Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 19 May 2022
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Methodological Issues in Consciousness Research View all 6 articles

Tackling the Electro-Topography of the Selves Through the Sphere Model of Consciousness

  • 1Research Institute for Neuroscience, Education and Didactics, Patrizio Paoletti Foundation, Assisi, Italy
  • 2Department of Criminology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel

In the current hypothesis paper, we propose a novel examination of consciousness and self-awareness through the neuro-phenomenological theoretical model known as the Sphere Model of Consciousness (SMC). Our aim is to create a practical instrument to address several methodological issues in consciousness research. We present a preliminary attempt to validate the SMC via a simplified electrophysiological topographic map of the Self. This map depicts the gradual shift from faster to slower frequency bands that appears to mirror the dynamic between the various SMC states of Self. In order to explore our hypothesis that the SMC’s different states of Self correspond to specific frequency bands, we present a mini-review of studies examining the electrophysiological activity that occurs within the different states of Self and in the context of specific meditation types. The theoretical argument presented here is that the SMC’s hierarchical organization of three states of the Self mirrors the hierarchical organization of Focused Attention, Open Monitoring, and Non-Dual meditation types. This is followed by testable predictions and potential applications of the SMC and the hypotheses derived from it. To our knowledge, this is the first integrated electrophysiological account that combines types of Self and meditation practices. We suggest this electro-topographic framework of the Selves enables easier, clearer conceptualization of the connections between meditation types as well as increased understanding of wakefulness states and altered states of consciousness.

Introduction

For centuries, scientists and philosophers have been tackling the question of what consciousness is, in order to understand what underlies human behavior, feeling, and action (Barrett and Satpute, 2013). Consciousness is a volatile phenomenon that is not easily defined, yet it determines our perception of what we consider as objective reality. While most operational definitions offer some attempt to characterize its various aspects, such definitions do not give a completely satisfying account of the way we know we are aware of our self and our existence (for a detailed argument, see Ardila, 2016). Consciousness is often regarded as consisting of two main components: awareness (the “content” of consciousness), and wakefulness or arousal (the “level” of consciousness). In addition to perception of the events of the “outside world,” awareness also refers to the subjective experience of internal phenomena: our perception of inner “events,” whether or not related to external ones, that comprises our subjective reality (Vithoulkas and Muresanu, 2014). This may also include self-awareness, i.e., awareness of one’s internal world of thoughts, reflections, imagination, and emotions as an expression of the Self. There is also a more elaborate level of self-awareness involving higher-order cognitive mechanisms such as attentional focus on one’s own mental states that is considered a distinct form of reflexive awareness (Peters, 2009), or “self as context” (Grieger, 1985).

Neuroscientific research has adopted different notions of the Self, the most frequently used of which is the binary distinction between the Narrative Self (NS) and the Minimal Self (MS) (James, 1890/1950; Gallagher, 2000; Legrand, 2006, 2007; Zahavi and Gallagher, 2008; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Gallagher and Zahavi, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020). In the SMC, a third state is added, called Overcoming of the Self, indicating the complete absence of any sense of self (Paoletti, 2002a,b; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020). In the last decade, each of these three selves has been regarded in contemplative neuroscience as a particular configuration of self-awareness and mental contents (Gallagher, 2000; Jerath et al., 2015; Sugimura et al., 2021). There have also been attempts to link self-states to higher-order cognitive functions (i.e., executive functions), although the existing research is still scant (Ardila, 2016; Wade et al., 2018). Nonetheless, there is some evidence suggesting that the Self can be examined as a multidimensional construct denoted by: (1) First-person subjective reporting–such as the self-awareness experience; (2) Behavioral characteristics expressed through executive functions; and (3) Third-person objective measurements, such as electrophysiology (Fingelkurts et al., 2020). Accordingly, several theoretical models of selves have been proposed based on first-person phenomenology (James, 1890/1950; Gallagher, 2000; Legrand, 2006, 2007; Zahavi and Gallagher, 2008; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Fingelkurts et al., 2020; Gallagher and Zahavi, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020); however, the challenge of empirically examining and validating them remains.

In this theoretical paper, we propose a novel examination of consciousness and self-awareness through the lens of the neuro-phenomenological theoretical model known as the Sphere Model of Consciousness (SMC; Paoletti, 2002a,b; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020). Although there are methodological challenges involved in examining and validating this model, we suggest ways to overcome these challenges through an “electro-topography of the Selves” that depicts cognitive and electrophysiological correlates of the different states of the Self, induced by different meditative practices. Of course, the subjective phenomenology of first-person experience remains an indispensable part of any consciousness research (Varela, 1996; Depraz et al., 2000). Thus, our aim is to create a practical instrument bridging first person reports and electrophysiology, in order to resolve some of the methodological challenges of consciousness research, as outlined below.

First, we will show how each state of the Self connects with a particular type of self-awareness and first-person phenomenology of experience, and then link these to executive functions. Second, by isolating some of the specific electrophysiological and neuronal correlates of each Self and meditative practice, we will suggest that hierarchies of meditation and the Selves (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021) share a similar electro-topography. As the hierarchical electrophysiological correlates of Selves have yet to be conceptualized within one unified model, we will detail the electrophysiology of the Selves, followed by the electrophysiology of meditation and each specific Self, all within the framework of the Sphere Model of Consciousness (SMC) (Figure 1A; Paoletti, 2002a,b; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) The Sphere Model of Consciousness. (B) Different Selves in the SMC and their main suggested electrophysiological correlate. The Narrative Self is related to Gamma and Beta, the Minimal Self is related to a shift toward Alpha and Theta (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014), and Overcoming the Self is mostly related to Delta activity (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013; Paoletti et al., 2020). Adapted from Paoletti and Ben-Soussan (2019, 2020).

The Sphere Model of Consciousness, Hierarchy of Selves, and Executive Functions

The three Selves conceptualized in the SMC are: the Narrative Self, the Minimal Self, and the Overcoming of the Self (SMC; Paoletti, 2002a,b; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020; see Figure 1A). They are represented as concentric circles around a center (Figure 1B), with the Narrative Self (NS) on the periphery, the Minimal Self (MS) in the middle, and Overcoming of the Self (OCS) in the center.

Each Self is characterized by a particular state of self-awareness and first-person phenomenology that are expressed through features driven by functionality and cognition. In turn, the specific features of each state of the Self facilitate regulatory processes supported by executive functions (EFs) (Barkley, 2001; Nigg, 2017), as we will see shortly. The Narrative Self is considered as a self-image built through autobiographical memories and projections into the future; it involves awareness of personal identity and its continuity through time, as well as conceptual contents (Oatley, 2007). The Minimal Self emerges from the awareness of a situated living body as a sensorimotor unit that enables selfhood in the physical world in the “here and now” (James, 1890/1950; Gallagher, 2000), has a short temporal extension, and is endowed with a sense of action, property, and first person non-conceptual content. In our previous papers about the SMC, we specified the addition of a third state, called Overcoming of the Self (see Figure 1B), in which all sense of self disappears yet subjective experiences are still able to be experienced and eventually reported (Paoletti, 2002a,b; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020).

As we have said, each of the above-mentioned Selves is linked to core self-awareness features driven by functionality and cognition, and they facilitate regulatory processes supported by executive functions (EFs) (Barkley, 2001; Nigg, 2017). EFs are a set of higher-order cognitive functions often linked to the prefrontal cortex of the brain, consisting of three key components–working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility–from which more complex and higher-order EFs are built (e.g., reasoning and planning) (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013). EFs are essential for controlling unregulated behaviors, whether external (e.g., physical actions), as well as internal (e.g., meditative practices) (van Gaal et al., 2009; Hofmann et al., 2012). Furthermore, effective self-regulation processes require self-awareness of one’s actions, thoughts, and feelings as well as the ability to make desired changes when needed (Heatherton, 2011). The multidimensional nature of EFs creates complexity regarding how they are conceptualized theoretically (Anderson, 2002; Welsh et al., 2006). This complexity is evident in the examination of latent components of EFs (e.g., shifting, inhibition) in relation to other multidimensional constructs, such as Self and awareness. The convergent and discriminant validity of these constructs is not clear, and the nature of their differences remains to be determined.

The different Selves–Narrative Self, Minimal Self, and Overcoming of the Self–may involve different usages of EFs. As we have seen above, the Narrative Self, depicted toward the periphery of the SMC sphere, is a more or less coherent self (or self-image) that is constituted of a past and a future in the various narratives that we and others tell about ourselves. It relies on declarative and episodic memories of the past or projection into the future (Vago and David, 2012) and, for an evaluative and personal perspective to be constituted and maintained over time, executive and volitional processes are necessary (Bortolan, 2020). Indeed, narrative abilities required to establish the Narrative Self are associated with core components of EFs and the higher-order cognitive functions that emerge from them, such as working-memory, inhibition, and set-shifting (Friend and Bates, 2014). However, once the Narrative Self is constructed and established, one can act more automatically with little forethought based on accumulated and internalized learning and experience. Thus, in the Narrative Self, a person can operate with relatively low self-awareness.

In contrast, the Minimal Self, depicted between the periphery and the center of the SMC sphere, is usually connected to conscious experience and a specific kind of self-awareness in which experiences are assimilated immediately as one’s own experiences without need for any inferential processes (Lane, 2020). This form of self-experience in the Minimal Self is connected with pre-reflective self-consciousness (Bortolan, 2020). As Gallagher and Zahavi (2020) explain, on the one hand, self-experience is “non-observational” because it does not depend on any kind of introspective attitude taken by the subject toward the experience itself. On the other, it is “non-objectifying” because through it the self is not treated as an object, but rather as the subject of a conscious state. It can be claimed that in the Minimal Self state, a person deliberately reduces the use of executive processes and at the same time increases the level of bodily self-awareness.

Finally, as the level of consciousness of the Self approaches the center of the SMC sphere, toward the Overcoming of the Self, the necessity of EFs may decrease considerably, or even cease completely.1 It is also entirely possible that the use of EFs in different Selves may not only vary in how much they are involved but also in the way they are involved. For example, a recent review (Gallant, 2016) highlighted a specific, instead of general, improvement in inhibition following mindfulness meditation, but showed inconsistent benefits in memory and cognitive flexibility, the other two main components of EFs (Miyake et al., 2000).

