Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 09 August 2022
Sec. Performance Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Rhythm Across the Arts and Sciences: A Synergy of Research View all 11 articles

The sweet spot between predictability and surprise: musical groove in brain, body, and social interactions

  • 1Center for Music in the Brain, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University and The Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 2Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

Groove—defined as the pleasurable urge to move to a rhythm—depends on a fine-tuned interplay between predictability arising from repetitive rhythmic patterns, and surprise arising from rhythmic deviations, for example in the form of syncopation. The perfect balance between predictability and surprise is commonly found in rhythmic patterns with a moderate level of rhythmic complexity and represents the sweet spot of the groove experience. In contrast, rhythms with low or high complexity are usually associated with a weaker experience of groove because they are too boring to be engaging or too complex to be interpreted, respectively. Consequently, the relationship between rhythmic complexity and groove experience can be described by an inverted U-shaped function. We interpret this inverted U shape in light of the theory of predictive processing and provide perspectives on how rhythmic complexity and groove can help us to understand the underlying neural mechanisms linking temporal predictions, movement, and reward. A better understanding of these mechanisms can guide future approaches to improve treatments for patients with motor impairments, such as Parkinson’s disease, and to investigate prosocial aspects of interpersonal interactions that feature music, such as dancing. Finally, we present some open questions and ideas for future research.

Introduction

In the field of musicology, the term groove was coined in the context of African-American musical genres, such as R&B, jazz, soul, disco, funk, and hip-hop, where it can refer to esthetic qualities of the music, specific rhythmic patterns, or the musicians’ way of effortlessly synchronizing and interacting with each other (Senn et al., 2019; Câmara and Danielsen, 2020; Duman et al., 2021). In contrast to this multifaceted understanding, recent studies in music perception and cognition agree on a sharper definition of groove as the pleasurable urge to move one’s body in relation to the rhythm of music (Madison, 2006; Janata et al., 2012; Stupacher et al., 2013; Senn et al., 2020). These different approaches to defining groove are discussed, for example, by Câmara and Danielsen (2020), who distinguish three aspects of groove: (1) a rhythmic pattern and performance, (2) a pleasurable urge to move, and (3) a state of being.

Here, we focus on the pleasurable, movement-inducing aspect of groove. In this approach, groove is genre-independent, in that every rhythmic pattern and performance evoking the pleasurable urge to move possesses the quality of groove. Although soul, disco, funk and related genres may be more likely to induce groove, the pleasurable urge to move can also be experienced while listening to rock, jazz, electronic dance music, and many other genres.

We argue that groove, when defined as the pleasurable urge to move to a rhythm, depends on a fine-tuned interplay between predictability and surprise. The predictability arises from repetitive rhythmic patterns, and the surprise arises from slight deviations from these patterns, for example in the form of syncopation. This tension represents the sweet spot of the groove experience, and is commonly found in rhythmic patterns that are simple enough for us to interpret and predict, but complex enough to keep us challenged and engaged (Vuust and Witek, 2014; Witek et al., 2014). We will use this perspective to discuss the experience of groove in body, brain, and social interactions.

Groove in body and brain

The neural mechanisms that are engaged from the moment we start to listen to music to the moment we are tapping our foot in time with the beat or start dancing, rely on the human brain’s ability to integrate external stimuli with internal representations, expectations, or predictions (Koelsch et al., 2019; Pando-Naude et al., 2021). This continuous flow of stimulus-driven bottom-up information and top-down processes requires specialized neural processing, such as audio-motor coupling (Jäncke, 2012), a phenomenon driven by temporal predictions (Vuust et al., 2009; Schröger et al., 2015) that is associated with reward, pleasure, and other cognitive and emotional mechanisms (Koelsch, 2010, 2014, 2020; Koelsch et al., 2013; Salimpoor et al., 2015). How accurately we can predict a rhythm and how pleasurable a rhythm is, therefore depend, on the one hand, on an individual’s long-term priors, such as listening biography, cultural background, musical expertise, dance training, and general cognitive and motor abilities, and on the other hand, on the rhythm’s complexity. Both aspects are integrated in the predictive coding framework (PC), which proposes that the brain minimizes prediction errors by using Bayesian inference when comparing a real-time internal model to a given sensory input (Friston, 2005). When listening to music, this means that we constantly check and update the predictive model of a rhythm by comparing it to the actual musical input (Vuust and Witek, 2014).

