Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 June 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Moderating Effects of Striving to Avoid Inferiority on Income and Mental Health

Updated
  • 1Graduate School of Psychology, Mejiro University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2National Center for Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Research, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3School of Health Sciences, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom

Many people experience feelings of inferiority in their life. The concept of striving to avoid inferiority is a belief associated with the unwanted fear of being overlooked, missing out on opportunities for advancement, and active rejection. This study examined the effect of striving to avoid inferiority on mental health and well-being. We hypothesized that striving to avoid inferiority would modify the relationship among socioeconomic status, mental health, and well-being, therefore examined the effect of striving to avoid inferiority on the relationship between annual income, mental health, and well-being. The participants were 491 adults (241 men and 250 women). The results indicated that insecure striving (IS) in competition with others positively correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress, whereas secure non-striving (SNS) in acceptance of inferiority positively correlated with the satisfaction with life and negatively correlated with depression. The effect of striving to avoid inferiority on the relationship among annual income, mental health, and well-being indicated that SNS affected the relationships between annual income and well-being, annual income and depression, income and anxiety, and the interaction between IS and SNS. Moreover, the relationship between income and stress was influenced by SNS and the interaction between IS and SNS. These results indicated that SNS or the interaction between IS and SNS were the only variables regulating the relationship among annual income, mental health, and well-being. These findings suggested that accepting feelings of inferiority or striving to avoid inferiority influences the mental health and well-being of people.

Introduction

Gilbert (1989, 2005a,b), and Dykman (1998) stated that people who considered themselves insecure or unsafe in social relationships were more inclined to feel inferior. This is related to the psychological pressure forcing them to make efforts to avoid undesirable situations, such as “not being noticed” and “being rejected.” Such tendencies are often discussed in the contexts of perfectionism, and many studies revealed their relations to mental health problems (Dunkley and Blankstein, 2000; Dunkley et al., 2003, 2006, 2009). These reports underpin the critical roles of fear of rejection or being overlooked and involuntary effort to avoid inferiority.

The belief that one must strive to avoid inferiority is defined as “striving to avoid inferiority” (Gilbert et al., 2007). The concept of striving to avoid inferiority is a belief associated with the fear of being overlooked, missing out on opportunities for advancement, and active rejection. It has been observed to predict depression and anxiety more than inferiority (Gilbert et al., 2007). Therefore, individuals with a high level of striving to avoid inferiority view others as rejecting and shaming. In addition, even in a situation in which an individual is aware that he or she is not inferior to others, the pressure and fear of past inferiority may activate the striving to avoid inferiority, which may threaten mental health. Oppositely, Gilbert (1989, 2005a,b), and Dykman (1998) stated that there are a certain number of people who regard themselves as accepting and helpful, do not fear inferiority or mistakes, and feel safe in social relationships.

Here, the striving to avoid inferiority has been described as a two-factor structure, called insecure striving (IS) and secure non-striving (SNS; Gilbert et al., 2007). IS is associated with being overlooked, missing out, and active rejection. Ferreira et al. (2013) observed that IS was associated with low social status and low self-esteem among women. Ferreira et al. (2013) argued that IS had a significant moderating effect on low social status and influenced thinness and dieting behaviors among women, supporting the hypothesis that the desire to lose weight arises as a competitive weapon to secure social status. In fact, a more recent study revealed IS can be a key moderator between shame and disordered eating (Ferreira and Mendes, 2020). Gilbert et al. (2007), who proposed this concept, observed that IS was associated with depression, anxiety, and stress in depressed populations, as well as feelings of inferiority, beliefs that others do not particularly value them, and beliefs that they were being looked down upon. The relationship between IS and distress is replicated in undergraduate student and athlete samples (Walton et al., 2020; To et al., 2021). Other survey studies identified that IS was correlated with daily hassles and paranoia positively, and with self-mastery negatively (Anderson and Freeman, 2013). A recent review concluded that upward mobility can be a risk for lower health state because of its competitive environment and effortful strivings (Chen et al., 2022), also suggesting IS relates to psychopathological symptoms.

Conversely, SNS is associated with feelings of acceptance of oneself and by others, regardless of success or failure, whether one has accomplished something or not (Bellew et al., 2006). Therefore, there was no need to address SNS as acutely as IS was addressed. SNS comprises feelings that focus on the sense of being accepted by others, such as “If I make mistakes‚ I know other people will still like me” and “I do not have to prove myself to feel part of a group.” Thus, SNS is a factor behind the sense of security of being socially accepted by others. The aforementioned study revealed SNS correlates to daily hassles and paranoia negatively (Anderson and Freeman, 2013).

