- 1School of Business, Maynooth University, Maynooth, Ireland
- 2Palacky University, Olomouc, Czechia
- 3School of Fine Arts, Aristotle University, Thessaloniki, Greece
Autonomy is a key characteristic of attachment relations that varies as a function of attachment orientations and is also a key personality characteristic of leadership perceptions. In the presented research, we reasoned that the relationship between attachment and autonomy-related preference for specific leaders and leadership behavior would be a function of individuals’ insecure attachment strategies. We tested our hypotheses in two studies. Study 1 used Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) modeling to test expectations based on a cross-sectional design, while Study 2 utilized a vignette-based experimental design. We find that anxious individuals attributed less positive evaluations to an autonomous leadership style (Study 1), while avoidant persons attributed higher leader competence to an autonomous leader description (Study 2). Compared to less anxious participants, highly anxious participants attributed lower competence to the autonomous leader description. By examining how individual differences in attachment orientations can indirectly influence the ideal leader categorization process, the present set of studies lends support to the importance of attachment orientations and related working models in leader perception and contribute to the literature on leader-follower fit. Using a survey and experimental approach, we examine how followers’ attachment schemas can shape the leader influence process, specifically concerning a preference for an autonomous leadership style.
Introduction
Individuals form mental representations of desired leadership attributes and behavior they would like to see in their leaders which form the foundation of implicit leadership theories (ILTs; Cronshaw and Lord, 1987). Largely unconsciously held, ILTs allow individuals to distinguish leaders (those that fit perceivers’ ILTs) from non-leaders (those that do not fit perceivers’ ILTs). Indeed, one of ILTs’ key functions is the easing of cognitive load by automatically and effortlessly matching perceived leader traits to the already held ideal ILTs, which, in turn, shape both preferred and non-preferred leader attributions (Schyns and Schilling, 2010). Yet, less is known about individual-level variation in followers’ leader preferences and consequent leader-follower fit (Junker and van Dick, 2014).
The present set of studies examined how individual differences in followers’ attachment orientations, varying on the avoidance and anxiety dimensions, indirectly influence preferred leader characteristics. In particular, we focus on a preference for and attributions, such as perceived competence, to leaders who exhibit an autonomous or a less autonomous leadership style. Autonomous leadership constitutes one of several attributes of an ideal leader prototype (House et al., 2004) and adult attachment orientations are particularly suited to account for followers’ idealized and preferred leadership mental images (Gruda and Kafetsios, 2020). We propose that perceptions of leaders who exhibit higher autonomy characteristics (e.g., independence) are associated with followers’ avoidant and anxious attachment orientations in theoretically meaningful ways. In doing so, the present set of studies add to the understanding of socio-cognitive processes relatied to followers’ attachment orientations (Harms, 2011) and, consequently, leader-follower fit (Epitropaki et al., 2013).
Preferred Leaders and Attachment Orientations
Whenever followers engage in leader identification decisions, they implicitly compare perceived leaders to the mental image of an ideal leader (Shondrick et al., 2010; Foti et al., 2012). A good match between the perceived leader and followers’ cognitively represented ideal image of how a leader should be and should not be like will result in more favorable “reactions toward [and interactions with] the leader” (Foti et al., 2012: 1). There are different types of leader schema, including ideal, typical, effective, political (Epitropaki et al., 2013). As noted by Junker and van Dick (2014, p. 3) “ideal leader prototypes differ substantially from typical [leader] prototypes” in that “ideal prototypes can be quite extreme and be more on the periphery of the [leader] category”. Put differently, ideal leadership behavior does not equate to actual (or typical) leader behavior; rather, ideal leadership is based on followers’ desired expectations of how leaders should or should not be like. However, this is not to say that ideal leader prototypes are somehow less important than typical leader prototypes in leader-follower interactions.
