Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 December 2022
Sec. Addictive Behaviors

The effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress and mediating role of ruminations: Evidence from Chinese and Czech university students

  • 1Department of Psychology and Abnormal Psychology, Faculty of Education, Palacký University Olomouc, Olomouc, Czechia
  • 2Translational Neuroscience and Aging Program, Center for Translational Medicine, International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital, Brno, Czechia

Introduction: The rise in the capabilities of mobile devices and the associated increase in the proportion of time we spend on them has not only positive benefits but also several risks, including mobile phone addiction and its consequences. The complex mechanisms of the impact of this addiction on mental health, especially in a cross-cultural context, however, remain relatively unknown. The aim of this cross-cultural study was to investigate the mediating role of rumination on the association between mobile phone addiction and perceived stress.

Methods: A population of 358 Chinese and 282 Czech university students was tested using a battery of validated psychological tests that included a short version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale, the Ruminative Response Scale, and the Perceived Stress Scale.

Results: The results showed significant cross-cultural differences with Czech students manifesting greater rumination (d = 0.79) and perceived stress (d = 0.42) and Chinese students showing greater mobile phone addiction (d = 1.01). Mediation analyses showed that the effect of mobile phone addiction on stress levels was mediated through the rumination in both populations (45.6% and 80.9% of the explained variance for Chinese and Czech students, respectively) and did not differ between the two countries (estimate of difference [95%CI] = −0.052[−0.166, 0.037], p = 0.27). In contrast, the significant direct effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress was only present in Chinese students, where it was marginally larger than the indirect effect. In Czech students, the direct effect was not manifested and the difference between countries was significant (estimate of difference [95%CI] = 0.242 [0.035, 0.413], p < 0.001). In all of the cases, the association between the variables was positive, i.e., as one grew, so did the other. Finally, a moderated-mediation analysis confirmed that country of origin significantly moderated only the direct relationship between mobile phone dependence and perceived stress (p = 0.002).

Discussion: These results suggest that the mechanism of interaction between excessive mobile phone use and perceived stress is culturally conditioned, which may limit the transferability of research findings in a global context and requires further cross-cultural studies.

Introduction

The evolution of mobile device capabilities has been almost ‘meteoric’ in recent decades, and today’s mobile phones are capable of doing things that were reserved only for computers a few years ago. This makes many people use their phones more and more as their primary technological tool for communication, gaming, reading, studying or shopping. Along with this, we can see a significant increase in the role of mobile phones in social networking. According to App Annie (2020), a mobile data and analytics company, which recently released the State of Mobile 2022 report, global mobile device usage in 2021 is at a record high of 3.8 trillion hours (Olson et al., 2022). Furthermore, a meta-analysis by McGill University showed that Chinese users ranked first in the world in mobile phone addiction rates compared with European countries such as Germany and France (Olson et al., 2022), as China’s young population uses mobile phones too often and for too long (Liu et al., 2017). These so-called ‘heads down’ groups of university students can be seen everywhere.

Although mobile phones offer a number of benefits and conveniences for university students (and the general population), such as the ability to communicate quickly with family, teachers and classmates (Park and Lee, 2012), the possibility of meeting, interacting and communicating with distant friends (Auter, 2007), a tool for studying and filling leisure time, a means of reducing stress and tension as well as the ability to purchase goods that are not available locally (Hossain, 2019), the excessive use of the phone can also have a number of negative impacts. Inappropriate use of mobile phones may hinder face-to-face communication (Krasnova et al., 2016) and reduce interpersonal intimacy (Roberts and David, 2016). Using mobile phones in class and studying can be a distraction for students and be a cause of academic procrastination or lower academic performance (Balkis, 2013; Byun et al., 2013; Ibrahim et al., 2018). Moreover, excessive mobile phone use can result in physical and psychological problems or the development of a mobile phone addiction.

Mobile phone addiction was first described by Young as a type of technology addiction, which further includes for example Facebook addiction, internet addiction or video games addiction (Young, 2007). It is considered as an addictive behavior, as it encompasses some of the general characteristics of addictive behavior: lack of self-control, withdrawal symptoms, and tolerance (Griffiths, 1995; Griffiths, 2005). Mobile phone addiction can have a variety of negative consequences. Excessive smartphone use has been linked to a higher risk of anxiety and depression (Thomée et al., 2011; Rosen et al., 2013; Firth et al., 2017; Matar Boumosleh and Jaalouk, 2017; Lei et al., 2020; Cheng et al., 2021; Ratan et al., 2021), poorer health (Schoeni et al., 2015), sleep problems, pain and migraine, cognitive impairment, poorer self-esteem (Wacks and Weinstein, 2021), fatigue (Feng and Sun, 2022), neurological disorders (Ratan et al., 2021) or increased perceived stress (Chiu, 2014; Kuang-Tsan and Fu-Yuan, 2017; Reinecke et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2019), particularly in adolescents and young adults (Matar Boumosleh and Jaalouk, 2017; Cha and Seo, 2018; Loleska and Pop-Jordanova, 2021). In fact, the significant increase of this type of addiction in the young population is emerging as one of the priority public health issues. Moreover, despite a number of studies examining this issue, comparative studies accounting for the effects of different cultural backgrounds are still lacking.

