Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 18 March 2022
Sec. Psychology of Aging

Lower Education and Reading and Writing Habits Are Associated With Poorer Oral Discourse Production in Typical Adults and Older Adults

  • 1Department of Linguistics, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul (PUCRS), Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 2Département de Réadaptation, Faculté de Médecine, Centre Interdisciplinaire de Recherche en Réadaptation et Intégration Sociale (CIRRIS), Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 3Brain Institute of Rio Grande do Sul, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul (PUCRS), Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 4National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq), Brasília, Brazil

During normal aging there is a decline in cognitive functions that includes deficits in oral discourse production. A higher level of education and more frequent reading and writing habits (RWH) might delay the onset of the cognitive decline during aging. This study aimed at investigating the effect of education and RWH on oral discourse production in older adults. Picture-based narratives were collected from 117 healthy adults, aged between 51 and 82 years (68.6 ± 6.38) with 0–20 years of formal education (10.1 ± 5.69). Measures of macro, microlinguistic and modalizations were computed and entered as dependent variables in hierarchical regression analyses that included age, education and RWH as regressors. Results revealed that higher education explained a better performance at the macrostructure and microstructure dimensions. Higher frequency of RWH explained the production of fewer modalizations. These results demonstrate the positive effect of education and RWH in oral discourse production in older adults. Therefore, higher attention should be given to these social factors.

Introduction

Healthy aging involves changes in cognitive functions, including a deficit in oral discourse production (Marini et al., 2005). To generate discourse, the speaker must integrate linguistic and non-linguistic skills to produce and structure a narrative. As a complex task, discourse production is a rich source of information in clinical interviews and cognitive assessment. Thus, discourse production is a valuable tool to support the detection of language and cognitive impairments. Moreover, it is an ecologically valid task, representative of language use in daily life.

During typical aging, discourse comprehension is relatively spared, while discourse production is affected (Ska et al., 2009; Martin et al., 2018). Oral discourse production, the focus of the present study, can be divided into two main dimensions (van Dijk, 2010). The first dimension is the macrostructure. It refers to the semantic information that provides global unity to discourse. The second dimension is the microstructure. It concerns the structure of an individual proposition and its internal relations (van Dijk, 2010). Several studies bring evidence that the ability to deal with the macrostructural dimension of speech decreases more significantly than the microstructural dimension of speech during typical aging. Notably, the oral discourse produced by older adults contains fewer main ideas compared with that produced by younger adults (Glosser and Deser, 1992; Capilouto et al., 2005, 2016; Marini et al., 2005; Wright et al., 2011, 2014). In addition to these two dimensions, meta-discursive strategies, or modalizations, refer to a participant’s comments about the story content or his/her performance during an oral discourse production task (Cardebat et al., 1993; Lira et al., 2018). Studies on modalizations in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have been developed (Duong et al., 2003, 2005; Toledo et al., 2018), but when it comes to typical aging studies are scarce. For instance, le Dorze and Bédard (1998) compared the use of modalizations by young, middle-aged, and older adults while producing a narrative story based on a single picture (Bank Robbery Picture) (Nespoulous et al., 1986). They observed that older adults included more comments about their difficulties during a narrative task as compared to young and middle-aged adults.

Several factors can delay the onset of cognitive decline during aging. These factors can be grouped under the concept of cognitive reserve. Cognitive reserve establishes that activities that stimulate the brain are linked to an increase in brain resilience to changes in cognitive processing resulting from typical and atypical aging (Stern et al., 2020). Education is one of the factors most associated with cognitive reserve. There is consensus that education is a protective factor that delays the emergence of the cognitive aspects of neurological pathologies (Nitrini et al., 2009; Cotrena et al., 2016; Quintas et al., 2017). In picture-based tasks, studies report better performance in highly educated adults compared with less-educated ones (Ardila and Rosselli, 1996; Duong and Ska, 2001). Few studies addressed the effects of education on the oral discourse production of typical adults. These studies provide evidence that the level of education influences the quantity and completeness of the informational content in tasks based on single pictures, as well as the production of main ideas and cohesive links in tasks based on a sequence of pictures (le Dorze and Bédard, 1998; MacKenzie, 2000; Mackenzie et al., 2007). The present study aims to bring some light to the influence of education in discourse macro and microstructure as well as to modalizations in typical aging.