An ideal leader schema does not only include characteristics associated with an ideal leader, but also characteristics that an ideal leader should not possess (i.e., characteristics of non-leaders). In support of this distinction is evidence that effective leadership does not equate to favorable leadership, in that leader attributes can be categorized both into effective and unfavorable or vice versa (Schyns et al., 2011). Therefore, attributes that describe typical leaders do not necessarily describe, or might even be irrelevant to, ideal leaders (Junker and van Dick, 2014). Hence, ideal leader prototypes and the respective ideal leader categorization process are vital in most leader-follower interactions. The studies presented in this paper specifically focus on the ideal leader prototype and preferred leadership behaviors.1
Ideal leader schemas are closely associated with self-schemas. Previous research has shown that ideal leader prototypes are established in a self-serving manner (e.g., Dunning and Hayes, 1996). Specifically, individuals are likely to include characteristics that are similar to themselves in their ideal leader prototype. That is, individuals are likely to merge their self-concept with the leader schema to produce an ideal leader prototype. For example, outgoing and social individuals are more likely to look for sociability in their ideal leaders than individuals who do not consider themselves outgoing. Likewise, concerning negative characteristics, followers are likely to de-emphasize their own negative characteristics in the definition of their ideal leaders (Foti et al., 2012). It is this self-perception factor in the ideal leader categorization process that provides a well-founded link to attachment theory and adult attachment orientations.
Attachment working models involve self and other schemas, and the aggregation of memories of significant others across relationships, that lead to forming impressions and expectations of others in particular roles such as leadership roles (Kafetsios and Gruda, 2018; Gruda and Kafetsios, 2020). An avoidant working model involves a negative view of and distance from others, with an emphasis and focus on the self, while an anxious attachment working model involves a more positive view of others compared to the self. This positive view of others typically leads anxiously attached individuals to experience a strong need for intimacy and increased proximity and closeness to important others (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007).
Several studies have demonstrated that followers’ attachment orientations are associated with followers’ leadership perceptions and leader prototypes. For example, Keller (2003) proposed that follower attachment orientations influence idealized leadership mental images, as these images mirror descriptions of parental traits. Similarly, Hansbrough (2012), found that anxious followers are biased in evaluating their leaders as more transformational, in line with their dominant attachment orientation. More recently, Gruda and Kafetsios (2020) have shown that attachment orientations predict the transfer of leadership expectations. These studies highlight the nature of leader-follower perceptions and leadership categorization processes (Junker and van Dick, 2014). In the present paper, we focused on a particular characteristic of idealized leadership, namely, autonomous leadership.
Attachment Orientations and Autonomous Leadership
Autonomy is a seminal aspect of relationships and is closely linked to attachment orientations and the corresponding working models of self and others. Attachment and relations theorists stress the significance of autonomy in child-caregiver interactions. Adolescents and children with secure attachment orientations are involved in interactions with parents characterized by healthy autonomy as reflected in cognitive and emotional indicators of those interactions (Allen et al., 2002; McElhaney et al., 2009). And while avoidant adolescents exhibit low levels of relatedness in interactions with their parents, anxious attachment is associated with over-engaging parenting (Allen and Hauser, 1996). The anxious–ambivalent parenting style also is characterized as ‘intrusive parenting’ (Rholes et al., 1995). These interactions with an authority figure shape expectations (working models) of autonomous leadership and interactions with leaders in later life, depending on which attachment orientation is the most dominant.
Regarding ideal leadership, autonomy is a “leadership dimension that refers to independent and individualistic leadership attributes” (House et al., 2004, p 14). An autonomous leader is characterized by “a high degree of independence from superiors and a high degree of social distance from subordinates, a tendency to be aloof and to work alone” (House et al., 2004, p. 7). An autonomous leader may also display self-governing behavior, for example, acting separately from others (Dorfman et al., 2012, p. 508). Autonomy also constitutes a key leadership characteristic to do with self-determination (Chiniara and Bentein, 2016).
Avoidant Attachment Working Models
Avoidant attachment is characterized by an emphasis on autonomy and self-reliance, a reluctance to trust others, and a relatively low tolerance for interpersonal intimacy and interdependence (Popper and Amit, 2009). Others are also often devalued to inflate avoidant individuals’ own capabilities and self-worth (Rom and Mikulincer, 2003). As Richards and Hackett (2012, p. 689) point out: “followers high in attachment avoidance will behave in ways aimed at verifying their self-concept of a socially distant ‘lone wolf.” Hence, we hypothesize that characteristics such as self-reliance and autonomy are part of avoidant individuals’ ideal leader prototype, resulting in a higher preference for such a leadership style:
H1: Higher avoidant attachment orientation is associated with a higher preference for and higher positive attributions to an autonomous leadership style.