Groove and predictive processing

Within the context of groove, PC has predominately been deployed to interpret the inverted U-shaped relationship between groove ratings and the degree of rhythmic complexity (Vuust and Witek, 2014; Vuust et al., 2018; Koelsch et al., 2019). Rhythmic complexity can depend on different factors, such as meter or microtiming, but is most often operationalized as syncopation, which is when notes occur on weak metrical positions, and are followed by silences on stronger metrical positions (Longuet-Higgins and Lee, 1984). The inverted U shape suggests that moderately syncopated rhythms elicit the strongest sensation of groove (Sioros et al., 2014; Witek et al., 2014; Matthews et al., 2019; Stupacher et al., 2022). Under PC, this effect arises from the fact that moderately syncopated rhythms give rise to the greatest number of strongly weighted prediction errors. Prediction errors result from a mismatch between an internally generated model—here, the beat and meter—and the sensory input (Friston, 2005). Together, beat and meter form a predictive scaffold that determines how strongly we expect a note to occur at each time point (Vuust and Witek, 2014).

Panels A and B of Figure 1 illustrate how beat- and meter-based temporal predictions can be conceptualized as probability distributions (Large and Jones, 1999; Danielsen et al., 2019; Koelsch et al., 2019; Cannon, 2021), with their mean and spread reflecting the accuracy and certainty of these predictions, respectively. Prediction certainty determines the weight of the prediction error, that is, the degree to which it affects the metrical model. Syncopations violate meter-based predictions, and thus can introduce uncertainty into the metrical model and the subsequent predictions. Therefore, all else being equal, the degree of syncopation in a rhythm determines both the number and certainty of prediction errors. As shown in panels A and B of Figure 1, moderately syncopated rhythms combine a moderate number of prediction errors with a moderate degree of certainty. They therefore elicit the strongest top-down engagement to update and maintain the metrical model, that is, to minimize further prediction errors and uncertainty.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Beat-based predictions, prediction uncertainty, and prediction errors for three rhythms with low, medium, and high degrees of complexity, taken from Matthews et al. (2019). Dark blue rectangles denote the onsets of the rhythms and black rectangles denote the underlying beat with the height indicating the metric weight of each beat point according to (Longuet-Higgins and Lee, 1984). The black traces represent metrical models with beat-based predictions delineated as probability distributions wherein the mean of the distribution reflects the predicted onset times, and the width of the distribution reflects the certainty of that prediction. As can be seen across the three traces, the predictions and their certainty depend on the degree of syncopation, the metric weights of each beat, and the progression through the rhythm. That is, prediction accuracy and certainty start out relatively low for all three rhythms as it takes several onsets before a beat and meter is induced. Meter-based predictions can occur for each metric level relevant to a given rhythm, and depending on musical training (Palmer and Krumhansl, 1990), however, for simplicity, only beat-based predictions at the quarter note level are shown. (B) The inverted U shape arises from the product of the number of prediction errors and prediction certainty. Prediction errors increase from low syncopation rhythms to high, while the degree of prediction certainty decreases. Multiplying these functions reveals that moderately syncopated rhythms elicit the greatest number of strongly weighted prediction errors. (C) Groove ratings in Parkinson’s disease patients (N = 24) and healthy individuals (N = 27) from Pando-Naude et al. (in preparation). The inverted U-shaped relationship is shifted from moderately complex rhythms in healthy individuals, toward less complex rhythms in PD patients. Blue diamonds indicate mean values. Asterisks indicate significant differences (p < 0.05) in pairwise comparisons adjusted with the Tukey method. Boxplots: The centerline represents the median. The lower and upper ends of the boxes correspond to the first and third quartiles. Whiskers represent lowest and highest values within 1.5 × interquartile range (IQR) from the lower and upper quartiles, respectively. Dots represent values outside 1.5 × IQR. (D) The tendency of an inverted U shape in relation to the level of syncopation can also be found in social bonding with another person, as measured by Inclusion of Other in the Self (Stupacher et al., 2020).

According to PC, there are two ways to minimize prediction errors and thereby reduce uncertainty (Friston, 2003). One way is to change the model to better fit the input, either by shifting the phase of the meter, i.e., ‘resetting,’ to better align with the rhythm (Fitch and Rosenfeld, 2007), or switching to a different meter altogether, e.g., from a duple to a triple meter. Recent empirical and theoretical work implicates the motor system in the phase resetting and dynamic maintenance of temporal predictive processes during rhythmic auditory tasks (Morillon and Baillet, 2017; Rimmele et al., 2018). The second way to minimize prediction error is to move one’s body in time with the beat (Koelsch et al., 2019). Moving to the beat results in rhythmically timed proprioceptive input that reinforces the metrical model. Therefore, by optimally challenging the predictive model, moderately syncopated rhythms lead to greater engagement of the underlying motor systems, which is experienced as an automatic urge to move.

