Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 July 2022
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Rhythm in Human Cognition and Action: Health and Pathology View all 11 articles

Synchrony During Online Encounters Affects Social Affiliation and Theory of Mind but Not Empathy

  • 1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3International Laboratory for Brain, Music and Sound Research BRAMS, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 4Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music – CRBLM, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 5Haskins Laboratories, Yale University, New Haven, CI, United States

Moving together in time affects human social affiliation and cognition. However, it is unclear whether these effects hold for on-line video meetings and whether they extend to empathy (understanding or sharing others' emotions) and theory of mind (ToM; attribution of mental states to others). 126 young adult participants met through online video in unacquainted pairs. Participants either performed 3 min of synchronous arm movements paced by sounds (n = 40), asynchronous movements (n = 46) or a small talk condition (n = 40). In a subsequent empathy task, participants engaged in a conversation. A video recording of this conversation was played back, and each participant rated, at predetermined time points, how they felt and how they thought their partner felt. From this we calculated empathic accuracy (accuracy of the estimation of the other's emotions) and emotional congruence (emotion sharing). ToM was measured by showing videos of geometrical shapes interacting and asking the participants to describe what happened, measuring the amount of intentionality. We found that participants in the synchrony condition rated feeling greater closeness and similarity to their partners relative to the asynchronous condition. Further, participants in the synchrony group tended to ascribe more intentionality to the abstract shapes than participants in asynchrony condition, suggesting greater ToM. Synchrony and asynchrony groups did not reliably differ in empathic accuracy nor emotional congruence. These results suggest that moving in synchrony has effects on social affiliation measures even in online encounters. These effects extend to ToM tendencies but not empathic accuracy or emotion sharing. These results highlight the potential of synchronous movement in online encounters to affect a subset of social cognition and affiliation measures.

Introduction

People moving in synchrony occurs in cultures across the globe in activities ranging from rituals and ceremonies to military marching (McNeill, 1997). Synchronous movement appears early in life (Cirelli, 2018). Synchronization can occur spontaneously during an interaction or can be intentional as when ensemble musicians adjust their movements to each other (bidirectionally) or when orchestra members follow the beat imposed (unilaterally) by a conductor. Synchrony has a wide range of effects on human social cognition, promoting social bonding (Huron, 2001) and enhancing social cohesion (Hove and Risen, 2009; Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009) and cooperation (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009) both within and outside behaviorally synchronous groups (Reddish et al., 2014; Sullivan et al., 2015) (but see also Gelfand et al., 2020). The aim of the present study was to investigate whether these effects hold for online encounters between unacquainted pairs of young adults and whether these effects extend to more complex socio-cognitive abilities such as theory of mind (e.g., ToM) and empathy.

Moving in synchrony with somebody, whether spontaneous or imposed, has previously been shown to increase the sense of connection and liking (Hove and Risen, 2009; Tarr et al., 2015, 2016) and boost individual perception of closeness and feelings of similarity (Paladino et al., 2010; Valdesolo et al., 2010; Vacharkulksemsuk and Fredrickson, 2012; Reddish et al., 2013; Tarr et al., 2016). Synchrony has also been shown to increase positive affect (Tschacher et al., 2014) and to improve memory recall of words spoken by the person that one synchronizes with (Macrae et al., 2008). Drawing on a meta-analysis including more than 4,000 participants, Mogan and colleagues concluded that moving in synchrony increases four main dimensions of social relationships: social behaviors, such as prosocial actions, social bonding, including the feeling of similarity and closeness, social cognition, such as the ability to memorize or paying attention to others, and positive affect, such as mood, happiness, self-esteem, and general life satisfaction (Mogan et al., 2017). Altogether these studies show that synchrony affects several dimensions of social affiliation such as closeness, prosocial behavior, and perceived similarity. Studies in this field have been typically conducted in person with participants being in the same room while performing the synchronous movements. Therefore, it is not clear whether synchrony can also affect social affiliation when participants meet online rather than in physical presence. This is not a trivial question as there are a number of key differences between online (video) and in-person meetings which may influence the social dynamics of these encounters (Day and Schneider, 2002). First of all, the fixed camera in online meetings forces people to stay relatively still in order to be visible hindering the regulation of social distance which is known to be an important part of natural conversation (Patterson, 1996). Second, in online encounters some sensory information is not shared between the interacting partners such as external sounds and odors, which can have an impact on perceived interaction and emotional engagement with the other person (Johnson et al., 2006; Sohn, 2011). These features may explain why it is more difficult to recreate a sense of 'being there together' in online encounters (Parkinson and Lea, 2011). Given the differences between online and in-person social interactions, we cannot assume that the effects of synchrony on social affiliation, which have been tested virtually exclusively in-person, generalize to online interactions. The first aim of the present study is to test whether, in online video meetings, synchrony affects social affiliation.

The second set of questions we address relates to socio-cognitive skills. Recently it was suggested that the effects of synchrony extend beyond social affiliation (e.g., closeness, social bonding) to also include people's socio-cognitive skills (Baimel, 2015). In particular, the present study focuses on the tendency to attribute mental states and intentionality to others, typically referred to as ToM, on the one hand, and on the accuracy of perceiving other's emotions and on the extent of sharing them, typically referred to as cognitive and affective empathy, respectively.

Connections between synchrony and empathy have been investigated, focusing predominantly on subjective reports. In one study participants rhythmically moved cups in time with sounds presented through headphones (Baimel et al., 2018). They did so in trios where either the sounds were synchronous (i.e., same tempo for all participants) or asynchronous (i.e., different tempi for different participants). As a result, participants performed movements at the same time in the synchrony but not in the asynchrony condition. Participants moving synchronously rated themselves as better in understanding others' emotions, but not in sharing these emotions, relative to participants moving asynchronously (Baimel et al., 2018). This latter result differs from Koehne et al. (2016) who tested participants in a unilateral synchrony task where participants, in a leader role, were followed by a computer algorithm with a high or low degree of synchrony. Participants (healthy controls) who were followed synchronously self-reported higher affective empathy (i.e., sharing emotions) relative to those who were followed asynchronously. Taken together, synchrony increases subjective understanding of the emotions of others (cognitive empathy) and possibly also self-reported sharing of the emotions of others (affective empathy). However, these studies relied on self-reported empathy measures. There are often considerable discrepancies between self-reported measures of empathy (e.g., using questionnaires) and objective measures of people' ability to read and share emotions. Several studies found little to no correlation between objective and self-reported empathy measures (Levenson and Ruef, 1992; Ickes, 1993; Realo et al., 2003; Jospe et al., 2020). Indeed, only a small portion of the variance in objective measures may be explained by self-reported questionnaires: around 1% according to some estimates (Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019). This is not surprising if we consider that self-report questionnaires (but not objective measures) rely on metacognitive insight into one's capacities and are sensible to well-documented biases: social desirability (Sedikides et al., 2003) and the Dunning-Kruger effect (where poor performers overestimate their abilities and high performers often underestimate their abilities, Ames and Kammrath, 2004).

The effect of synchrony on objective measures of empathy remains less clear. The discrepancy between objective and self-reported empathy measures implies that we cannot simply extrapolate the findings from self-report measures to objective empathic performance. Studies that did use objective measures of empathy tended not to find effects of synchrony. For example, synchronous movements did not significantly affect participants' ability to attribute the correct emotions to a corresponding image of the eye region (Baimel et al., 2018). Using this same Reading the Mind in the Eyes task, Koehne et al. (2016) found no correlation with perceived synchrony (in a unilateral synchrony task). These findings are important but leave open one possibility. The studies had participants rate emotions of unknown people in pictures, whereas the effects of synchrony may be specific to the synchronizing partners. Indeed, the issue of whether the effects of synchronous movements are specific to those we synchronize with is still open: some studies report that synchrony selectively boosts helping behaviors toward the people we synchronize with (Cirelli, 2018) while others show that moving in synchrony induces an effect that generalizes even to those who did not move in synchrony with us (Reddish et al., 2014, 2016). Thus, in the case of empathy, one would need to measure the accuracy or reading the emotions of the people that one synchronizes with. This is exactly what was done more recently, in a study using the Empathic Accuracy paradigm in which two partners first engage in a conversation and are then asked a series of questions on their own and their partner's feelings and thoughts. The study found that the degree of success in inferring others' emotions and thoughts (empathic accuracy) was not significantly associated with spontaneous behavioral synchrony during the conversation (Fujiwara and Daibo, 2021). Although highly relevant, this study relied on spontaneous behavioral synchrony which tends to be less stable than imposed synchrony (Richardson et al., 2005, 2007; Schmidt and Richardson, 2008). For this reason, it is possible that the resulting effects of synchrony on empathy were too small to detect. Imposing synchrony in a stable manner may affect empathic accuracy more robustly, as we will test here. Further, the Fujiwara and Daibo study measured accuracy for inferring both emotions and thoughts and so the absence of effect may be because synchrony has a different effect on awareness of thoughts (Theory of Mind) and awareness of emotions (empathy).