In addition, SNS had a significant negative relationship with variables associated with IS (Gilbert et al., 2009). Bellew et al. (2006) discovered that SNS was associated with few depressive symptoms, less anxiety concerning appearance, less negative eating habits, and less anxiety concerning rejection. In addition, they observed that feelings of security were associated with making favorable social comparisons.

One of the domains and socioeconomic topics about which people might feel inferior is income. Certain reports have shown that low socioeconomic status is associated with poor mental health (Lorant et al., 2003). Kahneman and Deaton (2010) divided happiness into the following two types: emotional and long-term happiness, and observed that emotional happiness correlated with income and feelings of “joy” and “satisfaction” for those whose annual income was less than $75,000. However, when the annual income exceeded $75,000, the relationship between income and feelings of joy and satisfaction weakened. Frank (2013) stated that income is a status for which satisfaction can be obtained, compared with those of others. Therefore, the association between income and happiness or satisfaction can be influenced in comparison with the income of other people. It was reported that the incidence of mental illness and crime is higher in competitive societies than that in caring societies (Arrindell et al., 2003). Thus, it is possible that the relationship between “socioeconomic status” and “happiness and mental health” is influenced by the striving to avoid inferiority. Anderson and Freeman (Anderson and Freeman, 2013) investigated a relationship between socioeconomic status and IS indirectly by using money-related hassles, and found a significant correlation between them. Another report also showed a significant relation between ill-being and socioeconomic status (Headey et al., 1984). However, the relationship among socioeconomic status, happiness or well-being, and the striving to avoid inferiority has not been enough clarified.

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between striving to avoid inferiority and mental health and well-being. In addition, hypothesizing that the striving to avoid inferiority is a factor that adjusts the relationship between socioeconomic status, mental health and well-being, the purpose of this study was to examine the effect of the striving to avoid inferiority on the relationship among annual income, mental health, and well-being.

Materials and Methods

Procedure

We conducted an anonymous, individual-response, internet-based survey of the general adult (over 20 years old) population in Japan in November 2020, using the panel survey service by Questant. We received 547 responses. Of these, 491 (241 males, 250 females; mean age, 45.01 years, SD, 13.99) were valid respondents, excluding those with incomplete survey items.

In the online questionnaire, it was stated that participation in the survey was not compulsory, that there would be no disadvantage if respondents refused to answer, the results of the survey would be used only for research purposes, and individual responses would not be published in their entirety. The results of the survey will be used for research purposes only, and individual responses will not be publicized. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Mejiro University in the Humanities and Social Sciences (Approval No. 20 person-014).

Questionnaire

We asked questions regarding gender, age, marital status, and employment status. The respondents were asked to choose from the following options: full-time (approximately 40 h per week), part-time (a few hours per week), self-employed (irregular/unfixed working hours), retired, housewife (husband), student, other, and unanswered.

Household income, which is a socioeconomic condition, was used to measure income in Japanese yen. Household income is defined as the annual income of the entire household, including the face value before taxes and insurance premiums, as well as extra and supplementary income.

The Striving to Avoid Inferiority Scale (SAIS) developed by Gilbert et al. (2007) was used. The SAIS was translated into Japanese by two psychologists (KA and YK), and a meeting was held to integrate the translations. The integrated translation was backtranslated by a Japanese–English translation company specializing in psychology. The two backtranslations were sent to the scale developer and were confirmed to be equivalent to the original version. The SAIS comprised two scales, Parts 1 and 2, but only Part 1 was used which was already verified its factor structure. This scale comprised two factors: IS (e.g., I need to seek success to be valued by others) and SNS (e.g., people around me like me even if I make mistakes). A total of 31 items were measured (Table 1). Five items (0, never; 1, almost never; 2, sometimes; 3, almost always; and 4, always) were used.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of the target population.

To measure mental health, we used the DASS-15 scale (Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-15) developed by Adachi et al. (2013), which is the Japanese version of the DASS 21-item version developed by Lovibond and Lovibond (1995). This scale comprised three factors with 5 items on each scale, namely depression, anxiety, and stressA four-point scale (0, not at all; 1, sometimes; 2, quite often; and 3, very often) was used.