Uncertainty reduction also engenders the pleasurable component of groove. Within PC, this uncertainty reduction, as driven by improving the correspondence between model and input, corresponds to learning. In this context, learning is thought to be inherently rewarding as it serves the innate drive for competence (Ryan and Deci, 2000) and satisfies the intrinsic motivation for information gain (i.e., curiosity; Schmidhuber, 2010; Kidd and Hayden, 2015; Friston et al., 2017; Gottlieb and Oudeyer, 2018). Therefore, organisms are intrinsically motivated to seek out and attend to activities that afford maximal uncertainty reduction, in other words “learnable activities that are just beyond [their] current predictive capacities” (Oudeyer et al., 2016, p.9). In this context, moderately syncopated rhythms elicit pleasure by maximizing the intrinsic reward derived from actively refining our predictive processes via covert or overt motor processes. Recent work has demonstrated a direct link between music-evoked reward and moderate levels of prediction error and uncertainty in the context of melodic and harmonic expectations (Cheung et al., 2019; Gold et al., 2019b; Shany et al., 2019). We believe that applying a similar approach to groove will be crucial to understanding why a rhythmic sweet spot has such power to move us, and to uncovering the neural mechanisms that drive this effect.

Intriguingly, the basal ganglia (BG), a set of subcortical nuclei involved in motivation and motor control, along with dopaminergic transmission within the BG, have been implicated in all of the processes discussed above, including beat and meter-based perceptual and motor timing (Schubotz et al., 2000; Grahn and Brett, 2007; Grahn and Rowe, 2009; Schwartze et al., 2011; Kung et al., 2013), music-evoked pleasure (Salimpoor et al., 2011, 2013; Gold et al., 2019a), and prediction certainty (Friston et al., 2014; Owens et al., 2018; Gershman and Uchida, 2019). This suggests that the BG and dopamine play a crucial role in groove, a perspective that is supported by a recent fMRI study linking groove ratings to activity within limbic- and motor-associated BG nuclei (Matthews et al., 2020). Based on these results, the authors proposed a theoretical model wherein beat-based temporal predictions and the associated reward are integrated in the BG via parallel striato-cortical loops, particularly the limbic, motor, and associative loops (Alexander et al., 1986; Obeso et al., 2008).

Groove and Parkinson’s disease

One way to test the role of the BG and dopamine in groove is to compare Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients with healthy controls. PD results from dopaminergic dysfunctions in the BG caused by neuronal degeneration of the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc). Such dopaminergic depletion disrupts disinhibitory mechanisms between the BG and the motor thalamus, altering the fine-tuning between initiation and suppression of the activity in the motor loop, and giving rise to the characteristic motor symptomatology. In terms of groove, a recent study showed that the inverted U-shaped relationship between rhythmic complexity and pleasurable desire to move is shifted from moderately complex rhythms in healthy individuals, toward less complex rhythms in PD patients, who seem to prefer little incongruence between the internal predictive model and the stimulus (Figure 1C; Pando-Naude et al., in preparation). Notably, PD patients do not show an overall reduction in groove ratings, suggesting that PD does not reduce the overall urge to move to the rhythm, but only alters which types of stimuli elicit these responses, potentially as a function of altered predictive processes.

Research into auditory-motor activity and the neural correlates of rhythm perception (Schubotz et al., 2000; Grahn and Brett, 2007; Chen et al., 2008; Bengtsson et al., 2009; Grahn and Rowe, 2009; Grahn, 2012) has led to new approaches for developing movement therapies. Rhythmic auditory stimulation improves motor deficits in patients with PD by providing a regularly-timed cue, such as a metronome, with which patients can synchronize their gait (Thaut et al., 1996; Pau et al., 2016; Dalla Bella et al., 2017; Lei et al., 2019). However, the low rhythmic complexity of the metronome may restrict the method’s benefits (Dalla Bella et al., 2015; Cochen De Cock et al., 2018). In contrast, ecologically valid stimuli incorporating both rhythmic and harmonic elements may lead to a richer set of predictions, potentially promoting better guidance for temporal models of movements (Vuust et al., 2014). Dancing might offer an even more ecologically valid and rich PD intervention that can improve gait symmetry, decrease dual task costs (Fontanesi and DeSouza, 2021), and reduce disease severity (Krotinger and Loui, 2021). As discussed above, moderately complex stimuli are likely to increase engagement of motor timing and reward processes involving the BG (Matthews et al., 2020), potentially boosting motor benefits by increasing dopaminergic signaling in these nuclei (Salimpoor et al., 2013; Hansen et al., 2017; Matthews et al., 2020). A challenge for future research on music-supported movement and dance therapies is to investigate whether an individual PD patient’s preference for a certain level of rhythmic complexity may depend on the progression of the disease. One objective of future studies could therefore be to find individualized sweet spots of rhythmic complexity for PD patients with different levels of auditory, sensorimotor, and timing deficits.