Anxious Attachment Working Models
Anxiously attached individuals prefer both physical and emotional closeness and with others and begin to resort to increased proximity and support-seeking behavior, to overcome their constant worry over abandonment and fear of loneliness (Popper and Amit, 2009). Anxious individuals also increasingly look to others for help and safe-haven, oftentimes inflating others’ abilities and capabilities in line with a positive model of others (Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991). Hence, in terms of ideal leadership, anxious individuals likely envision and are drawn to leaders who strengthen them as followers and who constitute a possible safe-haven (Rom and Mikulincer, 2003). Hence, we argue that characteristics such as self-reliance and autonomy do not form part of anxiously individuals’ ideal leader prototype, resulting in a lower preference for such a leadership style:
H2: Higher anxious attachment orientation is associated with a lower preference for and less positive attributions to an autonomous leadership style.
Overview of Studies
Study 1 provided a first examination of the preference for and positive attributions to an autonomous leadership style using the ideal leader prototype attribute list by House et al. (2004). Study 2 experimentally tested whether attachment orientations shape competence attributions to an autonomous or less autonomous leadership style thus revealing a preference for such a leadership style. In both studies we adopted a two-dimensional approach to measuring followers’ attachment orientations (Fraley et al., 2015), including the interaction between avoidance and anxiety (see Gruda and Kafetsios, 2020; Gruda and Kafetsios, 2021).
Study 1
Sample and Procedure
The first study followed a two-wave design, with 2 weeks between each wave. We collected a sample of 298 U.S. participant responses on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) platform via the CloudResearch dashboard. To lower the likelihood of MTurk workers to click through surveys randomly, we included: (a) various pre-tested attention check questions including both direct and bogus items according to best practices (DeSimone et al., 2015), (b) time screens to prevent participants from answering items too quickly (Wood et al., 2017), and (c) included only participants who had a high approval on the MTurk platform.
In the first wave, we collected responses on individual differences, including attachment orientations, personality, and demographic questions. Most participants indicated whether their most recent job position was that of an employee (59.7%) or middle management (15.44%). Female participants accounted for 54.7% of the sample (134 males; 1 gender unknown). The average age of participants was 42.59 years (SD = 11.99), with an average work experience of 17.69 years (SD = 11.98).
In the second wave, participants were shown and indicated their agreement to the complete ideal leader prototype attribute list (House et al., 2004), to allow participants to social-cognitively contrast items against each other (Stapel and Suls, 2007) and to ensure that participants’ leader prototype is activated. All items are shown in random order to each participant, in line with House et al. (2004).
Measures
Autonomous Leader Ideal Prototype
The ideal leader prototype scale instructs the participant to rate several “… behaviors and characteristics that can be used to describe leaders […] accompanied by a short definition to clarify its meaning.” Items were rated using a scale ranging from: (p. 1) “Greatly inhibits a person from being an outstanding leader” to (p. 7) “Contributes greatly to a person being an outstanding leader.”
The latent factor “autonomy” was formed comprising four behavioral trait items describing an autonomous leadership style: “autonomous” (description: acts independently, does not rely on others), “independent” (does not rely on others; self-governing), “individualistic” (behaves in a different manner than peers), “unique” (an unusual person; has characteristics of behaviors that are different from most others). The Cronbach alpha for autonomy (α = 0.60) equals the reliability factor in the original GLOBE study (α = 0.59; House et al., 2004).
Attachment Orientations
Attachment orientations were assessed using the adapted Experiences in Close Relationships scale (ECR) by Richards and Schat (2011). The adapted ECR scale replaced references to romantic partners in the items with “other people” or “others” and is used in more general contexts, such as organizational settings. The ECR consists of 36 items on two 18-item subscales measuring anxious (α = 0.96, M = 2.98, SD = 1.36) and avoidant attachment (α = 0.96; M = 3.88, SD = 1.4). Participants rated their agreement for each subscale on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Big Five Personality
Given some overlap between attachment and Big Five personality dimensions (Noftle and Shaver, 2006), we account for these traits as well using the Mini IPIP scale (Goldberg et al., 2006). Openness to experience (α = 0.81), conscientiousness (α = 0.81), extraversion (α = 0.87), agreeableness (α = 0.85) and neuroticism (α = 0.81) were measured with ten items each, rated on a 5-point scale ranging from (1) “very inaccurate” to (5) “very accurate.”
Demographics included age, gender, and job position.
Results
An overview of descriptive statistics and pairwise correlations are reported in Table 1.