Toward an Electro-Topography of the Selves

In the last few years, there have been several attempts to link different features of self-awareness (e.g., identity, witnessing) with their electrophysiological correlates (Raffone et al., 2019; Fingelkurts et al., 2020; Sugimura et al., 2021), indicating a noticeable interest of neuroscientific research in finding a method to correlate first-person phenomenology and objective measures. One such attempt at a three-dimensional construct model for complex experiential selfhood was proposed, focusing on cortical alpha activity (Fingelkurts et al., 2020). It suggests that increased frontal alpha connectivity during the experience of a “non-symbolic, non-linguistic sense of self-presenting being,” such as a sharper, more vivid experience of being a witnessing agent for oneself, possibly reflects being more conscious and able to access the external world, as well as the first-person perspective (Fingelkurts et al., 2020). Furthermore, temporal dynamics reflected in the brain’s electrophysiological activity have been linked by Sugimura et al. (2021) to another core feature of self-awareness, namely, the sense of consistency of self. In turn, the subjective sense of an integrated and coherent Self across time, conceptualized as identity (Erikson, 1968), has been correlated to better cognitive performance, as well as to enhanced moral judgment and creative problem solving (Dietrich and Kanso, 2010; Travis et al., 2011; Charles et al., 2014).

Models of the self, such as those proposed by Fingelkurts et al. (2020) and Sugimura et al. (2021) focus mostly on alpha cortical sources. Yet, contemplative studies, which serve as excellent means of examining the states of Self, have also emphasized the importance of additional bands as well (Travis et al., 2010; Glicksohn and Berkovich-Ohana, 2011, 2012; Kerr et al., 2011; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Flor-Henry et al., 2017; Wahbeh et al., 2018; Glicksohn and Ben-Soussan, 2020). Taking this into consideration, we suggest the inclusion of additional frequency bands in order to find more comprehensive electrophysiological marker evidence differentiating the different Selves.

Toward this aim, we present a preliminary attempt to integrate into the SMC a simplified topographic map of the Selves by band, in which the NS is related to higher frequency bands, while the transition toward the center of the sphere that is associated with the MS is related to a shifting toward lower frequency bands (see Figure 1B; Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013; Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Paoletti et al., 2020), ultimately leading toward the state of OCS (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The hierarchy of Selves in the Sphere Model of Consciousness corresponding to the hierarchy of meditation and their primary electrophysiological correlates.

We also posit that the electro-topography of the Selves aligns with a hierarchy of meditation practices, from Focused Attention (FA) to Open Monitoring (OM) to Non-Dual (ND) meditation (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021).2 Meditation provides us an excellent tool for investigating the nature of the Self, especially in the rare states of consciousness in which self-processing changes (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021). Understanding how meditation modulates neural oscillations3 may help elucidate the relationship between these brain oscillations and cognitive processes, as well as the different states of Self they may induce. Accordingly, we will show how these three related meditation practices, namely, FA, OM, and ND, follow a similar pattern as the Selves of increasingly slower frequency bands4 approaching the center of the SMC sphere.

Thus, we will attempt to show that hierarchical organization based on the level of consciousness of the three types of the Self, which mirrors the hierarchical organization of meditation practice types, can be electrophysiologically measured through frequency bands. This electrophysiological topographic map of the self, reflecting a gradual shift from faster frequency bands, namely, Gamma (30–70 Hz) and Beta (13–30 Hz), to slower ones, namely, Alpha (8–12 Hz), Theta (4–8 Hz), and eventually Delta (1–4 Hz) as we get closer to the center of the Sphere. Thus, we propose three main hypotheses that can be examined through evidence-based electro-topography as specified below:

Hypothesis 1: The Narrative Self Primarily Involves Gamma and Beta

Empirical grounding for the first hypothesis is based on the finding that Gamma and Beta bands are often correlated with EFs such as focused attention and cognitive effort (Ward, 2003). Since Gamma and Beta are attributed both to FA and working memory, which is essential for the Narrative Self (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021), we posit that the Narrative Self will be electrophysiologically related to FA meditation.

In fact, Gamma has been consistently reported to increase during engaging self-referential processing and focused attention in different cognitive tasks (Engel et al., 2001; Fell et al., 2003), both of which are related to the Narrative Self (Mantini et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2008), while a general decrease in this band has been reported during more relaxed states of meditation.

Beta is typically associated with sensorimotor processing (Symons et al., 2016). This is supported by studies that have shown a decrease in Beta during OM (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013; Faber et al., 2015), Transcendental Meditation (Tomljenoviæ et al., 2016), and following an intensive 3-month meditation retreat (Slagter et al., 2007). Decreased Beta activity might be understood as decreased active processing consistent with the traditional understanding that Beta activity is associated with a more active and aroused brain-processing state (Slagter et al., 2009). The findings regarding this frequency band mirror those regarding Gamma frequencies: increasing in relation to the Narrative Self state and decreasing in more relaxed states. Thus, our hypothesis that the Narrative Self is related to FA meditation (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021), is centered on the notion that both will elicit primarily Gamma and Beta frequencies.

Hypothesis 2: The Minimal Self Primarily Involves Alpha and Theta

Empirical grounding for the second hypothesis is centered around findings that Alpha and Theta frequencies have been found to increase in more relaxed states, lower arousal, in association with meditation, and with an internally directed focus of attention (Cooper et al., 2003; Takahashi et al., 2005; Lomas et al., 2015; Cona et al., 2020), all of which are related to the Minimal Self (see Section “The Sphere Model of Consciousness, Hierarchy of Selves, and Executive Functions” above). Many studies have found increased Alpha to be associated with meditation, such as Zen and Transcendental Meditation (Kasamatsu and Hirai, 1966; Wallace et al., 1971; Banquet, 1973; Murata et al., 1994, 2004; Yamamoto et al., 2006; see also Baerentsen et al., 2001 for a possible explanation of frontal cortical-subcortical system dominance in Zen meditation initiation).

There are different possible reasons for this relationship between Alpha frequency and meditation. DeLosAngeles et al. (2016) suggested that the widely reported increased Alpha power in meditation could be interpretable as a marker of practice or ease in task performance rather than specific to meditation. Beauregard et al. (2009) have instead suggested that higher frontal and temporal Alpha during meditation is an index of reduced cortical arousal associated with a relaxation response. In addition, as Kerr et al. (2013) beautifully described in their review, with somatic focus, mindfulness’ top-down alpha rhythm modulation “enhances gain control which, in turn, sensitizes practitioners to better detect and regulate when the mind wanders from its somatic focus” (pp. 1), a feature fundamentally associated with the Minimal Self. In turn, enhanced regulation of somatic mind-wandering may be an important early stage of mindfulness training that leads to enhanced cognitive regulation and metacognition.

While both OM and FA are related to increased frontal Alpha amplitude and synchrony (Travis, 2001), a recent study reported that OM resulted in an increase in Alpha power, compared to FA, to meditation naïve controls (Himalayan Yoga), and to mind wandering (Braboszcz et al., 2017). There is also evidence that experienced meditators have increased prefrontal and parietal Alpha power during sleep (Dentico et al., 2016; regarding the sleep-wakefulness continuum, see also subsection The Sleep-Wakefulness Continuum below). One reason for this difference between OM and FA is that the top-down regulation of alpha is considered a key mechanism by which advanced OM practitioners learn to disengage attention in order to maintain greater attentional flexibility (Kerr et al., 2013). Alpha is not the only frequency that seems to be particularly related to OM; frontal and parietal Theta coherence have been found to increase in particular during this type of meditation (Cahn et al., 2013).

In addition, both Alpha and Theta are closely related to executive functions such as working memory (Sauseng et al., 2005a). Alpha frequency also plays a critical role in inhibition (Klimesch et al., 2007; Klimesch, 2011) and cognitive flexibility (Fink et al., 2009; Wolff et al., 2017), the other two main components of executive functions.

Theta is also associated with other relevant findings. Notably, Sugimura et al. (2021) reported that participants who tested well in having a strong sense of who they are (i.e., identity synthesis) exhibited increased Theta waves with low noise contamination in their frontal lobe, while those who reported feelings of a temporally changeable and fragmented self (i.e., identity confusion) displayed more Beta waves with high noise interference in the centroparietal lobe. In addition, frontocentral Alpha waves correlated negatively with identity confusion, and frontal Theta waves showed positive relationships with identity synthesis (Sugimura et al., 2021). Additionally, Aftanas and Golocheikine (2001) suggest that Theta is related to thoughtless awareness and bliss.

Taken together, these findings are particularly significant for linking the increase in Alpha and Theta bands to the phenomenology of the Minimal Self. A stronger sense of oneself is, in fact, reported during the Minimal Self state, which is characterized by bodily perception and a more consistent and continuous self-perception compared to the Narrative Self. Moreover, presuming that the Minimal Self is in alignment with OM meditation, both should elicit Alpha and Theta frequencies in particular.

Hypothesis 3: Overcoming of the Self Primarily Involves Delta Frequency

Empirical grounding for the third hypothesis centers on the finding that Delta is the main frequency associated with non-dual meditative states and higher states of consciousness (Berman and Stevens, 2015). In fact lower frequency bands, namely, Delta (for a recent review, see Wahbeh et al., 2018), are related to meditation’s “core,” and thus are depicted toward the center of the SMC sphere (Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020). This leads us to the third hypothesis that Delta should be primarily associated with Overcoming of the Self (see Section “The Sphere Model of Consciousness, Hierarchy of Selves, and Executive Functions”), and that Non-Dual meditation, which we theorize corresponds with the OCS, should also be particularly associated with Delta.

Delta is reported to increase in deep meditation, especially with higher states of consciousness (Mason et al., 1997; Parker et al., 2013; Berman and Stevens, 2015; Parker, 2017). Unusual Delta activity generated in deep meditation is affiliated with non-conceptual awareness; it may enhance the capacity to suddenly recognize complex, subtle informational patterns that serve to provide novel, relevant solutions to complex problems through insight (Horan, 2009). Consistent with our current hypothesis and supported by previous meditation research Berman and Stevens (2015) found increased Delta, Theta, and Alpha activity during meditation (for a review, see Ivanovski and Malhi, 2007; Wahbeh et al., 2018).