To measure subjective well-being, we used the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) developed by Sumino (Sumino, 1994), which is a Japanese version of the SWLS developed by Diener et al. (1985). This scale comprised five items responded on a 7-point scale [1 (not at all) to 7 (totally)].

Analysis

After confirming the reliability of the scales, the mean, standard deviation, minimum, and maximum values of the scores were calculated. Furthermore, a correlation analysis between each variable was conducted. In addition, to examine if the SAIS subscales and household income influenced changes in the SWLS and DASS subscales, we used the SWLS or DASS subscales of depression, anxiety, and stress as the objective variables, and we used the household income, SAIS subscales of IS and SNS, and interaction between household income and IS as the explanatory variables. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted with the following explanatory variables: the interaction between household income and IS; household income and SNS; and household income, IS, and SNS. In addition, a simple slope analysis was conducted to clarify the effects of the interactions.

Descriptive statistics, correlation analysis, hierarchical multiple regression analysis, and simple slope analysis were conducted using HAD ver. 16.3, a free software (Shimizu, 2016).

Results

Demographic Data

The demographic data are shown in Table 1. A total of 241 (49.08%) participants were male and 250 (50.92%) were female. The mean age was 45.01 (SD = 13.99), and the median age was 45, with a range of 21–69 years. A total of 182 (37.06%) participants had no spouse (never married), 39 (7.94%) were bereaved or separated (divorced), 269 (54.79%) had a spouse (married), and one (0.20%) did not answer. Regarding employment status, 219 (44.60%) were full-time workers, 70 (14.26%) were part-time workers, 24 (4.89%) were self-employed, 26 (5.30%) were retired, 92 (18.74%) were full-time housewives (husbands), 16 (3.26%) were students, 38 (7.74%) were others, and 6 (1.22%) did not answer.

The descriptive statistics were calculated and are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics.

The mean household income was 601.19 million yen (SD = 529.21), the minimum was 0 million yen, and the maximum was 85 million yen.

The scores for each subscale were summed, and the scores for each subfactor were calculated. The mean score of “IS” was 51.01 (SD = 12.08), minimum was 19, maximum was 94, and alpha coefficient was alpha = 0.94. The mean score for “SNS” was 36.69 (SD = 7.93), minimum was 12, maximum was 60, and alpha coefficient was alpha = 0.91.

The scores for each factor were calculated by summing the scores for each subscale. The mean score of “depression” was 8.91 (SD = 3.78), minimum was 5, maximum was 20, and alpha coefficient was alpha = 0.91. The mean score for “anxiety” was 7.49 (SD = 3.34), with a minimum of 5, maximum of 20, and alpha coefficient of alpha = 0.91. The mean score for “stress” was 8.61 (SD = 3.45), with a minimum of 5, maximum of 20, and alpha coefficient of alpha = 0.88.

The wellbeing scores were calculated by summing the scores of all five items. The mean score was 17.59 (SD = 6.79), minimum was 5, maximum was 35, and alpha coefficient was alpha = 0.93.

Correlation Analysis

A correlation analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between striving to avoid inferiority and mental health, well-being, and household income. The results are shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation analysis.

The correlation analysis showed that IS had a significant positive correlation (r = 0.34, p = 0.01) with “depression”; significant positive correlation (r = 0.32, p < 0.01) with “anxiety”; and significant positive correlation (r = 0.35, p < 0.01) with “stress.”

SNS was shown to have a significant positive correlation (r = 0.40, p < 0.01) with “SWLS” and a significant negative correlation (r = −0.33, p < 0.01) with “depression”; significant negative correlation (r = −0.24, p < 0.01) with “anxiety”; and significant negative correlation (r = −0.27, p < 0.01) with “stress.”

Moderating Effects of Striving to Avoid Inferiority on Household Income and Wellbeing.

A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to examine the effects of the SAIS subscales and household income on changes in SWLS.

The procedure of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with SWLS as the objective variable was as follows: in Step 1, the SAIS subscales, IS, SNS, and household income were entered into the regression equation. In Step 2, we added the interaction terms of IS and SNS, IS and household income, and SNS and household income to the variables in Step 1. In Step 3, the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were added to the variables in Steps 1 and 2 (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis with SWLS as the objective variable.

