Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 06 December 2022
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Reviews in Psychology for Clinical Settings View all 5 articles

Which outcome variables are associated with psychological inflexibility/flexibility for chronic pain patients? A three level meta-analysis

  • School of Educational Science, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, China

The psychological flexibility model can be seen as a basis for an integrated and progressive psychological approach to chronic pain management. Some researchers suggest that psychological flexibility and inflexibility represent distinct processes and constructs. This meta-analysis is the first to provide a summary estimate of the overall effect size for the relationship between psychological (in)flexibility and common outcomes among chronic pain patients. The research protocol was registered in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO, https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/), registration number CRD42021285705. Four databases were searched (PsycINFO; PubMed; Web of Science, CINAHL) along with reference lists. Thirty-six cross-sectional studies were included (7,779 participants). Meta-analyses (random effects model) indicated a significant medium negative association between psychological flexibility and pain intensity or functional impairment. The present study also indicated a significant small to medium association between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity, a nearly large association between psychological inflexibility and functional impairment as well as the quality of life, and a large association between psychological inflexibility and anxiety/depression. Due to the limited number of included studies, the relationship between risk behavior and psychological inflexibility may not be significant. Types of countries and instruments measuring psychological inflexibility may explain part of the heterogeneity. These findings may carry significant implications for chronic pain patients regarding the potential relationship between psychological inflexibility or flexibility and these outcomes. It may consequently form the basis for more robust testing of causal and manipulable relationships.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/, identifier: CRD42021285705.

Introduction

Chronic pain is one of the most common health conditions worldwide in the general population (Brooks et al., 2017; Si et al., 2019; Rhodes et al., 2021) and is a source of distress and disability that affects all aspects of a patient's life. For example, chronic physical pain is linked to various psychological distress, such as anxiety, stress, and depression (Goldbart et al., 2020; Esteve et al., 2021). Furthermore, individuals in a state of psychological distress experience more intense pain, leading to a reciprocal reinforcement between psychological distress and pain (Davis et al., 2004; Goldbart et al., 2020). Chronic pain entails economic costs higher than other diseases, such as heart disease, diabetes, and cancer (Esteve et al., 2021). In addition to the monetary costs of treatment, other costs (e.g., disability compensation, loss of productivity, sleep problems, and increased healthcare utilization) are also common (Turk and Burwinkle, 2005). Therefore, it is imperative to develop an effective chronic pain management program for chronic pain patients.

There are a variety of treatments for chronic pain. However, medical and other previously traditional treatments are costly and of questionable effectiveness in treating persistent pain (Hoffman et al., 2007; Rhodes et al., 2021). Conversely, psychological treatments can benefit patients with chronic pain, specifically in terms of daily functioning and health-related quality of life (Hoffman et al., 2007; Rhodes et al., 2021). One of the noteworthy psychological treatments is acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). The meta-analyses of ACT for chronic pain show that ACT was more effective than controls (i.e., no alternative intervention or treatment as usual) in terms of functioning, pain intensity, depression, and anxiety (Hughes et al., 2017; Trindade et al., 2021). ACT targets psychological flexibility, which is the primary determinant of mental health and effective action (Dionne et al., 2013). Psychological flexibility refers to an individual's ability to focus on the present moment, move toward their goals, and persist or change behaviors to serve valued ends. The opposite is psychological inflexibility which could be dysfunctional, ultimately contributing to and exacerbating psychopathology (Hayes et al., 2006; Daks et al., 2020; Fang and Ding, 2020). Psychological flexibility is established through six core ACT processes, i.e., acceptance, defusion, present moment, self-as-content, committed action, and values. In contrast, the opposite six dimensions constitute psychological inflexibility: lack of present moment awareness, lack of contact with values, inaction, self-as-content, fusion, and experiential avoidance (Hayes et al., 2006; Ding and Zheng, 2022).

The psychological flexibility model can be seen as a basis for an integrated and progressive psychological approach to chronic pain management (McCracken and Morley, 2014). This model fully integrates cognitive and environmental influences as the core processes of health and risk behaviors (McCracken and Morley, 2014). In chronic pain, psychological inflexibility is considered to be an important determinant of mood swings and ineffective living (Wicksell et al., 2008). It also leads to short-term relief behaviors, such as physical inactivity and avoidance of activities (i.e., remaining still or not going out of doors). However, these behaviors often prevent them from activities that increase their quality of life (e.g., visiting friends, working, or playing sports) (Maathz et al., 2020; Rhodes et al., 2021). Over time, the patient's strategy of avoiding negative psychological events produces a narrow and inflexible pattern of action, resulting in increased disability (Wicksell et al., 2008). In comparison, the flexibility model suggests that pain and suffering are inherent aspects of human life. Psychological flexibility, on the other hand, is characterized by accepting inner experiences, being mindful, and participating in actions aligned with individual goals and values, which may relieve the psychological burden and increase wellbeing (Maathz et al., 2020; Rhodes et al., 2021). Moreover, psychological flexibility is a malleable factor which is amenable to change through ACT (Hayes et al., 2012). It is known that outcome variables are usually organized into four categories, such as functioning (e.g., goal progress, pain interference, functional impairment, and subjective accounts of functioning), quality of life, mental health symptomology (e.g., symptoms of depression and/or anxiety), and observable behavioral changes (e.g., smoking cessation and risk medication-related behaviors) (Stockton et al., 2019). For the person with chronic pain, pain intensity has been the primary target for pain measurement (Rhodes et al., 2021). Therefore, understanding the relationship between psychological (in)flexibility and these outcomes for patients with chronic pain may have important implications for intervention and, thus, for organizations and patient care.

The previous study of patients with chronic pain has supported the role of the various components of psychological flexibility in their wellbeing and daily functioning (McCracken and Velleman, 2010), such as acceptance (Varallo et al., 2021, 2022), defusion, present moment, self-as-content (Nigol and Di Benedetto, 2020), committed action, and values (McCracken and Vowles, 2008). However, some studies suggest that global psychological flexibility may work better as a predictor of treatment outcome than measures of various components from the psychological flexibility model (Gilpin et al., 2019; Åkerblom et al., 2021). Besides, some meta-analytic results suggested that the individual dimensions of flexibility and inflexibility might represent distinct (yet related) processes and constructs (Rolffs et al., 2018; Daks and Rogge, 2020). Nonetheless, many studies have mixed the two, i.e., using instruments that measure inflexibility to measure flexibility (Dubey et al., 2020; Goldbart et al., 2020). Some researchers suggest that the acceptance and action questionnaire-II (AAQ-II), which has been widely used for measuring psychological flexibility, may be more appropriate for psychological inflexibility (Gilpin et al., 2019; Åkerblom et al., 2021). It is worth noting that different results also appeared for the same relationship between outcome variables, such as the relationship between psychological flexibility and pain severity. For example, one study (Allen et al., 2018) showed that the correlation coefficient of psychological flexibility and pain severity was not significant, while in another study, the effect size tended to be large (Vallejo et al., 2021).