In addition, combining hypothesis 1, 2, and 3 demonstrating a gradual slowing of the frequencies as we approach the center of the SMC sphere, we can also observe a differentiation between general meditation and non-dual states (in which the participant transcends the separation between self and other). The opposite trend was observed for Gamma activity, which was higher during the meditation sessions compared to non-dual states (Berman and Stevens, 2015). Similarly, Berkovich-Ohana et al. (2012), who examined participants who possessed one of three levels of mindfulness expertise versus non-experienced controls, found that mindfulness practitioners generally exhibited reduced resting-state frontal low Gamma power as compared to controls. They also found decreased resting-state Gamma functional connectivity—representing Default Monde Network (DMN) deactivation—among the long-term practitioners, suggesting a trait or long-lasting effect of reduced mind-wandering and self-related processing that is generally associated with the Narrative Self (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2012, 2013). In addition, creativity, as measured by ideational fluency and flexibility, which was higher among the long-term practitioners than the short-term practitioners and control participants, was negatively correlated with Gamma interhemispheric functional connectivity (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2017). Thus, one should keep in mind that different meditation techniques can produce different electrophysiological results, depending, among other things, on the extent of experience of the practitioner, and the experimental design (Berman and Stevens, 2015).

In sum, recent electrophysiological studies of meditation consistently demonstrate increased slower frequency bands, and either decreased DMN activation or measures of enhanced sensory, and attentional processing with concomitantly decreased automated reactivity (for a review, see Britton et al., 2014) which could be integrated in the SMC and the hierarchy of Selves. See Table 1 for our mini-review linking different meditation practices and the phenomenological description of the Selves to the electrophysiological findings. The literature review presented in Table 1 was conducted in order to examine our hypothesis that there is a hierarchical order of the electrophysiological correlates that corresponds with the hierarchical order of the three types of the selves and related meditation practices.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Synthesis table summing our mini-review linking different meditation practices and the phenomenological description of the Selves to the electrophysiological findings.

Discussion on the Model and Current Hypothesis

Of the articles reviewed, 83% support our model of the electro-topography of the Selves with increasingly slower frequency bands as one approaches the center of the SMC sphere (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Numerical summary of literature mini-review findings.

To further support our hypothesis, we will now also present a number of our previous study results regarding the relationship between frequency bands and self-awareness through: (1) Mindfulness meditation, (2) Quadrato Motor Training (QMT), and (3) The Whole-Body Perceptual Deprivation chamber (OVO-WBPD) (Table 3). The last two are techniques that were created from the SMC itself. The results support the relationship between electrophysiological frequency bands and the SMC. More specifically:

1. Mindfulness meditation. We previously reported higher fluency and flexibility, which are measures of creativity, among two long-term mindfulness practitioners as compared to short-term mindfulness practitioners and control participants, which were negatively correlated with trait default-mode gamma inter-hemispheric functional connectivity (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2017). This study indicates that a form of mindfulness meditation that shares similarities with QMT can reduce the role of the DMN and the involvement of gamma frequency band. In terms of the model, mindfulness meditation can promote distancing from Narrative Self and from fast frequency bands like gamma. This is in line with the proposed electro-topography, since processes and networks (DMN) commonly related to the Narrative Self have also been found associated with gamma frequency outside the context of the SMC and the scope of the present paper (e.g., Engel et al., 2001; Laufs et al., 2003a,b; Chen et al., 2008; Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2012).

2. The QMT is a specifically structured movement meditation in which the participants make a step into one of 12 possible directions according to verbal instruction (Paoletti and Salvagio, 2011; Dotan Ben-Soussan et al., 2013). QMT relates mainly to processes associated with the Minimal Self as it requires ongoing second-by-second mindful awareness to the body and to the upcoming command (Ben-Soussan et al., 2019b; Leshem et al., 2020).

3. The OVO-WBPD chamber is an altered sensory environment in the form of a human-sized egg (uovo in Italian literally means “egg”). Based on the SMC, the OVO-WBPD was specifically built with the aim of facilitating an immersive experience and an increased state of presence (Paoletti, 2002a). While the QMT is related more to the Minimal Self, the experience in the OVO-WBPD is more related to the Overcoming of the Self, since it induces a state of absorption and dissolves spatial boundaries (Ben-Soussan et al., 2019a).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of studies relating Quadrato Motor Training (QMT), OVO Whole Body Perceptual Deprivation chamber (OVO-WBPD), and mindfulness meditation to different states of Self and their electrophysiological correlates.

Taken together, these techniques and their electrophysiological correlates (shown in Table 3) suggest a gradual shift from the Narrative Self and higher frequency bands toward the Minimal Self and then toward Overcoming of the Self and lower frequency bands that we are proposing with the present mini-review.

In Table 4, we also highlight the spatial localization of the EEG results reported in this mini-review. It is possible to see that Alpha and Theta frequency bands display the most consistent results with predominant frontal activity, while Gamma and Delta frequency bands are less spatially localized. One possible explanation for these results could be the scale-dependent mechanism highlighted by Von Stein and Sarnthein (2000) for visual processing. The authors proposed that the more local the synchronization, the higher the frequency involved. In particular, they showed that “local interactions during visual processing involve gamma frequency dynamics, semantical interactions between temporal and parietal cortex involve beta frequency dynamics, and very long-range interactions […] a low theta or alpha frequency range.”

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of main studies and their localization.

Although these conclusion concern visual processing, they appear highly relevant because they are consistent with the findings reported in Table 4 and they offer a cogent explanation as to why the Delta frequency band is more globally distributed than other “faster” frequencies. In addition, Von Stein and Sarnthein (2000) stated that Alpha and Theta frequencies seem to be “specifically involved in processing of internal mental context” which is in line with the role that these frequency bands have in the proposed electro-topography model and in the previous literature (Takahashi et al., 2005; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Klimesch, 2012; Benedek, 2018; Cona et al., 2020), further supporting our hypothesis.

Suggested Hypothesis Testing and Application

First-Person Phenomenology and Self-Awareness

Probably the biggest challenge in the study of consciousness is how to deal with the first-person subjective experience within an objective measurable framework. According to the SMC, all phenomenal characteristics of experience can be placed along three axes: Time, Emotion, and Self-Determination (see Figure 1; Paoletti, 2002a; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020, 2021). The highest degree of self-awareness can be represented as an equal relationship between the periphery of the sphere, or the extremes of the axes on which the features of first-person experience are placed, and the center, where we place consciousness-as-such/non-dual consciousness. While the spherical matrix of the model provides a diagram for representing first-person experience, our hypotheses on the electro-topography of the Selves might enable us in future studies to find neurophysiological correlates for first-person experience. For instance, recalling Raffone et al. (2019) argument about the Beta frequency (the possibility of different levels or grades of self-awareness within each dimension of Self), we might observe this frequency in a state of self-projection as well in FA meditation with good self-awareness.

Our model may also serve as a guideline to refine the research on correlates to self-awareness phenomenology: when Beta is present, for example, we hypothesize a connection with the Narrative Self, but can we also detect differences in its coherence, amplitude, and localization in specific brain areas? What would that imply? For example, can we detect electrophysiological brain correlates in areas suggested in different models as the main areas for consciousness correlation–like Broadman Area 10 (Raffone and Srinivasan, 2009) or the precuneus (Josipovic, 2019)–according to the Self we are observing?

Since several works have already been published on the topic, we will only focus briefly on the problem of ineffability (see Section “The Problem of Ineffability”), and then present several difficulties that have not (with few exceptions) been sufficiently addressed yet in the research literature, such as the sleep-wakefulness continuum (see also Section “The Sleep-Wakefulness Continuum”) and the distinction between higher and altered states of consciousness (see Section “The Importance of Differentiating Between Altered and Higher States of Consciousness”). As detailed above (Section “The Sphere Model of Consciousness, Hierarchy of Selves, and Executive Functions”), the Minimal Self and the Narrative Self appear to be more directly related to executive functions (EFs), while the Overcoming of the Self presents us with the problem of ineffability, which will now be shortly addressed.

The Problem of Ineffability

In order to electrophysiologically and behaviorally map the different Selves, we need to distinguish between consciousness and its contents and, consequently, deal with the challenge of ineffability. One of the main obstacles in studying and quantifying higher states of consciousness is the ineffability of subjective experiences. When one’s sense of self “disappears,” as is reported in first-person accounts of the Overcoming of the Self, we encounter something hardly measurable by the same behavioral and cognitive parameters used to assess other states of the Self which are mostly identified with/described by the processes involved (e.g., Narrative Self is associated with autobiographical processes and conceptual contents; Gallagher, 2000; Oatley, 2007). Yet, taking neurophenomenological studies for reference, we can electrophysiologically measure whether consciousness-without-content or non-dual awareness is in line with some of the correlates for that state, as suggested by the most recent models of consciousness proposed by Raffone and Srinivasan (2009); Metzinger (2018), and Josipovic (2019). These models conceptualize consciousness-without-content, non-dual awareness, and the Overcoming of the Self (in the case of the SMC) which can be easily measured using EEG. Indeed, various attempts are being made to do so (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2017). For this reason, the hypothesized electrophysiological model could provide an interesting index (i.e., increase in Delta frequency band) to identify the state in which any sense of self disappears and reportable cognitive processes and behavioral measures become less informative.

The Sleep-Wakefulness Continuum

Another possible application for the proposed electro-topographic model is in the examination and differentiation of various states of wakefulness. More specifically, the sleep-wakefulness continuum is often treated as dichotomous, but various cultures have described different states of awareness outside this dichotomy (Walsh and Vaughan, 1993). One of these states is the outcome of transcendent meditation: a deeply restful state but with fully alert inner wakefulness (Alexander and Sands, 1993). Utilizing this state of consciousness as an example of an “in-between state” along the sleep-wakefulness continuum, we will apply our proposed model to explore if the proposed electro-topography matches the electroencephalography findings in the literature.

Further, considering the phenomenological description of transcendental consciousness as a profound state of relaxation but with preserved awareness, and following the SMC-based hypotheses presented in this paper, we postulate that this state of consciousness will be found associated with increased activity in Alpha and Theta bands (Wallace et al., 1971; Banquet, 1973; Mason et al., 1997; Yamamoto et al., 2006).