Studies on empathy have focused primarily on cognitive (emotion understanding) rather than affective (emotion sharing) empathy. Theoretical accounts predict that behavioral synchrony should lead to alignment of affective states (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2019) but to our knowledge, this has been tested using subjective, not objective measures of emotion sharing. Using subjective measures, Koehne et al. (2016) found that participants self-reported more emotion sharing with synchronous vs. asynchronous partners. However, again this is a self-report measure, and it remains unclear whether when measured objectively, synchronous participants share more emotions.

In sum, synchrony affects subjective cognitive empathy, but no effects have been found using objective measures, possibly because they used spontaneous synchrony or stimuli other than the person we synchronize with. In the present study we asked dyads to synchronize with a metronome and then measured empathy toward the synchronization partner in order to investigate whether objective empathy (both cognitive and affective) is influenced by synchrony.

The present study also addresses the question of whether moving in synchrony affects tendency to attribute mental states to others, namely Theory of Mind (ToM) (Wimmer and Perner, 1983). In this study we explore this issue in adult participants whereas prior work on ToM, independent of synchrony, focused predominantly on children and atypical populations. Recent work shows that ToM continues to develop during adulthood (Apperly et al., 2010; Klindt et al., 2017) and new tasks have been developed to assess adults' actual and self-reported advanced ToM skills (Apperly, 2011; Devine and Lecce, 2021). Previous research offers some preliminary evidence that synchrony affects self-reported mentalizing. For example, participants who in small groups moved synchronously to a metronome increased the extent to which mental states are ascribed to the other members of the group (Baimel et al., 2018). Similarly, perceived synchrony, in a unilateral synchronization experimental task, correlated with the perceived ability to understand the thoughts and intentions of their partner (Koehne et al., 2016). Together, these studies suggest that moving in synchrony with a partner increases self-reported understanding of the partner's mental states. It remains unclear whether synchrony affects people's actual attribution of mental states using performance-based measures of ToM. Performance-based tasks differ from self-report questionnaires in that they do not assess people's beliefs about their own ToM but, rather, examine the extent to which people actually attribute mental states to selected stimuli such as characters in vignettes (e.g., Happé, 1994) or videos (e.g., Murray et al., 2017), or to abstract geometrical moving shapes (e.g., White et al., 2011). While emerging research has shown that self-reported questionnaires may correlate with performance-based measures (Bukowski and Samson, 2017; Clutterbuck et al., 2021), it is not clear whether the effects of synchrony found in self-reports extend to performance-based ToM measures. To date the only study conducted on this topic is that of Koehne et al. (2016). After having performed the unilateral synchrony task (see above), participants completed the Movie to Assess Social Cognition (MASC), a video-based task in which participants watch a short movie about four characters getting together for a dinner party and to answer to a series of questions concerning the characters' mental states (Dziobek et al., 2006). Authors found that individual performance in produced synchrony (follower's success in adjusting his or her movements to the leader to produce synchrony) were unrelated to performance in the MASC. Two features of this study should, however, be noted here. First, correlations are based on a restricted sample of 22 healthy controls; second, as the authors pointed out, there may have been too low variability in MASC scores to detect correlations with synchrony. In the present study we aimed to recruit a larger sample and employ a different performance-based ToM task to test whether synchrony affects ToM. The task chosen is the Triangle task (Castelli et al., 2000), that measures participants' attribution of intentionality as they describe a video of moving geometric shapes in absence of any contextual verbal or non-verbal cues other than movement.

In the present study we recruited participants who were divided into dyads that were unacquainted. These dyads met online through video conference and performed a series of periodic hands movement (clapping) paced by a metronome, either in synchrony or asynchrony with their partner. However, both these conditions involved rhythmic arm movements and fixed gaze position that may be perceived as unnatural. As a result, participants could experience these conditions as awkward on the one hand or as fun and engaging on the other hand. Such perceptions could affect the social affiliation measures that we collect here. Thus, to put the results from the synchrony and asynchrony conditions in perspective, we included a more conventional interaction: the small talk condition in which participants freely talked about a set of predefined questions. In this group, participants were not restricted in their arm, head and eye movements. Thus, this condition serves as a relatively ecological baseline to which any differences between synchrony and asynchrony can be interpreted. The aims of the study were to examine the effect of synchrony on (1) social affiliation (perceived similarity, closeness, likeability, and future friendship), (2) the tendency to attribute mental states and intentionality to others (ToM), and (3) on the accuracy of perceiving other's emotions and sharing them. Empathy was measured using the empathic accuracy paradigm, an ecological task that constitutes a relatively objectively measure of whether participants in dyads can accurately infer each other's emotions (cognitive empathy) and share those emotions (affective empathy). ToM was measured using two tasks: a self-report questionnaire (measuring the extent to which mental states were attributed to the dyadic partner) and a performance-based measure (measuring the degree of intentionality ascribed to abstract moving shapes in a video). The study was conducted entirely online during the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy (spring 2020) which was a period of a degree of imposed social isolation. To control for potential effects of this social isolation, we measured social contacts, wellbeing, and loneliness.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred twenty-six Young Adults Were Recruited Through Social Networks and the Newsletter of the Psychology Department. Participants Had a Mean age of 23.59 years (SD = 3.21, Range = 19–32 years) and Were Randomly Assigned to one of the three Experimental Conditions: Synchrony (N = 40; 30 Females), Asynchrony (N = 46; 35 Females) and Small Talk (N = 40; 30 Females). All Participants Were Fluent Italian Speakers. Criteria for Inclusion Were Written Consent and age Between 19 and 35 years. The Study Was Approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences of the University of Pavia (Approval # 048/20). Prior to Participation, all Participants Were Informed About the Aims of the Study and Signed the Informed Consent, According to the Declaration of Helsinki. The Dataset Is Available Online at osf.io/Jchzb/.

Procedure

Participants were contacted via mail and asked to complete a questionnaire assessing baseline variables: demographic information, quantity of social contacts, shyness, wellbeing, and loneliness. In the second phase, participants were paired in dyads according to the following rules: being unacquainted, close in age (maximum age difference was 3 years) and of the same gender. Dyads were randomly attributed to one of three experimental conditions: synchrony, asynchrony, and small talk. After the manipulation, all participants, first, took part in the empathic accuracy procedure, then completed the two ToM tasks (the Triangle task and the Mental State Attribution questionnaire) and, finally, filled out a series of questions evaluating social affiliation (closeness, similarity, degree of liking, and possibility of a future friendship; details below). The experimental procedure is represented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure. Participants first completed a questionnaire assessing baseline variables, second, engaged in synchronous/asynchronous movement or small talk, third, completed the Empathic accuracy procedure and finally, completed the ToM tasks and a series of questions evaluating social affiliation. In the synchrony and asynchrony conditions subjects clapped their hands in time with a metronome sound presented via headphones while looking at their partner. In the synchrony condition the metronome had the same BPM (beats per minutes) for both partners. In the asynchrony condition the metronome had a different BPM for the partners. Movements were therefore aligned in time in the synchrony but not in the asynchrony condition. In the figure the lines represent the time and the arrows represent when the metronome sound occurred, in time with which participants clapped their hands. In the small talk condition, participants took part in a semi-structured conversation.

The entire video call took ~90 min.

Synchrony and Asynchrony Manipulation

In the synchrony condition participants were asked to make a cyclic movement with their hands (touching the palms of the hands and then putting them opened in front of the camera) in time with a metronome (periodic beep) sound that was presented through Zoom at the same tempo for both partners (65 beats per minute (bpm) for half the dyads and 55 bpm for the other half). These particular tempi were chosen for consistency with prior studies (Reddish et al., 2013, 2014). Participants were instructed to perform the movement continuously while looking at their partner. In the asynchrony condition participants received the same instructions and followed the same procedure as those belonging to the synchrony group. The only difference was that in the asynchrony condition, the two participants were received a different tempo (55 bpm and 65 bpm) (see Figure 1) and as a result their movements did not generally align in time. Participants in both these conditions were not allowed to talk to each other.

Immediately following the movements, participants were asked whether they had perceived Internet connection problems. Results showed that 13 participants in the synchrony group (32.5%) and 14 in asynchrony group (30.4%) reported that they perceived Internet connectivity issues. These numbers were not significantly different between groups (X2(2,85) = 0.08, p = 0.78).

Small Talk Condition

In the small talk condition participants (instead of performing movements either in synchrony or asynchrony with their partner) took part in a semi-structured conversation based on 24 questions of the small talk task developed by Sedikides et al. (1999). The subjects were instructed to go through the 24 questions in order, taking turns answering one question. Participants were not restricted in their movements, and they could see each other during the conversation.