As mentioned above, to date, no meta-analysis has summarized the relationship between psychological flexibility/inflexibility and these outcome variables in people with chronic pain. A meta-analysis of this subject is important to gain a more coherent understanding of these relationships, as the literature indicates chronic pain patients are already subject to intervention strategies designed to enhance psychological flexibility or reduce psychological inflexibility to improve their lives. Further analytical evidence is needed to examine whether and how psychological (in)flexibility could be associated with these outcomes for chronic pain patients, which would form the basis for more robust testing of causal and manipulable relationships.

Thus, the primary aim of this review was to identify and integrate all published findings on associations between psychological flexibility/inflexibility and these five outcomes, and address an analytic question about the magnitude and direction of the associations within chronic pain patients. A second aim is to determine which variables potentially moderate the relationship between psychological (in)flexibility and outcomes. We hypothesized that the following four moderators would systematically influence the effect: (1) the type of measurement, (2) the age of target sample, (3) the country, and (4) the proportion of females. A third research goal is to address descriptive questions about how these variables are being measured for chronic pain.

Methods

Selection of studies

The meta-analysis was reported following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) statement (Page et al., 2021). The research protocol was registered in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO, https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/) to minimize the risk of bias in this systematic review, registration number CRD42021285705.

We searched PsycINFO, PubMed, Web of Science, and CINAHL, all of which were done on October 1, 2022. No date restrictions were applied to the search to maximize the search strategy. The main search terms used included keywords and free words: (“experiential avoidance” OR “psychological acceptance” OR “psychological flexibility” OR “psychological inflexibility”) AND (“chronic pain” OR cancer OR fibromyalgia). In addition, reference lists of eligible studies, relevant review articles, and relevant meta-analyses were manually searched to minimize potential publication bias.

Inclusion criteria

(a) The sample population included chronic pain patients, included cancer patients, and fibromyalgia patients;

(b) Psychological flexibility or psychological inflexibility was measured.

(c) The relationship between psychological (in)flexibility and outcome measures (i.e., pain severity/intensity, functioning, quality of life/wellbeing, mental health symptomology, and behavioral changes) were reported with Pearson's r correlation coefficient or standardized regression coefficients (β).

If studies reported standardized regression coefficients, they would be converted into correlation coefficients using the equation: r = β + 0.05λ (λ = 1 if β is positive and λ = 0 if β is negative) (Liu et al., 2021).

Exclusion criteria

(a) Review, meta-analysis, or theoretical articles;

(b) Without reporting Pearson's r correlation coefficient or standardized regression coefficients.

Difficulties in deciding the selection were discussed between the two authors. Any ambiguity about studying eligibility was settled via discussion. A full consensus was reached between the two authors according to the criteria.

Data extraction and coding

We completed the data extraction and inspection together. If there were disagreements, agreements would be reached through a full consultation. Extracted data include: authors and year of publication, country, instruments, study characteristics (e.g., sample size, mean age, percentage of female), and effect sizes.

We created dummy variables for countries and tools of outcome measures (i.e., psychological flexibility or inflexibility, psychological distress, dysfunction, pain intensity, behavioral outcomes, and quality of life). We divided countries into Eastern and Western according to their geographical distribution. If the measurement was used only in one effect size, it would be coded as “other” to reduce the number of dummy variables (Ding and Zheng, 2022). The dummy variables were mutually exclusive.

Data analysis

There is a discrepancy between the instruments measuring psychological flexibility and the constructs the authors claim to be measured. For example, AAQ-II (Bond et al., 2011) is more appropriate for measuring psychological inflexibility (Kashdan et al., 2020), while some studies use this to measure alternative constructs such as “psychological acceptance” or “experiential avoidance.” As such, the AAQ-II was considered a measure of psychological inflexibility in the present study. The directions of their effects were adjusted accordingly regardless of the construct defined within the individual study. Other constructs were categorized according to the author's classification. For example, if the authors use The Brief Pain Response Inventory (BPRI) to measure psychological flexibility, this study classified it as psychological flexibility.

If studies reported a range of outcome measures, the authors selected the outcome measure related to pain severity/intensity, functioning, quality of life/wellbeing, psychological distress, negative effects, positive effects, and behavioral outcomes. All relevant effect sizes would be extracted for the same category because studies report multiple effect sizes due to several relevant outcome measures and/or subscales of overarching concepts, e.g., “physical disability” and “psychosocial disability” as subscales of functioning (Jurkovich et al., 1995).

Due to differences in measurement tools, the effect sizes were analyzed using the random effects model in R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2016) with the packages metafor and meta (Viechtbauer, 2010). A three-level meta-analytic model was used to synthesize effect sizes and conduct moderator analyses (Cheung, 2014). This model can explore three sources of variance: sampling variance of the observed effect sizes (Level 1), the variance between effect sizes from the same study (Level 2), and variance between studies (Cheung, 2014; Ding and Zheng, 2022). The significant variance of level 2 and level 3 indicated a heterogeneous effect size distribution. In addition, heterogeneity was also tested using the 75% rule (Assink and Carlijn, 2016), which contends that heterogeneity can be considered substantial if <75% of the overall variance can be attributed to level 1. Therefore, potential moderating effects would be examined according to the significant variance of level 2 or level 3 and the 75% rule. We first assess the (potential) moderating effects separately in the univariate model. If the effects were significant, we extended the meta-analytic model by adding all significant moderating variables simultaneously (Assink and Carlijn, 2016). Additionally, if there was no significant difference between the three-level model and the two-level model, then the results of the two-level model would be used according to Occam's razor (Harrer et al., 2021). The heterogeneity among the results of the two-level model would be tested by the Q test and the I2 test (Higgins et al., 2022). If I2 > 50%, it is suggested to have moderate-to-high heterogeneity. All model parameters were estimated using the restricted maximum likelihood method.