Another factor associated with cognitive reserve is reading and writing habits (RWH). These habits have recently gained a greater focus of attention regarding their role in typical cognitive aging. A recent study indicates that RWH can contribute to cognitive performance even more than education (Cotrena et al., 2016). Moreover, evidence attests to the positive effects of RWH on executive functions, attention, memory (Pawlowski et al., 2012; Moraes et al., 2013; Sörman et al., 2018), as well as language processing (Pagliarin et al., 2015; Kochhann et al., 2018; Tessaro et al., 2020). For example, Moraes et al. (2013) found that RWH better predicted phonemic fluency than education. Kochhann et al. (2018) found that RWH were the second-best predictor, after verbal fluency, of mild cognitive impairment and AD. Cotrena et al. (2016) found that RWH predicted speed and accuracy in the Hayling test (Burgess and Shallice, 1997), a linguistic measure of executive functions. Sörman et al. (2018) found that the habit of reading books was associated with higher levels of verbal fluency and episodic recall. Tessaro et al. (2020) found that RWH were associated with the total number of words produced in the phonemic verbal fluency task. Pagliarin et al. (2015) found that the combination of education and RWHs better predicted the linguistic performance in oral discourse, in measures such as the number of words, information units (IU), and scenes, rather than one of the variables in isolation.

Overall, these studies corroborate the importance of RWH in cognition. However, to the best of our knowledge, no studies have further investigated the effects of RWH on oral discourse production in typical adults. The effect of education is still scarce as well, as mentioned before. Toward this aim, we used the same picture sequence task and linguistic variables investigated by Lira et al. (2018). Lira et al. (2018) identified the items in the macrolinguistic dimension of oral discourse that better differentiated individuals with AD from typical older adults with a picture sequence task. The present study aims to investigate the effect of education and the frequency of RWH on oral discourse production in typical adults. We hypothesize that higher education levels and a higher frequency of RWH would be associated with better performance on the macro and microstructure dimensions of oral discourse production, as well as with fewer modalizations.

Method

Ethical and Data Collection Procedures

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at the university where the study was developed under report number 560.073, CAAE registry number 21006913.0.0000.5336. Participation in the study was voluntary, and participants provided their written informed consent before joining the study. Participants were tested individually in a laboratory setting. The participants were recruited at general courses offered for members of the community at the university and at community centers close to it.

Participants

One hundred and seventeen adults participated in the study (see Table 1 for their sociodemographic and neuropsychological characteristics). Their ages ranged from 51 to 82 (mean = 68.6; standard deviation, SD = 6.38) and education level ranged from 0 to 20 years of formal schooling (mean = 10.1; SD = 5.69). They were recruited at community centers, in an urban context in the most southern state in, Brazil. Participants were mainly blue-collar individuals from low to middle-to-low socioeconomic status (SES; see Table 1). The SES scores of the participants in this study ranged from 13 to 41 (mean = 24.2; SD = 6.05). The scores for RWHs in the present sample ranged from 0 to 26 (mean = 12.2; SD = 6.27). SES was measured with the Questionário de condição social e questionário de uso de medicamentos, taken from ABEP—Associação Brasileira de Empresa e Pesquisa (2015), which establishes the following cut-off points: lower SES = 0–16; middle = 17–28; upper middle = 29–44; upper = 45–100. RWHs were quantified according to the weekly frequency of reading and writing activities of different types of digital and printed texts. There were four questions for reading habits and four questions for writing habits. Each question had a maximum score of four for a maximum total score of 16 points per modality (reading and writing), thus 32 points in total. The possible scores for each question were as follows: daily (4 points); a few days a week (3 points); once a week (2 points); rarely (1 point), and never (0 points). All participants underwent a battery of linguistic tasks as part of a larger study to map age and dementia-related changes in language processing (BALE–Battery for Language Assessment in Aging) (Hübner et al., 2019). To further characterize the sample, phonological short-term memory and working memory were evaluated with the WAIS-III Digit Span and Backward Digit Span subtests (Wechsler, 1997). Semantic and episodic memories were assessed with two subtests of the BALE. The semantic memory task (naming task) consists of 60 black and white drawings, divided into animate and inanimate and high and low-frequency items, presented in groups of four. The episodic memory task (verbal learning task) consists of identifying and naming 16 figures from different semantic categories and comprised three phases (learning, immediate recall, and 20-min recall) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive analyses.

All participants were native Brazilian Portuguese speakers and did not speak other languages. All participants had a general cognitive performance within the normal range as measured by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; mean = 27.4; SD = 2.6) (Laks et al., 2003). We followed the Brazilian scoring procedure provided by Laks et al. (2003). This scoring procedure is adapted for the Brazilian population and takes into consideration age and level of education. Participants did not present with depression, as measured by the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Yesavage et al., 1982), and did not present any functional problems that attested any cognitive decline indicative of dementia, as measured by the Pfeffer Questionnaire (Pfeffer et al., 1982). As self-reported, they did not present with a current or previous history of neurological disorders, nor a current or previous history of substance abuse, or untreated vision and/or hearing problems.