Method of Statistical Analysis
Keeping with previous studies using the GLOBE scale, we treated autonomy as a latent variable and computed a one-factor model with all autonomous leadership items loading onto a single latent factor (see House et al., 2004). The chi-square (χ2) statistic, the comparative fit index (CFI), the non-normed fit index (NNFI), and the root-mean-square error of approximation were used to assess model fit (RMSE; Bentler and Bonett, 1980, Bentler, 1990). A one-factor model without predictors was fitted, adding a direct path between the variables unique and individualistic, as they are conceptually close. This model provided the best fit (χ2 = 0.95, p > 0.33; CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.01; NNFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.00).
We specified a Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) model (Jöreskog and Goldberger, 1975; Muthén, 1989) due to two main reasons. Firstly, MIMIC models allow the testing of direct and indirect effects of attachment orientations on autonomy as a latent variable and the dimension’s individual items (i.e., unique, individualized, independent, and autonomous), respectively. Secondly, the MIMIC approach allows us to assess the impact of interactions using continuous predictors, such as anxious and avoidant attachment, on a latent variable within a structural equation model (Woods et al., 2009). A decent fit for the structure to the data was found with regard to autonomy and predictors (χ2 = 17.14, p > 0.10; CFI = 0.96; SRMR = 0.04; NNFI = 0.93; RMSEA = 0.04, see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Maximum-likelihood parameter estimates for the hypothesized model without controls (Study 1). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; n = 297; standardized coefficients. RMSEA: 0.043, CFI: 0.956, SRMR: 0.04, χ2 (11) = 17.14, p > 0.14.
A significant negative relationship between anxious attachment and preference for an autonomous leadership style was found (b = −0.18, p = 0.01). There were also indirect effects of anxious attachment on the various items of the autonomy dimension (independent: b = −0.15; individualistic: b = −0.07; autonomous: b = −0.18; unique: b = −0.04; all p < 0.05). On the other hand, the relationship between avoidant attachment and autonomous leadership was not found to be significant (b = 0.02, p > 0.10).
Introducing additional controls such as the Big-5 personality dimensions and demographics improved model fit (χ2 = 37.19, RSMEA = 0.02, SRMR = 0.03, CFI = 0.98, NNFI = 0.98; p = 0.37). To better understand these findings, we further examined our hypotheses regarding autonomous leadership using an experimental design in Study 2.
Study 2
Study 2 expanded on findings from Study 1 testing the effects of autonomous and less autonomous ILTs in an experimental design. To assess preference for and positive attributions to an autonomous leadership style based on attachment orientations, we used perceived leader competence as a possible positive attribute. In the vignette-based experimental setting, we examined whether participants’ leader preference and degree of (higher or lower) positive attributions to autonomous leadership vary when evaluating descriptions of a highly autonomous or less autonomous manager.
Sample and Procedure
We recruited 400 U.S. participants via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk to take part in this experiment. Our final sample included 193 female participants (206 males; 1 unanswered), ranging from 20–75 years of age (M = 36.76, SD = 10.80), with an average work experience of 15.59 years.
Participants were provided with an informed consent form and told that they would be asked to complete measures on individual characteristics. These measures consisted of the same measures as in Study 1, namely attachment (Richards and Schat, 2011) and Big Five personality traits (Goldberg et al., 2006). Secondly, participants were randomly assigned to either an autonomous leader condition or a control condition and then asked to evaluate the presented leader description on competence. Finally, a manipulation check was conducted, and participants completed demographic measures. After completion, participants were fully debriefed and thanked for their help.
Vignettes
Our goal was primarily to create two vignettes, identical in sentence structure and word count, examining the variance due to individual attachment orientations differences. Both vignettes counted 147 words and 8 sentences. In both vignettes we used the same introductory section to describe the leader’s back story:
“Mark Smith is Director of Sales for a major appliance firm. Mark assumed his position two years ago following his attainment of an MBA degree with a specialization in marketing. In this position, he has gained the respect of both his subordinates and his superiors. His superiors evaluate him as a capable worker, and his subordinates have indicated that they enjoy working for him. Mark is currently in charge of 12 subordinates.”
The section above was followed by a different story for each leader style condition. For example, the autonomous leader condition included sentences such as “Mark achieves what he sets out and does not rely on others’ help,” while the less autonomous leader condition includes sentences such as “Mark achieves what he sets out and relies strongly on other’s input.”