All extracted effects were converted to Fisher's Z values and weighted by sample size before analysis. These effects were then meta-analyzed, and the results were subsequently converted back to correlations for interpretation (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). Following Cohen's convention, the magnitude of effect for r is classified as small (0.10), medium (0.30), or large (0.50) with 95% CI. Outliers of the effect size were inspected and removed. Outliers were defined as deviations greater than three standard deviations from the mean (Chiang et al., 2022).

Publication bias was first tested by Egger's regression test for funnel plot asymmetry (Kim et al., 2021; Higgins et al., 2022). We included sampling variance as a moderator and re-estimated the three-level model, and the significant effect of sampling variance was considered as an indicator of publication bias (Kim et al., 2021). Besides, a fail-safe N analysis was conducted to estimate the number of missing studies that would be needed to reduce the overall effect size insignificantly. If N was larger than 5k+10 (k was the number of included relevant studies in the present meta-analysis), then publication bias would be suggested as minimal. If publication bias were to be found, trim and fill analyses were performed to provide an adjusted average effect size (Duval and Tweedie, 2000) to correct. Sensitivity analyses were also conducted to test whether estimates were robust.

Results

Description of studies

Studies characteristics

Initially, 1,373 citations were identified by searching electronic bibliographic databases and reference lists. After detecting duplicates and screening titles and abstracts for relevance, 102 articles were identified as being potentially eligible for further assessment. After reading the full text of each article, 36 studies met the criteria and were included (see Figure 1 for the details).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study flow diagram.

The characteristics of the 36 included studies are summarized in Table 1. Sample sizes for included studies ranged from 34 to 1,522 (total participants = 7,779). The mean age of participants in these studies ranged from 18.86 to 73.8. The proportion of females ranged from 0 to 100%.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Study characteristics and key findings.

The measurements

Thirteen studies used the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS) to measure psychological inflexibility, and AAQ-II was used in 19 studies. Psychological flexibility was measured with AAQ (two studies), The Brief Pain Response Inventory (BPRI) (one study), the Comprehensive Assessment of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Processes (CompACT-18) (one study), and Flexibility Index Test-60 (FIT-60).

Anxiety was measured with Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)-Anxiety (eight studies), Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS)-20 (four studies), Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (one study), Self-rating Anxiety Scale (one study), and Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) (one study), Anxiety Sensitivity Index-III (ASI-III) (one study), Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) (one study). Depression was measured with HADS-Depression (eight studies), Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-9 (four studies), BCMDI (three studies), DASS-21-Depression (three studies), PROMIS (one study), and Self-rating Depression Scale (one studies). Functional impairment was mostly measured with Brief Pain Inventory-functional impairment subscale (BPI) (five studies), and Sickness Impact Profile (SIP) (four studies), Pain Disability Index (PDI) (four studies). Quality of life was measured with the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS), Short Form-12 Health Survey (SF-12), World Health Organization Quality of Life scale 3 (WHOQOL-BREF), and Stroke Impact Scale (SIS). More details can be seen in Table 1.

Meta-analyses

Psychological inflexibility/flexibility and pain intensity

There was no significant difference between the two-level and three-level models, so we used the results of the two-level model according to Occam's razor (Harrer et al., 2021). Aggregating across 26 correlational studies that examined the relationship between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity, the overall effect size was statistically significant and nearly medium (r = 0.26, 95% CI = 0.22, 0.30, p < 0.001, n = 4,839, I2 = 43.7%; Q = 44.37, df = 25, p < 0.001). The results were presented in a forest plot in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity.

Moderating effects of mean age, percentage of females, country, and measurement tools were evaluated separately in univariate models. We found a significant moderating effect of the country and measurement of psychological inflexibility on the association in univariate models, as shown by the results of the omnibus test [F(1, 24) =5.22, p < 0.05; F(1, 24) =4.65, p < 0.05]. When the two significant moderating variables were added simultaneously, only the regression coefficient of the eastern country (−0.11) was significant [t (96) = −2.20, p < 0.005], and the test for residual heterogeneity was not significant (Q = 32.33, df = 24, p > 0.05). It was suggested that the relationship between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity is larger in the western country than in the eastern country.

There was no publication bias in the Egger tests (p > 0.1) and fail-safe N analysis (N = 3,515) on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity, and further sensitivity analysis showed that the results were robust.

Four correlational studies examined the relationship between psychological flexibility and pain intensity. The overall effect size was statistically significant and medium (r = −0.32, 95% CI = −0.49, −0.14, p < 0.001, I2 = 91.21%; Q = 49.65, df = 3, p < 0.001). The I2 and Q statistic indicated high variation between studies attributable to heterogeneity.

Psychological inflexibility/flexibility and functional impairment

The results indicated a nearly large pooled effect size estimate (r = 0.49, 95% CI = 0.43, 0.52, p < 0.001) for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and functional impairment. The results are presented in Figure 3, and more details can be seen in Table 2. There were significant variances within the same studies (i.e., level 2 variance). However, no significant moderating variables were found in this study. That is, mean age, country, and measurements were not potential moderating variables. Possible reasons were summarized in the Discussion section.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plot for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and functional impairment.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The relationship between psychological (in)flexibility and outcomes, and heterogeneity.

There was no publication bias in the Egger tests (p > 0.1) and fail-safe N analysis (N = 16,016) on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and dysfunction, and further sensitivity analysis showed that the results were robust.

Three studies, including five correlations, examined the relationship between psychological flexibility and functional impairment. The overall effect size was statistically significant and medium (r = −0.40, 95% CI = −0.60, −0.17, p < 0.001). There were significant variances within level 2. Fail-safe N analysis (N = 238) suggested that there was no publication bias, and sensitivity analysis showed that the results were robust.

Psychological inflexibility/flexibility and anxiety

Aggregating across 14 correlations in 12 studies that examined the relationship between psychological inflexibility and anxiety, the overall effect size was statistically significant and large (r = 0.55, 95% CI = 0.43, 0.52, p < 0.001). The results are presented in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest plot for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and anxiety.

There were significant variances within level 2. Moderating effects of mean age, percentage of females, country, and tools of measurement were evaluated separately in univariate models. We found a significant moderating effect of the measurements of psychological inflexibility [F (1, 12) = 5.82, p < 0.05]. It indicated a significantly larger pooled effect size estimate for AAQ-II (r = 0.69, 95% CI = 0.56, 0.83, p < 0.001) than that for PIPS (r = 0.49, 95% CI = 0.35, 0.64, p < 0.001). No significant moderating effects were found for the percentage of females, mean age, and country.