Picture Sequence Task

The picture sequence task is known as “The dog story,” a subtest of the Battery for Language Assessment in Aging (Hübner et al., 2019). Participants are asked to tell the story based on seven scenes, following this instruction: “I will show you a story with scenes. Each scene is a moment in the story, which has a beginning, middle, and end. I will ask you to take a good look at the scenes and try to understand the story. Then, I am going to ask you to tell me this story as if you were going to tell it to a friend. Are you ready? Can we start?” The scenes are presented in the correct sequence and remained in front of the participant so that s/he can observe them while telling the story. Participants are given time to observe the scenes and, if necessary, the instruction is repeated. There is no time limit for the accomplishment of the task. The examiner only interferes to encourage the participant if s/he does not show initiative to produce the story. In this case, expressions such as “tell me more,” “can you continue?,” “Uhum,” “and then, what happens?” are addressed. All discourse samples were audio-recorded for later transcription and data scoring. Two separate transcripts were made by two independent researchers (BLCM and LCH) and checked, and disagreements were discussed in consensus.

Oral Narrative Variables Computation

We used 19 variables. Their explanation can be seen in Table 2. Following Lira et al. (2018), we analyzed one variable for modalizations (Mods), two for macrostructure [macropropositions (Mps) and appropriated global coherence (GCoh)] and 16 for microstructure [content-related complete propositions (CCPs); no-content-related complete propositions (NoCCPs); incomplete propositions (IncPs); cohesive devices (CohDs) and its five subtypes: referential (Ref), lexical (Lex), conjunction (Conj), ellipsis (Eli), or structural (Est); cohesive errors (CohEs) and its five subtypes: no reference (Eref), conjunction error (Econj), information error (Einfo), missing element (Emissing), or inappropriate sentence (Esent); and appropriated local coherence (LCoh)].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Linguistic variables used in the study, based on Lira et al. (2018).

Following Lira et al. (2018), we analyzed CCPs, NoCCPs, IncPs, Mps, and Mods as a proportion of all the propositions. The total of the CohDs and CohEs, as well as their subtypes, represent the ratio of the sum of the words produced. GCoh and LCoh were considered as absolute numbers.

Data Analysis

We analyzed the data using the Tidyverse package (Wickham et al., 2019), implemented in RStudio (R Core Team, 2020).1 First, we examined the data for skewness and kurtosis. The values were within the acceptable respective ranges (−2 to +2 for skewness and −9 to +9 for kurtosis) (Schmider et al., 2010), and thus, no transformations were performed (see Table 1). In our regression models, we entered one dependent variable per construct of interest (macrostructure, microstructure, and modalizations). Since macrostructure and microstructure were composed of several variables, we computed two composite scores, one for microstructure and one for macrostructure. For some variables, a higher score indicates better performance; for other variables, a higher score denotes worse performance. For instance, a better score in global coherence indicates better performance, while more cohesive errors indicate a worse performance Thus, we rendered all the variables in the same direction (i.e., higher scores associated with better performance). To that end, we subtracted each individual score from its maximum number. In this way, a higher score always means better performance. Then, we calculated two composite scores by summing the scores of the variables related to the macrostrucuture (Mps, GCoh) and the microstructure (CCPs, NoCCPs, IncPs, CohDs, Ref, Lex, Conj, Eli, Est, CohEs, Eref, Econj, Einfo, Emissing, Esent, and LCoh). Next, we ran a single hierarchical regression model for each one of the constructs of interest (macrostructure, microstructure, and modalizations). This method allows examining the variation in the dependent variable with each subsequent addition of an independent variable (Schmider et al., 2010). We grouped the independent variables in the regression models in three separate steps. Step 1 included age and status socioeconomic, step 2 included education, and step 3 included frequency of RWHs. We examined variance inflation factor (VIF) and Tolerance to assess for multicollinearity. The reference value for the VIF was <4, and for the tolerance, the reference value was >02. The values for VIF and Tolerance were within the acceptable ranges, and thus, there was no multicollinearity issue in this analysis. Model improvement was evaluated using ΔF-statistic. Improvement in the explained variance was calculated using ΔR2. Statistical significance level was assumed at p < 0.05. All the data are available in Supplementary Table 1.

Results

Table 1 provides the means, standard deviations, ranges (minimum and maximal values), and skewness for the demographic and neuropsychological variables of the sample, as well as for all the linguistic measures (dependent variables).

Results of the hierarchical regression analyses, including values of change in R2R2) and standardized coefficients (β) for the predictor variables at each step are presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized βs, R2s, and ΔR2 for the three hierarchical regression analyses (macrostructure, microstructure, and modalizations).