For example, Theta plays important role in memory encoding and consolidation during sleep (for a review, see Rasch and Born, 2007). Nonetheless, as Kirov et al. (2009) demonstrated, application of transcranial slow oscillation stimulation (tSOS) can enhance the process of memory consolidation not only during sleep (Marshall et al., 2006), but also in wakeful states. This enhancement was accompanied by a widespread increase of Theta activity (Kirov et al., 2009). Thus, the hypothesized correlation between the Theta frequency and the Minimal Self could help in understanding differences in states of awareness along the sleep-wakefulness continuum, and to further expanding its currently dichotomous definition into more subtle distinctions between different wakefulness states.

The Importance of Differentiating Between Altered and Higher States of Consciousness

So far, we have demonstrated how the hypothesized model is in alignment with different meditation traditions. In addition, we further suggested that the current hypothesized electro-topography could be useful in guiding new contemplative neuroscience investigations into differentiating Altered States (AS) from Higher States (HS) of consciousness. Altered States (AS) are traditionally defined as “a qualitative alteration in the overall pattern of mental functioning, such that the experiencer feels consciousness radically differently from the ‘normal’ way it functions. It should be noted that an AS is not defined by a particular content of consciousness, behavior, or physiologic change, but in terms of overall patterning.” (Tart, 1972; for a review on neurobiological basis of AS, see also Vaitl et al., 2005).

However, not all AS are alike. While higher states of consciousness are accompanied by improved executive functionality, the opposite is true for many cases of drug-induced AS. Moreover, electrophysiologically, HS are accompanied by slower frequency bands and are considered more integrated states (Mason et al., 1997), while EEG and MRI studies of drug-induced AS report an opposite trend that could be regarded as fragmentation and consequent deregulation.

Moreover, an important distinction must be made between general AS and HS. According to Tart (1972), higher states, in particular, can be associated with superior cognitive functioning or can be more profound than other states; these states can include insights into oneself, insights into others, intuitive understanding of the nature of the universe, or comprehension of an individual’s place in the overall scheme of things (in relation to insight, see also Section The Sphere Model of Consciousness and Self Are Dynamic below). Although this description is phenomenologically exhaustive, it is not sufficient (as acknowledged by the author) to distinguish “any unequivocally higher state” Electrophysiology, therefore, could be a reliable method to reach a more precise discrimination between HS and AS.

Electrophysiological studies and the proposed model can, indeed, help in differentiating between AS and HS of consciousness. Although this is not the aim of the paper, we will focus briefly on the differentiation between contemplative-induced HS of consciousness versus drug-induced AS, in order to provide a preliminary framework. To examine our hypothesis, we reviewed the literature from the perspective of its applicability to SMC electro-topography. We found that the main difference between AS and HS is primarily in Alpha and higher frequencies: HS are consistently related to increased Alpha activity while AS displays reduced Alpha activity in this EEG band (see Table 5). Taken together, in HS, deep states of meditation and attention are achieved through discipline and focus which largely involve EF training. While achievement of an AS is easier, often being driven by consumed substances, there is a lesser degree of control over the whole experience. This lesser degree of intentional and skillful navigation of the experiences could undermine the replicability and increase possible dangers related to drug-induced AS compared to the safety and replicability of HS. A good example for this, would be a professional athlete or dancer that can intentionally and reliably perform difficult and potentially dangerous movements compared to a novice that perform the same actions by chance, risking injuries and without being able to replicate or even know how that happened. This example is also quite fitting to the findings of Fink et al. (2009), who found increased Alpha activity in professional dancers compared to novices during mental creation of dances.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Summary of examined studies regarding Alpha Frequency in Altered States (AS) and Higher States (HS) differentiation.

To sum up, HS could be linked to increased Alpha activity because these states entail effort and inhibition in controlling the process, while AS could be related to disinhibition and loss of control as reflected by overall decreased Alpha activity. This could be explained by the proposed electro-topography model as the result of increased internally directed attention and embodiment (Minimal Self) during the process of achieving HS, which is lacking during a drug-induced AS.

Limitations and Considerations in the Interpretation of the Model

Despite the supporting evidence, there are limitations to the proposed model that should be acknowledged. First, the electrotopographic model may seem oversimplified because, as depicted, the examined frequency bands often underlie a wide range of different cognitive functions. However, we are not suggesting that a given meditation type, for instance, is exclusively associated with one frequency band’s activity, but rather we hypothesize that a specific meditation type relies more on that frequency than others. Of necessity, the graphic depiction of this requires some simplification.

In addition, there are two patterns of results that apparently contradict to our hypothesis. First, regarding our hypothesis that the Minimal Self primarily involves Alpha and Theta frequencies, paradoxically, some findings in the literature have reported increased Alpha and Theta activity during mind-wandering (Rodriguez-Larios et al., 2020; Rodriguez-Larios and Alaerts, 2021), which is related to the Narrative Self. However, this could also be attributed to retrieval and manipulation of memory information (Bastiaansen et al., 2002; Jensen et al., 2002; Sauseng et al., 2002, 2004, 2005a,2005b; Moran et al., 2010; see also Staresina and Wimber, 2019 for a review). Second, increased Gamma/decreased Delta during deep meditation could be related to the central role that Gamma plays in brain mechanisms underlying information processing (Lehmann et al., 2001). Increased Gamma activity could also be related to the spiritual/religious/mystic aspects of the meditative state (Beauregard et al., 2009) and therefore to higher states of consciousness with fullness of content, while lower frequency bands (Delta, Theta, and Alpha) could be related to states of thoughtless emptiness (meaning higher yet content-less states). Perhaps further discrimination between “deeper” states, taking into consideration not only the level of consciousness involved but also the content of those states, would be worthwhile to explore in future research.

A further aspect to consider in future research is the strong relationship among phenomenological experiences, EFs, and electrophysiology that have been reported across many studies exploring the concept of Self. Future studies should explore these relationships in a systematic way, taking into consideration different styles of meditation (see Gallant, 2016 for a review examining EFs with regard to mindfulness meditation).

What is more, as mentioned in the Introduction, the subjective dimension of self-awareness remains inescapable. For instance, based on Raffone et al. (2019) review, we can argument about the possibility of different levels or grades of self-awareness within each dimension of Self, namely, we might observe Beta frequency in a state of self-projection as well in FA meditation with good self-awareness.

The Sphere Model of Consciousness and Self Are Dynamic

The proposed model is dynamic in several noteworthy ways. As the Self is dynamic, so might one oscillate between meditative states during meditation practice. For example, movement between FA and OM often occurs (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021). In the same way, mechanisms and functions underlying the Self could also be dynamic: an active and intentional effort could dynamically restructure self-awareness, consciousness, and EFs in different ways. Indeed, two of the most studied phenomena that share some form of self-awareness and restructuring with meditation are cognitive reappraisal5 and insight.6

Many studies identify an association between Gamma and the process of achieving deeper states of meditation, as well as with the degree of visual imagery involved in these states. In particular, the Gamma band could be associated with the hyper-focused attention required to achieve non-dual states, but not with the non-dual state itself (Berman and Stevens, 2015). This could be due to the relationship between Gamma, and meditation, cognitive reappraisal, and insight7 (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004): These processes rely on a restructuring of awareness producing similar effects (e.g., sudden and clear solutions; new perception of emotional stimuli). However, these could also be reached through different mechanisms: active and intentional efforts to solve a problem rely, at least partly, on executive functions (Sandkühler and Bhattacharya, 2008; Schmeichel et al., 2008). Meditation, in contrast, may rely on deeper and more self-less states that involve less conceptual processing.

From an electrophysiological point of view, as discussed earlier, different investigations have found decreased prefrontal Alpha activity during reappraisal (Parvaz et al., 2012; Choi et al., 2016; Li et al., 2021) in association with increased prefrontal Theta activity (Ertl et al., 2013). Possible generators of frontal Theta oscillations have been suggested to reside in the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC)/anterior cingulate complex (ACC) (Pizzagalli et al., 2002; Mulert et al., 2007a,b). However, although the ACC is notably one of the areas most associated with insight (Dietrich and Kanso, 2010), it is thought to prompt weak, subconscious solutions (Kounios and Beeman, 2009).

What could be the reason for these similarities in brain activity? One reason for shared areas and electrophysiological correlates is that both cognitive reappraisal and insight have been associated with core EFs, such as working memory processes (Schmeichel et al., 2008). For example, frontal Theta activity in the ACC has been found to increase with task difficulty and memory demands (Gevins et al., 1997; Lazarev, 1998; Kahana et al., 1999; Wilson et al., 1999; Krause et al., 2000; but see Bastiaansen et al., 2002). Successful retrieval is also associated with activity in the Theta band (Weiss and Rappelsberger, 2000; Klimesch et al., 2001; Sederberg et al., 2003). Thus, such similarities could result from the involvement of wider, more global functions that underlie all the Self states.

Conclusion

In conclusion, to our knowledge, this is the first attempt to systematically integrate electrophysiological accounts with different Selves and meditation practices. Our core proposal is that there is an electro-topographic hierarchy of the Selves that mirrors the hierarchy of meditation types. We have shown a parallel between FA/OM/ND and NS/MS/OCS in which Gamma and Beta = the Narrative Self and Focused Attention meditation; Alpha and Theta = the Minimal Self and Open Monitoring meditation; and Delta = Overcoming of the Self and Non-Dual meditation. Our evidence should be viewed as preliminary since these are complex processes that involve many different areas, networks, and frequency bands all at once. Future studies should examine the parallel between the electrophysiological change and the neurophenomenological shift between Selves, employing different training paradigms that utilize whole brain analysis of different frequency bands.

Moreover, we also highlighted how the frequency bands involved in both hierarchies are associated with EFs. Future behavioral and electrophysiological studies should aim to assess how EFs in general, and their components in specific, could play a critical role in structuring the Self and in the switching between different states (for a very recent review on mapping EFs and their components using electrical subcortical stimulation, see Landers et al., 2021).

Additionally, given the aforementioned role of neuronal noise in “identity confusion” (Sugimura et al., 2021) and the lack of it in feelings of identity consistency and integration, future studies should also explore possible parallels between phenomenological and electrophysiological silence, which is a very common feature in meditative practices (for empirical and theoretical contributions to neural, psychological, and contemplative correlates of silence, see Ben-Soussan et al., 2021).