Although the link between mobile phone addiction and stress is well documented, the mechanisms underlying this relationship are not yet fully understood. Given that increased stress in the context of this addiction occurs in conjunction with a lack of coping mechanisms, depression and increased susceptibility to addiction (Matar Boumosleh and Jaalouk, 2017), ruminative thinking may be one of the mechanisms explaining the above relation. Ruminative thinking (or rumination) is the tendency to focus attention on the symptoms of one’s distress and on its possible causes and consequences as opposed to its solutions. As such, it is perceived as a dysfunctional cognitive coping strategy (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Samtani and Moulds, 2017) that represents an excessive and intrusive fixation on negative experiences and feelings. Several studies have suggested that rumination is a consequence of internet addiction (Davis, 2001) as individuals with higher addiction have a deficit in the ability to regulate affective emotions, leading to a stage of maintained negative affectivity with lower positive affectivity (Joormann and Quinn, 2014). The indirect effect of mobile phone addiction also plays a role as increased smartphone time negatively affects a range of offline activities, including interpersonal relationships and academic achievements, which increases the stress load (Kuss and Griffiths, 2011) which in turn can further saturate negative affectivity and rumination (Liu et al., 2017). Furthermore, rumination has also previously been linked to stress levels (Kaiseler et al., 2017; Lian et al., 2021) within the diathesis stress model in which rumination caused by a negative life context (e.g., addiction) may increase subsequent experiences of stress and other psychopathologies such as depressive symptoms (Morrison and O’Connor, 2005).

In addition, the issue of mobile phone addiction and its negative consequences has become even more important in the last 2 years as the COVID-19 pandemic has had a number of negative impacts on humans. The reduction in the possibilities for face-to-face interaction, lockdowns and social distancing (Nurunnabi et al., 2021; Viner et al., 2021), increased uncertainty about the future, financial implications (Cao et al., 2020; Russo and Terraneo, 2020), health and mental health risks (Son et al., 2020; Browning et al., 2021; Fruehwirth et al., 2021; Křeménková et al., 2021; Xu et al., 2021) as well as the inevitability of having to spend a great amount of time online which is also flooded with more negative content (Ratan et al., 2021; Serra et al., 2021) may play a role in the increased development of mobile phone addiction and the magnification of its negative consequences and mental health issues in general, especially in individuals who have less resources to cope with challenging situations and adversities.

Surprisingly, although previous studies have both described the mediating role of rumination in the context of mobile phone addiction (Wang et al., 2018; Lian et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2021; Li et al., 2022; Peng et al., 2022) and have indicated cross-cultural differences in perceived stress (Lee et al., 2022; Mohamed et al., 2022), ruminative thinking (Murdock et al., 2019) and mobile phone addiction (Yang et al., 2019; Olson et al., 2022), there is no direct evidence available on the cultural conditioning of this mediating relationship. To explore the above-mentioned mechanism and in part fill the gap in understanding the role of cross-cultural differences, the aim of this study was to examine the mediating role of ruminative thinking on the relationship between mobile phone addiction and perceived stress in two different cultural settings and to compare them against each other.

Materials and methods

Study design and sample

At the beginning of this cross-cultural study, we approached 756 students from the Faculty of Education, Palacký University via their official university emails and 413 students from the Faculty of Educational Sciences, Chongqing Normal University in Sichuan province via Wechat social app concerning their participation in the study. Of these, 282 (68.3%) Czech and 358 (47.4%) Chinese students were eventually involved. Data were collected using an online questionnaire (Google Forms in Czech Republic and Wenjuanxing in China) in two identical forms in Czech and Chinese. Data collection took place from February to April 2022.

Measure and instruments

Mobile phone addiction (‘mpa’) was measured using the Short Version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS-SV, Kwon et al., 2013). The scale consists of 10 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = totally disagree, 6 = totally agree), with higher scores indicating a higher level of mobile phone addiction. McDonald’s omega in this study was good to excellent at 0.93 and 0.86 for the Czech and Chinese samples, respectively.

Perceived stress (‘pss’) was measured using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS, Cohen et al., 1983; Cohen and Williamson, 1988). The scale has 10 items and is scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always. A higher the score indicates increased perceived stress. Studies have shown good reliability in different cultural settings. McDonald’s omega in this study was good at 0.78 and 0.77 for the Czech and Chinese samples, respectively.

Rumination (‘rmn’) was assessed using the Rumination Response Scale (RPS, Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). This 22-item scale measures the tendency to ruminative thinking. The items are answered on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = never to 4 = always, with a higher total score reflecting a greater tendency toward ruminative thinking. McDonald’s omega in this study was excellent at 0.94 and 0.96 for the Czech and Chinese samples, respectively and the tool demonstrated good applicability in the Chinese population (Li et al., 2019).

The English versions of SAS-SV and RPS were initially translated to Czech by the standard double-blind approach (two independent translations to Czech, two back-translations to English by a native-speaker and a psychologist, comparison of the original and back-translated versions). The Czech version of the PSS and Chinese versions of all questionnaires were available from previous studies.

Data analysis

No missing values were detected. The reliability of each instrument (separately for each country) was verified using McDonald’s omega. Due to the non-normal data distribution, all variables were standardized to Z-scores prior to the analyses. Associations between variables were first analyzed using Pearson’s correlation, differences between correlation coefficients by z-test with Fisher’s r-to-z transformation, and group differences using Welch t-test. Complex interactions between variables were analyzed using a series of casual mediation analyses (with 1,000 simulations) and the effect of country of origin on the mediation effect was then tested using a test of significant differences in direct and indirect effects (with 200 simulations) and a moderated-mediation analysis (Tingley et al., 2014). Data analyses and visualizations were performed using RStudio (v. 1.4.1717 with R environment v. 4.1.3) using cocor, dplyr, flexplot, GGally, ggplot2, jmv, JSmediation, mediation, rstatix, and stats packages.

Ethical consideration

The authors assert that all of the procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All of the participants were informed of the confidentiality of their answers and signed an online informed consent form prior to the completion of the questionnaire. No specific information enabling the identification of specific students (such as IP address, student name or ID number, specific field of study, etc.) was obtained as part of the online data collection. The research protocol of the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Education.