There was no publication bias in the Egger tests (p > 0.1) and fail-safe N analysis (N = 6,057) on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and anxiety, and further sensitivity analysis showed that the results were robust.

Only one study examined the relationship between psychological flexibility and anxiety, r = −0.57.

Psychological inflexibility and depression

There were twenty correlations in sixteen studies that examined the relationship between psychological inflexibility and depression. It showed a large pooled effect size estimate (r = 0.57, 95% CI = 0.51, 0.62, p < 0.001). The results were presented in a forest plot in Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Forest plot for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and depression.

There were significant variances within level 3. Moderating effects of mean age, percentage of females, country, the tools of measurement were evaluated separately in univariate models. We found a significantly moderating effect of the country [F (1, 18) = 5.78, p < 0.05]. It indicated a significant larger pooled effect size estimate for the western country (r = 0.68, 95% CI = 0.61, 0.76, p < 0.001) than that for the eastern country (Δr = −0.20, 95% CI = −0.37, −0.02, p < 0.05). No significant moderating effects were found for the percentage of females, mean age, and measurements.

There was no publication bias in the Egger tests (p > 0.1) and fail-safe N analysis (N = 10,769) on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and depression, and further sensitivity analysis showed that the result was robust.

No study examined the relationship between psychological flexibility and depression.

Psychological inflexibility/flexibility and quality of life

Aggregating across 12 correlations in 9 studies that examined the relationship between psychological inflexibility and quality of life, the results indicated a nearly large pooled effect size estimate (r = −0.47, 95% CI = −0.53, −0.41, p < 0.001). The results were presented in a forest plot in Figure 6.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Forest plot for the relationship between psychological inflexibility and quality of life.

There were significant variances within level 2. Moderating effects of mean age, percentage of females, country, and tools of measurement were evaluated separately in univariate models. However, no significant moderating effects were found.

There was no publication bias in Egger tests (p > 0.1) and fail-safe N analysis (N = 2,114) on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and quality of life, and further sensitivity analysis showed that the result was robust.

Only one study examined the relationship between psychological flexibility and quality of life, r = 0.37.

Psychological inflexibility and risk behaviors

Two studies examined the relationship between psychological inflexibility and risk behaviors (i.e., opioid use). It showed a non-significant pooled effect size estimate (r = 0.41, 95% CI = −0.18, 0.77, p > 0.1; I2 = 96.3%; Q = 27.1, df = 1, p < 0.001).

Discussion

To date, no meta-analysis has summarized the relationship of psychological flexibility / inflexibility with these outcome variables in people with chronic pain for patients and clinical workers. A meta-analysis of this topic is important to better understand these relationships. Many studies have shown that ACT can improve the lives and reduce the symptoms of chronic pain patients by increasing their psychological flexibility or reducing psychological inflexibility. The elucidation of these relationships may provide possible explanatory mechanisms as to why ACT works in patients with chronic pain. The current study provided the first truly comprehensive perspective on the relationship between psychological flexibility/inflexibility and pain intensity/severity, quality of life, psychological distress, functioning, and risk behavior, and provided a conceptual and empirical basis for future work.

The present meta-analysis indicates a significant medium negative association between psychological flexibility and pain intensity or functional impairment. The present study also indicates a significant small to medium association between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity. There is a nearly large association between psychological inflexibility and functional impairment or the quality of life, and a large association between psychological inflexibility and anxiety/depression. Due to the limited number of included studies, the relationship between risk behavior and psychological inflexibility may not be significant. It is important to note that the relationship between psychological inflexibility and risk behavior was insignificant, and more future research is needed. There were not enough studies to aggregate the effect sizes of the relationship between psychological flexibility and anxiety, depression, quality of life, and risk behaviors. Previous research has suggested that psychological flexibility and inflexibility might represent distinct (yet related) processes and constructs (Rolffs et al., 2018; Daks and Rogge, 2020). Therefore, more research is needed to distinguish the two constructs.

This study is a research meta-analysis of correlational relationships. Therefore, it raises questions about causal inference, particularly about the possibility of inverse direction. However, pain catastrophizing, one of the strongest psychological predictors of pain outcomes (Schutze et al., 2018), can be explained by some essential key elements, such as repetitive negative thinking and rumination (Petrini and Arendt-Nielsen, 2020) which are some kind of overlapping with cognitive fusion (one of the core components of psychological flexibility) (McCracken et al., 2014). Besides, psychological flexibility and pain catastrophizing are considered as process mechanisms underlying the efficacy of ACT for pain patients (Trompetter et al., 2015). Furthermore, interventional studies in ACT provide a means to draw causal relationships because they aim to reduce chronic pain patients' psychological inflexibility and enhance their psychological flexibility (Hughes et al., 2017; Simpson et al., 2017; Trindade et al., 2021). These meta-analyses concluded that patients had significantly lower pain intensity, functioning, anxiety, depression, and psychological inflexibility after the ACT intervention than the control group. ACT is also one of the best evidence for reducing catastrophizing (Schutze et al., 2018). Nonetheless, these meta-analyses did not summarize the effects of ACT on the quality of life and risk behaviors in patients with chronic pain. Given the present study's findings, future intervention trials in the ACT may benefit from exploring the effects on quality of life and risk behaviors in chronic pain patients.

This study found that the type of country was a moderating variable in the relationship between psychological inflexibility and pain intensity or depression, significantly higher in chronic pain patients in western countries than in eastern countries. It is consistent with previous cross-sectional studies, which suggest that patients in Eastern countries may have a greater tolerance for pain (Thong et al., 2017; Yotnuengnit et al., 2022). This may be related to underlying cultural differences: stoicism is seen as a positive quality in Eastern cultures, whereas the inability to tolerate pain is seen as a sign of weakness (Narayan, 2010; Thong et al., 2017). We also found that the instruments of psychological inflexibility (i.e., AAQ-II, PIPS) moderate the relationship between psychological inflexibility and anxiety. The pooled effect sizes measured with AAQ-II were significantly larger than that with PIPS. This may be due to the construct and discriminant validity of AAQ-II. Some researchers highlighted questions surrounding the psychometric properties of the AAQ-II (Cherry et al., 2021; Garner and Golijani-Moghaddam, 2021), which suggested that the AAQ-II may measure psychological distress rather than psychological inflexibility. Thus, the correlation between psychological flexibility and anxiety, as measured by the AAQ-II, was higher. But this does not explain why the different instruments of psychological inflexibility had no impact on the relationship between psychological inflexibility and depression. In any case, it is necessary to develop alternative measures to more accurately capture the process of psychological inflexibility, which has been done by many authors (Francis et al., 2016; Rolffs et al., 2018). However, the validity of these instruments also deserves to be examined in future studies.