Although a comprehensive electrophysiological model encompassing all frequency bands and their dynamics—including detailed temporal and spatial localization evidence—is still far from being achieved, there are relevant findings in recent literature that offer potential direction for future studies. For example, a recent work of Rodriguez-Larios et al. (2020) examined the Alpha-Theta cross-frequency coupling and considered these two frequency bands and their dynamics during meditation. This is particularly interesting because authors not only examined Alpha and Theta bands from the point of view of a linear increased/decreased activity, but also how reciprocal changes in these frequencies’ activity (especially their ratio) could facilitate meditation training. Such dynamics were also explored in the field of neurofeedback, in which the importance of the Alpha/Theta ratio in relation to creativity and artistic performance was emphasized (Gruzelier, 2009; Gruzelier et al., 2014a,b). These findings may offer directions for expanding and refining our proposed electro-topography. Moreover, these directions should orient and guide the field of consciousness studies and contemplative neuroscience, which should systematically start reporting all the different frequency bands and to integrate evidence concerning electroencephalographic dynamics into the various models that currently exist.

In summary, based on the presented evidence, we suggest that the Narrative Self may be related to reduced self-awareness in the “here and now” compared to the Minimal Self that is more mindful. Further, shifting between the Narrative Self to the Minimal Self correlates with an increase in self-awareness which, in turn, correlates with slower frequency bands, namely, Alpha and Theta oscillation (Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020). The third state of Overcoming of the Self correlates with increasingly slower frequency bands, namely, Delta. Similarly, different kinds of meditations align with the same electrophysiological hierarchy. However, this does not necessarily imply that one meditation is better than another in enhancing self-awareness, only that there is a gradual shift in self-awareness’ phenomenology (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021).

Our proposed model may aid in resolving some inconsistencies between different frequency bands and locations pointed to in previous studies as the main correlates of consciousness and meditative practices. According to our proposal, these different frequency bands and locations may result from the different attentional states and related Selves that were examined. The model can be tested and applied in different contexts such as in the examination of advanced meditative states, higher states versus altered states, and levels of wakefulness. Accordingly, this new electro-topographic framework of the Self and meditation may more easily facilitate greater understanding of the connections between them, with implications for research in wakefulness states, altered states of consciousness, and executive functions.

Author Contributions

PP was the creator of the Sphere Model of Consciousness and contributed parts related to the neuro-psychological applications of his model. TB-S mostly contributed to the electro-topographic map of the self. RL mostly contributed parts related to the executive functions. MP conducted the mini-review. All authors contributed to the article hypothesis, writing, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ In relation to this, in certain meditation practices, attention must eventually be released in favor of a form of bare or non-preferential attention, while in non-dual meditation practices, attention is released altogether (Dunne, 2011) as it claims to employ reflexive awareness that permits non-dual witnessing of the subject-object dichotomy and results in a pure (non-dual) awareness. In fact, according to some traditions, our inability to ordinarily detect non-dual awareness “is due to an obscuration of this reflexive property by mistaken cognitions arising from substrate consciousness. Although NDA [non-dual awareness] is experienced in meditation as a vivid presence of empty awareness that knows Itself directly without mediation by conceptual thought, substrate consciousness is experienced as a pleasantly restful absorbed state, akin to deep sleep yet not entirely unconscious” (Josipovic, 2014, pp. 2). In addition, the voluntary “letting go” in “states of flow,” a transitory state between the MN and OCS, is related to altered states of consciousness (ASC). This deliberate reduction in the use of executive processes can also occur in the experience of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Flow may result from a voluntary act occurring in both meditation and sports, a tuning in that involves “letting go” of goal (Jackson and Csikszentmihalyi, 1999; Lutz, 2009; Brown and Leary, 2016; Paoletti et al., 2020). It is important to note that alpha activity has been found to be related to the experience of flow (Katahira et al., 2018) and considering the involvement of alpha frequency both in flow and as main difference between Altered States and Higher States, this voluntary “letting go” could be critical in discriminating between these states of consciousness.
  2. ^ Another reason for the similarities between electrophysiological correlates of the electro-topography of the Selves and meditation types could be related to the different executive functions required for each of these states, which may be related to different levels of self-awareness, as we have seen in Section “The Sphere Model of Consciousness, Hierarchy of Selves, and Executive Functions.”
  3. ^ In particular, neural oscillations can be evaluated in a local region or between various regions of the brain. The strength of a particular oscillatory frequency in a particular region can be analyzed using a power analysis. Coherence, on the other hand, is the degree of coupling of a particular frequency between two different brain regions and can be used as an indicator of functional connectivity. EEG and MEG can both be used to investigate power and coherence within a particular frequency band.
  4. ^ FA meditation requires voluntary focusing attention on a chosen object, OM meditation involves non-reactive monitoring of the content of experience from moment to moment (Lutz et al., 2009), and ND meditation aims for awareness that is unchanging regardless of what happens in experience (Josipovic, 2010; Dunne, 2011; Metzinger, 2020). ND awareness is characterized by its reflexive property—it knows itself to be conscious without relying on conceptual cognition (Josipovic, 2014; Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021). These meditation types also reflect different neural activity. FA, such as in the case of Tibetan Buddhist, Buddhist, and Chinese practices (Travis et al., 2010), has been mostly associated with gamma and beta activity (Hinterberger et al., 2014), OM with theta activity (Lee et al., 2018) and ND with delta activity (Knyazev, 2012; Berman and Stevens, 2015).
  5. ^ Reappraisal refers to the act of changing the way one thinks about emotional events (for a review, see John and Gross, 2004). In particular, cognitive reappraisal strategies have been investigated mainly in relation to aversive stimuli (Ochsner et al., 2004; Banks et al., 2007; Kanske et al., 2011; Schulze et al., 2011). This cognitive strategy for emotional regulation has been found to be strongly related to contemplative practices. Garland et al. (2009, 2010, 2011) and Garland and Howard (2013) theorized that mindfulness training can facilitate cognitive reappraisal. Moreover, Hanley et al. (2014) demonstrated that a substantial proportion of practitioners who use a wide range of contemplative practices reportedly engaged in mindful reappraisal techniques as a means of coping with serious stressors or the hassles of everyday life.
  6. ^ Insight is a sudden comprehension, the famous “Eureka!” that can result in a new interpretation, a reorganization or restructuring of the elements of a situation and can point to the solution to a problem (Sternberg and Davidson, 1995). This definition seems to be very similar to the following description of a sudden transformation occurring during meditation: “A common experience for long term meditators is that the ‘new’ knowledge being encoded is, paradoxically, the experience of a thought-free state transforming all phenomena into novelty. Phenomena become new because “there is no succession of perception of this knowledge; it takes in all things simultaneously, at a glance” (Nikhilananda, 1956, pp. 203; from Horan, 2009).
  7. ^ This could also explain the difference in Gamma between experienced and novice meditators (i.e., less Gamma = less effort among experienced meditators in achieving a non-dual state). For example, long-term Vipassana meditation contributes to increased occipital Gamma power and is related to long-term meditational expertise and enhanced sensory awareness (Cahn et al., 2010).

References

Abdullah, A. A., and Omar, Z. (2011). The effect of temporal EEG signals while listening to Quran recitation. Int. J. Adv. Sci. Eng. Inform. Technol. 1, 372–375. doi: 10.18517/ijaseit.1.4.77

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aftanas, L., and Golosheykin, S. (2005). Impact of regular meditation practice on EEG activity at rest and during evoked negative emotions. Int. J. Neurosci. 115, 893–909. doi: 10.1080/00207450590897969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aftanas, L. I., and Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: high-resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neurosci. Lett. 310, 57–60. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02094-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, C. N., and Sands, D. (1993). “Meditation and relaxation,” in McGill’s Guide of the Social Sciences: Psychology, ed. F. N. McGill (Pasadena, CA: Salem Press), 1499–1505.

Google Scholar

Al-Galal, S. A., and Alshaikhli, I. F. T. (2017). Analyzing brainwaves while listening to quranic recitation compared with listening to music based on EEG signals. Int. J. Percept. Cogn. Comput. 3, 1–5.

Google Scholar

Anderson, V. (2002). Executive function in children: introduction. Child Neuropsychol. 8, 69–70. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.69.8725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A. (2016). Is “self-consciousness” equivalent to “executive function”? Psychol. Neurosci. 9:215. doi: 10.1037/pne0000052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baerentsen, K. B., Hartvig, N. V., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., and Mammen, J. (2001). Onset of meditation explored with fMRI. Neuroimage 6:297. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(01)91640-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baijal, S., and Srinivasan, N. (2010). Theta activity and meditative states: spectral changes during concentrative meditation. Cogn. Process. 11, 31–38. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0272-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., and Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala–frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 303–312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banquet, J. P. (1973). Spectral analysis of the EEG in meditation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 35, 143–151. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(73)90170-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barcelona, J., Fahlman, M., Churakova, Y., Canjels, R., Mallare, J., and Van Den Heuvel, M. I. (2020). Frontal alpha asymmetry during prayerful and resting states: an EEG study in Catholic sisters. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 155, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychol. Rev. 11, 1–29. doi: 10.1023/a:1009085417776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 361–372. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, M. C., Posthuma, D., Groot, P. F., and De Geus, E. J. (2002). Event-related alpha and theta responses in a visuo-spatial working memory task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1882–1893. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00303-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, M., Courtemanche, J., and Paquette, V. (2009). Brain activity in near-death experiencers during a meditative state. Resuscitation 80, 1006–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2009.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M. (2018). “Internally directed attention in creative cognition,” in The Cambridge Handbook of the Neuroscience of Creativity, eds R. E. Jung and O. Vartanian (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 180–194. doi: 10.1017/9781316556238.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Avirame, K., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Harpaz, Y., and Ben-Shachar, M. (2014a). Changes in cerebellar activity and inter-hemispheric coherence accompany improved reading performance following Quadrato motor training. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:81. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2014b). A suspended act: increased reflectivity and gender-dependent electrophysiological change following Quadrato Motor Training. Front. Psychol. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., Ohana, A. B., Donchin, O., and Goldstein, A. (2011). Step in time: changes in EEG coherence during a time estimation task following Quadrato Motor Training. Proc. Fechner Day 27, 239–244.

Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., De Fano, A., Mauro, F., Marson, F., Modica, M., et al. (2019b). Embodied time: time production in advanced Quadrato and Aikido practitioners. PsyCh J. 8, 8–16. doi: 10.1002/pchj.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Mauro, F., Lasaponara, S., Glicksohn, J., Marson, F., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2019a). Fully immersed: state absorption and electrophysiological effects of the OVO whole-body perceptual deprivation chamber. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 165–184. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Soussan, T. D., Srinivasan, N., Glicksohn, J., Beziau, J. Y., Carducci, F., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2021). Neurophysiology of silence: neuroscientific, psychological, educational and contemplative perspectives. Front. Psychol. 12:675614. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front. Psychol. 4:912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., Ben-Soussan, T. D., and Goldstein, A. (2017). Creativity is enhanced by long-term mindfulness training and is negatively correlated with trait default-mode-related low-gamma inter-hemispheric connectivity. Mindfulness 8, 717–727. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0649-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2012). Mindfulness-induced changes in gamma band activity–implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 700–710. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.07.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, A. E., and Stevens, L. (2015). EEG manifestations of nondual experiences in meditators. Conscious. Cogn. 31, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bortolan, A. (2020). Affectivity and the distinction between minimal and narrative self. Continent. Philos. Rev. 53, 67–84. doi: 10.1111/mcn.13141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braboszcz, C., Cahn, B. R., Levy, J., Fernandez, M., and Delorme, A. (2017). Increased gamma brainwave amplitude compared to control in three different meditation traditions. PLoS One 12:e0170647. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, W. B., Lindahl, J. R., Cahn, B. R., Davis, J. H., and Goldman, R. E. (2014). Awakening is not a metaphor: the effects of Buddhist meditation practices on basic wakefulness. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1307:64. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Leary, M. R. (eds) (2016). The Oxford Handbook of Hypo-Egoic Phenomena. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., and Polich, J. (2010). Occipital gamma activation during Vipassana meditation. Cogn. Process. 11, 39–56. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0352-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., and Polich, J. (2013). Event-related delta, theta, alpha and gamma correlates to auditory oddball processing during Vipassana meditation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 100–111. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Rucker, J., Day, C. M., Erritzoe, D., Kaelen, M., et al. (2016). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 619–627.

Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., Shanahan, M., Feilding, A., Tagliazucchi, E., et al. (2014). The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:20. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, G. L., Travis, F., and Smith, J. (2014). Policing and spirituality: their impact on brain integration and consciousness. J. Manag. Spirit. Relig. 11, 230–244. doi: 10.1080/14766086.2014.887479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, A. C., Feng, W., Zhao, H., Yin, Y., and Wang, P. (2008). EEG default mode network in the human brain: spectral regional field powers. Neuroimage 41, 561–574. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, D., Sekiya, T., Minote, N., and Watanuki, S. (2016). Relative left frontal activity in reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion: evidence from frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA). Int. J. Psychophysiol. 109, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cona, G., Chiossi, F., Di Tomasso, S., Pellegrino, G., Piccione, F., Bisiacchi, P., et al. (2020). Theta and alpha oscillations as signatures of internal and external attention to delayed intentions: a magnetoencephalography (MEG) study. NeuroImage 205:116295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J., Burgess, A. P., and Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00107-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

DeLosAngeles, D., Williams, G., Burston, J., Fitzgibbon, S. P., Lewis, T. W., Grummett, T. S., et al. (2016). Electroencephalographic correlates of states of concentrative meditation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 110, 27–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dentico, D., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B. A., Smith, R., Zennig, C., Lutz, A., et al. (2016). Short meditation trainings enhance non-REM sleep low-frequency oscillations. PLoS One 11:e0148961. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depraz, N., Varela, F. J., and Vermersch, P. (2000). “The gesture of awareness: an account of its structural dynamics,” in Investigating Phenomenal Consciousness, ed. M. Velmans (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 121–136. doi: 10.1075/aicr.13.10dep

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168.

Google Scholar

Dietrich, A., and Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychol. Bull. 136:822. doi: 10.1037/a0019749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:582. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotan Ben-Soussan, T., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Berkovich-Ohana, A., and Donchin, O. (2013). Into the square and out of the box: the effects of Quadrato Motor Training on creativity and alpha coherence. PLoS One 8:e55023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doufesh, H., Faisal, T., Lim, K. S., and Ibrahim, F. (2012). EEG spectral analysis on muslim prayers. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 37, 11–18. doi: 10.1007/s10484-011-9170-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doufesh, H., Ibrahim, F., Ismail, N. A., and Wan Ahmad, W. A. (2014). Effect of Muslim prayer (Salat) on α electroencephalography and its relationship with autonomic nervous system activity. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 20, 558–562. doi: 10.1089/acm.2013.0426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. R., Hartigan, J. A., and Mikulas, W. L. (1999). Concentration and mindfulness meditations: unique forms of consciousness? Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 24, 147–165. doi: 10.1023/a:1023498629385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunne, J. (2011). Toward an understanding of non-dual mindfulness. Contemp. Buddhism 12, 71–88. doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564820

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebert, U., Grossmann, M., Oertel, R., Gramatté, T., and Kirch, W. (2001). Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling of the electroencephalogram effects of scopolamine in healthy volunteers. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 41, 51–60. doi: 10.1177/00912700122009836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis (No. 7). New York, NY: WW Norton & company.

Google Scholar

Ertl, M., Hildebrandt, M., Ourina, K., Leicht, G., and Mulert, C. (2013). Emotion regulation by cognitive reappraisal—the role of frontal theta oscillations. NeuroImage 81, 412–421. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, P. L., Lehmann, D., Gianotti, L. R., Milz, P., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Held, M., et al. (2015). Zazen meditation and no-task resting EEG compared with LORETA intracortical source localization. Cogn. Process. 16, 87–96. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0637-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, P. L., Steiner, M. E., Lehmann, D., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Jancke, L., Esslen, M., et al. (2008). Deactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex in experienced Zen meditators. Brain Topogr. 20:172.

Google Scholar

Fell, J., Axmacher, N., and Haupt, S. (2010). From alpha to gamma: electrophysiological correlates of meditation-related states of consciousness. Med. Hypotheses 75, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2010.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., Fernandez, G., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., and Fries, P. (2003). Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention? Brain Res. Rev. 42, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00178-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., and Kallio-Tamminen, T. (2020). Selfhood triumvirate: from phenomenology to brain activity and back again. Conscious. Cogn. 86:103031. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, A., Graif, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain correlates underlying creative thinking: EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers. NeuroImage 46, 854–862. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, M. (1969). EEG and human psychopharmacology. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. 9, 241–258. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.09.040169.001325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flor-Henry, P., Shapiro, Y., and Sombrun, C. (2017). Brain changes during a shamanic trance: altered modes of consciousness, hemispheric laterality, and systemic psychobiology. Cogent. Psychol. 4:1313522. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1313522

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friend, M., and Bates, R. P. (2014). The union of narrative and executive function: different but complementary. Front. Psychol. 5:469. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01417-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S., and Zahavi, D. (2020). The Phenomenological Mind. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Gallant, S. N. (2016). Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: breaking down the benefit. Conscious. Cogn. 40, 116–130. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, E., Gaylord, S., and Park, J. (2009). The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal. Explore 5, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, E. L., Fredrickson, B. L., Kring, A. M., Johnson, D. P., Meyer, P. S., and Penn, D. L. (2010). Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: insights from the broaden and build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 849–864. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, E. L., Gaylord, S. A., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2011). Positive reappraisal coping mediates the stress-reductive effect of mindfulness: an upward spiral process. Mindfulness 2, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, E. L., and Howard, M. O. (2013). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement reduces pain attentional bias in chronic pain patients. Psychother. Psychosom. 82, 311–318. doi: 10.1159/000348868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevins, A., Smith, M. E., McEvoy, L., and Yu, D. (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cerebr. Cortex 7, 374–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.4.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glicksohn, J., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2020). Immersion, absorption, and spiritual experience: some preliminary findings. Front. Psychol. 11:2118. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glicksohn, J., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2011). From trance to transcendence: a neurocognitive approach. J. Mind Behav. 32, 49–62.

Google Scholar

Glicksohn, J., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2012). “Absorption, immersion, and consciousness,” in Video Game Play and Consciousness, ed. J. Gackenbach (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 83–99.

Google Scholar

Glicksohn, J., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Mauro, F., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2019). Individual EEG alpha profiles are gender-dependent and indicate subjective experiences in whole-body perceptual deprivation. Neuropsychologia 125, 81–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grieger, R. M. (1985). From a linear to a contextual model of the ABCs of RET. J. Ratio. Emot. Ther. 3, 75–99.

Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. (2009). A theory of alpha/theta neurofeedback, creative performance enhancement, long distance functional connectivity and psychological integration. Cogn. Process. 10, 101–109. doi: 10.1007/s10339-008-0248-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H., Foks, M., Steffert, T., Chen, M. L., and Ros, T. (2014a). Beneficial outcome from EEG-neurofeedback on creative music performance, attention and well-being in school children. Biol. Psychol. 95, 86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H., Thompson, T., Redding, E., Brandt, R., and Steffert, T. (2014b). Application of alpha/theta neurofeedback and heart rate variability training to young contemporary dancers: state anxiety and creativity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanley, A., Garland, E. L., and Black, D. S. (2014). Use of mindful reappraisal coping among meditation practitioners. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 294–301. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 363–390. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinterberger, T., Schmidt, S., Kamei, T., and Walach, H. (2014). Decreased electrophysiological activity represents the conscious state of emptiness in meditation. Front. Psychol. 5:99. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180.

Google Scholar

Horan, R. (2009). The neuropsychological connection between creativity and meditation. Creativ. Res. J. 21, 199–222. doi: 10.1037/hop0000035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huels, E. R., Kim, H., Lee, U., Bel-Bahar, T., Colmenero, A. V., Nelson, A., et al. (2021). Neural correlates of the shamanic state of consciousness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:140. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.610466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanovski, B., and Malhi, G. S. (2007). The psychological and neurophysiological concomitants of mindfulness forms of meditation. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 19, 76–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00175.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in Sports. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York, NY: Dover.