By comparing the experience of groove in PD patients and healthy controls, we contribute to our understanding of the underlying neural mechanisms linking temporal predictions, movement, and reward. In turn, a better understanding of these mechanisms can guide future approaches to better treat motor deficits, and improve the quality of PD patients’ personal and social life.

Groove in social interactions

When people come together to listen to or make music, the level of rhythmic complexity that hits an individual’s sweet spot for an optimal groove experience depends on their biology and cultural background. In his interviews with musicians, Charles Keil noted that “each person has a unique feel for time and that bringing different or discrepant personalities together generates different kinds of groove” (Keil, 1995, p.8). Keil calls the intentional deviations in timing that result from the constant relating and negotiating between players participatory discrepancies and hypothesizes that they are necessary to make music involving and socially meaningful (Keil, 1987, 1995). This type of involvement may be especially strong with syncopated rhythms, as performers—live or recorded—can invite listeners and dancers to participate in the relating and negotiating by filling in the gaps in the syncopated rhythmic structure (Witek, 2017).

Groove and dance

Music and dance are so intertwined that some cultures do not distinguish between them (Haugen, 2021), and groove is an important element in understanding this connection (Foster Vander Elst et al., 2021). Fitch argues that “if we want to understand the rhythmic origins of a musical style, it behooves us to know how contemporaries would have moved to that music” (2016, p.6). Dance is also an intrinsically social activity that encourages social bonding (Tarr et al., 2014, 2015; Launay et al., 2016). Indeed, compared to synchronizing movements in silence or with a metronome, music can increase social closeness with another person (Stupacher et al., 2017, 2021). Furthermore, the number of people who can easily converse together is usually limited to four (Robertson et al., 2017), but much larger groups regularly form when people dance together. It has therefore been posited that the prosocial and emotional effects of group music-making and dancing might be evolutionary adaptations (Huron, 2001; Loersch and Arbuckle, 2013; Trainor, 2015).

From the perspective of predictive processing, beat and meter are mental models that can be shared by all dancers and musicians in a group. If the models are alike, i.e., ‘distributed’ across dancers, they facilitate synchronous movements, which in turn can promote shared affective experiences (Witek, 2019). In addition to strict synchronization, dance is commonly also concerned with the expression of creativity and “individual flourish” (Merker et al., 2009) within the framework of a mental model of beat and meter. When dancing together, moderately syncopated rhythms may provide the ideal stimulus for facilitating both united synchrony and individual creativity. On the one hand, moderately syncopated rhythms include enough notes on strong metrical positions that allow the dancers to form shared beat- and meter-based predictions. On the other hand, these rhythms also include notes on weak metrical positions, “injecting energy” into upbeats (Fitch, 2016), and pauses on strong metrical positions, inviting dancers to fill the “open spaces” with creative movements (Witek, 2017). Therefore, moderately syncopated rhythms may provide a common temporal framework within which dancers can share basic movements, but also inspire each other with individual creative movements—a combination that may facilitate the experience of shared emotions. This perspective is supported by a recent study suggesting that social bonding with another person tends to follow an inverted U shape in relation to the degree of syncopation (Figure 1D; Stupacher et al., 2020). A certain level of complexity is also preferred when playing the Mirror Game, in which two individuals move as coordinated and synchronously as possible (Ravreby et al., 2022). Ravreby and colleagues found that social bonding with the partner increases with increasing interpersonal synchronization, but also with movement complexity. Although simple movements benefit from greater synchronization accuracy, more complex and novel movements may be introduced to keep each other interested and engaged.