Empathic Accuracy

The empathic accuracy procedure (adapted from Blanke et al., 2016) consists of a semi-structured conversation in which participants are asked to talk, in turn, for 3 min about a positive and a negative event that happened in their life (Figure 2). The listener is allowed to interrupt the partner to ask questions. At the end of this 12-min conversation, the researcher showed participants the recorded video of their conversation twice, interrupting the video every 90 s (“tape stop”). At each tape stop, the researcher asked participants to report their own (in the first viewing) and estimate their partner's (in the second viewing) emotions answering the following questions: “How do you feel?” and “How does your partner feel?”. Participants answered these questions for each of nine emotions (five positive: happy, excited, content, comfortable, balanced and four negatives: nervous, sad, uncomfortable, tense) using a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much) (see Measures). The answers given by the subjects were confidential (invisible to their dyadic partner) (see Figure 2). After completing the empathic accuracy procedure, participants were administered the two ToM tasks, the Triangle task and the Mental State Attribution questionnaire, and the social affiliation questionnaires.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Empathic accuracy procedure and an example of empathic scores computation. Two participants (A, B) took part in a semi-structured conversation in which both participants talk about positive and negative events in their life. At the end of talk, the researcher showed participants the recorded video of their conversation twice, interrupting the video at eight time points (tape stops). At each tape stop, the researcher asked participants to rate their own (in the first viewing) and their partner's (in the second viewing) emotions. Based on these answers, we computed empathic accuracy (EA) and emotional congruence (EC) as shown, for an example portion of the data in the table. SA, self-reported feelings of participant A; SB, self-reported feelings of participant B; OA, other-rating of A: estimation by A of B's feelings; OB, other-rating of B: estimation by B of A's feelings; EAA, empathic accuracy of participant A; EAB, empathic accuracy of participant B; EC, emotional congruence of the dyad.

Measures

Baseline Variables

General Information Questionnaire

We asked participants to report on their age, gender, and general information about the social situation that they were experiencing (i.e., days of isolation, number of times they went out during the last week).

Social Contacts

We administered an in-house questionnaire to assess the frequency of social contacts (e.g., phone call, texting, and videocalls) during the last week. Possible answers range from 0 (never) to 4 (more than once a day) and were summed into a total score ranging from 0 to 70 (α = 0.64).

Wellbeing

We used the Italian version of the Warnick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS; Gremigni and Stewart-Brown, 2011). WEMWBS is a 14-item scale covering both hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of mental health including positive affect (feelings of optimism, cheerfulness, and relaxation), satisfying interpersonal relationships and positive functioning (energy, clear thinking, self-acceptance, personal development, competence, and autonomy). Participants were required to tick the box which best described their own experience over the past 2 weeks using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = none of the time; 5 = all of the time). Item scores were summed into a single total score of wellbeing ranging from a minimum of 14 to a maximum of 70, with higher scores representing higher levels of mental wellbeing (α = 0.85).

Loneliness

Participants filled out the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (R-UCLA; Russell et al., 1980). This scale consisted of 20 items, 10 positively (e.g., “There are people I can turn to”) and 10 negatively worded (e.g., “I feel isolated from others”). Answers were based on a 4-points Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (often). Once items were reverse scored, all of them were summed to create an overall index of loneliness, ranging from a minimum of 20 to a maximum of 80 with higher scores indicating greater loneliness (α = 0.79).

Shyness

Participants responded to the Italian version of the Revised Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale (RCBS-14; Marcone and Nigro, 2001). The scale consists of 14 items to which subjects responded on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (completely disagree) to 4 (completely agree). Responses were summed into a single shyness score (range 0–56) (α = 0.89).

Variables of Interest (Post-manipulation)

Empathy

This was measured using the empathic accuracy task (adapted from Blanke et al., 2016) which consists of a semi-structured conversation between two participants (see procedure 2.2.1). On the basis of participants' answers to the questions of “How do you feel?” and “How does your partner feel?,” we computed a score of empathic accuracy as the similarity between the participant's self-reported feelings and the empathizer's judgment of the experimental partner's feelings (cognitive empathy) and emotional congruence as the similarity between the two participants' self-reported feelings (affective empathy) (Figure 2). Specifically, for empathic accuracy, we calculated the sum of squared differences of the judgment of the empathizer and the self-report of the partner across the eight tape stops for each emotion, which were then averaged to yield one empathic accuracy score for positive and one for negative emotions in each participant. The reason positive and negative emotions were kept separate is because empathy for positive and negative emotions are considered as two distinct but related constructs with different properties and correlates (Rothbart et al., 2000; Sallquist et al., 2009). Analogously, emotional congruence was calculated via the sum of squared differences between both partners' emotion ratings across the tape stops (see Figure 2). Note that while empathic accuracy yielded an individual score for each participant, emotional congruence yielded a single score per dyad.

Theory of Mind (ToM)

ToM was measured using a self-report questionnaire, the Mental State Attribution questionnaire (Baimel et al., 2018), and a performance-based task, the Triangle task (Castelli et al., 2000).

The Mental State Attribution questionnaire consists of 15 items evaluating participants' tendency to view the experimental partner as someone who owns mental states (emotions, thoughts, desires) (Baimel et al., 2018). Responses were on a 7-points scale (1 = completely disagree; 7 = completely agree; summed score range: 15–105; α = 0.87).

The Triangle task (Castelli et al., 2000) evaluates the extent to which people attribute intentionality to geometric shapes on the basis of their movements. It has been shown to reliably differentiate between high-functioning ASD groups and verbal ability matched control groups (Abell et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2017) and has been used in studies with neurotypical adults (for example Devine and Hughes, 2019). From the three categories of videos (random, goal-directed and ToM) in the original study, the present study selected two ToM videos of moving geometric shapes (triangles) that behaved in such a way as to imply teasing and joking. Each animation lasted ~40 s. After watching each video clip, participants were asked to write down what happened in each clip. The score for each clip reflected the degree to which participants described the video in terms of complex intentional mental states, according to the original guidelines (Castelli et al., 2000, Appendix II, Intentionality score). The intentionality score for each description ranged from 0 (no deliberate action, e.g., “bouncing”, “rotating”) to 5 (deliberate action aimed at affecting another's mental state, e.g., “persuading”, “pretending”, and “deceiving”). Two raters independently coded 25% of the responses and interrater agreement was established using Cohen's kappa (κ = 0.77). All remaining responses were coded jointly by the raters. Disagreements were resolved through discussion between the raters. During this entire procedure the coders were blinded to the experimental condition. The summed score for the two videos could range from 0 to 10 points.

Closeness

We assessed how close each participant felt to their experimental partner using the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (IOS) (Aron et al., 1992). Participants were asked to report how close they felt to the partner by picking one out of seven “Venn diagrams”: each was a pair of more or less overlapping circles (Figure 4D). On one end of the continuum, the circles are completely separate, and, on the other end, the circles are virtually entirely overlapping (Range: 1–7 with higher values meaning greater closeness).

Perceived Similarity, Liking and Future Friendship

Participants rated on a 9-point scale from 1 (not at all similar) to 9 (very similar) (Range: 1–9) the following statement: “How similar do you feel to the participant with whom you take part in this study?” (perceived similarity). Participants responded on a 9-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much) the following two statements: “How much do you like the participant with whom you take part in this study?” (likeability) and “In the future, to what extent do you feel that you could be friends with the participant with whom you take part in this study?” (future friendship) (Sedikides et al., 1999).

Data Analysis

We first removed outliers in each dependent variable using the boxplot rule (Tukey, 1977): a data point is considered an outlier if it is more than 1.5 times the interquartile range above the upper quartile or below the lower quartile.

We performed a randomization check by running a series of ANOVAs with experimental group (synchrony, asynchrony, and small talk) as the independent variable and each baseline variable in turn as dependent variable in order to investigate whether there were differences between experimental groups at the outset (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and group comparisons (ANOVA) on baseline variables.

To test our main hypotheses, we performed a series of ANCOVAs with experimental condition (synchrony, asynchrony, and small talk) as the between-participants factor, variables of interest (see details below) as dependent variables and gender as covariate (to control for gender differences). For each variable of interest, we further checked whether there was a significant correlation with any of the baseline variables (general information, social contacts, shyness, loneliness, and wellbeing) and, if so, these were included as covariates. Follow-up tests were performed using planned orthogonal contrasts: first, we investigated the effect of synchrony using a synchrony vs. asynchrony contrast and, second, we contrasted synchrony and asynchrony together vs. small talk. The rationale for the planned latter contrast is that synchrony and asynchrony conditions involve precisely prescribed rhythmic movements that are potentially experienced as unnatural and rigid, in similar ways, whereas the small talk condition is a more common, ecological interaction. For reference, separate contrasts (synchrony vs. small talk and asynchrony vs. small talk) are reported in the Supplementary Materials section. Empathic accuracy and emotional congruence were analyzed using a series of ANCOVAs controlling for gender with experimental condition (synchrony, asynchrony, and small talk) as between-participants factor and the valence of emotion (positive and negative) as within-subject factor. Where significant, we then examined the effect of experimental condition on positive and negative empathic indexes separately using ANOVAs. Finally, when the ANOVA follow-up contrasts were not significant for contrasts of interest (synchrony vs. asynchrony) in the empathic measures (empathic accuracy and emotional congruence), we calculated the Bayes Factor BF10 (i.e., evidence for alternative vs. null hypothesis). The Bayes Factor, when smaller than 1, quantifies the amount of evidence in favor of the null hypothesis, that there is no difference between groups. Benchmark scores: BF10 between 1 and 1/3 are considered to be weak, between 1/3 and 1/10 are considered moderate, and >1/10 are considered strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (Jeffreys, 1961).