In the present study, no moderating variables were found that explained the heterogeneity of the relationship between psychological inflexibility and dysfunction. This may be due to the fact that the present study did not distinguish between psychological and physiological functioning, and included the both two distinct subcomponents in the heading of functioning. A recent study has specifically explored the relationship between the six subcomponents of psychological flexibility and functioning (Ding and Zheng, 2022). This study identified the measurement instrument of functioning as a possible factor contributing to heterogeneity (Ding and Zheng, 2022). Perhaps this study can give deeper insight into the relationship between psychological flexibility and functioning.

Given these associations, it seems that the way chronic pain patients relate to the difficult experiences they encounter may be implicated in their pain intensity, quality of life, functioning, and mental health symptomology. For instance, chronic pain patients who can focus on the present situation and move toward their goals may also experience improved professional quality of life and functioning, and lower levels of pain intensity or mental health symptomology. This may be due to the fact that psychological flexibility could provide less painful experiences and psychological distress in the pursuit of value-oriented behavior and reduce long-term distress and increase the ability to cope with chronic pain (Hayes et al., 2014; Goldbart et al., 2020). While for those who were trapped in their negative thinking or avoidance (i.e., psychological inflexibility), they may experience more anxiety, depression, dysfunction, and lower levels of quality of life which in turn may exacerbate the pain over time (Maathz et al., 2020; Rhodes et al., 2021). As such, targeting psychological inflexibility/flexibility may likely be valuable for chronic pain patients. As current literature highlights, ACT toward improving chronic pain patients' psychological flexibility seems to have desirable effectiveness in managing chronic pain (Hughes et al., 2017; Trindade et al., 2021). However, training in psychological flexibility should not be considered a panacea, and the responsibility for mental health, quality of life, or functioning should not be considered exclusively lying within the individual.

There were some limitations in the present study. First, a major weakness of this meta-analysis is that the studies' methodological quality was not rated. It was suggested that rating would be difficult due to the lack of clear methodological standards and relevant detail in the Methods sections of these studies (Levin et al., 2012; Low et al., 2020). This study excluded unpublished studies, providing a general approach to ensure methodological quality, but also raising the risk of publication bias affecting the results. A more comprehensive search of the published and unpublished literature may be useful for further research in this area. Second, the studies included were based on cross-sectional data, so the direction of causality remains unclear. Thus, the results need to be interpreted with caution. Third, we did not contact any study authors whose publications provide insufficient data about the correlation coefficient. It may have reduced the total number of studies included in analyses. Despite these limitations, the current study provided the first truly comprehensive perspective on the relationship between psychological flexibility/inflexibility and pain intensity/severity, quality of life, psychological distress, and functioning, providing a conceptual and empirical basis for future work.

Future research should distinguish psychological flexibility from inflexibility to better understand the psychological flexibility model in chronic pain. This meta-analysis suggests that the magnitude of effect sizes between psychological inflexibility/flexibility and functional impairment is different. Although recent research has shown that psychological flexibility and inflexibility are two different concepts (Rolffs et al., 2018; Daks and Rogge, 2020), no additional studies have shown differences in effect sizes between psychological inflexibility/flexibility and mental symptomology for chronic pain patients. In addition, if future research can combine psychological flexibility/inflexibility with the strong predictors of pain outcomes (such as pain catastrophizing), it will be more helpful to understand the mechanisms by which the psychological flexibility model works in patients with pain. Besides, it would also be beneficial to include more behavioral measures in studies for people with chronic pain. As the goal of ACT to enhance psychological flexibility is to help individuals pursue value-based goals, measures to assess behavior or behavioral change would be more useful to evaluate the psychological flexibility model in chronic pain. In addition, more research on long-term outcomes would be worthwhile. Although some evidence suggested psychological flexibility plays an important role in the development of pain and in long-term adaptation to pain (Linton and Shaw, 2011), the studies included were mostly cross-sectional. Also, data should be expanded beyond self-report data and collected from multiple informants to reduce reporting biases. Furthermore, it would be helpful to develop tools or use measures related to chronic pain. And, clinical workers should take note of efficient and cost-effective interventions in reducing chronic pain and functional impairment as well as improving mental health or quality of life for chronic pain patients.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