Pre-test of Vignettes
To ensure that participants perceived each vignette correctly, they were asked to rate each vignette on the four items of autonomous leadership as outlined in Study 1. A subsequent t-test using the aggregated autonomy score showed a significant difference (t = −20.88, p < 0.00) between the autonomous (M = 4.12, SD = 0.79) and less autonomous leadership condition (M = 2.38, SD = 0.87). These results suggest that the manipulations had their intended effects.
Measures
We included the same independent variable measures as in Study 1, namely attachment and Big Five personality traits. We also included age and gender as controls. Perceived leader competence was measured using one item, with answer choices ranging from (1) “not at all” to (5) “extremely.”
Results
Correlations and reliability alphas are reported in Table 2. Using ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions with a heteroscedastic-robust estimate of the variance, we regressed the continuous variable – reflecting the degree of competence attributed to the described leader – on the manipulated variable leader condition, as well as anxious and avoidant attachment and their interaction (Table 3).
Avoidant attachment significantly and negatively related to the perception of leader competence (b = −0.27, p < 0.001) as a main effect. Moreover, in the high autonomous leader condition, avoidant attachment was related significantly and positively to perceived leader competence. To gain a better understanding of this interaction, we conducted a simple slopes test (see Figure 2), using the full range of the moderator to plot our results (Bauer and Curran, 2005).

Figure 2. Regression of the interaction of avoidance attachment and autonomous leadership on perceived leader competence conditions (Study 2).
In line with H2, regarding high avoidant attachment, there was a significant increase in the ratings of perceived leader competence between (high/low) autonomous leadership conditions (simple slope = 1.049, t = 2.074, p = 0.039). In low avoidant attachment, the difference between (high/low) autonomous leadership conditions was not significant (simple slope = −0.57, t = −1.8, p = −0.073).
On the other hand, anxious attachment significantly predicted overall leader competence perceptions (b = −0.37, p < 0.001). However, the interaction between anxious attachment and the autonomous leader conditions was not significant (p > 0.10). The interaction of anxious and avoidant attachment showed a significant effect with regard to general attributions of leader competence (b = 0.08, p < 0.001) in the less autonomous leader condition.
Discussion
Attachment theory in organizational settings has witnessed an intense research interest in recent years (Harms, 2011; Yip et al., 2018). Yet, a focus on the socio-cognitive processes that drive leader-follower interaction has been scarce. The present studies examined how followers’ avoidant and anxious attachment orientations can shape perceptions of autonomy in leader-follower interaction. Autonomy is a key characteristic of attachment dynamics that varies as a function of attachment orientations (Allen et al., 2002; McElhaney et al., 2009) and also a key personality characteristic of leadership perceptions across cultures (House et al., 2004). We reasoned that since attachment orientations involve working models (self-other perception schemas) based on interactions with autonomous or less autonomous leaders/caregivers, attachment orientations would guide individuals’ preference for a leadership style and related behaviors higher or lower in autonomy. Hence, the relationship between attachment and preference for specific leaders and leadership behavior would be a function of individuals’ sense of security or insecurity (Gillath and Hart, 2010).
Study 1 found that individuals with an anxious attachment orientation tend to hold a lower preference for an autonomous leadership style. Individuals who score higher on anxious attachment likely exhibit a higher preference for leaders who display positive approach-related behavior, that is, a leader who is less likely to be autonomous and independent and likely to be more consultative, collaborative, and team-oriented. By perceiving others as more capable, anxious individuals effectively may fulfill their own attachment needs and therefore increase proximity and dependence on others.
On the other hand, avoidant participants responded better to an autonomous leadership style. For example, using a vignette-based experimental design, Study 2 allowed to establish causal links between the activation of leadership schemas high or lower in autonomy and leader evaluations for persons differing in attachment orientations. Indeed, individuals who scored high on avoidant attachment had higher positive attributions and a higher preference for an autonomous leadership style (H1). In the presented case, these individuals attributed higher perceived leader competence to the highly autonomous leader description, and lower perceived competence to the less autonomous leader description. Confrontation with a description of a less autonomous leader seems to have activated avoidant individuals’ affiliation-related thoughts and behavioral strategies, and have led them to judge this fictional leader as much less competent than the highly autonomous leader who better fitted their own ideal leader expectations. This lends support to the importance of attachment orientations and related working models in leader perception (Keller, 1999; Hansbrough, 2012). Avoidant individuals are self-reliant and autonomous and have generally negative views of others, based on previous consistent socialization experiences that distance one-selves from other attachment figures (Collins and Feeney, 2004). Hence, avoidant persons seem to have developed implicit leadership schemas that tend to favor self-reliant, autonomous, and self-governing leaders. Such leaders would provide a closer match and a better leader-follower fit to avoidant individuals’ ideal leader prototype.