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with SWLS as the objective variable are shown in Table 5. The results showed that the interaction terms of SNS, household income, and SNS and household income were positively associated with SWLS. The increase in R2 from Steps 1 to 2 and Steps 2 to 3 was 0.01 (F = 1.92, p < 0.01) and 0.00 (F = 0.05, p < 0.01), respectively. Cohen’s (Cohen, 1992) effect estimates for small, medium, and large increases in R2 were 0.02, 0.13, and 0.26, respectively; however, the variance explanatory rate of the effect of the interaction term on the target variable was generally between 1 and 3% (Champoux and Peters, 1987), suggesting that even 1% can be interpreted as a significant result (Fairchild and McQuillin, 2010).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis with depression as the objective variable.

To examine the content of the significant interaction, we conducted a simple slope analysis to examine the effect of household income on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD. The results are shown in Figure 1. The results showed that when SNS was high, household income did not have a significant effect on SWLS (β = 0.09, nonsignificant (ns)). However, when SNS was low, household income had a significant effect on SWLS (β = 0.36, p < 0.01).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term SNS × household income for SWLS purposes.

Moderating Effects of Striving to Avoid Inferiority on Household Income and Mental Health

Another hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to examine the effects of the SAIS subscales and household income on changes in mental health.

The procedure of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with depression as the objective variable was as follows: in Step 1, IS, SNS, and household income, which are subscales of the SAIS, were entered into the regression equation. In Step 2, we added the interaction terms of IS and SNS, IS and household income, and SNS and household income to the variables in Step 1. In Step 3, the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were added to the variables in Steps 1 and 2.

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with depression as the objective variable are shown in Table 5, where the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income positively associated with depression, and SNS and household income were negatively associated with depression. The increase in R2 from Steps 1 to 2 and Steps 2 to 3 was 0.02 (F = 3.94, p < 0.01) and 0.00 (F = 1.65, p < 0.01), respectively.

To examine the content of the significant interaction, the effect of household income on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD was confirmed by a simple slope analysis. The results are shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simple effect analysis of the interaction term SNS × household income for depression.

As observed from the SNS mean + 1 SD, household income did not have a significant effect on depression when SNS was high (β = −0.02, ns).

For the SNS mean − 1 SD, household income had a significant negative effect on depression when SNS was low (β = −0.41, p < 0.01).

The procedure of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with anxiety as the objective variable was as follows: In Step 1, the subscales of the SAIS, IS, SNS, and household income were entered into the regression equation. In Step 2, the interaction terms of IS and SNS, IS and household income, and SNS and household income were added to the variables in Step 1. In Step 3, the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were added to the variables in Steps 1 and 2.

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with anxiety as the objective variable are shown in Table 6, where the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were positively associated with anxiety, while SNS and household income were negatively associated with anxiety. The increase in R2 from Steps 1 to 2 and Steps 2 to 3 was 0.02 (F = 2.99, p < 0.01) and 0.01 (F = 9.37, p < 0.01), respectively.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis with anxiety as the objective variable.

To examine the content of the significant interaction, we used a simple slope analysis to examine the effect of household income on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD, and the effect of IS on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD for each household income group. The results are shown in Figures 35.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term SNS × household income for anxiety.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term of IS × SNS × low household income group for anxiety.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term of IS × SNS × high household income group for anxiety.

As observed from the mean + 1 SD of SNS, household income did not have a significant effect on anxiety when SNS was high (β = 0.06, ns). When SNS was high, IS in the low-income group did not have a significant effect on anxiety (β = 0.16, ns), and IS in the high-income group did not have a significant effect on anxiety (β = 0.16, ns). IS had a significant positive effect on anxiety (β = 0.57, p < 0.01).

When SNS was low, the low-household income group had a significant positive effect on anxiety (β = 0.32, p < 0.01), and the high-household income group had no significant effect on IS (β = 0.06, ns).

The procedure for the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with stress as the objective variable was as follows: in Step 1, the subscales of the SAIS, IS, SNS, and household income were entered into the regression equation. In Step 2, we added the interaction terms of IS and SNS, IS and household income, and SNS and household income to the variables in Step 1. In Step 3, the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were added to the variables in Steps 1 and 2.

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis with stress as the objective variable are shown in Table 7, where the interaction terms of IS, SNS, and household income were positively associated with stress, while SNS and household income were negatively associated with stress. The increase in R2 from Steps 1 to 2 and from Steps 2 to 3 was 0.01 (F = 3.26, p < 0.01) and 0.01 (F = 6.07, p < 0.01), respectively.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis with stress as the objective variable.