Google Scholar

Jensen, O., Gelfand, J., Kounios, J., and Lisman, J. E. (2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9–12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cerebr. Cortex 12, 877–882. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerath, R., Crawford, M. W., and Barnes, V. A. (2015). A unified 3D default space consciousness model combining neurological and physiological processes that underlie conscious experience. Front. Psychol. 6:1204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., and Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. J. Person. 72, 1301–1334. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josipovic, Z. (2010). Duality and nonduality in meditation research. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 1119–1121. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josipovic, Z. (2014). Neural correlates of nondual awareness in meditation. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1307, 9–18. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josipovic, Z. (2019). Nondual awareness: consciousness-as-such as non-representational reflexivity. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 273–298. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., et al. (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biol. 2:e97. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahana, M. J., Sekuler, R., Caplan, J. B., Kirschen, M., and Madsen, J. R. (1999). Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature 399, 781–784. doi: 10.1038/21645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., Heissler, J., Schönfelder, S., Bongers, A., and Wessa, M. (2011). How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1379–1388. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasamatsu, A., and Hirai, T. (1966). An electroencephalographic study on the Zen meditation (Zazen). Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 20, 315–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1966.tb02646.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katahira, K., Yamazaki, Y., Yamaoka, C., Ozaki, H., Nakagawa, S., and Nagata, N. (2018). EEG correlates of the flow state: a combination of increased frontal theta and moderate frontocentral alpha rhythm in the mental arithmetic task. Front. Psychol. 9:300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, C. E., Jones, S. R., Wan, Q., Pritchett, D. L., Wasserman, R. H., Wexler, A., et al. (2011). Effects of mindfulness meditation training on anticipatory alpha modulation in primary somatosensory cortex. Brain Res. Bull. 85, 96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., and Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirov, R., Weiss, C., Siebner, H. R., Born, J., and Marshall, L. (2009). Slow oscillation electrical brain stimulation during waking promotes EEG theta activity and memory encoding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 15460–15465. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904438106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2011). Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: the P1 inhibition timing hypothesis. Brain Res. 1408, 52–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Stadler, W., Pöllhuber, D., Sauseng, P., and Röhm, D. (2001). Episodic retrieval is reflected by a process specific increase in human electroencephalographic theta activity. Neurosci. Lett. 302, 49–52. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01656-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knyazev, G. G. (2012). EEG delta oscillations as a correlate of basic homeostatic and motivational processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 677–695. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kometer, M., Pokorny, T., Seifritz, E., and Volleinweider, F. X. (2015). Psilocybin-induced spiritual experiences and insightfulness are associated with synchronization of neuronal oscillations. Psychopharmacology 232, 3663–3676. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4026-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kounios, J., and Beeman, M. (2009). The Aha! moment: the cognitive neuroscience of insight. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 18, 210–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01638.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, C. M., Sillanmäki, L., Koivisto, M., Saarela, C., Häggqvist, A., Laine, M., et al. (2000). The effects of memory load on event-related EEG desynchronization and synchronization. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 2071–2078. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00429-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubota, Y., Sato, W., Toichi, M., Murai, T., Okada, T., Hayashi, A., et al. (2001). Frontal midline theta rhythm is correlated with cardiac autonomic activities during the performance of an attention demanding meditation procedure. Cogn. Brain Res. 11, 281–287. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00086-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landers, M. J., Sitskoorn, M. M., Rutten, G. J. M., Mandonnet, E., and De Baene, W. (2021). A systematic review of the use of subcortical intraoperative electrical stimulation mapping for monitoring of executive deficits and neglect: what is the evidence so far? Acta Neurochirurgica 164, 177–191. doi: 10.1007/s00701-021-05012-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, T. J. (2020). The minimal self hypothesis. Conscious. Cogn. 85:103029. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasaponara, S., Mauro, F., Ben-Soussan, T. D., Carducci, F., Tombini, M., Quattrocchi, C. C., et al. (2016). Electrophysiological indexes of eyes open and closed resting states conditions following the Quadrato Motor Training. Int. J. Bioelectromagn. 18, 99–108.

Google Scholar

Lasaponara, S., Mauro, F., Carducci, F., Paoletti, P., Tombini, M., Quattrocchi, C. C., et al. (2017). Increased alpha band functional connectivity following the Quadrato Motor Training: a longitudinal study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:282. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufs, H., Kleinschmidt, A., Beyerle, A., Eger, E., Salek-Haddadi, A., Preibisch, C., et al. (2003a). EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity. Neuroimage 19, 1463–1476. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00286-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufs, H., Krakow, K., Sterzer, P., Eger, E., Beyerle, A., Salek-Haddadi, A., et al. (2003b). Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 11053–11058. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1831638100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laukkonen, R. E., and Slagter, H. A. (2021). From many to (n) one: meditation and the plasticity of the predictive mind. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 199–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarev, V. V. (1998). On the intercorrelation of some frequency and amplitude parameters of the human EEG and its functional significance. Communication I: multidimensional neurodynamic organization of functional states of the brain during intellectual, perceptive and motor activity in normal subjects. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 28, 77–98. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00068-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D. J., Kulubya, E., Goldin, P., Goodarzi, A., and Girgis, F. (2018). Review of the neural oscillations underlying meditation. Front. Neurosci. 12:178. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrand, D. (2006). The bodily self: the sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 5, 89–118.

Google Scholar

Legrand, D. (2007). Pre-reflective self-as-subject from experiential and empirical perspectives. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 583–599.

Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R., and Pizzagalli, D. (2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 108, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00116-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Tei, S., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Milz, P., and Kochi, K. (2012). Reduced functional connectivity between cortical sources in five meditation traditions detected with lagged coherence using EEG tomography. Neuroimage 60, 1574–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leshem, R., De Fano, A., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2020). The implications of motor and cognitive inhibition for hot and cool executive functions: the case of quadrato motor training. Front. Psychol. 11:940. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Li, Y., and Cao, D. (2021). The effectiveness of emotion cognitive reappraisal as measured by self-reported response and its link to EEG alpha asymmetry. Behav. Brain Res. 400:113042. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, P. C., Huang, M. L., and Chang, K. M. (2003). EEG alpha blocking correlated with perception of inner light during Zen meditation. Am. J. Chinese Med. 31, 629–642. doi: 10.1142/S0192415X03001272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., and Fu, C. H. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 401–410. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., and Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 16369–16373. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407401101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169.

Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., Greischar, L. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: neural and behavioral evidence. J. Neurosci. 29, 13418–13427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1614-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, J. (2009). Flow and sense of coherence: two aspects of the same dynamic? Glob. Health Promot. 16, 63–67. doi: 10.1177/1757975909339774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Perrucci, M. G., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170–13175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700668104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., Molle, M., and Born, J. (2006). Oscillating current stimulation–slow oscillation stimulation during sleep. [preprint]. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzetti, L., Di Lanzo, C., Zappasodi, F., Chella, F., Raffone, A., and Pizzella, V. (2014). Magnetoencephalographic alpha band connectivity reveals differential default mode network interactions during focused attention and open monitoring meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:832. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, L. I., Alexander, C. N., Travis, F. T., Marsh, G., Orme-Johnson, D. W., Gackenbach, J., et al. (1997). Electrophysiological correlates of higher states of consciousness during sleep in long-term: practitioners of the transcendental meditation program. Sleep 20, 102–110. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.2.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzinger, T. (2018). “Why is mind wandering interesting for philosophers,” in The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming, eds K. C. R. Fox and K. Christoff (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 97–111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzinger, T. (2020). Minimal phenomenal experience: meditation, tonic alertness, and the phenomenology of “pure” consciousness. Philos. Mind Sci. 1, 1–44. doi: 10.33735/phimisci.2020.i.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, R. J., Campo, P., Maestu, F., Reilly, R. B., Dolan, R. J., and Strange, B. A. (2010). Peak frequency in the theta and alpha bands correlates with human working memory capacity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:200. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulert, C., Juckel, G., Brunnmeier, M., Karch, S., Leicht, G., Mergl, R., et al. (2007a). Prediction of treatment response in major depression: integration of concepts. J. Affect. Disord. 98, 215–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulert, C., Juckel, G., Brunnmeier, M., Karch, S., Leicht, G., Mergl, R., et al. (2007b). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity in the theta band predicts response to antidepressive medication. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 38, 78–81. doi: 10.1177/155005940703800209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, T., Koshino, Y., Omori, M., Murata, I., Nishio, M., Sakamoto, K., et al. (1994). Quantitative EEG study on Zen meditation (zaZen). Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 48, 881–890. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1994.tb03090.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, T., Takahashi, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Yoshida, H., et al. (2004). Individual trait anxiety levels characterizing the properties of Zen meditation. Neuropsychobiology 50, 189–194. doi: 10.1159/000079113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Moran, R. J., Brookes, M. J., Williams, T. M., Errtizoe, D., et al. (2013). Broadband cortical desynchronization underlies the human psychedelic state. J. Neurosci. 33, 15171–15183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2063-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neufeld, M. Y., Rabey, M. J., Parmet, Y., Sifris, P., Treves, T. A., and Korczyn, A. D. (1994). Effects of a single intravenous dose of scopolamine on the quantitative EEG in Alzheimer’s disease patients and age-matched controls. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, 407–412. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90162-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 361–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikhilananda, S. W. (1956). Raja Yoga. New York, NY: Ramakrishna-Vivekananda Center.

Google Scholar

Oatley, K. (2007). “Narrative modes of consciousness and selfhood,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, eds P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press), 375–402. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511816789.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23, 483–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osipova, D., Ahveninen, J., Kaakkola, S., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Huttunen, J., and Pekkonen, E. (2003). Effects of scopolamine on MEG spectral power and coherence in elderly subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 1902–1907. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00165-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paoletti, P. (2002a). Flussi, Territori, Luogo [Flows, Territories, Place]. Madeira: M.E.D. Publishing.

Google Scholar

Paoletti, P. (2002b). Flussi, Territori, Luogo II [Flows, Territories, Place II]. Madeira: M.E.D. Publishing.

Google Scholar

Paoletti, P., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2019). The sphere model of consciousness: from geometrical to neuro-psycho-educational perspectives. Logica Univ. 13, 395–415. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.548813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paoletti, P., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2020). Reflections on inner and outer silence and consciousness without contents according to the sphere model of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 11:1807. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paoletti, P., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2021). “Emotional intelligence, identification, and self-awareness according to the sphere model of consciousness,” in The Science of Emotional Intelligence, ed. S. G. Taukeni (London: Intech Open).