Modalizations

In the first step of the regression analysis, neither age nor socioeconomic status significantly predicted modalizations [R2 = 0.042, F(2, 109) = 2.409, p = 0.094], and the addition of education in Step 2 did not accounted for a significant increase in the variance of modalizations beyond of that explained by the previous sets of predictors [R2 = 0.067, ΔR2 = 0.024, F(3, 108) = 2.595, p = 0.056]. However, in Step 3, RWH accounted for a significant amount of variance in modalizations [R2 = 0.106, ΔR2 = 0.038, F(4, 107) = 3.179, p = 0.016] beyond the variance explained by the variables entered in the two previous steps. RWH significantly contributed to the change in variance in Mods [β = −0.252, t(107) = −2.160, p = 0.033], indicating that the higher the frequency of RWH, the fewer the comments on the content of the story or on the participant’s own performance during the task (modalizations) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Beta and confidence intervals for each regressor variable of the three hierarchical linear regression models (macrostructure, microstructure, and modalizations). The dots represent the beta, and the lines depict the confidence intervals for each regressor variable of the models.

Macrostructure

In the first step of the analysis, socioeconomic status accounted for a significant amount of variance in macrostructure score [R2 = 0.068, F(2, 109) = 3.99, p = 0.021]. Socioeconomic status was found to significantly contribute to the change in variance in macrostructure [β = 0.234, t(109) = 2.505, p = 0.013], indicating that the higher the socioeconomic status, the better the performance at the macrostructure level. The addition of education in the step 2 accounted for a significant increase in the variance of macrostructure [R2 = 0.167, ΔR2 = 0.099, F(3, 108) = 7.234, p ≤ 0.001] beyond that explained by the previous sets of predictors. Education significantly contributed to the change in variance in macrostructure [β = 0.371, t(108) = 3.585, p ≤ 0.001]. In step 3, the addition of RWH did not accounted for a significant amount of variance in macrostructure score [R2 = 0.067, ΔR2 ≤ 0.001, F(4, 107) = 5.384, p ≤ 0.001] (Figure 1).

Microstructure

In the first step of the regression analysis, neither age nor socioeconomic status significantly predicted microstructure score [R2 = 0.037, F(2, 109) = 2.112, p = 0.125]. However, the addition of education in step 2 accounted for a significant increase in the variance of microstructure beyond that explained by the previous sets of predictors [R2 = 0.108, ΔR2 = 0.071, F(3, 108) = 4.397, p = 0.005]. Education significantly contributed to the change in the microstructure level [β = 0.315, t(108) = 2.944, p = 0.003], indicating that the higher the education, the better their performance at the microstructure level. In step 3, the addition of RWH did not account for a significant amount of variance in microstructure score (Figure 1).

Discussion

This study aimed at investigating the effects of education and RWH on oral discourse production in typical adults. These factors are known to increase cognitive reserve during aging (Stern et al., 2020). We hypothesized that higher education levels and frequency of RWH would be positively associated with better performance on the macro- and microstructure dimensions of oral discourse production, as well as with fewer modalizations. Our results indicate that higher education explained the production of higher macro and microstructure scores. Moreover, our results indicate that higher frequency of RWH explained the production of fewer modalizations.

While the study developed by Lira et al. (2018) showed significant differences between AD participants and healthy controls regarding the macro- and microstructure dimensions, mainly macropropositions, global coherence, and the ellipsis subtype of cohesive devices [regarding modalizations, Lira et al. (2018) did not find differences between AD and healthy controls groups], our results demonstrate variations in oral discourse production as a function of education and frequency of RWH present in typical adults. Taken together, our results corroborate previous studies that support the positive effects of education (Ardila and Rosselli, 1996; Juncos-Rabadán, 1996; le Dorze and Bédard, 1998; Ardila et al., 2000; MacKenzie, 2000; Duong and Ska, 2001; Bennett et al., 2003; Hong et al., 2011; Nogueira et al., 2016) and frequency of RWH (Pawlowski et al., 2012; Moraes et al., 2013; Cotrena et al., 2016; Kochhann et al., 2018; Sörman et al., 2018; Tessaro et al., 2020) on human cognition. Our findings are discussed below in terms of (a) modalizations, (b) macrostructure, and (c) microstructure.

Modalizations

Our results showed that as the frequency of RWH increases, the number of modalizations—participants’ comments about their performance during the task—decreases. Studies investigating modalizations in AD patients found that they produced a higher amount of modalizations than healthy participants [Duong et al., 2003; Toledo et al., 2018; but see Cardebat et al., 1993; Lira et al., 2018 for contrasting results]. Regarding typical adults and older adults, le Dorze and Bédard (1998) found that older adults made more comments about their difficulties during a picture description task as compared to younger adults. To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first one to address the variations in modalizations in oral discourse production as a function of the frequency of RWH in typical adults and older adults. It is possible that individuals who maintain a high frequency of RWH have less difficulty in understanding the story and, therefore, are less susceptible to distractions and to the production of irrelevant information.