Results

Sample demographics

The research population consisted of two cohorts of university students. The Czech population consisted of 282 students (mean age ± SD, 25.1 ± 7.5, 87.6% females), while the Chinese population consisted of 358 students (mean age ± SD, 21.6 ± 2.0, 68.2% females). The baseline characteristics of both samples are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main characteristics of the research cohorts.

Associations between variables

The initial analysis showed significant correlations between all three variables across the dataset and within both countries (Figure 1) with small to medium effect sizes for the relationship between mobile phone addiction and rumination and perceived stress and large effect sizes for the relationship between rumination and perceived stress. However, the correlation coefficients did not differ between the two countries (Prmn ~ pss = 0.146, Pmpa ~ rmn = 0.152, Prmn ~ pss = 0.502).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Correlations between main outcomes. Correlation matrix showing the density plot of the z-scores of each variable, the scatterplot of the distribution of values in both countries and the strength of the correlations between the variables. ***p < 0.001.

Furthermore, we also observed significant cross-cultural differences in rumination [t(590.43) = −9.930, p < 0.001, d = 0.79 (moderate)] and perceived stress levels [t(568.18) = −5.270, p < 0.001, d = 0.42 (small)] with higher values in Czech students and in mobile phone addiction [t(497.67) = 12.483, p < 0.001, d = 1.01 (large)] with higher levels in Chinese students.

Mediation analysis

Subsequently, we examined the effect of mobile phone addiction on the level of perceived stress and the possible role of rumination as a mediator in this relationship. A casual mediation analysis showed a significant both direct and indirect effect in Chinese students (Table 2; Figures 2A,C). The direct effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress was marginally greater with higher perceived stress and greater rumination associated with greater phone addiction. The mediation effect explained 45.6% of the net total effect. In contrast, Czech students did not show a direct effect of phone addiction on perceived stress. Here, mobile phone addiction was positively related only to rumination, through which it also influenced the level of perceived stress (Table 2; Figures 2B,C). This mediation effect explained 80.9% of the net total effect.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Estimates of direct and indirect effects with quasi–Bayesian confidence intervals for both countries.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Casual mediation and moderated–mediation models. Simple mediation diagrams showing a, b, c, and c’ path coefficients for (A) Chinese and (B) Czech students, representing unstandardized regression weights with standardized regression weights in parentheses. The square brackets contain the SE. ***p < 0.001. The c path coefficient represents the total effect, the c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct effect. (C) Country–specific estimates of direct and indirect effects of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress with rumination as the mediator. (D) Direct effects of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress in both countries considering the mediating effect of rumination.

Cross-cultural differences

To verify the effect of students’ country of origin on the observed differences in mediation effects, we statistically tested these differences. The results showed that the indirect effects did not differ between the two groups of students (estimate of difference [95% CI] = −0.052 [−0.166, 0.037], p = 0.27), while the direct effect was significantly larger for Chinese students (estimate of difference [95% CI] = 0.242 [0.035, 0.413], p < 0.001).

This was further confirmed by the moderated-mediation analysis (with students’ country of origin as the moderator), which showed that country of origin did not moderate the relationship between phone addiction (independent variable) and rumination (mediator; a*Mod: t(636) = 0. 03, p = 0.976) or the relationship between rumination and perceived stress (dependent variable; b*Mod: t(634) = 1.87, p = 0.062) but significantly moderated the direct relationship between mobile phone addiction and perceived stress (c’*Mod: t(634) = 3.18, p = 0.002; Figure 2B).

Discussion

The aim of this cross-cultural study was to investigate the relationship between mobile phone addiction, ruminative thinking, and perceived stress and the mediating role of rumination in two different cultural settings. The rationale for this study arose from three observations. First, previous studies have provided evidence for the mediating role of ruminative thinking in the context of mobile phone addiction and mental health (Wang et al., 2018; Lian et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2021; Li et al., 2022; Peng et al., 2022). Second, there is separate evidence for cross-cultural differences in perceived stress (Lee et al., 2022; Mohamed et al., 2022), ruminative thinking (Murdock et al., 2019) and mobile phone addiction (Yang et al., 2019; Olson et al., 2022). And third, and most importantly, there is surprisingly no direct evidence of cultural conditioning of the aforementioned mediating relationship. This study thus seeks to fill this gap in our knowledge.

We observed a significant small to moderate positive relationship between these variables, which was the same for both student populations. These findings are consistent with previous studies that described an adverse effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress (Chiu, 2014; Kuang-Tsan and Fu-Yuan, 2017; Reinecke et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2019) and an increase in rumination (Morrison and O’Connor, 2005; Joormann and Quinn, 2014; Kaiseler et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017; Lian et al., 2021). We hypothesize that this effect is exerted directly through the potential influence of ‘withdrawal’ symptoms on internal emotional turmoil as well as indirectly through the negative impact of excessive smartphone time on other activities in students’ lives, including their studies. Other potential influence may be that for some individuals their mobile device is an important surrogate tool for their social functioning or for maintaining self-concept and self-esteem. However, Li, Liu, and Dong showed that problematic smartphone use was rather associated with lower self-esteem, depression and interpersonal confidence (Li et al., 2019). In line with Billieux et al. (2015) it appears that although students in some cases resort to excessive online communication, messaging and social media as a means of fulfilling their needs and gaining reassurance and self-worth, individuals predisposed to depressive and anxiety symptoms in particular are in fact more likely to promote a vicious cycle of constantly seeking new and novel sources of self-affirmation and acceptance (Elhai et al., 2019). This can be particularly problematic when the content consumed on a mobile phone is dominated by negative feedback and information, which the individual finds harder to process due to poorer coping skills (Matar Boumosleh and Jaalouk, 2017). It should also be taken into account that the ongoing Covid-19 pandemic may have affected the identified relationships. This potential effect will be summarized below in the mediation analysis section.