We report ηp2 partial effect sizes (Keppel, 1991).

Results

Preliminary Analyses and Descriptive Statistics

Randomization check: ANOVAs showed no significant differences between the three groups on any of the baseline variables (all ps > 0.07, Table 1), indicating that there was no evidence for group differences at the outset.

Comparing values on the wellbeing scale with normative scores, we found that for the majority of our sample, wellbeing was above clinical levels (70.6%). Of the remaining 29.3%, 12.6% of participants reported a level of wellbeing indicating a probable depression and 16.7% a possible depression.

Descriptive statistics of the post-manipulation focus variables are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics on post-manipulation variables.

Manipulation Check

In order to assess the amount of synchrony in the experimental groups (nominal synchrony and asynchrony conditions) we performed an analysis that was inspired by Motion Energy Analysis (Ramseyer, 2020). For each dyad, we extracted the part of the video recording where participants made movements paced by the metronome. We analyzed each of the two persons' video separately, converted into grayscale (Figure 3A). For each consecutive two frames, we calculated the pixel-by-pixel intensity difference. Taking the mean absolute value of these differences across the frame, this yielded a single value estimating the overall amount of change in the frame relative to the next. The corresponding time course was filtered (5th order Butterworth bandpass filter 0.75–1.25 Hz) to extract the periodicity in the vicinity of the pacing frequency (Figure 3B). In order to account for possible small delays in the signal, we calculated the maximum Pearson cross-correlation between the signals with a maximum shift of 25 frames (1 s). This yielded a single Pearson correlation value per dyad which was Fisher r-to-z transformed and compared between synchrony and asynchrony conditions (Figure 3C). The image change time courses showed greater correlation in the synchrony condition (Pearson z mean = 0.59, SD = 0.28) than in the asynchrony condition (Pearson z mean = 0.32, SD = 0.20) [t (41.50) = 3.81, p < 0.001, Cohen d = 1.11, 95% CI (0.47, 1.76)]. This suggests that the nominal synchrony dyads indeed moved more synchronously than dyads in the nominal asynchrony condition.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Manipulation check. Manipulation check indicating that the nominal synchrony dyads moved more in synchrony than the asynchrony condition. See text for detail. (A) Differences in pixel intensity were calculated across the image and averaged. (B) Raw mean absolute image change showed periodic patterns in both participants (bar graph in the background) that were extracted using bandpass filtering (line in the foreground). Pearson correlation was calculated between these traces (bottom panel). (C) The dyads in the nominal synchrony condition showed greater correlation among their movement traces than participants in the asynchrony condition. Bars represent average, error bars indicate standard error of the mean and dots indicate individual dyads.

Synchrony and Social Affiliation

We found a significant effect of the experimental group on perceived similarity, F (2, 122) = 4.14, p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.68 (Figure 4A). Planned contrasts revealed statistical trends toward significance with participants in the synchrony condition rating their partners as more similar to themselves than participants in the asynchrony condition, t (122) = 2.17, p = 0.063, and participants taking part in the synchrony and asynchrony groups with respect to those belonging to the small talk one, t (122) = 2.13, p = 0.067. The ANOVA on likeability scores showed a statistical trend toward significance for the effect of the experimental group, F (2,110) = 2.82, p = 0.063, ηp2 = 0.45 (Figure 4B). Planned contrasts showed no significant difference between synchrony and asynchrony groups t (110) = 1.18, p = 0.42. The synchrony and asynchrony vs. small talk contrast was marginally significant, with synchrony/asynchrony participants condition showing higher scores than those in the small talk condition, t (110) = 2.16, p = 0.64. For perceived closeness, results revealed a significant main effect of experimental group F (2, 122) = 3.07, p = 0.050, ηp2 = 0.05 (Figure 4C). Planned contrasts showed that the synchrony group reported more closeness than the asynchrony group, t (122) = 2.48, p = 0.029. The contrast between synchrony/asynchrony and small talk groups was not significant, t (117) = 0.80, p = 0.67. No significant difference between groups was found on the future friendship question, F (2, 121) = 1.66, p = 0.20, ηp2 = 0.03.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effects of synchrony on social affiliation, empathy and theory of mind. Participants in the synchrony group reported greater similarity (A), likeability (B), and closeness (C) to their partners than participants in asynchrony and small talk groups. Closeness was measured using the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) scale of which a sample item is shown in the inlay. Participants in the synchrony group attributed greater intentionality to triangles in a video (D). For the empathic measures, empathic accuracy (E) and emotional congruence (F) results are shown separately for negative and positive emotions, and no group differences were found. In all panels dots represent participants' single scores; bars represent group averages and error bars the standard error of the mean. syn, synchrony; asyn, asynchrony; st, small talk.

Overall, these data showed that participants in the synchrony condition were more likely to perceive their experimental partner as close and, marginally, as more similar than in the asynchrony group but did not rate more liking or future friendship possibility.

Synchrony and Empathic Accuracy, Emotional Congruence

For empathic accuracy, we first conducted a 3 x 2 mixed ANCOVA with group as between-subject variable, valence of the emotions (positive and negative) as within-subjects factor and empathic accuracy as dependent variable (controlling for gender). Results showed no significant effect of the experimental group, F (2, 120) = 1.30, p = 0.275, ηp2 = 0.02, and no significant interaction, F (2, 120) = 1.75, p = 0.178, ηp2 = 0.03 (Figure 4E). A significant effect was found for the valence of the emotions, F (1, 120) = 19.75, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14, indicating that empathic accuracy was greater for negative than for positive emotions. Since the main effect of group was not significant, we proceeded to calculate the Bayes Factor of the principal contrast of interest between synchrony and asynchrony and found BF10 = 0.23 for positive and BF10 = 0.29 for negative emotions. This indicates moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis that there is no difference between synchrony and asynchrony groups in empathic accuracy for both positive and negative emotions.

For emotional congruence there was no significant effect of group, F (2, 118) = 1.76, p = 0.176, ηp2 = 0.03, but a significant main effect of the valence of the emotions, F (1, 118) = 27.68, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19, with emotional congruence being greater for negative emotions than for positive emotions (Figure 4F). The interaction between experimental group and valence of emotions was significant, F (2, 118) = 3.71, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.06. We then followed up with two separate ANCOVAs on positive and negative emotional congruence, respectively. For positive emotional congruence, the effect of group was not significant, F (2, 118) = 0.23, p = 0.79, ηp2 = 3.92, but for negative emotional congruence it was, F (2, 118) = 5.53, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.09. Planned group contrasts for negative emotional congruence revealed no significant difference between synchrony vs. asynchrony group, t (118) = 1.05, p = 0.50, but a significantly greater emotional congruence in the synchrony and asynchrony vs. the small talk group, t (118) = −3.11, p = 0.005. In sum, we did not find differences in emotional congruence between synchrony and asynchrony groups, whereas negative emotional congruence was greater in synchrony/asynchrony groups relative to the small talk. To assess the evidence for the finding that emotional congruence does not differ between groups, we calculated the Bayes Factor of the difference between synchrony and asynchrony groups and found BF10 = 0.24 for positive (moderate evidence) and BF10 = 0.37 (some evidence) for negative emotions. Overall, synchrony and asynchrony groups did not differ in assessing or sharing others' emotions.

Synchrony and Theory of Mind

The ANCOVA (controlling for gender) on the Triangle intentionality attribution scores showed a significant main effect of experimental group, F (2, 122) = 3.39, p = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.06 (Figure 4D). Planned contrasts revealed that participants in the synchrony condition were more likely to attribute intentions to the triangles than participants in the asynchrony condition, t (122) = 2.65, p = 0.009. No significant difference was found between the synchrony and asynchrony vs. the small talk group, t (122) = 0.71, p = 0.73. For the Mental State Attribution questionnaire, no significant differences between groups were found, F (2, 120) = 1.49, p = 0.23, ηp2 = 0.02. Overall, these results showed that participants in the synchrony group showed greater tendency to describe the video of the triangles in mental state terms than in the asynchrony group, but we did not find evidence that they were more likely to see other people as carrying mental states.