Indeed, modalizations can be interpreted as a rupture in the discursive macrostructure, since when individuals discuss their performance they tend to deviate from the theme of the narrative (Cardebat et al., 1993; Lira et al., 2018). In addition, from a pragmatic point of view, the presence of modalizations might reflect the awareness of their difficulties to maintain the central theme of the discourse (Lira et al., 2018; Toledo et al., 2018).

In our study, RWH contributed to explaining variation in modalizations, but did not explain variation at the macro and microstructure levels. Indeed, in previous studies with typical adult and older adult populations, the habit of reading books was associated with higher levels of verbal fluency (Sörman et al., 2018) and the frequency of RWH was associated with the total number of words produced in the phonemic verbal fluency task (Tessaro et al., 2020). Based on this evidence, it could be possible to postulate that individuals who maintain a frequency of RWH in their lives may have a richer vocabulary. Further studies need to address the issue of the relationship between the frequency of RWH and discourse production, which is a complex linguistic ability that goes beyond the word level. Besides, future studies could analyze the impact of RWH in other discourse tasks, as well as analyze picture sequences, like in our study, by adopting automatic language analysis, such as speech connectedness (Mota et al., 2018), to bring more conclusive evidence on the role of frequency of RWH at the discourse level.

Macrostructure

According to van Dijk (2010), the macrostructural dimension refers to the most relevant or prominent topic in the semantic information of the discourse. Our results reveal that as education increases, the performance at the macrostructure dimension increases as well. This finding corroborates other studies, which indicated that individuals with a higher level of education present more informative content in their narratives and better recognize and use the structure of the story than individuals with a lower level of education (Juncos-Rabadán, 1996; le Dorze and Bédard, 1998).

Many studies presented in the literature indicate that global coherence is the most affected aspect of oral discourse production in people with AD (Glosser and Deser, 1991; Chapman et al., 1995; Laine et al., 1998; Dijkstra et al., 2002, 2004; Ash et al., 2007; Brandão et al., 2013; Lima et al., 2014; Drummond et al., 2015; Lira et al., 2018; Toledo et al., 2018; Pistono et al., 2019). Our results indicate that coherence also plays an important role in typical aging. Moreover, our results corroborate the findings of Mackenzie (2000), who also found that individuals with higher education levels performed better than individuals with lower educational levels on the macrostructure dimension.

Microstructure

According to van Dijk (2010), the microstructural dimension of the discourse refers to the structure of an individual proposition and its internal relations. Our results indicate that as education increases, the performance at the microstructure dimension increases as well. Mackenzie et al. (2007) found that participants with lower educational levels produced more tangential sentences in their narratives than participants with higher educational levels. In another study, Mackenzie (2000) found that participants with a lower level of education produced shorter and less complete narratives in comparison with the more educated participants. Other studies of cohesion in healthy adults and older adults have reported similar findings (Juncos-Rabadán, 1996; le Dorze and Bédard, 1998). Juncos-Rabadán (1996) found that older adults with a higher level of education used more cohesive links in their narratives, while older adults with a lower level of education used a greater number of descriptive sentences and deictic elements in their narratives. These results may indicate that picture-based tasks are sensitive to the level of education and might also provide a sensitive indicator of the linguistic competence of healthy adults. In such tasks, participants are limited to the content of the pictures and cannot resort to compensatory strategies as, for example, in a rehearsed autobiography of a family narrative (Wright et al., 2011). Overall, these results stress the importance of education in the microstructural dimension of the discourse.

It is important to note that age did not predict any of the dependent variables, unlike other similar studies that addressed this effect, such as Capilouto et al.’s (2016). However, Capilouto, Wright, and Maddy used a single picture description task. Another difference between our study and Capilouto, Wright, and Maddy’s study that could explain the contrasting results is the age range of participants. Capilouto et al. (2016) divided their participants into three groups according to their age, ranging from 20 to 89. In our study, age ranged from 51 to 82 years old.

The fact that age did not predict any of the linguistic measures may indicate that the most important factors for oral discourse production are social factors and not the age when considering adult and older adult populations. Previous studies have revealed that, as age increases, there are discursive gains. Marini et al. (2005) found that the productions of participants aged 40–74 years presented more main ideas in the task based on a single figure than the productions of participants aged 20–39 and between 75 and 84 years. This can be interpreted as an improvement in the narrative capacity related to aging, which decreases only in older people. Thus, middle-aged groups may have discursive gains, and education plays one of the most important roles in discourse production.

Considering that the present study aims to investigate language at the level of individual differences, one limitation of the study is that it does not consider the overall intellectual or any other measure of general functioning. Therefore, future studies should analyze the impact of general intelligence together with social variables in the oral discourse production of older adults.