We also observed differences between the two student populations. Consistent with recent studies, Chinese students showed significantly higher cell phone addiction (App Annie, 2020; Olson et al., 2022). One reason for this difference may be the different cultural and philosophical backgrounds in the two countries. While the Czech Republic is considered a ‘Western’ country with many features of a consumerist society, due to its recent past (satellite country of the USSR and the restrictive communist regime), a certain delay in the development of consumerism and due to the effect of a relatively small land area and still sustaining face-to-face interpersonal contacts and bonds, the intensity and depth of connection to the ‘online world’ of mobile phones and social networks is still rather weaker. China, on the other hand, as a typical Eastern country with a long-term farming civilization, is characterized by significantly higher collectivism compared to the Western world (Triandis and Gelfand, 2012). Several studies have found that individuals with a higher tendency to collectivism have higher interpersonal sensitivity, that is, the tendency to be overly sensitive to the emotional and behavioral performance of others, especially the criticism and rejection of others (Yamaguchi, 1994; Scharf et al., 2017) and may experience emotional distress such as social anxiety and reluctance to express their emotions and social needs. The following three characteristics of the mobile phone network can make these interpersonally oversensitive individuals more likely to become dependent: anonymity, convenience and escape (Xia et al., 2003). Various social apps have made mobile social networking more convenient because the anonymity of mobile social networking allows people to do what they want without worrying too much about what they say or do will bring harm to them; the convenience gives people more opportunities to connect with strangers and have supportive social relationships. This is more important for people who cannot meet their social needs in reality, so people who are interpersonally sensitive are more likely to escape reality and immerse themselves in mobile social interaction (You et al., 2019). Furthermore, influenced by collectivism and Confucianism, the Chinese attribute great importance to the synchronicity of the collective and the opinion of their peers (Yum, 1988). There is a phenomenon among young Chinese people nowadays that they gather together among their classmates or friends to play online games on mobile phones and those who are reluctant are forced to participate by the peer pressure (Han and Qi, 2005). The need not to disappoint their peers drives these young people to participate in these activities but as the frequency of mobile gaming increases, so does their level of stress (Assor et al., 2020). Besides higher collectivism, other possible contributing factors may be the significantly larger size of China and more frequent long distances between students and their family and friends, which can be overcome regularly only by using online tools. Chinese students may therefore be more likely to use mobile phones to kill leisure time, relieve stress and as entertainment means. Also, the effect of the Covid-19 pandemic circumstances, which will be discussed below, may have played a role here.

Mediation and moderated-mediation analyses revealed cross-cultural differences between the two groups of student populations. While the effect of mobile phone addiction on stress in Chinese students is approximately split in half between a direct effect and a mediating effect through the level of rumination, in Czech students the stress levels are only affected through ruminative thinking (explaining a full 80% of the variance in perceived stress). For this group of students, the problematic smartphone use is not directly related to the level of stress.

The observed mediating effect of rumination is consistent with similar studies which also observed the mediating role of rumination on the relationship between mobile phone addiction and stress (Liu et al., 2017; Lian et al., 2021) and in the opposite direction and under similar circumstances of the Covid-19 pandemic (Peng et al., 2022). These results suggest that maladaptive behaviors associated with excessive smartphone use negatively affect students’ emotional state and their response to adverse circumstances in the form of increased rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). This negative attunement then contributes to a more pronounced experience of stress. An example of such a sequence is the effect on self-image in individuals with lower self-esteem and a stronger external source of self-concept (which is more typically the case for adolescents and young adults). A greater need for external validation of self-worth through acceptance on social media increases the need for activity and consumption of content on these apps. Negative feedback or the mere absence of sufficient feedback can increase a person’s risk of developing ruminative thoughts, which in turn increases the drive to achieve their goal through greater activity on social media, thereby increasing their stress levels or rates of depression (Feinstein et al., 2013). Another source of more negative emotionality and rumination and increased stress as a result of excessive smartphone use may be the so-called fear of missing out (FOMO) phenomenon which is based on the conviction that one is either not in the know or missing out on information, events, experiences or life decisions that could make his/her life better and which has been repeatedly identified as an element of mobile phone addiction and a possible source of rumination and stress in both European and Chinese students (Yang et al., 2019, 2021; Li et al., 2020; Brailovskaia et al., 2021).

The direct effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress was observed only in Chinese students. This difference may have several causes. As we observed, the degree of mobile phone addiction was lower among Czech students. As a result, the negative impact of the symptoms of this addiction, such as worrying, withdrawal symptoms or escape to the online environment with a reduction of activities in the offline world, could be lower, which then does not create as many daily-life disturbances (Pavia et al., 2016). The richer variety of direct interpersonal activities available to Czech students compared with Chinese students, for whom these opportunities seem to be reduced, may also have played a role. Finally, social peer pressure may also play a role in this context, as has already been suggested above in the context of the phenomenon among young Chinese of coming together to play online games. The perceived need to engage in these activities (also in the context of the collectivism principle mentioned above), even when one is not realistically interested, leads to a person forcing oneself to engage (Han and Qi, 2005; Assor et al., 2020). However, the increasing frequency of these activities raises, among other things, the pressure on the person and his or her perceived stress. Regardless, these results suggest that the socio-cultural background needs to be taken into account in research and prevention and intervention of problematic smartphone use and mobile phone addiction.