Both dancing and performing music in a group involve moving in time together. However, dance and music improve sensorimotor integration in both shared and unique ways (Giacosa et al., 2016, 2019; Karpati et al., 2017). For example, musicians perform better than dancers when synchronizing finger taps with auditory rhythms, whereas expert dancers perform better than musicians when imitating whole-body dance movements (Karpati et al., 2016). To date, most of the research investigating long-term-priors, such as the effects of expertise and listening biographies on groove is concerned with effects of musical training and preferred musical style. Musicians show greater neural activity in motor-related areas of the brain when listening to high-groove music (Stupacher et al., 2013), and a more pronounced U-shaped relationship between groove ratings and degree of syncopation (Matthews et al., 2019, 2022). Additionally, Senn and colleagues note that “listeners’ susceptibility to bodily entrainment as a response to music is strengthened when the music agrees with their taste” (Senn et al., 2018, p.26). The rare studies on groove that have tested the effect of dance experience are limited to behavioral paradigms, which suggest that participants with greater frequency and enjoyment of dancing experience more groove (Witek et al., 2014; Matthews et al., 2019). However, while both of these studies differentiate sharply between musicians and non-musicians based on years of training, the differentiation between dancers and non-dancers is less clear. Given the unique ways in which dancing affects whole-body sensorimotor integration (Giacosa et al., 2016, 2019), it would be beneficial for future work on the experience and neurophysiology of groove to use similarly sharp selection criteria for dancers. Groove is a universal phenomenon, but some effects may be more pronounced in experienced dancers, who are experienced in using their entire bodies to actively and creatively engage with music.

Conclusion

In dance, music making, and music listening, groove research can help us to better understand the interactions between temporal processing, movement, social behavior, and pleasure in both the general population and individuals with motor impairments. Elucidating the neural mechanisms underlying groove—especially the role of the basal ganglia—will contribute to our understanding of the integration of motor driven predictive timing and reward processes more generally.

One open question is how the experience of groove differs when comparing individual versus collective situations of music listening or dancing. It is also unclear how the type of rhythm-related movements affects groove experiences. Future research could, for example, compare whole body engagement in dance with specific movements of particular body parts, such as finger tapping or playing an instrument. Another future direction of groove research could be to investigate activities outside the field of music. Senn and colleagues define the verb to groove as “playing music together in an effortless and rhythmically well-coordinated manner” (Senn et al., 2020, p.46). This use of groove can also be applied to other coordinated interindividual activities, such as playing team sports, verbal communication, or gestural mimicking. Based on the relationship between rhythmic complexity and groove in music, it could be expected that in these activities rhythmic complexity follow similar U-shaped functions when measuring pleasure and engagement.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

JS: conceptualization and writing – original draft, review and editing. TEM, VP-N, and OVFE: writing – original draft, review and editing. PV: writing – review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The Center for Music in the Brain is funded by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF 117). JS was supported by an Erwin Schrödinger fellowship from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) (J-4288). This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) (J-4288).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alexander, G. E., Delong, M. R., and Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381.

Google Scholar

Bengtsson, S. L., Ullén, F., Henrik Ehrsson, H., Hashimoto, T., Kito, T., Naito, E., et al. (2009). Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex 45, 62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Câmara, G. S., and Danielsen, A. (2020). “Groove,” in The Oxford Handbook of Critical Concepts in Music Theory, eds A. Rehding and S. Rings (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 271–294.

Google Scholar

Cannon, J. (2021). Expectancy-based rhythmic entrainment as continuous Bayesian inference. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17:e1009025. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cerebr. Cortex 18, 2844–2854. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, V. K. M., Harrison, P. M. C., Meyer, L., Pearce, M. T., Haynes, J.-D., and Koelsch, S. (2019). Uncertainty and surprise jointly predict musical pleasure and amygdala, hippocampus, and auditory cortex activity. Curr. Biol. 29, 4084.e4–4092.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.09.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochen De Cock, V., Dotov, D. G., Ihalainen, P., Bégel, V., Galtier, F., Lebrun, C., et al. (2018). Rhythmic abilities and musical training in Parkinson’s disease: do they help? NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 4:8. doi: 10.1038/s41531-018-0043-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Benoit, C.-E., Farrugia, N., Keller, P. E., Obrig, H., Mainka, S., et al. (2017). Gait improvement via rhythmic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease is linked to rhythmic skills. Sci. Rep. 7:42005. doi: 10.1038/srep42005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Benoit, C.-E., Farrugia, N., Schwartze, M., and Kotz, S. A. (2015). Effects of musically cued gait training in Parkinson’s disease: beyond a motor benefit. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 77–85. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielsen, A., Nymoen, K., Anderson, E., Câmara, G. S., Langerød, M. T., Thompson, M. R., et al. (2019). Where is the beat in that note? Effects of attack, duration, and frequency on the perceived timing of musical and quasi-musical sounds. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 45, 402–418. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duman, D., Snape, N., Toiviainen, P., and Luck, G. (2021). Redefining groove. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mrp6v

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2016). Dance, music, meter and groove: a forgotten partnership. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:64. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T., and Rosenfeld, A. J. (2007). Perception and production of syncopated rhythms. Music Percept. 25, 43–58.