In sum, our results demonstrate the positive effect of education on the macro- and microlinguistic aspects of oral discourse production during typical aging, as well as the positive effects of frequently engaging in RWHs. Moreover, the use of narrative tasks based on a sequence of pictures seems to be valid to detect differences in oral production between healthy adult and older adult populations regarding their schooling and RWHs, showing its efficiency as a tool to be used in the clinics and in research. Oral narrative productions represent an ecologically valid way to elucidate discourse, which goes beyond the word and sentence levels, therefore favoring the analyses of coherence, cohesion, together with other aspects at the micro- and macrostructural levels, present in daily conversations. Finally, based on our results, which showed the impact of education and RWHs on oral narrative production in healthy adulthood and aging, greater attention should be paid to education and RWHs, since these can prevent or delay the development of neurodegenerative diseases, such as AD. This is especially relevant in underdeveloped or developing countries, where the increase of dementia in the near future is associated with low socioeconomic status and low educational level.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the PUCRS University Research Ethics Committee (report number 560.073, CAAE registry number 21006913.0.0000.5336). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

BM contributions to included analysis, interpretation of the data, writing, and drafting the submitted material. MW supervised the data analyses and performed a critical revision of the manuscript. LS performed a critical revision of the manuscript. LH supervised the data collection and performed a critical revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We thank the following institutions for financial support for the development of this research: (1) BM, Doctoral Scholarship by CNPq (National Council of Technologic and Scientific Development) (grant number 140436/2018-9); (2) LH, Chamada MCTIC/CNPq #28/2018; Universal #4524362508342994; Programa de Internacionalização da Pós-Graduação no Rio Grande do Sul (RS) – Edital FAPERGS #012/2013, and grants as productivity researcher at CNPq. The authors thank CAPES (PROEX) and the Graduate Course in Letras at PUCRS for financial support for this publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.740337/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.R-project.org/

References

Almeida, O. P., and Almeida, S. A. (1999). Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Depressão em Geriatria (GDS) versão reduzida. Arq. de Neuro Psiquiatr. 57, 421–426.

Google Scholar

Ardila, A., and Rosselli, M. (1996). Spontaneous language production and aging: sex and educational effects. Int. J. Neurosci. 87, 71–78. doi: 10.3109/00207459608990754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solis, F., Rosselli, M., and Gómez, C. (2000). Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 495–513. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(99)00040-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ash, S., Moore, P., Vesely, L., and Grossman, M. (2007). The decline of narrative discourse in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Lang. 103, 181–182. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.07.105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Schneider, J. A., Evans, D. A., Arnold, S. E., et al. (2003). Mendes de Leon CF. Neurology 60, 1909–1915.

Google Scholar

Brandão, L., Lima, T. M., and Parente, M. (2013). A de MP, Peña-Casanova J. Discourse coherence and its relation with cognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Psicol. em Pesqui. 7, 99–107.

Google Scholar

Brasil, C., and de, C. E. (2015). Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa (ABEP). Critério Brasil 1, 21–46.

Google Scholar

Burgess, P. W., and Shallice, T. (1997). “The relationship between prospective and retrospective memory: Neuropsychological evidence,” in Cognitive models of memory, ed. M. A. Conway (Cambridge: MIT Press), 247–272.

Google Scholar

Capilouto, G. J., Wright, H. H., and Maddy, K. M. (2016). Microlinguistic processes that contribute to the ability to relay main events: influence of age. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 23, 445–463. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1118006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capilouto, G., Wright, H. H., and Wagovich, S. A. (2005). CIU and main event analyses of the structured discourse of older and younger adults. J. commun. Disord. 38, 431–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardebat, D., Demonet, J.-F., and Doyon, B. (1993). “Narrative discourse in dementia,” in Narrative Discourse in Neurologically Impaired and Normal Aging Adults, eds Y. Joanette and H. H. Brownell (San Diego: Singular Publishing Group), 317–322. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2019.0798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, S. B., Ulatowska, H. K., King, K., Johnson, J. K., and McIntire, D. D. (1995). Discourse in early Alzheimer’s disease versus normal advanced aging. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathology. 4, 124–129. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360.0404.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotrena, C., Branco, L. D., Cardoso, C. O., Wong, C. E. I., and Fonseca, R. P. (2016). The predictive impact of biological and sociocultural factors on executive processing: the role of age, education, and frequency of reading and writing habits. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 23, 75–84. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2015.1012760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, K., Bourgeois, M. S., Allen, R. S., and Burgio, L. D. (2004). Conversational coherence: discourse analysis of older adults with and without dementia. J. Neurolinguistics 17, 263–283. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3805.1054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, K., Bourgeois, M., Petrie, G., Burgio, L., and Allen-Burge, R. (2002). My recaller is on vacation: discourse analysis of nursing-home residents with dementia. Discourse Process. 33, 53–76. doi: 10.1207/S15326950DP3301_03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drummond, C., Coutinho, G., Fonseca, R. P. R. P., Assunção, N., Teldeschi, A., de Oliveira-Souza, R., et al. (2015). Deficits in narrative discourse elicited by visual stimuli are already present in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:96. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00096/abstract