Discussion

The present study investigated the effect of synchronous movement on social affiliation, empathy and ToM. We tested unacquainted participants who met on-line in dyads and performed 3 min of synchronous or asynchronous movements, or a semi-structured small talk conversation. We examined self-reported social affiliation with the dyadic partner (similarity, closeness, likeability, possibility of a future friendship), ToM (attribution of intentionality and mental states, using an objective and a subjective measure) and objective measures of empathy (empathic accuracy and emotional congruence). First, we found that participants in the synchrony group reported greater closeness to their partner relative to participants in the asynchrony and small talk groups, and a statistical trend toward greater similarity. Second, participants in the synchrony group were more likely to attribute intentions to abstract moving shapes than participants in the asynchrony and small talk groups. No differences were found between groups in attributing mental states to their partners. Finally, participants in the synchrony and asynchrony groups showed similar levels of accuracy in estimating (empathic accuracy) and sharing (emotional congruence) of their partner's emotions.

Synchrony and Social Affiliation

The present study found that the synchrony group reported greater closeness toward their partner relative to the asynchrony group, in line with prior in-person studies, whereas for a number of other social affiliation measures (similarity, likeability and future friendship) results were only marginal or absent. This suggests that in online encounters at least a subset of in-person effects of synchrony on social affiliation can be replicated.

Our finding that synchrony increases perceived closeness fits with existing studies that have typically been conducted in person (Paladino et al., 2010; Vacharkulksemsuk and Fredrickson, 2012; Reddish et al., 2013; Fessler and Holbrook, 2014; Lumsden et al., 2014; Dong et al., 2015; Tarr et al., 2016). Taken together these results show that moving in synchrony makes people feel closer to each other and this is true both when participants met online and offline. The effect of synchrony on closeness is in accordance with the self-other blurring hypothesis. According with this view, synchronizing with someone, compared to other coordinated behavior such as asynchronous or sequential action, blurs the boundaries between the self and other (Hove, 2008). This is thought to be because typically during synchronous movement, two people perform the same movement at the same time. This match between one's own action and the observation of another performing the same action makes it more difficult for the brain to distinguish between self and other (Paladino et al., 2010; Mazzurega et al., 2011; Tarr et al., 2014; Rennung and Göritz, 2016). This blurring of the self-other boundary would lead to greater perceived closeness or even a sense of oneness with a group (Swann et al., 2012). As for similarity, our results are partially in line with previous literature which reported an increase of the sense of similarity after synchronizing with someone compared to asynchronous or control conditions (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Schachner and Garvin, 2010; Valdesolo et al., 2010; Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011; Dong et al., 2015). The present study also indicated a trend for greater similarity in the synchrony group (relative to the asynchrony group) although this did not reach statistical significance. With regard to likeability, our data did not replicate a prior finding of an increase in liking after synchronous movement (Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011).

We did not find an effect of synchrony on the perceived possibility to develop a future friendship with the experimental partner. As far as we know, our study is the first one testing whether this indicator of social affiliation is affected by synchrony. We expected participants in the synchrony group to be more willing to become friends than participants in the asynchrony group. This is because synchrony typically increases a range of social affiliation dimensions (Hove and Risen, 2009; Paladino et al., 2010; Valdesolo et al., 2010; Vacharkulksemsuk and Fredrickson, 2012; Reddish et al., 2013; Tarr et al., 2015, 2016) that presumably make one more likely to want to form a future friendship (Sedikides et al., 1999). However, we were not able to find support for this hypothesis.

This pattern of findings may reflect one of two hypothetical scenarios. A first possibility is that in-person, synchrony can affect a range of social affiliation measures, whereas online it affects only closeness. We speculate that the reason other affiliation measures (similarity, likeability) may not be affected by online synchrony in the same way as in-person synchrony is because of a lack of relevant visual cues, such as eye contact and physical proximity. Indeed, previous literature suggested that during in-person interactions, similarity and likeability are sensitive to such information (Peters, 2007). However, it remains challenging for this account to explain why the effect of online synchrony would be specific to closeness and not hold for related measures such as similarity and likeability. A second hypothetical scenario is that online synchrony actuality affects all measures of social affiliation, but the effects on likeability and the possibility of a future friendship were smaller and, therefore, did not reach significance here. This account is in line with theories of synchrony's social effects in which synchrony should affects multiple measures of social affiliation across the board (Hu et al., 2022). It remains unclear whether the effect on likeability and the possibility of a future friendship would be smaller. Since prior literature has typically merged all these variables (similarity, closeness, etc.) into a single, aggregate social affiliation measure, we do not have prior estimates for the effect sizes on the individual variables.

Our study is the first, to our knowledge, to document the social effects of synchrony in people meeting through video conferencing. There are some distantly related studies that found that synchronous movements between avatars (stick figures) can lead to greater perceived closeness and sympathy ratings (Stupacher et al., 2017a,b, 2020, 2021). If indeed such results rely on similar synchrony-to-social processes as operating between humans, as is sometimes tacitly assumed, then this lends credence to the idea that social effects of synchrony are not restricted to humans meeting in person. However, these studies differ in important ways from the present work, that makes such extrapolations tentative.

To summarize, our findings suggest that synchrony increases closeness in online environments. On the contrary, findings of other social affiliation measures were less clear: no detected difference in likeability and future friendship and only a statistical trend on similarity.

Synchrony and Empathy

Our data showed no difference between synchrony and asynchrony conditions for both empathic accuracy - the accuracy in understanding other's emotions - and emotional congruence - sharing emotions with another person.

It has been proposed that moving in synchrony would be associated with greater emotion sharing (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2019). Indeed, on a subjective level this has been confirmed by a study in which participants taking part in a unilateral synchronization estimated that they shared emotions with their partners more than participants in an asynchrony condition (Koehne et al., 2016), although (Baimel et al., 2018), asking a similar question, did not find this effect. Here we tested whether this effect may hold on an objective level as well, by directly comparing the ratings of felt emotions by the two partners (emotional congruence). We found no difference between the synchrony and asynchrony conditions. Taken together, this suggests that although participants may have thought they shared the emotions of their partner (based on prior work), they did not actually do so (based on the present study). Synchrony may, thus, introduce a positive subjective bias in emotional congruence, making us believe we share more emotions with others than we really do. We speculate that this positive subjective bias may help us feel closer to the people we synchronize with, and that it can perform this function without requiring that we actually feel the same. Since in this study we did not collect subjective measures of emotion sharing we cannot test this association between closeness and subjective emotion sharing directly. The mismatch between subjective and objective measures of emotion sharing is reminiscent of similar mismatches that have been documented in the empathic accuracy literature. Indeed, subjective measures of empathic accuracy (thinking we know what the other feels) often differ from objective measures of empathy (actually knowing what the other feels) (Zaki et al., 2008; Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019). If indeed synchrony affects subjective but not objective emotion sharing, future theoretical accounts of synchrony may need to draw this distinction and explain why that is the case.

In terms of accuracy of understanding others' emotions (empathic accuracy), prior studies like us did not find an effect of synchrony. However, empathic accuracy was typically measured using tasks that evaluate the extent to which participants correctly identified the emotions of people other than the synchronizing partners, such as for example when inferring emotions from a picture of somebody's eyes (Baimel et al., 2018). This left open the possibility that synchrony selectively boosts empathy toward the people we synchronize with (as has similarly been demonstrated in the case of helping behaviors, Cirelli et al., 2014; Cirelli, 2018), which is what we tested here. However, empathic accuracy toward the synchronization partner was not significantly different between the synchrony and asynchrony conditions. One other study focusing on spontaneous not imposed synchronization, investigated empathic accuracy toward the synchronization partner (Fujiwara and Daibo, 2021) and similarly found no relation to spontaneous synchronization. This study used a composite measure of empathic accuracy that included awareness of both thoughts and emotions. As a result, it was not clear whether synchrony might have specifically affected empathy for emotions. Our result provides evidence that this is not the case. Taken together, converging lines of evidence including our study reveal that synchrony has little if any effect on empathy, regardless of how synchrony was achieved (spontaneous vs. imposed) or toward whom empathy was directed (synchrony partner or others).

The lack of effect of synchrony on empathic accuracy in our study may help nuance more broad-stroke claims that effects of prolonged music training on objective measures of empathy are mediated by synchrony. Long-term music training has been associated with some effects on empathic functioning (Thompson et al., 2003; Rabinowitch et al., 2013) and some works have speculated this is driven by synchrony inherent in the musical training (Rabinowitch, 2017). However, other components of long-term music training are likely to be responsible for these boosts in empathy. One component may be musical material itself. Indeed, studies on social affiliation show that presenting music, as opposed to a simple metronome or silence, leads to greater closeness and likeability, and that this is true regardless of synchrony (Stupacher et al., 2017a). Perhaps a similar knock-on effect would hold for empathy, namely that regardless of synchrony, music would boost empathic accuracy. This could explain why music training could promote empathic accuracy when synchrony alone does not. Another component is the long-term nature of the music training tested in the aforementioned studies. Indeed, improving empathic accuracy by means other than synchrony or music, usually takes extended periods of time (Mascaro et al., 2013; Lobchuk et al., 2016; Kraus, 2017; Haut et al., 2019) except for very short pharmacological interventions (Bartz et al., 2010). Thus, it is possible that our synchrony intervention was too short to have a reliable impact on empathic accuracy.