Google Scholar

Fontanesi, C., and DeSouza, J. F. X. (2021). Beauty that moves: dance for Parkinson’s effects on affect, self-efficacy, gait symmetry, and dual task performance. Front. Psychol. 11:600440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.600440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster Vander Elst, O., Vuust, P., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2021). Sweet anticipation and positive emotions in music, groove, and dance. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 39, 79–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2003). Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Netw. 16, 1325–1352. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2003.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Moutoussis, M., Behrens, T., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). The anatomy of choice: dopamine and decision-making. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369:598. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Lin, M., Frith, C. D., Pezzulo, G., Hobson, J. A., and Ondobaka, S. (2017). Active inference, curiosity and insight. Neural Comput. 29, 2633–2683. doi: 10.1162/NECO

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gershman, S. J., and Uchida, N. (2019). Believing in dopamine. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 703–714. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0220-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacosa, C., Karpati, F. J., Foster, N. E. V., Hyde, K. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2019). The descending motor tracts are different in dancers and musicians. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 3229–3246. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01963-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacosa, C., Karpati, F. J., Foster, N. E. V., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2016). Dance and music training have different effects on white matter diffusivity in sensorimotor pathways. NeuroImage 135, 273–286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, B. P., Mas-Herrero, E., Zeighami, Y., Benovoy, M., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2019a). Musical reward prediction errors engage the nucleus accumbens and motivate learning. PNAS 116, 3310–3315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1809855116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, B. P., Pearce, M. T., Mas-Herrero, E., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2019b). Predictability and uncertainty in the pleasure of music: a reward for learning? J. Neurosci. 39, 9397–9409. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0428-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottlieb, J., and Oudeyer, P. Y. (2018). Towards a neuroscience of active sampling and curiosity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 758–770. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0078-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A. (2012). Neural mechanisms of rhythm perception: current findings and future perspectives. Top. Cogn. Sci. 4, 585–606. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01213.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 893–906. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N., Dietz, M. J., and Vuust, P. (2017). Commentary: predictions and the brain: how musical sounds become rewarding. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:168. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haugen, M. R. (2021). Investigating music-dance relationships: a case study of Norwegian Telespringar. J. Music Theory 65, 17–38. doi: 10.1215/00222909-9124714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 43–61.

Google Scholar

Janata, P., Tomic, S. T., and Haberman, J. M. (2012). Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 54–75. doi: 10.1037/a0024208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäncke, L. (2012). The dynamic audio-motor system in pianists. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 246–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06416.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karpati, F. J., Giacosa, C., Foster, N. E. V., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2016). Sensorimotor integration is enhanced in dancers and musicians. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 893–903. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4524-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karpati, F. J., Giacosa, C., Foster, N. E. V., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2017). Dance and music share gray matter structural correlates. Brain Res. 1657, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keil, C. (1987). Participatory discrepancies and the power of music. Cult. Anthropol. 2, 275–283.

Google Scholar

Keil, C. (1995). The theory of participatory discrepancies: a progress report. Ethnomusicology 39, 1–19. doi: 10.2307/852198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, C., and Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88, 449–460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2010). Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 170–180. doi: 10.1038/nrn3666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2020). A coordinate-based meta-analysis of music-evoked emotions. NeuroImage 223:117350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Rohrmeier, M., Torrecuso, R., and Jentschke, S. (2013). Processing of hierarchical syntactic structure in music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15443–15448. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1300272110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Vuust, P., and Friston, K. (2019). Predictive processes and the peculiar case of music. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 63–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krotinger, A., and Loui, P. (2021). Rhythm and groove as cognitive mechanisms of dance intervention in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One 16:e0249933. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kung, S.-J., Chen, J. L., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). Interacting cortical and basal ganglia networks underlying finding and tapping to the musical beat. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 401–420. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Large, E. W., and Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol. Rev. 106, 119–159.