Finally, the potential impact of the Covid-19 pandemic circumstances should also be considered. Previous studies have shown that the pandemic had a negative impact on the mental well-being of many people, whether in terms of increased stress, anxiety or depression, which is closely associated with ruminative thinking (Daly et al., 2020; Novotný et al., 2020; Ye et al., 2020; Ramiz et al., 2021; Ettman et al., 2022; Jia et al., 2022; O’Connor et al., 2022). Similarly, the pandemic led to the necessity to spend much more time in the online environment with the online space being heavily laced with negative content. On the other hand, the data collection took place after 2 years of the pandemic, during which students had to cope with a number of adversities such as disruptions in their studies and the transition to online learning, uncertainty about their future studies and careers as well as difficulties at the societal level such as curfews, high numbers of infections, hospitalizations and deaths, financial implications, etc. We can assume that students developed strategies to cope with these issues during the pandemic. Moreover, by the time of data collection, the restrictive measures in both countries were already relatively relaxed, lectures were carried out face-to-face and students could already meet and travel relatively freely (except to places with a more serious situation in China such as Shanghai). Based on this, we hypothesize that the Covid-19 pandemic did not directly affect the observed relationships between the variables but rather accentuated the inter-individual differences between students depending on the availability of coping resources (Al Qudah et al., 2021; Kayis et al., 2021; Kovács et al., 2021). This is supported by the fact that the relationships reported in the present study and the mediating effect of rumination have been documented in studies both before (Liu et al., 2017; Lian et al., 2021) and during the Covid-19 pandemic (Peng et al., 2022).

Study limitations

The study has several limitations. First, women were over-represented in the research population, which may have had a partial effect on the relationships found. The main cause of this sex imbalance is the fact that faculties of education are mostly attended by women (regardless of country). Second, the cross-sectional nature of the study does not allow for a direct assessment of causality between the variables. Third, the research population consisted of students from faculties of education, which may have limited the ecological validity of the findings. Fourth, the use of the Wechat online tool in recruiting the respondents may have affected the representativeness of the sample (particularly in the context of the pattern and frequency of participants’ online presence). However, given that this tool is commonly and massively used by Chinese students, we do not think that potential respondents who are not online would be dropped by this approach. Finally, rumination and stress may be influenced by a number of other factors that were not included in this study and could potentially explain the mechanisms under investigation in greater depth. Future studies with more equal representation of both sexes, a more diverse sample, a wider range of variables and possibly of a longitudinal design should be carried out to further confirm our findings. The general and long-term validity of the results also needs to be verified in stable non-pandemic conditions.

Conclusion

This cross-cultural study is one of the few to examine the interaction between mobile phone addiction, ruminative thinking and perceived stress in different student populations, providing important insights into the role of the socio-cultural background. We observed that Czech students showed higher levels of rumination and perceived stress, while Chinese students showed significantly higher mobile phone addiction. Furthermore, the results showed that the effect of higher mobile phone addiction on perceived stress is mediated through rumination in both populations, as mobile phone addicted students exhibited greater excessive fixation on negative feelings, which then resulted in increased perceived stress. However, only Chinese students also demonstrated a direct association between mobile phone addiction and perceived stress levels.

These results suggest that the mechanism of interaction between excessive mobile phone use, ruminative thinking and perceived stress is culturally conditioned, which may limit the transferability of research findings in a global context. Given that smartphone use rates have been significantly increased in recent years, may have been further greatly exacerbated by the Covid-19 pandemic and are becoming a priority public health issue, particularly in the young population, a better understanding of cross-cultural differences is crucial for effective prevention and intervention.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Education, Palacký University Olomouc. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LV and HL collected data. JN made the statistical analysis. HL and JN wrote the draft of the manuscript. All authors conceived the idea of this study, contributed to the revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, and reviewed the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was supported by the Palacký University in Olomouc, grant numbers IGA_PDF_2022_004 and IGA_PDF_2022_028.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Al Qudah, M. F., Albursan, I. S., Hammad, H. I., Alzoubi, A. M., Bakhiet, S. F., Almanie, A. M., et al. (2021). Anxiety about COVID-19 infection, and its relation to smartphone addiction and demographic variables in middle eastern countries. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:11016. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

App Annie. (2020). The State of Mobile in 2020. Available at: https://gertkoot.files.wordpress.com/2020/01/2001_state_of_mobile_2020_main_en-1.pdf (Accessed May 14, 2022).

Google Scholar

Assor, A., Benita, M., Yitshaki, N., Geifman, Y., and Maree, W. (2020). Sense of authentic inner compass as a moral resource across cultures: possible implications for resisting negative peer-pressure and for parenting. J. Moral Educ. 49, 346–364. doi: 10.1080/03057240.2020.1727423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auter, P. J. (2007). Portable social groups: willingness to communicate, interpersonal communication gratifications, and cell phone use among young adults. Int. J. Mob. Commun. 5:139. doi: 10.1504/IJMC.2007.011813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balkis, M. (2013). Academic procrastination, academic life satisfaction and academic achievement: the mediation role of rational beliefs about studying. J. Cogn. Behav. Psychother. 13, 57–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0142-5