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duong, A., and Ska, B. (2001). Production of narratives: picture sequence facilitates organizational but not conceptual processing in less educated subjects. Brain Cogn. 46, 121–124. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(01)80047-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duong, A., Giroux, F., Tardif, A., and Ska, B. (2005). The heterogeneity of picture-supported narratives in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Lang. 93, 173–184. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duong, A., Tardif, A., and Ska, B. (2003). Discourse about discourse: what is it and how does it progress in Alzheimer’s disease? Brain Cogn. 53, 177–180. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00104-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glosser, G., and Deser, T. (1991). Patterns of discourse production among neurological patients with fluent language disorders. Brain Lang. 40, 67–88. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(91)90117-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glosser, G., and Deser, T. (1992). A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. J. Gerontol. 47, 266–272. doi: 10.1093/geronj/47.4.P266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, Y. J., Yoon, B., Shim, Y. S., Cho, A.-H., Lee, E.-S., Kim, Y.-I., et al. (2011). Effect of literacy and education on the visuoconstructional ability of non-demented elderly individuals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 934–939. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hübner, L. C., Loureiro, F., Smidarle, A., Tessaro, B., Siqueira, E. C. G., and Jerônimo, G. M. (2019). “Bateria de Avaliação da Linguagem no Envelhecimento - BALE,” in Tarefas de Avaliação Neuropsicológica. 1st ed, eds N. Zimmermann, F. Deleaere, and R. P. Fonseca (Rio de Janeiro: Memnon).

Google Scholar

Juncos-Rabadán, O. (1996). Narrative speech in the elderly: effects of age and education on telling stories. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 19, 669–685. doi: 10.1177/016502549601900313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochhann, R., Holz, M. R., Beber, B. C., Chaves, M. L. F., and Fonseca, R. P. (2018). Reading and writing habits as a predictor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychol. Neurosci. 11, 39–49. doi: 10.1037/pne0000125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laine, M., Laakso, M., Vuorinen, E., and Rinne, J. (1998). Coherence and informativeness of discourse in two dementia types. J. Neurolinguistics 11, 79–87. doi: 10.1016/s0911-6044(98)00006-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laks, J., Rubim Batista, E. M., Lima Guilherme, E. R., Barros Contino, A. L., Vieira Faria, M. E., Figueira, I., et al. (2003). Mini-mental state examination in community-dwelling elderly: preliminary data from Santo Antônio de Pádua, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. Arq. de Neuro Psiquiatr. 61, 782–785. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

le Dorze, G., and Bédard, C. (1998). Effects of age and education on the lexico-semantic content of connected speech in adults. J. Commun. Disord. 31, 53–71. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(97)00051-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, T. M., Brandão, L., Parente, M. A., de, M. P., and Peña-Casanova, J. (2014). Doença de Alzheimer: cognição e discurso narrativo com apoio em figuras. Rev. CEFAC 16, 1168–1177.

Google Scholar

Lira, J. O., de, Minett, T. S. C., Bertolucci, P. H. F., and Ortiz, K. Z. (2018). Evaluation of macrolinguistic aspects of the oral discourse in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatrics 31, 1343–1353. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKenzie, C. (2000). Adult spoken discourse: the influences of age and education. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 35, 269–285. doi: 10.1080/136828200247188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackenzie, C. (2000). The relevance of education and age in the assessment of discourse comprehension. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 14, 151–161. doi: 10.1080/026992000298887

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackenzie, C., Brady, M., Norrie, J., and Poedjianto, N. (2007). Picture description in neurologically normal adults: concepts and topic coherence. Aphasiology 21, 340–354. doi: 10.1080/02687030600911419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marini, A., Boewe, A., Caltagirone, C., and Carlomagno, S. (2005). Age-related Differences in the Production of Textual Descriptions. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 34, 439–463. doi: 10.1007/s10936-005-6203-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C. O., Pontbriand-Drolet, S., Daoust, V., Yamga, E., Amiri, M., Hübner, L. C., et al. (2018). Narrative discourse in young and older adults: behavioral and NIRS analyses. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:69. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moraes, A. L., Guimarães, L. S. P., Joanette, Y., Parente, M. A., de, M. P., Fonseca, R. P., et al. (2013). Effect of aging, education, reading and writing, semantic processing and depression symptoms on verbal fluency. Psicol. Reflex. e Crit. 26, 680–690.