Google Scholar

Launay, J., Tarr, B., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding. Ethology 122, 779–789. doi: 10.1111/eth.12528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, J., Conradi, N., Abel, C., Frisch, S., Brodski-Guerniero, A., Hildner, M., et al. (2019). Cognitive effects of rhythmic auditory stimulation in Parkinson’s disease: a P300 study. Brain Res. 1716, 70–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loersch, C., and Arbuckle, N. L. (2013). Unraveling the mystery of music: music as an evolved group process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 777–798. doi: 10.1037/a0033691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longuet-Higgins, H. C., and Lee, C. S. (1984). The rhythmic interpretation of monophonic music. Music Percept. 1, 424–441. doi: 10.2307/40285271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madison, G. (2006). Experiencing groove induced by music: consistency and phenomenology. Music Percept. 24, 201–208. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A. G., Heggli, O. A., Penhune, V. B., and Vuust, P. (2019). The sensation of groove is affected by the interaction of rhythmic and harmonic complexity. PLoS One 14:e0204539. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A. G., Lund, T., Vuust, P., and Penhune, V. B. (2020). The sensation of groove engages motor and reward networks. NeuroImage 214:116768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A. G., Thibodeau, J. L. N., Vuust, P., and Penhune, V. B. (2022). Perceived motor synchrony with the beat is more strongly related to groove than measured synchrony. Music Percept. 39, 423–442.

Google Scholar

Merker, B. H., Madison, G. S., and Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex 45, 4–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morillon, B., and Baillet, S. (2017). Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, E8913–E8921. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705373114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeso, J. A., Rodríguez-Oroz, M. C., Benitez-Temino, B., Blesa, F. J., Guridi, J., Marin, C., et al. (2008). Functional organization of the basal ganglia: therapeutic implications for Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 548–559. doi: 10.1002/mds.22062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oudeyer, P. Y., Gottlieb, J., and Lopes, M. (2016). “Intrinsic motivation, curiosity, and learning: theory and applications in educational technologies,” in Progress in Brain Research, eds S. Waxman, D. G. Stein, D. Swaab, and H. Fields (Amsterdam: Elsevier). doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, A. P., Allen, M., Ondobaka, S., and Friston, K. J. (2018). Interoceptive inference: from computational neuroscience to clinic. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 90, 174–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, C., and Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Mental representations for musical meter. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 728–741. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.4.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pando-Naude, V., Matthews, T., Højlund, A., Jakobsen, S., Østergaard, K., Johnsen, E., et al. (in preparation). Parkinson’s Disease Reduces the Urge to Move to Medium Complex Groove Rhythms.

Google Scholar

Pando-Naude, V., Patyczek, A., Bonetti, L., and Vuust, P. (2021). An ALE meta-analytic review of top-down and bottom-up processing of music in the brain. Sci. Rep. 11:20813. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00139-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pau, M., Corona, F., Pili, R., Casula, C., Sors, F., Agostini, T., et al. (2016). Effects of physical rehabilitation integrated with rhythmic auditory stimulation on spatio-temporal and kinematic parameters of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Neurol. 7:126. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravreby, I., Shilat, Y., and Yeshurun, Y. (2022). Liking as a balance between synchronization, complexity and novelty. Sci. Rep. 12:3181. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-06610-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimmele, J. M., Morillon, B., Poeppel, D., and Arnal, L. H. (2018). Proactive sensing of periodic and aperiodic auditory patterns. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 870–882. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, C., Tarr, B., Kempnich, M., and Dunbar, R. (2017). Rapid partner switching may facilitate increased broadcast group size in dance compared with conversation groups. Ethology 123, 736–747. doi: 10.1111/eth.12643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science 340, 216–219. doi: 10.1126/science.1231059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Zald, D. H., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., and McIntosh, A. R. (2015). Predictions and the brain: how musical sounds become rewarding. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidhuber, J. (2010). Formal theory of creativity & intrinsic motivation (1990-2010). IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 2, 1–18.

Google Scholar

Schröger, E., Kotz, S. A., and SanMiguel, I. (2015). Bridging prediction and attention in current research on perception and action. Brain Res. 11:1626.

