L. A., Ziser, S., Spector, A., and Orrell, M. (2016). Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 1791–1806. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216001137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, emotional enhancement of memory, emotional valence, word retrieval, verb fluency, action fluency

Citation: Paek EJ (2021) Emotional Valence Affects Word Retrieval During Verb Fluency Tasks in Alzheimer’s Dementia. Front. Psychol. 12:777116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.777116

Received: 14 September 2021; Accepted: 10 November 2021;
Published: 02 December 2021.

Edited by:

Matteo De Marco, Brunel University London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Oksana Itkes, University of Haifa, Israel
Jennifer Eastman, University of California San Francisco, United States

Copyright © 2021 Paek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eun Jin Paek, epaek@uthsc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.