SF: conceptualization and project administration. SF and DD: methodology, data collection, data curation, formal analysis, visualization, writing–original draft, and writing–review and editing. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by Philosophy and Social Science Planning Key Project of Anhui Province (Grant Number AHSKZ2020D37).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the support of Anhui Provincial Women's Federation and Anhui Provincial Department of Education 2022 Annual Women's Theory Research Key Project (2022-FNYJ-002), Open Fund Project of Key Laboratory of Philosophy and Social Science of Anhui Province on Adolescent Mental Health, and Crisis Intelligence Intervention and Anhui Topnotch Talents of Disciplines in Universities and Colleges (SF).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Åkerblom, S., Perrin, S., Rivano Fischer, M., and McCracken, L. M. (2021). Predictors and mediators of outcome in cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic pain: the contributions of psychological flexibility. J. Behav. Med. 44, 111–122. doi: 10.1007/s10865-020-00168-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Allen, T. M., Struemph, K. L., Toledo-Tamula, M. A., Wolters, P. L., Baldwin, A., Widemann, B., et al. (2018). The relationship between heart rate variability, psychological flexibility, and pain in neurofibromatosis type 1. Pain Pract. 18, 969–978. doi: 10.1111/papr.12695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assink, M. W., and Carlijn, J. M. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: a step-by-step tutorial. Quant. Methods Psychol. 12, 154–174. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Barke, A., Riecke, J., Rief, W., and Glombiewski, J. A. (2015). The Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS)—validation, factor structure and comparison to the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ) and other validated measures in German chronic back pain patients. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 16, 171. doi: 10.1186/s12891-015-0641-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Berrocal, M. C, Rivas Moya, T., Venditti, F., and Bernini, O. (2016). On the contribution of psychological flexibility to predict adjustment to breast cancer. Psicothema 28, 266–271. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., et al. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action questionnaire–II: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behav. Ther. 42, 676–688. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, J. M., Iwanaga, K., Chiu, C. Y., Cotton, B. P., Deiches, J., Morrison, B., et al. (2017). Relationships between self-determination theory and theory of planned behavior applied to physical activity and exercise behavior in chronic pain. Psychol. Health Med. 22, 814–822. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2017.1282161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Brown, S. L., Roush, J. F., Marshall, A. J., Jones, C., and Key, C. (2020). The intervening roles of psychological inflexibility and functional impairment in the relation between cancer-related pain and psychological distress. Int. J. Behav. Med. 27, 100–107. doi: 10.1007/s12529-019-09838-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Carvalho, S. A., Pinto-Gouveia, J., Gillanders, D., Lapa, T., Valentim, A., Santos, E., et al. (2020). Above and beyond emotional suffering: the unique contribution of compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding in chronic pain. Scand. J. Pain 20, 853–857. doi: 10.1515/sjpain-2020-0082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Cary, M. A., Brittain, D. R., and Gyurcsik, N. C. (2017). Differences in psychosocial responses to pain between sufficiently and insufficiently active adults with arthritis. Psychol. Health 32, 765–780. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1300258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherry, K. M., Hoeven, E. V., Patterson, T. S., and Lumley, M. N. (2021). Defining and measuring “psychological flexibility”: a narrative scoping review of diverse flexibility and rigidity constructs and perspectives. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 84, 101973. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, M. W. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: a structural equation modeling approach. Psychol. Methods 19, 211–229. doi: 10.1037/a0032968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, J. J., Lam, P. H., Chen, E., and Miller, G. E. (2022). Psychological stress during childhood and adolescence and its association with inflammation across the lifespan: a critical review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 148, 27–66. doi: 10.1037/bul0000351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daks, J. S., Peltz, J. S., and Rogge, R. D. (2020). Psychological flexibility and inflexibility as sources of resiliency and risk during a pandemic: modeling the cascade of COVID-19 stress on family systems with a contextual behavioral science lens. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 18, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daks, J. S., and Rogge, R. D. (2020). Examining the correlates of psychological flexibility in romantic relationship and family dynamics: a meta-analysis. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 18, 214–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. C., Zautra, A. J., and Smith, B. W. (2004). Chronic pain, stress, and the dynamics of affective differentiation. J. Pers. 72, 1133–1160. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00293.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, D., and Zheng, M. (2022). Associations between six core processes of psychological flexibility and functioning for chronic pain patients: a three-level meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 13, 893150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.893150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dionne, F., Ngo, T.-L., and Blais, M.-C. (2013). The psychological flexibility model: a new approach to mental health. Sante Ment. Que. 38, 111–130. doi: 10.7202/1023992ar