Overall, our study along with previous research, paints a picture where synchrony is not linked with greater accuracy in inferring or sharing others' emotions.

Synchrony and Theory of Mind

Synchronous participants attributed more to mental states to abstract geometrical shapes but not to their partner. Attribution of mental states to the partner was measured using a questionnaire. Attribution of mental states more broadly was tested using a task involving a set of videos of geometrical shapes moving so as to suggest human interaction, in the absence of any contextual verbal or non-verbal cues other than movement. We screened participants' descriptions of these videos for the degree of intentionality they attributed to the shapes and found that this was higher for participants in the synchrony compared to the asynchrony group. This suggests that synchrony has an effect on the attribution of intentionality. This result at first glance may seem to contradict a prior study that found that the quality of synchrony did not correlate with ToM performance (Koehne et al., 2016). In that study, participants, after a unilateral synchronization task, were shown videos of four human characters getting together for a dinner party and then had to respond to questions as to what the different characters were thinking, feeling or intending. The individual differences in scores on this task (Movie for Assessment of Social Cognition, MASC; Dziobek et al., 2006) did not correlate with the accuracy of the synchronization. We argue that this finding does not contradict but complements the present results. The Koehne study assessed the accuracy with which participants can infer other people's mental states which is a different aspect of ToM from what we tested here, namely the extent to which participants spontaneously ascribe mental states independently of whether they do so accurately or not. Thus, a preliminary picture seems to emerge from studies so far on synchrony and ToM and empathy where some subjective measures are affected by synchrony, but no accuracy measures are: no effect on empathic accuracy (our study and Fujiwara and Daibo, 2021), nor emotion sharing (our study), nor reading-the-mind-in-the-eyes (Baimel et al., 2018) nor assessing intentions in a movie (MASC; Koehne et al., 2016).

The Triangle task has been successfully used in studies involving typical adults (Devine and Hughes, 2019; Ceccato et al., 2020) and has shown convergent validity with other ToM tasks (Devine and Hughes, 2019; Lecce et al., 2021). Previous research has suggested that the detection of agency on the basis of motion cues, such as velocity changes and movements that appear interactive, may be a precursor of ToM (Blakemore et al., 2003) and relies on brain areas involved in understanding social information in human motion (Castelli et al., 2002). Given that previous research has shown that performance in the Triangle task is associated with social competence (Ceccato et al., 2020), at least in children, possibly the differences we found in this task in adults could also be relevant for social interactions.

Our results suggest that moving in synchrony has effects on other tasks that no longer involve the person we synchronized with (in our case, the Triangle task). This is analogous to prior work documenting effects of synchrony on prosocial behavior, showing that these effects extend beyond the particular people we synchronize with (Reddish et al., 2013; but see also Cirelli et al., 2014; Cirelli, 2018). However, in the present study, we did not observe an effect of synchrony on mental state attribution for the person synchronized with. This contrasts with results of Baimel et al. (2018) who reported that participants in the synchrony condition were more likely to attribute mental states to their partners than participants in the asynchrony condition. This failure to replicate the prior result could be, we speculate, because the effect of synchrony on ToM is smaller in dyads (as we tested) than in triads (as in Baimel et al., 2018). Indeed, although not addressing mental state attribution, previous literature has indicated that group size may moderate synchrony effects on other measures of social affiliation (prosocial behavior and positive affect): the larger the group, the bigger the effect (Mogan et al., 2017). Extrapolating from these findings, we speculate that in our case, the group size (dyads) may have been too small to yield effects that were observed previously for triads.

Our study taken together with prior work suggests that the link between synchrony and ToM may go both ways. We tested the effect of synchrony on ToM. Prior work has documented the opposite direction: whether pre-existing ToM differences lead to greater synchrony. Novembre et al. (2019) divided participants into high and low empathic perspective taking pairs (based on scores of the perspective taking subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, IRI). Participants in the high empathy group synchronized more accurately in time than participants with low empathy. In another set of studies, Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) individuals had lower synchronization performance relative to neurotypical controls (Gowen and Miall, 2007; Fitzpatrick et al., 2013, 2017; Marsh et al., 2013). One possibility is that this synchronization difference is related to the documented difference in mentalizing abilities in ASD individuals. However, other differences between typical and ASD individuals other than mentalizing could explain this effect, such as social perceptual processes (Klin et al., 2002). Taken together, existing literature may suggest a bidirectional relationship between synchrony and ToM, as has been proposed before (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2019). However, the studies for the two directions of this relationship did not use the same measures of ToM (subjective vs. performance-based) and synchrony (spontaneous vs. imposed). This leaves open the possibility that each direction of the relationship holds only for a specific ToM component considered or on the way that synchrony was manipulated, and thus it would not be fully bidirectional. It would be interesting for future studies to further hash out this relationship between ToM and synchrony by evaluating whether an intervention that increases ToM leads to greater synchrony after relative to the before.

In sum, our findings suggest that synchrony affects the tendency to spontaneously ascribe intentionality to moving geometric shapes but not the self-reported attribution of mental states to the synchronizing partner.

Secondary Findings

Participants in the synchrony and asynchrony conditions together, relative to the small talk condition, showed greater emotional congruence (emotion sharing) for negative emotions. This finding was selective: no differences were found between the synchrony and asynchrony groups, nor differences in empathic accuracy, nor differences for positive emotional congruence. The small talk condition was included as a benchmark of the social effects of a relatively natural, normal interaction. The emotional congruence effect observed can be due to many factors and our data do not allow us to determine which. For instance, the synchrony and asynchrony conditions both involved prescribed periodic movements paced by auditory sounds whereas the small talk condition did not. The small talk condition involved verbal interaction whereas the other conditions did not. Or again, the difference in emotion sharing could be due to the fact that the synchrony and asynchrony conditions both performed the same movement, thus giving rise to a mirroring of sort (even though not necessarily at the same time). The small talk condition allowed for sharing of personal details whereas the synchrony/asynchrony conditions did not. Note also that the small talk conversation could have given rise spontaneously to more or less synchronous movements, as has been previously observed, due to a number of factors (Fusaroli et al., 2013; Varlet et al., 2014; Schmidt and Fitzpatrick, 2019; Fujiwara and Daibo, 2021). Any of these differences, or yet others we have not listed, could be responsible for the emotion sharing difference observed here.

Both empathic accuracy and emotional congruence were greater for negative than for positive emotions. This was true across groups and, thus, not affected by synchrony or small talk. We are not aware of studies systematically investigating whether positive or negative emotions are more prone to be shared. Perhaps what we observed is because negative emotional information tends be more salient (Baumeister et al., 2001; Vaish et al., 2008; Fessler et al., 2015), since individuals in many cases tend to prefer sharing positive emotional content (Gillath et al., 2005). Prior literature has drawn distinctions between empathy for negative and positive emotions (Andreychik and Lewis, 2017). Empathy for others' positive emotions is associated with dispositional positive emotionality (propensity to experience frequent, intense, or enduring positive affect), engagement in behaviors aimed to enhance others' positive emotions and with engaging in random acts of kindness (Andreychik and Migliaccio, 2015). Empathy for others' negative emotions is associated with helping others when it is framed as avoiding them to suffer (Andreychik and Lewis, 2017). Our data does not allow us to distinguish whether greater sharing of negative emotions is a general phenomenon or an artifact of the conversation that we had our participants engage in. In our study, participants always first talked about a negative event, which may have led to a greater overall negative tone to the conversation which was reflected in greater sharing of negative emotions.

Future Directions and Limitations

Synchrony can occur spontaneously or intentionally. When intentional, it may come about through different processes that have different social outcomes. Cacioppo et al. (2014) distinguish between orchestration (when multiple participants follow a common stimulus), unilateral entrainment (when one of the participants is a leader that the others follow) or reciprocal entrainment (when the participants mutually adjust their movements). Although a growing literature documents differences in dynamics between these kinds of synchrony, the social effects of each have received little systematic comparison. The present study focuses on synchronizing with an imposed metronome (i.e., orchestration) as a number of prior studies did (Macrae et al., 2008; Hove and Risen, 2009; Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011; Reddish et al., 2013, 2016; Baimel et al., 2018). Although this allowed us greater control in terms of tempo over time and across dyads, it leaves open the possibility that social effects would have been different had we studied spontaneous or bilateral entrainment. Future studies could explore these possibilities. At least in principle, it is possible that bidirectional synchrony would have a stronger effect on ToM and empathy than a synchrony condition in which participants are required to synchronize with an external metronome because in that case there is no shared goal. A prior study that focused on social effects other than ToM and empathy indeed found that bidirectional synchronization (shared goal) has larger effects on measures of trust, cooperation and perceived similarity and closeness toward the synchronizing partners than a unilateral synchrony with a metronome (Reddish et al., 2013).