In Study 2, we also found a significant interaction effect between anxious and avoidant attachment. Such an interaction effect is usually considered equivalent to higher fearful attachment orientation, an attachment orientation also characterized by a higher degree of anxious attachment (Brennan et al., 1998). Since fearful attachment involves a higher level of anxiety, this could be taken as evidence partly supporting our expectations regarding anxious followers’ ILTs.
These key findings add to the discussion of leader identity and how leader identity is shaped as a result of leader-follower fit (DeRue and Ashford, 2010; Junker and van Dick, 2014). Work on leader identity has identified followers’ ILTs (DeRue and Ashford, 2010) critically influence the granting (and claiming) of leader identity to others who match followers’ ILTs. Self-identity marks another important antecedent in the evaluation of leader-follower relationships. Specifically, Jackson and Johnson (2012) attest that follower self-identity “may moderate the effects of […] leadership” (p. 488) so that some followers will respond more favorably to certain leadership styles than others, depending on relational leader-follower fit. Hence, attachment orientations could provide additional, individual-level insights in the formation and granting of leader identity under the assumption of leader-follower fit. By examining how individual differences in attachment orientations can indirectly influence the ideal leader categorization process, the present set of studies contribute to the literature on leader-follower fit and respond to calls for more research on this topic (Shondrick et al., 2010; Epitropaki et al., 2013).
Finally, the variation of results between studies (a trend in support of H1 in Study 1 and H2 in Study 2), could be due to either the non-static character of leadership cognitions overall (Lord et al., 2001) or organizational or contextual factors that may interact with participants’ attachment orientations in determining their implicit leadership schemas. Future research could examine contextual processes that make shifting to different modes of attachment-related implicit leadership schemas possible.
The results have some key practical applications and theoretical implications. Leader-follower fit is a key antecedent to work performance (Martin et al., 2016). Given the increasing multicultural character of work teams and autonomy being a key facet of leader schemas cross-culturally (House et al., 2004), examining leader-follower fit in terms of attachment related ILTs can increase accuracy in predicting group cohesiveness and effectiveness in international working teams. On the theory level, there is an increasing awareness on the interplay between insecure attachment and cultural orientations especially around the independent/interdependent cultural dimension (Kafetsios and Gruda, 2018; Kafetsios, 2021). This evidence underline the need to extend our understanding of how followers’ attachment orientations interact with leader autonomy traits (e.g., independent self-construal) with respect to relational and team outcomes at work. Therefore further cross-cultural research on this topc is distinctly needed.
Limitations and Future Research
This study is not without limitations. First, in both studies we relied on an online participant pool provided by Amazon MTurk. However, previous studies have shown that the MTurk population is similar to traditional participant pools (Paolacci and Chandler, 2014). Likewise, although the degree of control in an online environment is lower than traditional experiments conducted in the laboratory, the inclusion of various stated attention checks and robustness checks as described in the paper, provides us with confidence that the results are likely similar to expected results form a traditional participant pool completing surveys and the respective experiment in a laboratory setting.
Second, in Study 1 we found an association between attachment and the latent factor “Autonomy.” It is important to keep in mind that this factor originally was considered in the GLOBE study, a cross-cultural study of leadership. Hence, we acknowledge that the observed effects might present differently across cultures due to the association between attachment schemas and national cultures. As mentioned above, we encourage future research to examine in greater detail potential cross-cultural differences in the preference for autonomous leadership styles.
Conclusion
This study addresses a previously understudied link between followers’ attachment orientations and autonomy-related ILTs. Using a diverse set of methods, our findings suggest that followers’ attachment schemas can shape the leader influence process depending on followers’ attachment-related leader perceptions and the degree of autonomy exhibited by the respective leader.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by EMLYON Business School. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
DG: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing-original draft preparation. KK: investigation, writing-reviewing, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
1. ^Throughout this paper, we are solely concerned with ideal (or preferred) leader prototypes, not typical leader prototypes.