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubey, N., Podder, P., and Pandey, D. (2020). Knowledge of COVID-19 and its influence on mindfulness, cognitive emotion regulation and psychological flexibility in the Indian community. Front. Psychol. 11, 589365. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Dudek, J. E., Białaszek, W., and Ostaszewski, P. (2016). Quality of life in women with lipoedema: a contextual behavioral approach. Qual. Life Res. 25, 401–408. doi: 10.1007/s11136-015-1080-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteve, R., Marcos, E., Reyes-Pérez, Á., López-Martínez, A. E., and Ramírez-Maestre, C. (2021). Pain acceptance creates an emotional context that protects against the misuse of prescription opioids: a study in a sample of patients with chronic noncancer pain. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 3054. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18063054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, S., and Ding, D. (2020). The efficacy of group-based acceptance and commitment therapy on psychological capital and school engagement: a pilot study among Chinese adolescents. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 16, 134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Fish, R. A., Hogan, M. J., Morrison, T. G., Stewart, I., and McGuire, B. E. (2013). Willing and able: a closer look at pain Willingness and Activity Engagement on the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ-8). J. Pain 14, 233–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, A. W., Dawson, D. L., and Golijani-Moghaddam, N. (2016). The development and validation of the Comprehensive assessment of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy processes (CompACT). J. Context. Behav. Sci. 5, 134–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Gandolfi, M., Donisi, V., Battista, S., Picelli, A., Valè, N., Del Piccolo, L., et al. (2021). Health-related quality of life and psychological features in post-stroke patients with chronic pain: a cross-sectional study in the neuro-rehabilitation context of care. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 3089. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18063089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, E. V., and Golijani-Moghaddam, N. (2021). Relationship between psychological flexibility and work-related quality of life for healthcare professionals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 21, 98–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2021.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Gentili, C., Rickardsson, J., Zetterqvist, V., Simons, L. E., Lekander, M., and Wicksell, R. K. (2019). Psychological flexibility as a resilience factor in individuals with chronic pain. Front. Psychol. 10, 2016. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilpin, H. R., Stahl, D. R., and McCracken, L. M. (2019). A theoretically guided approach to identifying predictors of treatment outcome in Contextual Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for chronic pain. Eur. J. Pain 23, 354–366. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Goldbart, A., Bodner, E., and Shrira, A. (2020). The role of emotion covariation and psychological flexibility in coping with chronic physical pain: an integrative model. Psychol. Health 2020, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2020.1841766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* González-Fernández, S., Fernández-Rodríguez, C., Mota-Alonso, M. J., García-Teijido, P., Pedrosa, I., and Pérez-Álvarez, M. (2017). Emotional state and psychological flexibility in breast cancer survivors. Eur. J. Oncol. Nurs. 30, 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2017.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., and Ebert, D. D. (2021). Doing Meta-Analysis with R: A Hands-On Guide. New York, NY: Chapman and Hall/CRC. doi: 10.1201/9781003107347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S., Hogan, M., Dowd, H., Doherty, E., O'Higgins, S., Nic Gabhainn, S., et al. (2014). Comparing the clinical-effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of an internet-delivered Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) intervention with a waiting list control among adults with chronic pain: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 4, e005092. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-005092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., and Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: model, processes and outcomes. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. C., Pistorello, J., and Levin, M. E. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy as a unified model of behavior change. Couns. Psychol. 40, 976–1002. doi: 10.1177/0011000012460836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M., et al. (2022). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.3. Cochrane. Available online at: www.training.cochrane.org/handbook (accessed October 12, 2022).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hoffman, B. M., Papas, R. K., Chatkoff, D. K., and Kerns, R. D. (2007). Meta-analysis of psychological interventions for chronic low back pain. Health Psychol. 26, 1–9. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, L. S., Clark, J., Colclough, J. A., Dale, E., and McMillan, D. (2017). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analyses. Clin. J. Pain 33, 552–568. doi: 10.1097/ajp.0000000000000425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Hulbert-Williams, N. J., and Storey, L. (2016). Psychological flexibility correlates with patient-reported outcomes independent of clinical or sociodemographic characteristics. Support. Care Cancer 24, 2513–2521. doi: 10.1007/s00520-015-3050-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurkovich, G., Mock, C., MacKenzie, E., Burgess, A., Cushing, B., deLateur, B., et al. (1995). The sickness impact profile as a tool to evaluate functional outcome in trauma patients. J. Trauma Acute Care Surg. 39, 625–631. doi: 10.1097/00005373-199510000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., Disabato, D. J., Goodman, F. R., Doorley, J. D., and McKnight, P. E. (2020). Understanding psychological flexibility: a multimethod exploration of pursuing valued goals despite the presence of distress. Psychol. Assess. 32, 829–850. doi: 10.1037/pas0000834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Kato, T., Kadota, M., and Shimoda, S. (2021). Effects of coping flexibility in young women on depressive symptoms during chronic pain. Behav. Med. 47, 185–193. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2019.1708250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., Conijn, J. M., Gallagher, R. A. V., Reshetnikova, A. S., Heij, M., and Lutz, M. C. (2021). Using progress feedback to improve outcomes and reduce drop-out, treatment duration, and deterioration: a multilevel meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 85, 102002. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Koppert, T. Y., Jacobs, J. W. G., Lumley, M. A., and Geenen, R. (2021). The impact of COVID-19 stress on pain and fatigue in people with and without a central sensitivity syndrome. J. Psychosom. Res. 151, 110655. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2021.110655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Kwok, S. S., Chan, E. C., Chen, P. P., and Lo, B. C. (2016). The “self” in pain: the role of psychological inflexibility in chronic pain adjustment. J. Behav. Med. 39, 908–915. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9750-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, M. E., Hildebrandt, M. J., Lillis, J., and Hayes, S. C. (2012). The impact of treatment components suggested by the psychological flexibility model: a meta-analysis of laboratory-based component studies. Behav. Ther. 43, 741–756. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2012.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Lewson, A. B., Johns, S. A., Krueger, E., Chinh, K., Kidwell, K. M., and Mosher, C. E. (2021). Symptom experiences in post-treatment cancer survivors: associations with acceptance and commitment therapy constructs. Support. Care Cancer 29, 3487–3495. doi: 10.1007/s00520-020-05854-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linton, S. J., and Shaw, W. S. (2011). Impact of psychological factors in the experience of pain. Phys. Ther. 91, 700–711. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsey, M. W., and Wilson, D. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis. London: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Liu, H., Li, T. W., Liang, L., and Hou, W. K. (2021). Trauma exposure and mental health of prisoners and ex-prisoners: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 89, 102069. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, C. A., Schauenburg, H., and Dinger, U. (2020). Self-criticism and psychotherapy outcome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 75, 101808. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Lv, J., Zhu, L., Wu, X., Yue, H., and Cui, X. (2021). Study on the correlation between postoperative mental flexibility, negative emotions, and quality of life in patients with thyroid cancer. Gland Surg. 10, 2471–2476. doi: 10.21037/gs-21-424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Maathz, P., Flink, I. K., Engman, L., and Ekdahl, J. (2020). Psychological inflexibility as a predictor of sexual functioning among women with vulvovaginal pain: a prospective investigation. Pain Med. 21, 3596–3602. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnaa042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Marcelino, V., Paço, M., Dias, A., Almeida, V., Rocha, J. C., Azevedo, R., et al. (2022). The role of pain inflexibility and acceptance among headache and temporomandibular disorders patients. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 7974. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19137974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L., Barker, E., and Chilcot, J. (2014). Decentering, rumination, cognitive defusion, and psychological flexibility in people with chronic pain. J. Behav. Med. 37, 1215–1225. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9570-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* McCracken, L. M., Gutiérrez-Martínez, O., and Smyth, C. (2013). “Decentering” reflects psychological flexibility in people with chronic pain and correlates with their quality of functioning. Health Psychol. 32, 820–823. doi: 10.1037/a0028093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* McCracken, L. M., and Jones, R. (2012). Treatment for chronic pain for adults in the seventh and eighth decades of life: a preliminary study of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). Pain Med. 13, 860–867. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2012.01407.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L. M., and Morley, S. (2014). The psychological flexibility model: a basis for integration and progress in psychological approaches to chronic pain management. J. Pain 15, 221–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* McCracken, L. M., and Velleman, S. C. (2010). Psychological flexibility in adults with chronic pain: a study of acceptance, mindfulness, and values-based action in primary care. Pain 148, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L. M., and Vowles, K. E. (2008). A prospective analysis of acceptance of pain and values-based action in patients with chronic pain. Health Psychol. 27, 215–220. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* McCracken, L. M., Williams, J. L., and Tang, N. K. (2011). Psychological flexibility may reduce insomnia in persons with chronic pain: a preliminary retrospective study. Pain Med. 12, 904–912. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2011.01115.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* McCracken, L. M., and Zhao-O'Brien, J. (2010). General psychological acceptance and chronic pain: there is more to accept than the pain itself. Eur. J. Pain 14, 170–175. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Nagasawa, Y., Shibata, A., Ishii, K., and Oka, K. (2022). Psychological inflexibility and physical disability in older patients with chronic low back pain and knee pain. Pain Manag. 12, 829–835. doi: 10.2217/pmt-2022-0011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narayan, M. C. (2010). Culture's effects on pain assessment and management. Am. J. Nurs. 110, 38–47. doi: 10.1097/01.naj.0000370157.33223.6d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigol, S. H., and Di Benedetto, M. (2020). The relationship between mindfulness facets, depression, pain severity and pain interference. Psychol. Health Med. 25, 53–63. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2019.1619786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* and Novakov, I. (2021). Emotional state, fatigue, functional status and quality of life in breast cancer: exploring the moderating role of psychological inflexibility. Psychol. Health Med. 26, 877–886. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2020.1842896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 134, 178–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrini, L., and Arendt-Nielsen, L. (2020). Understanding pain catastrophizing: putting pieces together. Front. Psychol. 11, 603420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg,0.2020.603420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed October 12, 2022).