To examine the content of the significant interaction, we used a simple slope analysis to examine the effect of household income on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD, and the effect of IS on each value of the SNS mean ± 1 SD for each household income group. The results are shown in Figures 68.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term SNS × household income for stress.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term of IS × SNS × low household income group for stress.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Simple effects analysis of the interaction term of IS × SNS × high household income group for stress.

The results of the simple slope analysis are shown in Figures 68. First, the results of the mean value of SNS + 1 SD showed that household income did not have a significant effect on stress when SNS was high (β = 0.02, ns). When SNS was high, the IS of the high-household income group had a significant positive effect on stress (β = 0.56, p < 0.01).

When SNS was low, the low-household income group had a significant positive effect on stress (β = 0.31, p < 0.01), and the IS in the high-household income group had no significant effect on stress (β = 0.18, ns).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine (1) the relationship between striving to avoid inferiority and household income, mental health, and well-being and (2) whether striving to avoid inferiority is a factor that moderates the relationship between household income and mental health or well-being.

The results of correlation analysis showed that IS correlated with mental health and SNS correlated with well-being and mental health. These results consistent with previous studies (Bellew et al., 2006; Gilbert et al., 2007; Walton et al., 2020; To et al., 2021). Such injurious role of IS and protective role of SNS are shown as direct effects in the moderator analyses too. These findings strengthen the evidence that IS works as risk factor on mental health but SNS works as protective one. SNS was associated with well-being, but IS was not associated with well-being. This may be due to the interaction of IS with other factors such as SNS and income, as well as the fact that well-being and IS are not correlated. Besides, correlations between household income and mental health or well-being were small. These results may indicate that the relationships between household income and mental health or well-being are not linear as pointed out by Kahneman and Deaton (Kahneman and Deaton, 2010).

Next, we conducted hierarchical multiple regression analyses and results showed the following findings. Higher SNS and household income consistently related to better mental health and well-being but IS related to worse mental health similar to result of correlation analyses. Furthermore, individuals with either SNS or household income can maintain mental health and well-being. SNS is a sense of acceptance of oneself by others regardless success or failure, which can provide individuals with social safeness. This is consistent with an old report suggesting that social supportive network relates to well-being and ill-being (Headey et al., 1984), and suggesting that social connectedness is an essential part for people. About household income, previous studies pointed out that socioeconomic status relates to lower mental health (Pinquart and Sörensen, 2000; Lorant et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2010), and we can suggest that sufficient economic state is also an essential part.

Interestingly, the results on anxiety and stress suggested that when all of IS, SNS, and household income were higher, anxiety and stress also became higher. This means that protective roles of SNS and household income were weakened if higher IS coexists. Individuals with higher SNS, household income and IS seemed to have both of assets and reliable interpersonal relationship which preventing from depression and having satisfaction with life. However, if they had higher IS, they might be frightened to lose money or relationship and strive to avoid inferiority because they are haves. For example, if they have safe relationship with family including dependents, they need to earn money in competitive relationship at workplace because of the importance of dependents. Although it is difficult to determine why such interaction caused, such interaction of positive and negative factors has long been discussed (Headey et al., 1984), and balancing both of the aspects may contribute to better states (Trompetter et al., 2017).

For the above results, some suggestions can be made. First, the results suggested that financial support for low-income individuals was essential in improving mental health as suggested in previous research (Sareen et al., 2011). In the Finnish basic income experiment, respondents who received basic income reported less depression, sadness, and loneliness (Kela, 2020). Next, mental health can be improved by increasing SNS as previous studies have shown that a sense of acceptance or social support are related to mental health (Sugiyama, 2004; Harandi et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2021). Third, not only approaching income and SNS, it is also important to ease IS which can damage mental health even though individuals have higher income and social connectedness. IS is regarded as a part of competitive mentalities (Gilbert et al., 2009), which can be shifted to caring mentalities via psychotherapy such as compassion focused therapy (Gilbert, 2019). Therefore, developing compassion-based interventions for individuals with higher IS regardless of income or interpersonal satisfaction would be needed. At the social level, the liberal attitude of stirring up competition may also need to be reviewed.