References
Allen, J. P., and Hauser, S. T. (1996). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescent-family interactions as predictors of young adults’ states of mind regarding attachment. Dev. Psychopathol. 8, 793–809. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007434
Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., O’Connor, T. G., and Bell, K. L. (2002). Prediction of peer-rated adult hostility from autonomy struggles in adolescent–family interactions. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 123–137. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402001074
Bartholomew, K., and Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a four-category model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 226–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226
Bauer, D. J., and Curran, P. J. (2005). Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: inferential and graphical techniques. Multivar. Behav. Res. 40, 373–400. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4003_5
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol. Bull. 88, 588–606. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., and Shaver, P. R. (1998). “Self-Report Measurement of Adult Attachment,” Attachment Theory and Close Relationships, 46–76.
Chiniara, M., and Bentein, K. (2016). Linking servant leadership to individual performance: differentiating the mediating role of autonomy, competence and relatedness need satisfaction. Leadersh. Q. 27, 124–141. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.08.004
Collins, N. L., and Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment shape perceptions of social support: evidence from experimental and observational studies. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 363–383. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.363
Cronshaw, S. F., and Lord, R. G. (1987). Effects of categorization, attribution, and encoding processes on Leadership perceptions. J. Appl. Psychol. 72, 97–106. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.72.1.97
DeRue, D. S., and Ashford, S. J. (2010). Who will lead and who will follow? A social process of leadership identity construction in organizations. Acad. Manag. Rev. 35, 627–647. doi: 10.5465/amr.35.4.zok627
DeSimone, J. A., Harms, P. D., and DeSimone, A. J. (2015). Best practice recommendations for data screening. J. Organ. Behav. 36, 171–181. doi: 10.1002/job.1962
Dorfman, P., Javidan, M., Hanges, P., Dastmalchian, A., and House, R. (2012). GLOBE: A twenty year journey into the intriguing world of culture and leadership. J. World Bus. 47, 504–518. doi: 10.1016/j.jwb.2012.01.004
Dunning, D., and Hayes, A. F. (1996). Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 213–229. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.213
Epitropaki, O., Sy, T., Martin, R., Tram-Quon, S., and Topakas, A. (2013). Implicit Leadership and followership theories “in the wild”: taking stock of information-processing approaches to leadership and followership in organizational settings. Leadersh. Q. 24, 858–881. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.10.005
Foti, R. J., Bray, B. C., Thompson, N. J., and Allgood, S. F. (2012). Know thy self, know thy leader: contributions of a pattern-oriented approach to examining leader perceptions. Leadersh. Q. 23, 702–717. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.03.007
Fraley, R. C., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., and Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship-specific attachment orientations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 354–368. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000027
Gillath, O., and Hart, J. (2010). The effects of psychological security and insecurity on political attitudes and leadership preferences. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 122–134. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.614
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., et al. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. J. Res. Pers. 40, 84–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007
Gruda, D., and Kafetsios, K. (2020). Attachment orientations guide the transfer of Leadership judgments: culture matters. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46, 525–546. doi: 10.1177/0146167219865514
Gruda, D., and Kafetsios, K. (2021). I need a doctor, call me a doctor: attachment and the evaluation of general practitioners before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:7914. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18157914
Hansbrough, T. K. (2012). The construction of a transformational leader: follower attachment and Leadership perceptions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 42, 1533–1549. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00913.x
Harms, P. D. (2011). Adult attachment styles in the workplace. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 21, 285–296. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2010.10.006
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., and Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, Leadership, and Organizations: The GLOBE Study of 62 Societies, California: Sage publications.
Jackson, E. M., and Johnson, R. E. (2012). When opposites do (and do not) attract: interplay of leader and follower self-identities and its consequences for leader–member exchange. Leadersh. Q. 23, 488–501. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.12.003
Jöreskog, K. G., and Goldberger, A. S. (1975). Estimation of a model with multiple indicators and multiple causes of a single latent variable. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 70, 631–639.