Google Scholar

Schubotz, R. I., Friederici, A. D., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Time perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartze, M., Keller, P. E., Patel, A. D., and Kotz, S. A. (2011). The impact of basal ganglia lesions on sensorimotor synchronization, spontaneous motor tempo, and the detection of tempo changes. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 685–691. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senn, O., Bechtold, T., Rose, D., Câmara, G. S., Düvel, N., Jerjen, R., et al. (2020). Experience of groove questionnaire: instrument development and initial validation. Music Percept. 38, 46–65. doi: 10.1525/mp.2020.38.1.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senn, O., Kilchenmann, L., Bechtold, T., and Hoesl, F. (2018). Groove in drum patterns as a function of both rhythmic properties and listeners’ attitudes. PLoS One 13:e0199604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senn, O., Rose, D., Bechtold, T., Kilchenmann, L., Hoesl, F., Jerjen, R., et al. (2019). Preliminaries to a psychological model of musical groove. Front. Psychol. 10:1228. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shany, O., Singer, N., Gold, B. P., Jacoby, N., Tarrasch, R., Hendler, T., et al. (2019). Surprise-related activation in the nucleus accumbens interacts with music-induced pleasantness. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 459–470. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sioros, G., Miron, M., Davies, M., Gouyon, F., and Madison, G. (2014). Syncopation creates the sensation of groove in synthesized music examples. Front. Psychol. 5:136. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Hove, M. J., Novembre, G., Schütz-Bosbach, S., and Keller, P. E. (2013). Musical groove modulates motor cortex excitability: a TMS investigation. Brain Cogn. 82, 127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Maes, P.-J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization – If you move in time with the beat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Mikkelsen, J., and Vuust, P. (2021). Higher empathy is associated with stronger social bonding when moving together with music. Psychol. Music [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1177/03057356211050681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Wrede, M., and Vuust, P. (2022). A brief and efficient stimulus set to create the inverted U-shaped relationship between rhythmic complexity and the sensation of groove. PLoS One 17, e0266902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Witek, M. A. G., Vuoskoski, J. K., and Vuust, P. (2020). Cultural familiarity and individual musical taste differently affect social bonding when moving to music. Sci. Rep. 10:10015. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66529-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., and Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 11:20150767. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding: “Self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 5:1096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., Rice, R. R., Miller, R. A., Rathbun, J., and Brault, J. M. (1996). Rhythmic auditory stimulation in gait training for Parkinson’s disease patients. Mov. Disord. 11, 193–200. doi: 10.1002/mds.870110213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trainor, L. J. (2015). The origins of music in auditory scene analysis and the roles of evolution and culture in musical creation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140089. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuust, P., Gebauer, L. K., and Witek, M. A. (2014). Neural underpinnings of music: the polyrhythmic brain. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 829, 339–356. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1782-2_18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuust, P., Ostergaard, L., Pallesen, K. J., Bailey, C., and Roepstorff, A. (2009). Predictive coding of music – Brain responses to rhythmic incongruity. Cortex 45, 80–92. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuust, P., Witek, M., Dietz, M., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2018). Now you hear it: a novel predictive coding model for understanding rhythmic incongruity. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1423, 19–29. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuust, P., and Witek, M. A. G. (2014). Rhythmic complexity and predictive coding: a novel approach to modeling rhythm and meter perception in music. Front. Psychol. 5:1111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witek, M. A. G. (2017). Filling in: syncopation, pleasure and distributed embodiment in groove. Music Anal. 36, 138–160. doi: 10.1111/musa.12082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witek, M. A. G. (2019). “Feeling at one: socio-affective distribution, vibe and dance music consciousness,” in Music and Consciousness 2: Worlds, Practices, Modalities, eds R. Herbert, D. Clarke, and E. Clarke (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 93–112.

Google Scholar

Witek, M. A. G., Clarke, E. F., Wallentin, M., Kringelbach, M. L., and Vuust, P. (2014). Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS One 9:e94446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: music, entrainment, rhythm, movement, dance, Parkinson’s, predictive coding, syncopation

Citation: Stupacher J, Matthews TE, Pando-Naude V, Foster Vander Elst O and Vuust P (2022) The sweet spot between predictability and surprise: musical groove in brain, body, and social interactions. Front. Psychol. 13:906190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.906190

Received: 28 March 2022; Accepted: 19 July 2022;
Published: 09 August 2022.

Edited by:

Adina Mornell, University of Music and Performing Arts Munich, Germany

Reviewed by:

Joseph F X DeSouza, York University, Canada
Birgitta Burger, University of Hamburg, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Stupacher, Matthews, Pando-Naude, Foster Vander Elst and Vuust. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jan Stupacher, stupacher@clin.au.dk

ORCID: Jan Stupacher, orcid.org/0000-0002-2179-2508

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.