Google Scholar

Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered Mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2, 156–162. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brailovskaia, J., Stirnberg, J., Rozgonjuk, D., Margraf, J., and Elhai, J. D. (2021). From low sense of control to problematic smartphone use severity during Covid-19 outbreak: the mediating role of fear of missing out and the moderating role of repetitive negative thinking. PLoS One 16:e0261023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M. H. E. M., Larson, L. R., Sharaievska, I., Rigolon, A., McAnirlin, O., Mullenbach, L., et al. (2021). Psychological impacts from COVID-19 among university students: risk factors across seven states in the United States. PLoS One 16:e0245327. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byun, Y.-H., Ha, M., Kwon, H.-J., Hong, Y.-C., Leem, J.-H., Sakong, J., et al. (2013). Mobile phone use, blood Lead levels, and attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms in children: a longitudinal study. PLoS One 8:e59742. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, W., Fang, Z., Hou, G., Han, M., Xu, X., Dong, J., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of the COVID-19 epidemic on college students in China. Psychiatry Res. 287:112934. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cha, S.-S., and Seo, B.-K. (2018). Smartphone use and smartphone addiction in middle school students in Korea: prevalence, social networking service, and game use. Health Psychol. Open 5:205510291875504. doi: 10.1177/2055102918755046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S., An, D., Yao, Z., Liu, J. J.-W., Ning, X., Wong, J. P.-H., et al. (2021). Association between mental health knowledge level and depressive symptoms among Chinese college students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1850. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, S.-I. (2014). The relationship between life stress and smartphone addiction on Taiwanese university student: a mediation model of learning self-efficacy and social self-efficacy. Comput. Hum. Behav. 34, 49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Williamson, G. M. (1988). “Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States” in The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. The Social Psychology of Health. eds. S. Spacapan and S. Oskamp (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc), 31–67.

Google Scholar

Daly, M., Sutin, A. R., and Robinson, E. (2020). Longitudinal changes in mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from the UK household longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 52, 2549–2558. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720004432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 17, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., and Montag, C. (2019). Cognitive-and emotion-related dysfunctional coping processes: Transdiagnostic mechanisms explaining depression and Anxiety’s relations with problematic smartphone use. Curr. Addict. Rep. 6, 410–417. doi: 10.1007/s40429-019-00260-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ettman, C. K., Cohen, G. H., Abdalla, S. M., Sampson, L., Trinquart, L., Castrucci, B. C., et al. (2022). Persistent depressive symptoms during COVID-19: a national, population-representative, longitudinal study of U.S. adults. Lancet Reg. Health Am. 5:100091. doi: 10.1016/j.lana.2021.100091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J. A., Meuwly, N., and Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook and depressive symptoms: rumination as a mechanism. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2, 161–170. doi: 10.1037/a0033111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, B., and Sun, W. (2022). Bedtime procrastination and fatigue in Chinese college students: the mediating role of Mobile phone addiction. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 20, 1–14.doi: 10.1007/s11469-022-00796-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, J., Torous, J., Nicholas, J., Carney, R., Pratap, A., Rosenbaum, S., et al. (2017). The efficacy of smartphone-based mental health interventions for depressive symptoms: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. World Psychiatry 16, 287–298. doi: 10.1002/wps.20472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fruehwirth, J. C., Biswas, S., and Perreira, K. M. (2021). The Covid-19 pandemic and mental health of first-year college students: examining the effect of Covid-19 stressors using longitudinal data. PLoS One 16:e0247999. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, T., Li, J., Zhang, H., Gao, J., Kong, Y., Hu, Y., et al. (2018). The influence of alexithymia on mobile phone addiction: the role of depression, anxiety and stress. J. Affect. Disord. 225, 761–766. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. D. (1995). Technological addictions. Clinical psychology. Forum 76, 14–19.

Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. (2005). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J. Subst. Abus. 10, 191–197. doi: 10.1080/14659890500114359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, D. L., and Qi, Z. F. (2005). Psychological analysis of college students’ mobile phone addiction. Contemp. Youth Stud. 12, 34–38. https://doi.org/10.3969/j.issn.1006-1789.2005.12.010

Google Scholar

Hossain, S. F. A. (2019). Social networking and its role in media entrepreneurship. J. Media Manag. Entrep. 1, 73–86. doi: 10.4018/JMME.2019010105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibrahim, N. K., Baharoon, B. S., Banjar, W. F., Jar, A. A., Ashor, R. M., Aman, A. A., et al. (2018). Mobile phone addiction and its relationship to sleep quality and academic achievement of medical students at king Abdulaziz university, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia. J. Res. Health Sci. 18:e00420

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jia, R., Ayling, K., Chalder, T., Massey, A., Gasteiger, N., Broadbent, E., et al. (2022). The prevalence, incidence, prognosis and risk factors for symptoms of depression and anxiety in a UK cohort during the COVID-19 pandemic. BJPsych Open 8:e64. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2022.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joormann, J., and Quinn, M. E. (2014). Cognitive processes and emotion regulation in depression. Depress Anxiety 31, 308–315. doi: 10.1002/da.22264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiseler, M., Poolton, J. M., Backhouse, S. H., and Stanger, N. (2017). The relationship between mindfulness and life stress in student-athletes: the mediating role of coping effectiveness and decision rumination. Sport Psychol. 31, 288–298. doi: 10.1123/tsp.2016-0083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayis, A. R., Satici, B., Deniz, M. E., Satici, S. A., and Griffiths, M. D. (2021). Fear of COVID-19, loneliness, smartphone addiction, and mental wellbeing among the Turkish general population: a serial mediation model. Behav. Inform. Technol. 41, 2484–2496. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2021.1933181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovács, L. N., Baksa, D., Dobos, D., Eszlári, N., Gecse, K., Kocsel, N., et al. (2021). Perceived stress in the time of COVID-19: the association with brooding and COVID-related rumination in adults with and without migraine. BMC Psychol. 9:68. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00549-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasnova, H., Abramova, O., Notter, I., and Baumann, A. (2016). Why Phubbing is Toxic for Your Relationship: Understanding the Role of Smartphone Jealousy Among "generation y" Users. European Conference in Information Systems.