Although this study presented insightful findings, there are several limitations, and further studies are required. The first one is the target population of the study. The survey was conducted only in Japan. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct a similar survey outside Japan in the future and examine the similarities and differences. The second is the sample size. In previous studies investigated income, sample sizes of over 3,000 were used. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct a large-scale survey in the future to validate the results of this study. The third is the method of measuring income. The measurement of income was based on self-application; therefore, there is no objective information, and there is a possibility of false reporting. The fifth is that we did not examine gender differences. It is assumed that gender can impact on social role, job or income (Navarro and Salverda, 2019), and should be revealed in the future. Finally, since this study was conducted during a pandemic, it is suggested that IS and SNS were affected by the pandemic to an extent; therefore, it is desirable to conduct another survey in a non-pandemic situation.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Mejiro University in the Humanities and Social Sciences. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

AN and KA contributed to conception and design of the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. KA and YK translated SAIS into Japanese. KA conducted survey. AN performed the statistical analysis. YK wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant number 19H01764.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Paul Gilbert for continuous support with the research project.

Abbreviations

IS, Insecure striving; SNS, Secure non-striving; SWLS, Satisfaction with life scale; SAIS, Striving to avoid inferiority scale; DASS, Depression anxiety stress scale.

References

Adachi, K., Yoshino, M., and Ueno, T. (2013). “Standardization of the Japanese version of DASS (The depression anxiety stress scales) (II), in Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Japanese Society of Health Psychology; September 07, 2013.

Google Scholar

Anderson, F., and Freeman, D. (2013). Socioeconomic status and paranoia: the role of life hassles, self-mastery, and striving to avoid inferiority. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 201, 698–702. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31829c5047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrindell, W. A., Steptoe, A., and Wardle, J. (2003). Higher levels of state depression in masculine than in feminine nations. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 809–817. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00185-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellew, R., Gilbert, P., Mills, A., McEwan, K., and Gale, C. (2006). Eating attitudes and striving to avoid inferiority. Eat. Disord. 14, 313–322. doi: 10.1080/10640260600796242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Champoux, J. E., and Peters, W. S. (1987). Form, effect size and power in moderated regression analysis. J. Occup. Psychol. 60, 243–255. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1987.tb00257.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, E., Brody, G. H., and Miller, G. E. (2022). What are the health consequences of upward mobility? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73, 599–628. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-033020-122814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkley, D. M., and Blankstein, K. R. (2000). Self-critical perfectionism, coping, hassles, and current distress: A structural equation modeling approach. Cogn. Ther. Res. 24, 713–730. doi: 10.1023/A:1005543529245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkley, D. M., Sanislow, C. A., Grilo, C. M., and McGlashan, T. H. (2009). Self-criticism versus neuroticism in predicting depression and psychosocial impairment for 4 years in a clinical sample. Compr. Psychiatry 50, 335–346. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkley, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., and Blankstein, K. R. (2003). Self-critical perfectionism and daily affect: dispositional and situational influences on stress and coping. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 234–252. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkley, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., and Blankstein, K. R. (2006). Specific perfectionism components versus self-criticism in predicting maladjustment. Personal. Individ. Differ. 40, 665–676. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.08.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykman, B. M. (1998). Integrating cognitive and motivational factors in depression: initial tests of a goal-orientation approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 139–158. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairchild, A. J., and McQuillin, S. D. (2010). Evaluating mediation and moderation effects in school psychology: A presentation of methods and review of current practice. J. Sch. Psychol. 48, 53–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, C., and Mendes, C. (2020). Insecure striving as an exacerbator of the toxic effect of shame feelings on disordered eating. Eating Weight Disorder Stu. Anorexia Bulim. Obesity 25, 659–666. doi: 10.1007/s40519-019-00668-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, C., Pinto-Gouveia, J., and Duarte, C. (2013). Drive for thinness as a women’s strategy to avoid inferiority. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 13, 15–29.