Google Scholar

* Rhodes, A., Marks, D., Block-Lerner, J., and Lomauro, T. (2021). Psychological flexibility, pain characteristics and risk of opioid misuse in noncancerous chronic pain patients. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 28, 405–417. doi: 10.1007/s10880-020-09729-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolffs, J. L., Rogge, R. D., and Wilson, K. G. (2018). Disentangling components of flexibility via the hexaflex model: development and validation of the multidimensional psychological flexibility inventory (MPFI). Assessment 25, 458–482. doi: 10.1177/1073191116645905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutze, R., Rees, C., Smith, A., Slater, H., Campbell, J. M., and O'Sullivan, P. (2018). How can we best reduce pain catastrophizing in adults with chronic noncancer pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Pain 19, 233–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Scott, W., Daly, A., Yu, L., and McCracken, L. M. (2017). Treatment of chronic pain for adults 65 and over: analyses of outcomes and changes in psychological flexibility following interdisciplinary acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). Pain Med. 18, 252–264. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnw073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Sevier-Guy, L. J., Ferreira, N., Somerville, C., and Gillanders, D. (2021). Psychological flexibility and fear of recurrence in prostate cancer. Eur. J. Cancer Care (Engl) 2021, e13483. doi: 10.1111/ecc.13483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Si, H., Wang, C., Jin, Y., Tian, X., Qiao, X., Liu, N., et al. (2019). Prevalence, factors, and health impacts of chronic pain among community-dwelling older adults in China. Pain Manag. Nurs. 20, 365–372. doi: 10.1016/j.pmn.2019.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, P. A., Mars, T., and Esteves, J. E. (2017). A systematic review of randomised controlled trials using acceptance and commitment therapy as an intervention in the management of non-malignant, chronic pain in adults. Int. J. Osteopathic Med. 24, 18–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijosm.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stockton, D., Kellett, S., Berrios, R., Sirois, F., Wilkinson, N., and Miles, G. (2019). Identifying the underlying mechanisms of change during acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT): a systematic review of contemporary mediation studies. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 47, 332–362. doi: 10.1017/S1352465818000553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thong, I. S. K., Tan, G., Lee, T. Y. C., and Jensen, M. P. (2017). A comparison of pain beliefs and coping strategies and their association with chronic pain adjustment between Singapore and United States. Pain Med. 18, 1668–1678. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnw237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trindade, I. A., Guiomar, R., Carvalho, S. A., Duarte, J., Lapa, T., Menezes, P., et al. (2021). Efficacy of online-based acceptance and commitment therapy for chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Pain 22, 1328–1342. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2021.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trompetter, H. R., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Fox, J.-P., and Schreurs, K. M. G. (2015). Psychological flexibility and catastrophizing as associated change mechanisms during online Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for chronic pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 74, 50–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Trompetter, H. R., Bohlmeijer, E. T., van Baalen, B., Kleen, M., Köke, A., Reneman, M., et al. (2014). The Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS): exploration of psychometric properties in a heterogeneous chronic pain sample. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 30, 289–295. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turk, D. C., and Burwinkle, T. M. (2005). Clinical outcomes, cost-effectiveness, and the role of psychology in treatments for chronic pain sufferers. Professional Psychol. Res. Pract. 36, 602–610. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.36.6.602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Vallejo, M. A., Vallejo-Slocker, L., Offenbaecher, M., Hirsch, J. K., Toussaint, L. L., Kohls, N., et al. (2021). Psychological Flexibility Is Key for Reducing the Severity and Impact of Fibromyalgia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 7300. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18147300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varallo, G., Scarpina, F., Giusti, E. M., Suso-Ribera, C., Cattivelli, R., Guerrini Usubini, A., et al. (2021). The role of pain catastrophizing and pain acceptance in performance-based and self-reported physical functioning in individuals with fibromyalgia and obesity. J. Personal. Med. 11, 810. doi: 10.3390/jpm11080810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varallo, G., Suso-Ribera, C., Ghiggia, A., Veneruso, M., Cattivelli, R., Guerrini Usubini, A., et al. (2022). Catastrophizing, kinesiophobia, and acceptance as mediators of the relationship between perceived pain severity, self-reported and performance-based physical function in women with fibromyalgia and obesity. J. Pain Res. 15, 3017–3029. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S370718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Wicksell, R. K., Lekander, M., Sorjonen, K., and Olsson, G. L. (2010). The Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS)—statistical properties and model fit of an instrument to assess change processes in pain related disability. Eur. J. Pain 14, 771.e771–e714. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Wicksell, R. K., Renöfält, J., Olsson, G. L., Bond, F. W., and Melin, L. (2008). Avoidance and cognitive fusion–central components in pain related disability? Development and preliminary validation of the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS). Eur J Pain 12, 491–500. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Yang, S. Y., McCracken, L. M., and Moss-Morris, R. (2017). Psychological treatment needs for chronic pain in Singapore and the relevance of the psychological flexibility model. Pain Med. 18, 1679–1694. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnw175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yotnuengnit, P., Boonyapaisancharoen, P., Jamin, N., Schulte-Goecking, H., Azad, S.-C., Kraft, E., et al. (2022). Differences between chronic low back pain patients in Germany and Thailand in terms of nature of disease and socioeconomic and psychological factors. J. Back Musculoskelet. Rehabil. 35, 625–632. doi: 10.3233/BMR-210029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Zvolensky, M. J., Smit, T., Rogers, A. H., Matoska, C., Garey, L., Viana, A. G., et al. (2022). Exploring pain experience and anxiety sensitivity among Latinx adults in a federally qualified health center. J. Behav. Med. 45, 404–415. doi: 10.1007/s10865-022-00325-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Studies included in the meta-analysis.

Keywords: meta-analysis, psychological flexibility, psychological inflexibility, chronic pain, psychological distress, pain intensity, acceptance commitment therapy (ACT)

Citation: Fang S and Ding D (2022) Which outcome variables are associated with psychological inflexibility/flexibility for chronic pain patients? A three level meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:1069748. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1069748

Received: 14 October 2022; Accepted: 21 November 2022;
Published: 06 December 2022.

Edited by:

Andrea Poli, Università degli Studi di Pisa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Michael Finn, Helen DeVos Children's Hospital, United States
Giorgia Varallo, University of Parma, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Fang and Ding. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shuanghu Fang, fsh9075@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.