Junker, N. M., and van Dick, R. (2014). Implicit theories in organizational settings: A systematic review and research agenda of implicit leadership and followership theories. Lead. Q. 25, 1154–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.09.002
Kafetsios, K. (2021). Collective reactions to epidemic threat: attachment and cultural orientations predict early Covid-19 infection and mortality rates and trajectories. Social Psychol. Personal. Sci. 461. doi: 10.1177/19485506211053461
Kafetsios, K. G., and Gruda, D. (2018). Interdependent followers prefer avoidant leaders: followers’ cultural orientation moderates leaders’ avoidance relationships with followers’ work outcomes. Front. Commun. 3:9. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00009
Keller, T. (1999). Images of the familiar: individual differences and implicit leadership theories. Leadersh. Q. 10, 589–607. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(99)00033-8
Keller, T. (2003). Parental images as a guide to leadership sensemaking: an attachment perspective on implicit leadership theories. Leadersh. Q. 14, 141–160. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(03)00007-9
Lord, R. G., Brown, D. J., Harvey, J. L., and Hall, R. J. (2001). Contextual constraints on prototype generation and their multilevel consequences for leadership perceptions. Lead. Q. 12, 311–338. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(01)00081-9
Martin, R., Guillaume, Y., Thomas, G., Lee, A., and Epitropaki, O. (2016). Leader–member exchange (LMX) and performance: a meta-analytic review. Pers. Psychol. 69, 67–121. doi: 10.1111/peps.12100
McElhaney, K., Allen, J., Stephenson, J., Hare, A., Lerner, R., and Steinberg, L. (2009). “Handbook of Adolescent Psychology ”. New Jersey: John Wiley & son.
Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. New York: Guilford Press.
Muthén, B. O. (1989). Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika 54, 557–585. doi: 10.1007/BF02296397
Noftle, E. E., and Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the big five personality traits: associations and comparative ability to predict relationship quality. J. Res. Pers. 40, 179–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.11.003
Paolacci, G., and Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 184–188. doi: 10.1177/0963721414531598
Popper, M., and Amit, K. (2009). Attachment and leader’s development via experiences. Leadersh. Q. 20, 749–763. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.06.005
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., and Blakely, B. S. (1995). Adult attachment styles and mothers’ relationships with their young children. Pers. Relat. 2, 35–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1995.tb00076.x
Richards, D. A., and Hackett, R. D. (2012). Attachment and emotion regulation: compensatory interactions and leader-member exchange. Leadersh. Q. 23, 686–701. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.03.005
Richards, D. A., and Schat, A. (2011). Attachment at (not to) work: applying attachment theory to explain individual behavior in organizations. J. Appl. Psychol. 96, 169–182. doi: 10.1037/a0020372
Rom, E., and Mikulincer, M. (2003). Attachment theory and group processes: The association between attachment style and group-related representations, goals, memories, and functioning. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1220–1235. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1220
Schyns, B., Kiefer, T., Kerschreiter, R., and Tymon, A. (2011). teaching implicit Leadership theories to develop leaders and Leadership: how and why it can make a difference. Acad. Manag. Learn. Edu. 10, 397–408. doi: 10.5465/amle.2010.0015
Schyns, B., and Schilling, J. (2010). Implicit Leadership theories: think leader, think effective? J. Manag. Inq. 20, 141–150. doi: 10.1177/1056492610375989
Shondrick, S. J., Dinh, J. E., and Lord, R. G. (2010). Developments in implicit leadership theory and cognitive science: applications to improving measurement and understanding alternatives to hierarchical leadership. Leadersh. Q. 21, 959–978. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.004
Stapel, D. A., and Suls, J. (2007). “Several answers to four questions: reflections and conclusions,” in Assimilation and Contrast in Social Psychology (New York, NY: psychology press), 313–327.
Wood, D., Harms, P., Lowman, G. H., and DeSimone, J. A. (2017). Response speed and response consistency as mutually validating indicators of data quality in online samples. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 454–464. doi: 10.1177/1948550617703168
Woods, C. M., Oltmanns, T. F., and Turkheimer, E. (2009). Illustration of MIMIC-model DIF testing with the schedule for nonadaptive and adaptive personality. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 31, 320–330. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9118-9
Keywords: leadership, attachment theory, personality, indivdual characteristics, implicit leadership theories
Citation: Gruda D and Kafetsios K (2022) From Ideal to Real: Attachment Orientations Guide Preference for an Autonomous Leadership Style. Front. Psychol. 13:728343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.728343
Edited by:
Carlos Francisco De Sousa Reis, University of Coimbra, PortugalReviewed by:
Mohsen Khosravi, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, IranQueyu Ren, The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom
Wan Khairuzzaman Wan Ismail, Sulaiman Al Rajhi Colleges, Saudi Arabia
Copyright © 2022 Gruda and Kafetsios. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Dritjon Gruda, am9uLmdydWRhQG11Lmll