Google Scholar

Křeménková, L., Novotný, J. S., and Kvintová, J. (2021). Two waves of Covid-19 in university setting: mental health and underlying risk factors. Front. Psychol. 12:780071. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuang-Tsan, C., and Fu-Yuan, H. (2017). Study on relationship among university students’ life stress, smart Mobile phone addiction, and life satisfaction. J. Adult Dev. 24, 109–118. doi: 10.1007/s10804-016-9250-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuss, D. J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Online social networking and addiction—a review of the psychological literature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 8, 3528–3552. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, M., Lee, J.-Y., Won, W.-Y., Park, J.-W., Min, J.-A., Hahn, C., et al. (2013). Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PLoS One 8:e56936. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., Masuda, T., Ishii, K., Yasuda, Y., and Ohtsubo, Y. (2022). Cultural differences in the perception of daily stress between European Canadian and Japanese undergraduate students. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 014616722110703:014616722110703. doi: 10.1177/01461672211070360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, L. Y.-C., Ismail, M. A.-A., Mohammad, J. A.-M., and Yusoff, M. S. B. (2020). The relationship of smartphone addiction with psychological distress and neuroticism among university medical students. BMC Psychol. 8:97. doi: 10.1186/s40359-020-00466-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Griffiths, M. D., Mei, S., and Niu, Z. (2020). Fear of missing out and smartphone addiction mediates the relationship between positive and negative affect and sleep quality among Chinese university students. Front. Psychiatry 11:877. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Gu, S., Wang, Z., Li, H., Xu, X., Zhu, H., et al. (2019). Relationship between stressful life events and sleep quality: rumination as a mediator and resilience as a moderator. Front. Psych. 10:348. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C., Liu, D., and Dong, Y. (2019). Self-esteem and problematic smartphone use among adolescents: a moderated mediation model of depression and interpersonal trust. Front. Psychol. 10:2872. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Mu, W., Wang, Y., Xie, P., Zhang, Y., and Liu, T. (2022). Different roles of rumination and mindfulness among cyber-ostracized adolescents’ psychological well-being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:1222. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lian, S.-L., Sun, X.-J., Niu, G.-F., Yang, X.-J., Zhou, Z.-K., and Yang, C. (2021). Mobile phone addiction and psychological distress among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of rumination and moderating role of the capacity to be alone. J. Affect. Disord. 279, 701–710. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q.-Q., Yang, X.-J., Zhu, X.-W., and Zhang, D.-J. (2021). Attachment anxiety, loneliness, rumination and mobile phone dependence: a cross-sectional analysis of a moderated mediation model. Curr. Psychol. 40, 5134–5144. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00464-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q.-Q., Zhou, Z.-K., Yang, X.-J., Kong, F.-C., Niu, G.-F., and Fan, C.-Y. (2017). Mobile phone addiction and sleep quality among Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Comput. Hum. Behav. 72, 108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loleska, S., and Pop-Jordanova, N. (2021). Is smartphone addiction in the younger population a public health problem? Prilozi 42, 29–36. doi: 10.2478/prilozi-2021-0032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matar Boumosleh, J., and Jaalouk, D. (2017). Depression, anxiety, and smartphone addiction in university students-a cross sectional study. PLoS One 12:e0182239. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohamed, N. H., Beckstein, A., Hutchings, P. B., Pang, N. T. P., Dawood, S. R. S., Fadilah, R., et al. (2022). Cross-cultural differences in psychological health, perceived stress, and coping strategies of university students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. J. Mental Health 17, 65–77. doi: 10.5708/EJMH.17.2022.2.8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, R., and O’Connor, R. C. (2005). Predicting psychological distress in college students: the role of rumination and stress. J. Clin. Psychol. 61, 447–460. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdock, K. K., Carlucci, L., and Balsamo, M. (2019). A cross-cultural investigation of co-rumination via cellphone among emerging adults. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 38, 671–703. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2019.38.7.671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novotný, J. S., Gonzalez-Rivas, J. P., Kunzová, Š., Skladaná, M., Pospíšilová, A., Polcrová, A., et al. (2020). Risk factors underlying COVID-19 lockdown-induced mental distress. Front. Psych. 11:603014. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.603014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nurunnabi, M., Almusharraf, N., and Aldeghaither, D. (2021). Mental health and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic in higher education: evidence from G20 countries. J. Public Health Res. 9:2010. doi: 10.4081/jphr.2020.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D. B., Wilding, S., Ferguson, E., Cleare, S., Wetherall, K., McClelland, H., et al. (2022). Effects of COVID-19-related worry and rumination on mental health and loneliness during the pandemic: longitudinal analyses of adults in the UK COVID-19 mental health & wellbeing study. J. Ment. Health, 1–12. doi: 10.1080/09638237.2022.2069716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, J. A., Sandra, D. A., Colucci, É. S., Al Bikaii, A., Chmoulevitch, D., Nahas, J., et al. (2022). Smartphone addiction is increasing across the world: a meta-analysis of 24 countries. Comput. Hum. Behav. 129:107138. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, N., and Lee, H. (2012). Social implications of smartphone use: Korean college students’ smartphone use and psychological well-being. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 15, 491–497. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2011.0580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavia, L., Cavani, P., Di Blasi, M., and Giordano, C. (2016). Smartphone addiction inventory (SPAI): psychometric properties and confirmatory factor analysis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 63, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, Y., Zhou, H., Zhang, B., Mao, H., Hu, R., and Jiang, H. (2022). Perceived stress and mobile phone addiction among college students during the 2019 coronavirus disease: the mediating roles of rumination and the moderating role of self-control. Personal. Individ. Differ. 185:111222. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramiz, L., Contrand, B., Rojas Castro, M. Y., Dupuy, M., Lu, L., Sztal-Kutas, C., et al. (2021). A longitudinal study of mental health before and during COVID-19 lockdown in the French population. Glob. Health 17:29. doi: 10.1186/s12992-021-00682-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratan, Z. A., Parrish, A.-M., Zaman, S. B., Alotaibi, M. S., and Hosseinzadeh, H. (2021). Smartphone addiction and associated health outcomes in adult populations: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:12257. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182212257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinecke, L., Aufenanger, S., Beutel, M. E., Dreier, M., Quiring, O., Stark, B., et al. (2017). Digital stress over the life span: the effects of communication load and internet multitasking on perceived stress and psychological health impairments in a German probability sample. Media Psychol. 20, 90–115. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2015.1121832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, J. A., and David, M. E. (2016). My life has become a major distraction from my cell phone: partner phubbing and relationship satisfaction among romantic partners. Comput. Hum. Behav. 54, 134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, L. D., Whaling, K., Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., and Rokkum, J. (2013). The media and technology usage and attitudes scale: An empirical investigation. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2501–2511. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, C., and Terraneo, M. (2020). Mental well-being among workers: a cross-national analysis of job insecurity impact on the workforce. Soc. Indic. Res. 152, 421–442. doi: 10.1007/s11205-020-02441-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samtani, S., and Moulds, M. L. (2017). Assessing maladaptive repetitive thought in clinical disorders: a critical review of existing measures. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 53, 14–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharf, M., Rousseau, S., and Bsoul, S. (2017). Overparenting and Young adults’ interpersonal sensitivity: cultural and parental gender-related diversity. J. Child Fam. Stud. 26, 1356–1364. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0652-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoeni, A., Roser, K., and Röösli, M. (2015). Symptoms and cognitive functions in adolescents in relation to Mobile phone use during night. PLoS One 10:e0133528. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serra, G., Lo Scalzo, L., Giuffrè, M., Ferrara, P., and Corsello, G. (2021). Smartphone use and addiction during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic: cohort study on 184 Italian children and adolescents. Ital. J. Pediatr. 47:150. doi: 10.1186/s13052-021-01102-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Son, C., Hegde, S., Smith, A., Wang, X., and Sasangohar, F. (2020). Effects of COVID-19 on college students’ mental health in the United States: interview survey study. J. Med. Internet Res. 22:e21279. doi: 10.2196/21279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomée, S., Härenstam, A., and Hagberg, M. (2011). Mobile phone use and stress, sleep disturbances, and symptoms of depression among young adults - a prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health 11:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., and Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 59, 1–38. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., and Gelfand, M. J. (2012). “A theory of individualism and collectivism” in Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Ltd.), 498–520.