The present study did not include pre-manipulation measures of the variables of interest so we could not control for potential differences in baseline levels of social affiliation, ToM and empathy. Second, we did not measure the fine-grained accuracy of synchronous movements in each pair of participants (e.g., the time differences between the movements of the two partners). This could be helpful in future studies to ask whether pairs who are more accurately synchronous in time also yield greater effects on social affiliation and social cognition. Third, possibly our sample size was too small to detect more subtle effects. Further, the absence of effects on empathic measures (empathic accuracy and emotional congruence) reported here could be due to the online nature of the test. It is possible, at least in principle, that these effects would be observed had participants met in person. We think this possibility is less likely, given the degree of convergence between our result and that of prior studies on other social measures. However, future studies could test this more directly.

Our study was conducted during a time of exceptional social circumstances. In the spring of 2020, as data was collected, our Italian participants were in a state of lockdown where in-person social interactions were in many cases severely limited. In that situation online interactions may have provided the social support that is otherwise obtained in-person meetings (Pancani et al., 2021; Marinucci et al., 2022). This could have biased the results of the present study: perhaps participants were more open to engage in online interpersonal interactions than they would have been under conditions of less social isolation. Note that boosting closeness under these exceptional circumstances also highlights the potential of our intervention: that online encounters can, through even brief periods of synchronous movement, become a source of interpersonal closeness. Closeness, in turn, has been associated with greater emotional wellbeing during the pandemic (Cavallini et al., 2021). In this way, online social interactions may be beneficial when in-person interactions are not available (Waytz and Gray, 2018).

In conclusion, our study found that synchronous movements during online encounters increased a number of measures of social affiliation and Theory of Mind, but not empathic accuracy or emotion sharing. These results highlight the potential of synchronous movement in online encounters to affect a subset of social cognition and affiliation measures.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are publicly available online via https://osf.io/Jchzb/.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences of the University of Pavia. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and writing: CB, SL, and FV. Investigation and data collection: CB. Supervision: SL and FV. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Anna Segù and Alice Rigamonti for their assistance with data collection and we thank all participants for their collaboration in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886639/full#supplementary-material

References

Abell, F., Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn. Dev. 15, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00014-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, D. R., and Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. J. Nonverbal Behav. 28, 187–209. doi: 10.1023/B:JONB.0000039649.20015.0e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreychik, M. R., and Lewis, E. (2017). Will you help me to suffer less? How about to feel more joy? Positive and negative empathy are associated with different other-oriented motivations. Pers. Individ. Differ. 105, 139–149. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreychik, M. R., and Migliaccio, N. (2015). Empathizing with others' pain versus empathizing with others' joy: examining the separability of positive and negative empathy and their relation to different types of social behaviors and social emotions. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 274–291. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2015.1071256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of Theory of Mind. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Apperly, I., Carroll, D. J., Samson, D., Humphreys, G. W., Qureshi, A., and Moffitt, G. (2010). Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults' ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 1201–1217. doi: 10.1080/17470210903281582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baimel, A., Birch, S. A., and Norenzayan, A. (2018). Coordinating bodies and minds: Behavioral synchrony fosters mentalizing. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74, 281–290. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baimel, A. S. (2015). From Keeping Together in Time, to Keeping Together in Mind: Behavioral Synchrony and Theory of Mind (master's thesis). [Vancouver]: University of British Columbia

Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., Hollander, E., Ludwig, N. N., Kolevzon, A., and Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1426–1428. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., and Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Rev. General Psychol. 5, 323–370. doi: 10.37/1089-2680.5.4.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J., Boyer, P., Pachot-Clouard, M., Meltzoff, A., Segebarth, C., and Decety, J. (2003). The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex. 13, 837–844. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, E. S., Rauers, A., and Riediger, M. (2016). Does being empathic pay off?—Associations between performance-based measures of empathy and social adjustment in younger and older women. Emotion. 16, 671. doi: 10.1037/emo0000166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukowski, H., and Samson, D. (2017). New insights into the inter-individual variability in perspective taking. Vision. 1, 8. doi: 10.3390/vision1010008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, S., Zhou, H., Monteleone, G., Majka, E. A., Quinn, K. A., Ball, A. B., et al. (2014). You are in sync with me: neural correlates of interpersonal synchrony with a partner. Neuroscience 277, 842–858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain 125, 1839–1849. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., and Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 12, 314–325. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavallini, E., Rosi, A., van Vugt, F. T., Ceccato, I., Rapisarda, F., Vallarino, M., et al. (2021). Closeness to friends explains age differences in positive emotional experience during the lockdown period of COVID-19 pandemic. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 2623–2631. doi: 10.1007/s40520-021-01927-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceccato, I., Lecce, S., and Cavallini, E. (2020). Older adults' beliefs about their ability to understand others' mental states. J. Adult Dev. 27, 294–304. doi: 10.1007/s10804-020-09348-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K. (2018). How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early prosocial behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 20, 35–39. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369, 20130400. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clutterbuck, R. A., Callan, M. J., Taylor, E. C., Livingston, L. A., and Shah, P. (2021). Development and validation of the Four-Item Mentalising Index. Psychol. Assess. 33, 629–636 doi: 10.1037/pas0001004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, S. X., and Schneider, P. L. (2002). Psychotherapy using distance technology: A comparison of face-to-face, video, and audio treatment. J. Couns. Psychol. 49, 499. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.49.4.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, R. T., and Hughes, C. (2019). Let's talk: Parents' mental talk (not mind-mindedness or mindreading capacity) predicts children's false belief understanding. Child Dev. 90, 1236–1253. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, R. T., and Lecce, S. (2021). Theory of Mind in Middle Childhood and Adolescence: Integrating Multiple Perspectives. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780429326899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, P., Dai, X., and Wyer Jr, R. S. (2015). Actors conform, observers react: the effects of behavioral synchrony on conformity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 60. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., Kessler, J., Woike, J. K., Wolf, O. T., and Convit, A. (2006). Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fessler, D. M., and Holbrook, C. (2014). Marching into battle: synchronized walking diminishes the conceptualized formidability of an antagonist in men. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140592. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fessler, D. M. T., Pisor, A. C., and Navarrete, C. D. (2015). Negatively-biased credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs. PLoS One. 9, 95167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, P., Diorio, R., Richardson, M., and Schmidt, R. (2013). Dynamical methods for evaluating the time-dependent unfolding of social coordination in children with autism. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7, 21. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J. L., Duncan, A., Barnard, H., Richardson, M. J., and Schmidt, R. (2017). Evaluating the importance of social motor synchronization and motor skill for understanding autism. Autism Res. 10, 1687–1699. doi: 10.1002/aur.1808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujiwara, K., and Daibo, I. (2021). Empathic accuracy and interpersonal coordination: behavior matching can enhance accuracy but interactional synchrony may not. J. Soc. Psychol. 162, 1–18. doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01612-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusaroli, R., Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., and Tylén, K. (2013). Dialogue as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas Psychol. 32, 147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., Caluori, N., Jackson, J. C., and Taylor, M. K. (2020). The cultural evolutionary trade-off of ritualistic synchrony. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190432. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., and Mikulincer, M. (2005). Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts: exploring the neural correlates. Neuroimage 28, 835–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gowen, E., and Miall, R. (2007). The cerebellum and motor dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders. Cerebellum 6, 268–279. doi: 10.1080/14734220601184821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gremigni, P., and Stewart-Brown, S. (2011). Una misura del benessere mentale: validazione italiana della Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS). Giornale Italiano di Psicologia. 38, 485–508. doi: 10.1421/35174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haut, K. M., Dodell-Feder, D., Guty, E., Nahum, M., and Hooker, C. I. (2019). Change in objective measure of empathic accuracy following social cognitive training. Front. Psychiatry. 10, 894. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J. (2008). Shared circuits, shared time, and interpersonal synchrony. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 29–30. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07003202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Cheng, X., Pan, Y., and Hu, Y. (2022). The intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Acta Psychol. 224, 103513. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 930, 43–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05724.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ickes, W. (1993). Empathic accuracy. J. Personal. 61, 587–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1993.tb00783.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of Probability, 3rd Edn. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Google Scholar

Johnson, G. J., Bruner, G. C. II., and Kumar, A. (2006). Interactivity and its facets revisited: theory and empirical test. J. Advert. 35, 35–52. doi: 10.1088/1757-899X/392/6/062172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jospe, K., Genzer, S., klein Selle, N., Ong, D., Zaki, J., and Perry, A. (2020). The contribution of linguistic and visual cues to physiological synchrony and empathic accuracy. Cortex. 132, 296–308. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keppel, G. (1991). Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook. New York, NY: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., and Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social sit- uations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klindt, D., Devaine, M., and Daunizeau, J. (2017). Does the way we read others' mind change over the lifespan? Insights from a massive web poll of cognitive skills from childhood to late adulthood. Cortex. 86, 205–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., and Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition. 146, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W. (2017). Voice-only communication enhances empathic accuracy. Am. Psychol. 72, 644. doi: 10.1037/amp0000147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecce, S., Ronchi, L., and Devine, R. T. (2021). Mind what teacher says: teachers' propensity for mental-state language and children's theory of mind in middle childhood. Soc. Dev. doi: 10.1111/sode.12552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, R. W., and Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy: a physiological substrate. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 234. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobchuk, M., Halas, G., West, C., Harder, N., Tursunova, Z., and Ramraj, C. (2016). Development of a novel empathy-related video-feedback intervention to improve empathic accuracy of nursing students: a pilot study. Nurse Educ. Today. 46, 86–93. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2016.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lumsden, J., Miles, L. K., and Macrae, C. N. (2014). Sync or sink? Interpersonal synchrony impacts self-esteem. Front. Psychol. 5, 1064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., and Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: action synchrony and person perception. Cognition. 109, 152–156. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcone, R., and Nigro, G. (2001). La versione italiana della Revised Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale (RCBS 14-item). Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata. 234, 33–40.