Google Scholar

Mota, N. B., Sigman, M., Cecchi, G., Copelli, M., and Ribeiro, S. (2018). The maturation of speech structure in psychosis is resistant to formal education. npj Schizophr. 4:25. doi: 10.1038/s41537-018-0067-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nespoulous, J. L. L., Lecours, A. R., Lafond, D., Lemay, A., Puel, M., Joanette, Y., et al. (1986). Protocole Montréal-Toulouse d’examen linguistique de l’aphasie MIB. Montréal Centre Hospitalier C6te des Neiges 1, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Nitrini, R., Bottino, C. M. C., Albala, C., Custodio Capuñay, N. S., Ketzoian, C., Llibre Rodriguez, J. J., et al. (2009). Prevalence of dementia in Latin America: a collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Int. Psychogeriatrics 21, 622–630. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209009430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nogueira, D. S., Reis, E. A., and Vieira, A. (2016). Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia phoniatr. et logop. 68, 124–133. doi: 10.1159/000450640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagliarin, K. C., Gindri, G., Ortiz, K. Z., Parente, M. A. M. P., Joanette, Y., Nespoulous, J.-L., et al. (2015). Relationship between the Brazilian version of the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery and education, age and reading and writing characteristics: a cross-sectional study. Sao Paulo Med. J. 133, 298–306. doi: 10.1590/1516-3180.2014.8461610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pawlowski, J., Remor, E., de Mattos Pimenta Parente, M. A., de Salles, J. F., Fonseca, R. P., and Bandeira, D. R. (2012). The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Read. Writing. 25, 2275–2289. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeffer, R. I., Kurosaki, T. T., Harrah, C. H., Chance, J. M., and Filos, S. (1982). Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J. Gerontol. 37, 323–329. doi: 10.1093/geronj/37.3.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pistono, A., Jucla, M., Bézy, C., Lemesle, B., le Men, J., and Pariente, J. (2019). Discourse macrolinguistic impairment as a marker of linguistic and extralinguistic functions decline in early Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 54, 390–400. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quintas, J. L., Camargos, E. F., Melo, C. V. S., and Nóbrega, O. T. (2017). Influência da escolaridade e da idade em testes cognitivos. Geriat. Gerontol. Aging 11, 165–169.

Google Scholar

Schmider, E., Ziegler, M., Danay, E., Beyer, L., and Bühner, M. (2010). Is it really robust? Methodology 6, 147–151. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241/a000016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ska, B., Scherer, L. C., Flôres, O. C., Oliveira, CR de, Netto, T. M., and Fonseca, R. P. (2009). Theoretical, behavioral and neuroimage evidence on discourse processing aging. Psychol. Neurosci. 2, 101–109. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörman, D. E., Ljungberg, J. K., and Rönnlund, M. (2018). Reading habits among older adults in relation to level and 15-year changes in verbal fluency and episodic recall. Front. Psychol. 9:1872. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01872/full

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., et al. (2020). Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s Dement. 16, 1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Tessaro, B., Hermes-Pereira, A., Schilling, L. P., Fonseca, R. P., Kochhann, R., and Hübner, L. C. (2020). Verbal fluency in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in individuals with low educational level and its relationship with reading and writing habits. Dement. Neuropsychol. 14, 300–307. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642020dn14-030011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toledo, C. M., Aluísio, S. M., Santos, L. B., Brucki, S. M. D., Trés, E. S., Oliveira, M. O., et al. (2018). Analysis of macrolinguistic aspects of narratives from individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and no cognitive impairment. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: diagnosis. Assess. Dis. Monit. 10, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijk, T. A. (2010). Cognição: Discurso e Interação. 3. ed, São Paulo: Contexto

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. S. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III), 3rd Edn. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Sour. Softw. 43:1686. doi: 10.21105/joss.01686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, H. H., Capilouto, G. J., Srinivasan, C., and Fergadiotis, G. (2011). Story Processing Ability in Cognitively Healthy Younger and Older Adults. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 900–917. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0253)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, H. H., Koutsoftas, A. D., Capilouto, G. J., and Fergadiotis, G. (2014). Global coherence in younger and older adults: influence of cognitive processes and discourse type. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 21, 174–196. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2013.794894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. In. PARENTE, Mattos Pimenta. Cognição MA, envelhecimento, editors. J. Psychiatr. Res. 3, 37–49.

Google Scholar

Keywords: oral discourse, narrative discourse, education, reading and writing habits, typical aging, macrostructure, microstructure, modalization

Citation: Malcorra BLC, Wilson MA, Schilling LP and Hübner LC (2022) Lower Education and Reading and Writing Habits Are Associated With Poorer Oral Discourse Production in Typical Adults and Older Adults. Front. Psychol. 13:740337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.740337

Received: 14 July 2021; Accepted: 18 February 2022;
Published: 18 March 2022.

Edited by:

Antonino Vallesi, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sendy Caffarra, Stanford University, United States
Enrico Toffalini, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Malcorra, Wilson, Schilling and Hübner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bárbara Luzia Covatti Malcorra, barbaraluz.malcorra@gmail.com, barbara.malcorra@edu.pucrs.br

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.