Google Scholar

Paoletti, P., Ben-Soussan, T. D., and Glicksohn, J. (2020). “Inner navigation and theta activity: from movement to cognition and hypnosis according to the sphere model of consciousness,” in Hypnotherapy and Hypnosis, ed. C. Mordeniz (London: Intech Open).

Google Scholar

Paoletti, P., and Salvagio, A. (2011). Osservazione. Rome: 3P Publishing.

Google Scholar

Parker, S. (2017). Yoga nidrâ: an opportunity for collaboration to extend the science of sleep states. Sleep Vigilance 1, 57–63. doi: 10.1007/s41782-017-0026-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, S., Bharati, S. V., and Fernandez, M. (2013). Defining yoga-nidra: traditional accounts, physiological research, and future directions. Int. J. Yoga Ther. 23, 11–16. doi: 10.17761/ijyt.23.1.t636651v22018148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parvaz, M. A., MacNamara, A., Goldstein, R. Z., and Hajcak, G. (2012). Event-related induced frontal alpha as a marker of lateral prefrontal cortex activation during cognitive reappraisal. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 730–740. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0107-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, F. (2009). Consciousness and self-regulation. J. Mind Behav. 30, 267–290.

Google Scholar

Pizzagalli, D., Hendrick, A. M., Horras, K. A., and Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anterior cingulate theta activity is associated with degree of treatment response in major depression. Int. Congress Ser. 1232, 711–717. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5131(01)00810-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffone, A., Marzetti, L., Del Gratta, C., Perrucci, M. G., Romani, G. L., and Pizzella, V. (2019). Toward a brain theory of meditation. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 207–232. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffone, A., and Srinivasan, N. (2009). An adaptive workspace hypothesis about the neural correlates of consciousness: insights from neuroscience and meditation studies. Prog. Brain Res. 176, 161–180. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17620-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasch, B., and Born, J. (2007). Maintaining memories by reactivation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 698–703. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riba, J., Anderer, P., Jané, F., Saletu, B., and Barbanoj, M. J. (2004). Effects of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca on regional brain electrical activity in humans: a functional neuroimaging study using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Neuropsychobiology 50, 89–101. doi: 10.1159/000077946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riba, J., Anderer, P., Morte, A., Urbano, G., Jané, F., Saletu, B., et al. (2002). Topographic pharmaco-EEG mapping of the effects of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca in healthy volunteers. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 53, 613–628. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2125.2002.01609.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Larios, J., and Alaerts, K. (2021). EEG alpha–theta dynamics during mind wandering in the context of breath focus meditation: an experience sampling approach with novice meditation practitioners. Eur. J. Neurosci. 53, 1855–1868. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Larios, J., Wong, K. F., Lim, J., and Alaerts, K. (2020). Mindfulness training is associated with changes in alpha-theta cross-frequency dynamics during meditation. Mindfulness 11, 2695–2704. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01487-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandkühler, S., and Bhattacharya, J. (2008). Deconstructing insight: EEG correlates of insightful problem solving. PLoS One 3:e1459.

Google Scholar

Sannita, W. G., Maggi, L., and Rosadini, G. (1987). Effects of scopolamine (0.25–0.75 mg im) on the quantitative EEG and the neuropsychological status of healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiology 17, 199–205. doi: 10.1159/000118365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Hanslmayr, S., Schabus, M., and Gruber, W. R. (2004). Theta coupling in the human electroencephalogram during a working memory task. Neurosci. Lett. 354, 123–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., and Hanslmayr, S. (2005a). EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Hum. Brain Mapping 26, 148–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., and Doppelmayr, M. (2005b). Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Gruber, W., Doppelmayr, M., Stadler, W., and Schabus, M. (2002). The interplay between theta and alpha oscillations in the human electroencephalogram reflects the transfer of information between memory systems. Neurosci. Lett. 324, 121–124. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00225-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., and Demaree, H. A. (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95:1526. doi: 10.1037/a0013345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulze, L., Domes, G., Krüger, A., Berger, C., Fleischer, M., Prehn, K., et al. (2011). Neuronal correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with affective instability. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 564–573. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sederberg, P. B., Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., Donner, E. J., and Madsen, J. R. (2003). Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci. 23, 10809–10814. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-34-10809.2003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., et al. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol. 5:e138. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Nieuwenhuis, S., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Theta phase synchrony and conscious target perception: impact of intensive mental training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1536–1549. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sloan, E. P., Fenton, G. W., and Standage, K. P. (1992). Anticholinergic drug effects on quantitative electroencephalogram, visual evoked potential, and verbal memory. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 600–606. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90246-v

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staresina, B. P., and Wimber, M. (2019). A neural chronometry of memory recall. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 1071–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Davidson, J. E. (1995). The Nature of Insight. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Sugimura, K., Iwasa, Y., Kobayashi, R., Honda, T., Hashimoto, J., Kashihara, S., et al. (2021). Association between long-range temporal correlations in intrinsic EEG activity and subjective sense of identity. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79444-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Symons, A. E., El-Deredy, W., Schwartze, M., and Kotz, S. A. (2016). The functional role of neural oscillations in non-verbal emotional communication. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:239. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M., et al. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 55, 199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tart, C. T. (1972). Altered States of Consciousness. New York, NY: Doubleday.

Google Scholar

Tei, S., Faber, P. L., Lehmann, D., Tsujiuchi, T., Kumano, H., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., et al. (2009). Meditators and non-meditators: EEG source imaging during resting. Brain Topogr. 22, 158–165. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0107-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomljenoviæ, H., Begiæ, D., and Maštroviæ, Z. (2016). Changes in trait brainwave power and coherence, state and trait anxiety after three-month transcendental meditation (TM) practice. Psychiatr. Danubina 28, 1–72.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Travis, F. (2001). Autonomic and EEG patterns distinguish transcending from other experiences during transcendental meditation practice. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00143-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, F., Haaga, D. A., Hagelin, J., Tanner, M., Arenander, A., Nidich, S., et al. (2010). A self-referential default brain state: patterns of coherence, power, and eLORETA sources during eyes-closed rest and Transcendental Meditation practice. Cogn. Process. 11, 21–30. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0343-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, F., Harung, H. S., and Lagrosen, Y. (2011). Moral development, executive functioning, peak experiences and brain patterns in professional and amateur classical musicians: interpreted in light of a unified theory of performance. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1256–1264. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, F., Tecce, J., Arenander, A., and Wallace, R. K. (2002). Patterns of EEG coherence, power, and contingent negative variation characterize the integration of transcendental and waking states. Biol. Psychol. 61, 293–319. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00048-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaghefi, M., Nasrabadi, A. M., Golpayegani, S. M. R. H., Mohammadi, M. R., and Gharibzadeh, S. (2015). Spirituality and brain waves. J. Med. Eng. Technol. 39, 153–158. doi: 10.3109/03091902.2014.1001528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vago, D. R., and David, S. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A., et al. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychol. Bull. 131:98. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., and Lamme, V. A. (2009). Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. J. Exp. Psychol. 35:1129. doi: 10.1037/a0013551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lutterveld, R., Houlihan, S. D., Pal, P., Sacchet, M. D., McFarlane-Blake, C., Patel, P. R., et al. (2017). Source-space EEG neurofeedback links subjective experience with brain activity during effortless awareness meditation. Neuroimage 151, 117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J. Conscious. Stud. 3, 330–349.

Google Scholar

Vithoulkas, G., and Muresanu, D. F. (2014). Conscience and consciousness: a definition. J. Med. Life 7:104.

Google Scholar

Von Stein, A., and Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 38, 301–313. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00172-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, M., Prime, H., Jenkins, J. M., Yeates, K. O., Williams, T., and Lee, K. (2018). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2119–2140. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1459-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahbeh, H., Sagher, A., Back, W., Pundhir, P., and Travis, F. (2018). A systematic review of transcendent states across meditation and contemplative traditions. Explore 14, 19–35. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2017.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, R. K., Benson, H., and Wilson, A. F. (1971). A wakeful hypometabolic physiologic state. Am. J. Physiol. Legacy Content 221, 795–799. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1971.221.3.795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, R. E., and Vaughan, F. E. (1993). Paths Beyond Ego: The Transpersonal Vision. New York, NY: Perigee Books.

Google Scholar

Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 553–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., and Rappelsberger, P. (2000). Long-range EEG synchronization during word encoding correlates with successful memory performance. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 299–312. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsh, M. C., Friedman, S. L., and Spieker, S. J. (2006). “Executive functions in developing children: current conceptualizations and questions for the future,” in Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development, eds K. McCartney and D. Phillips (Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Publishing), 167–187. doi: 10.1002/9780470757703.ch9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, G. F., Swain, C. R., and Ullsperger, P. (1999). EEG power changes during a multiple level memory retention task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 32, 107–118. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00005-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, U., LeVan, P., Borghardt, T. L., Akin, B., Wittmann, M., Leyens, Y., et al. (2020). Content-free awareness: EEG-fcMRI correlates of consciousness as such in an expert meditator. Front. Psychol. 10:3064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, N., Zink, N., Stock, A. K., and Beste, C. (2017). On the relevance of the alpha frequency oscillation’s small-world network architecture for cognitive flexibility. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14490-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, S., Kitamura, Y., Yamada, N., Nakashima, Y., and Kuroda, S. (2006). Medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in the generation of alpha activity induced by transcendental meditation: a magnetoencephalographic study. Acta Med. Okayama 60, 51–58. doi: 10.18926/AMO/30752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahavi, D., and Gallagher, S. (2008). The (in) visibility of others: a reply to Herschbach. Philos. Explor. 11, 237–244.

Google Scholar

Keywords: consciousness, self, electrophysiology, meditation, executive functions, EEG, frequency

Citation: Paoletti P, Leshem R, Pellegrino M and Ben-Soussan TD (2022) Tackling the Electro-Topography of the Selves Through the Sphere Model of Consciousness. Front. Psychol. 13:836290. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.836290

Received: 15 December 2021; Accepted: 11 March 2022;
Published: 19 May 2022.

Edited by:

Luca Simione, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies (ISTC-CNR), Italy

Reviewed by:

Hiroaki Ishida, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Japan
Sepehrdad Rahimian, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russia

Copyright © 2022 Paoletti, Leshem, Pellegrino and Ben-Soussan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tal Dotan Ben-Soussan, research@fondazionepatriziopaoletti.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.