Google Scholar

Marinucci, M., Pancani, L., Aureli, N., and Riva, P. (2022). Online social connections as surrogates of face-to-face interactions: a longitudinal study under Covid-19 isolation. Comput. Hum. Behav. 128, 107102. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Verbalis, A. D., Schmidt, R. C., and Fein, D. (2013). Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7, 4. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mascaro, J. S., Rilling, J. K., Negi, L. T., and Raison, C.L. (2013). Compassion meditation enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 48–55. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., Paladino, M. P., and Schubert, T. W. (2011). Self-other bodily merging in the context of synchronous but arbitrary-related multisensory inputs. Exp. Brain Res. 213, 213–221. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2744-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeill, W. H. (1997). Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Mogan, R., Fischer, R., and Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, B. A., and Lilienfeld, S. O. (2019). Are self-report cognitive empathy ratings valid proxies for cognitive empathy ability? Negligible meta-analytic relations with behavioral task performance. Psychol. Assess. 31, 1062. doi: 10.1037/pas0000732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, K., Johnston, K., Cunnane, H., Kerr, C., Spain, D., Gillan, N., Hammond, N., Murphy, D., and Happé, F. (2017). A new test of advanced theory of mind: The “Strange Stories Film Task” captures social processing differences in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 10, 1120–1132. doi: 10.1002/aur.1744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novembre, G., Mitsopoulos, Z., and Keller, P. E. (2019). Empathic perspective taking promotes interpersonal coordination through music. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-48556-91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paladino, M. P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., and Schubert, T. W. (2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychol. Sci.. 21, 1202–1207. doi: 10.1177/0956797610379234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pancani, L., Marinucci, M., Aureli, N., and Riva, P. (2021). Forced social isolation and mental health: a study on 1,006 Italians under COVID-19 lockdown. Front. Psychol. 12, 1540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkinson, B., and Lea, M. (2011). “Video-linking emotions,” in Face-To-Face Communication Over the Internet: Emotions in a Web of Culture, Language and Technology, eds. A. Kappas and N. C. Krämer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 100–126. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511977589.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, M. (1996). “Social behavior and social cognition: A parallel process approach,” in What's Social About Social Cognition, eds J. L. Nye and A. M. Brower (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.), 87–105. doi: 10.4135/9781483327648.n4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, P. (2007). Gaining compliance through non-verbal communication. Pepp. Disp. Resol. LJ. 7, 87

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rabinowitch, T.-C., Cross, I., and Burnard, P. (2013). Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children. Psychol. Music. 41, 484–498. doi: 10.1177/0305735612440609

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinowitch, T. C. (2017). “Synchronisation-a musical substrate for positive social interaction and empathy,” in Music and Empathy, eds E. King and C. Waddington (London: Routledge), 89–96. doi: 10.4324/9781315596587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramseyer, F. T. (2020). Motion energy analysis (MEA): a primer on the assessment of motion from video. J. Couns. Psychol. 67, 536–549. doi: 10.1037/cou0000407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Realo, A., Allik, J., Nolvak, A., Valk, R., Ruus, T., Schmidt, M., and Eilola, T. (2003). Mind-reading ability: beliefs and performance. J. Res. Pers. 37, 420–445. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00021-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddish, P., Bulbulia, J., and Fischer, R. (2014). Does synchrony promote generalized prosociality? Religion Brain Behav. 4, 3–19. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2013.764545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddish, P., Fischer, R., and Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let's dance together: synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PloS One. 8, e71182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddish, P., Tong, E. M., Jong, J., Lanman, J. A., and Whitehouse, H. (2016). Collective synchrony increases prosociality towards non-performers and outgroup members. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 55, 722–738. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rennung, M., and Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Z. Psychol. 224, 168–189. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R., and Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 867–891. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., and Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 62. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., and Hershey, K. (2000). “Stability of temperament in childhood: laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years,” in Temperament and Personality Development Across the Life Span, eds. V. J. Molfese, D. L. Molfese, and R. R. McCrae (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 85–119.

Google Scholar

Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., and Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 472. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sallquist, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., and Gaertner, B. M. (2009). Assessment of preschoolers' positive empathy: concurrent and longitudinal relations with positive emotion, social competence, and sympathy. J. Posit. Psychol. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1080/17439760902819444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schachner, A., and Garvin, L. (2010). “Does synchrony really affect social variables? Effects on cooperation, conformity may not be robust,” in Proceedings of the Poster presented at the International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (Seattle, WA).

Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. C., and Fitzpatrick, P. (2019). Embodied synchronization and complexity in a verbal interaction. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 23, 199–228.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. C., and Richardson, M. J. (2008). “Dynamics of interpersonal coordination,” in Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics, eds A. Fuchs and V. Jirsa (Berlin: Springer), 281–308. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-74479-5_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reader, G. D., and Elliot, A. J. (1999). The relationship closeness induction task. Represent. Res. Soc. Psychol. 23, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 60. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Saporta, N., Marton-Alper, I. Z., and Gvirts, H. Z. (2019). Herding brains: a core neural mechanism for social alignment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohn, D. (2011). Anatomy of interaction experience: distinguishing sensory, semantic, and behavioral dimensions of interactivity. New Media Soc. 13, 1320–1335. doi: 10.1177/1461444811405806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Maes, P. J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2017a). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization–If you move in time with the beat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Mikkelsen, J., and Vuust, P. (2021). Higher empathy is associated with stronger social bonding when moving together with music. Psychol. Music. 1–16. doi: 10.1177/03057356211050681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Witek, M. A., Vuoskoski, J. K., Vuust, P. (2020). Cultural familiarity and individual musical taste differently affect social bonding when moving to music. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66529-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Wood, G., Witte, M. (2017b). Synchrony and sympathy: social entrainment with music compared to a metronome. Psychomusicol. Music Mind Brain. 27, 158. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, P., Gagnon, M., Gammage, K., Peters, S. (2015). Is the effect of behavioral synchrony on cooperative behavior mediated by pain threshold? J. Soc. Psychol. 155, 650–660. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swann, W. B. Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, Á., Whitehouse, H., and Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal: a theory of identity fusion. Psychol. Rev. 119, 441. doi: 10.1037/a0028589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150767. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Dunbar, R. I. (2014). Music and social bonding: “self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 5, 1096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Dunbar, R. I. (2016). Silent disco: dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 343–349. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., Husain, G. (2003). Perceiving prosody in speech: Effects of music lessons. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 530–532. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tschacher, W., Rees, G. M., Ramseyer, F. (2014). Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Front. Psychol. 5, 1323.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory Data Analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co.

Google Scholar

Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 399–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychol. Bull. 134, 383–403. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion. 11, 262. doi: 10.1037/a0021302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 693–695. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varlet, M., Marin, L., Capdevielle, D., Del-Monte, J., Schmidt, R. C., Salesse, R. N., et al. (2014). Difficulty leading interpersonal coordination: towards an embodied signature of social anxiety disorder. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waytz, A., Gray, K. (2018). Does online technology make us more or less sociable? A preliminary review and call for research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 473–491. doi: 10.1177/1745691617746509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, S. J., Coniston, D., Rogers, R., Frith, U. (2011). Developing the Frith-Happé animations: a quick and objective test of Theory of Mind for adults with autism. Autism Res. 4, 149–154. doi: 10.1002/aur.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiltermuth, S. S., Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wimmer, H., Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition. 13, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., Bolger, N., and Ochsner, K. (2008). It takes two: the interpersonal nature of empathic accuracy. Psychol. Sci. 19, 399–404. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02099.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: synchrony, theory of mind, empathy, social affiliation, online meetings, closeness

Citation: Basile C, Lecce S and van Vugt FT (2022) Synchrony During Online Encounters Affects Social Affiliation and Theory of Mind but Not Empathy. Front. Psychol. 13:886639. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886639

Received: 28 February 2022; Accepted: 07 June 2022;
Published: 11 July 2022.

Edited by:

Ilaria Gabbatore, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jonna Katariina Vuoskoski, University of Oslo, Norway
Ghilès Mostafaoui, École nationale supérieure de l'électronique et de ses applications, France

Copyright © 2022 Basile, Lecce and van Vugt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chiara Basile, chiara.basile01@universitadipavia.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.