Google Scholar

Frank, R. (2013). Falling Behind. University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (1989). Human Nature and Suffering. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2005a). Compassion and Cruelty: A Biopsychosocial Approach in Compassion: Conceptualisations, Research and Use in Psychotherapy. United States: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2005b). Social Mentalities: A Biopsychosocial and Evolutionary Reflection on Social Relationships in Interpersonal Cognition. United States: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2019). Psychotherapy for the 21st century: An integrative, evolutionary, contextual, biopsychosocial approach. Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 92, 164–189. doi: 10.1111/papt.12226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., Broomhead, C., Irons, C., McEwan, K., Bellew, R., Mills, A., et al. (2007). Development of a striving to avoid inferiority scale. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 633–648. doi: 10.1348/014466606X157789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Bellew, R., Mills, A., and Gale, C. (2009). The dark side of competition: how competitive behaviour and striving to avoid inferiority are linked to depression, anxiety, stress and self-harm. Psychol. Psychother. 82, 123–136. doi: 10.1348/147608308X379806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harandi, T. F., Taghinasab, M. M., and Nayeri, T. D. (2017). The correlation of social support with mental health: A meta-analysis. Electron. Physician 9, 5212–5222. doi: 10.19082/5212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headey, B., Holmström, E., and Wearing, A. (1984). Well-being and ill-being: Different dimensions? Soc. Indic. Res. 14, 115–139. doi: 10.1007/BF00293406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 16489–16493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kela (2020). Results of Finland’s basic income experiment: small employment effects, better perceived economic security and mental wellbeing. Available at: https://www.kela.fi/web/en/news-archive/-/asset_publisher/lN08GY2nIrZo/content/results-of-the-basic-income-experiment-small-employment-effects-better-perceived-economic-security-and-mental-wellbeing (Accessed September 28, 2021).

Google Scholar

Liu, C., Huang, N., Fu, M., Zhang, H., Feng, X. L., and Guo, J. (2021). Relationship between risk perception, social support, and mental health among general Chinese population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Risk Manage. Healthcare Policy 14, 1843–1853. doi: 10.2147/RMHP.S302521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorant, V., Deliège, D., Eaton, W., Robert, A., Philippot, P., and Ansseau, M. (2003). Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: A meta-analysis. Am. J. Epidemiol. 157, 98–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the Beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, M., and Salverda, W. (2019). Earner position and job and life satisfaction: do contributions to the household income have the same effect by gender and occupations? J. Happiness Stud. 20, 2227–2250. doi: 10.1007/s10902-018-0045-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Sörensen, S. (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 15, 187–224. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sareen, J., Afifi, T. O., McMillan, K. A., and Asmundson, G. J. (2011). Relationship between household income and mental disorders: findings from a population-based longitudinal study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 419–427. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimizu, Y. (2016). Free statistical analysis software HAD: An introduction to its functions and a proposal for its use in statistical learning, education, and research practice. J. Media Inf. Commun. Stud. 1, 59–73.

Google Scholar

Sugiyama, T. (2004). Do feelings of acceptance and rejection contribute to or accompany depression? A longitudinal study of three time points. Bull. Yamanashi Eiwa Univ. 3, A9–A16.

Google Scholar

Sumino, Y. (1994). “An attempt to create a Japanese version of the satisfaction With life scale (SWLS),” in Proceedings of the 58th annual meeting of the Japanese Psychological Association; August 20, 1994.

Google Scholar

To, P. Y. L., Lo, B. C. Y., Ng, T. K., Wong, B. P. H., and Choi, A. W. M. (2021). Striving to avoid inferiority and procrastination among university students: The mediating roles of stress and self-control. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:5570. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18115570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trompetter, H. R., Lamers, S. M. A., Westerhof, G. J., Fledderus, M., and Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2017). Both positive mental health and psychopathology should be monitored in psychotherapy: confirmation for the dual-factor model in acceptance and commitment therapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 91, 58–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, C. C., Baranoff, J., Gilbert, P., and Kirby, J. (2020). Self-compassion, social rank, and psychological distress in athletes of varying competitive levels. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 50:101733. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. L., Schmitz, N., and Dewa, C. S. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the risk of major depression: The Canadian National Population Health Survey. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 64, 447–452. doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.090910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: inferiority, mental health – related quality of life, well-being, income, social safeness, social comparison

Citation: Nagae A, Asano K and Kotera Y (2022) Moderating Effects of Striving to Avoid Inferiority on Income and Mental Health. Front. Psychol. 13:838991. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.838991

Received: 04 February 2022; Accepted: 17 May 2022;
Published: 03 June 2022.

Edited by:

Larisa Ivascu, Politehnica University of Timișoara, Romania

Reviewed by:

Paul Gilbert, NHS England, United Kingdom
Brian Pacheco, Nirmala Institute of Education, India

Copyright © 2022 Nagae, Asano and Kotera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kenichi Asano, kenichi.asano1225@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.