Google Scholar

Viner, R., Russell, S., Saulle, R., Croker, H., Stansfeld, C., Packer, J., et al. (2021). Impacts of school closures on physical and mental health of children and young people: a systematic review. Med Rxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2021.02.10.21251526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wacks, Y., and Weinstein, A. M. (2021). Excessive smartphone use is associated with health problems in adolescents and Young adults. Front. Psych. 12:669042. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.669042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P., Wang, X., Wu, Y., Xie, X., Wang, X., Zhao, F., et al. (2018). Social networking sites addiction and adolescent depression: a moderated mediation model of rumination and self-esteem. Personal. Individ. Differ. 127, 162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, C., Xiting, H., and Gang, B. (2003). Psychological researches on internet addiction. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 11, 355–359. https://doi.org/10.3969/j.issn.1671-3710.2003.03.020

Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Su, S., Jiang, Z., Guo, S., Lu, Q., Liu, L., et al. (2021). Prevalence and risk factors of mental health symptoms and suicidal behavior among university students in Wuhan, China during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychiatry 12:1161. doi: 10.3389/FPSYT.2021.695017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, T.-T., Wang, H.-Z., Fonseca, W., Zimmerman, M. A., Rost, D. H., Gaskin, J., et al. (2019). The relationship between academic stress and adolescents’ problematic smartphone usage. Addict. Res. Theory 27, 162–169. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2018.1488967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S. (1994). “Collectivism among the Japanese: a perspective from the self” in Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications. eds. U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç. Kâğitçibaşi, S.-C. Choi, and G. Yoon (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc), 175–188.

Google Scholar

Yang, Z., Asbury, K., and Griffiths, M. D. (2019). Do Chinese and British university students use smartphones differently? A cross-cultural mixed methods study. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 17, 644–657. doi: 10.1007/s11469-018-0024-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Liu, B., and Fang, J. (2021). Stress and problematic smartphone use severity: smartphone use frequency and fear of missing out as mediators. Front. Psych. 12:659288. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.659288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, B., Zhou, X., Im, H., Liu, M., Wang, X. Q., and Yang, Q. (2020). Epidemic rumination and resilience on college students’ depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of fatigue. Front. Public Health 8:560983. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.560983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

You, Z., Zhang, Y., Zhang, L., Xu, Y., and Chen, X. (2019). How does self-esteem affect mobile phone addiction? The mediating role of social anxiety and interpersonal sensitivity. Psychiatry Res. 271, 526–531. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. S. (2007). Cognitive behavior therapy with internet addicts: treatment outcomes and implications. Cyber Psychol. Behav. 10, 671–679. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yum, J. O. (1988). The impact of Confucianism on interpersonal relationships and communication patterns in East Asia. Commun. Monogr. 55, 374–388. doi: 10.1080/03637758809376178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mobile phone addiction, rumination, perceived stress, university students, mediation, cross-cultural differences, Czech Republic, China

Citation: Liu H, Novotný JS and Váchová L (2022) The effect of mobile phone addiction on perceived stress and mediating role of ruminations: Evidence from Chinese and Czech university students. Front. Psychol. 13:1057544. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1057544

Received: 04 October 2022; Accepted: 29 November 2022;
Published: 19 December 2022.

Edited by:

Giovanna Nigro, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marco Fabbri, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy
Milka Escalera Chávez, Autonomous University of San Luis Potosi, Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Liu, Novotný and Váchová. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucie Váchová, lucie.vachova@upol.cz

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.