Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 13 October 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Parental Mentalization: New Frontiers View all 8 articles

Reflective functioning in mothers with addictions: Differential relationships involving family history of mental illness and substance use

\r\nAmanda F. Lowell*Amanda F. Lowell1*Tal YatzivTal Yatziv1Elizabeth Peacock-ChambersElizabeth Peacock-Chambers2Amanda Zayde,Amanda Zayde3,4Cindy DeCosteCindy DeCoste5Nancy Suchman,Nancy Suchman1,5Thomas J. McMahon,Thomas J. McMahon1,5
  • 1Child Study Center, School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, University of Massachusetts Chan Medical School-Baystate, Springfield, MA, United States
  • 3Montefiore Medical Center, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 4Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States

Parental reflective functioning (RF) is often cited as an important domain in which mothers with addictions struggle in their roles as parents, though the links between addiction and RF remain unclear. Exposure to attachment trauma associated with parental mental illness and substance use is commonly associated with both addiction and lower RF. We thus examined how family history of parental mental illness and substance use may relate to the RF of mothers with addictions. One hundred ninety-four mothers in outpatient substance use treatment completed the Parent Development Interview and provided information about whether their mothers and fathers experienced mental illness or problems with substance use. Univariate ANOVAs revealed an interaction between family history of maternal mental illness and maternal substance use. Among mothers with a history of maternal substance use, those with a history of maternal mental illness had higher RF than those who had no history of maternal mental illness. Among mothers who did not report a family history of maternal mental illness, mothers who had a family history of maternal substance use exhibited significantly lower RF than mothers with no family history of maternal substance use. Exposure to paternal mental illness or substance use was not associated with mothers’ RF. These findings highlight the importance of disentangling the contributions of attachment trauma to mothers’ RF and utilizing interventions that support mothers’ capacity to reflect about how their early experiences of being cared for by a mother with a mental illness or addiction may impact their current caregiving behaviors.

Introduction

Mothers with addictions are often described in the literature as experiencing difficulties in their roles as parents. Firsthand accounts from mothers experiencing addiction describe challenges balancing recovery with caring for young children (Gazso, 2021), as well as feelings of ambivalence and guilt around the intersection of parenthood and substance use (Silva et al., 2013). Numerous empirical studies document that during interactions with their children, mothers with substance use disorders demonstrate poorer sensitivity to their children’s signals (Hatzis et al., 2017; Porreca et al., 2018; Romanowicz et al., 2019) as well as more harsh parenting practices (Kelley et al., 2015) and high rates of involvement with child welfare and legal systems (Paltrow and Flavin, 2013; Taplin and Mattick, 2015; Moreland et al., 2021). Poor parental reflective functioning (RF), or the capacity to make sense of one’s own and one’s child’s behaviors in terms of thoughts, emotions, wishes, and intentions (Slade, 2005), underlies many of these caregiving challenges (Suchman et al., 2004; Pajulo et al., 2006; Suchman et al., 2006; Macfie et al., 2020).

Notably, one’s level of RF originates within early attachment relationships. Ideally, infants are thought to learn about the minds of others by being held in mind by their caregivers (Fonagy and Target, 1997; Slade, 2005). However, caregiving contexts in which this process is disrupted may impede the development of RF capacities. Examining the caregiving contexts experienced by mothers with addictions may therefore be important in order to better understand what may be at play in the manifestation of their RF as they care for the next generation.

Parental mental illness and substance use in one’s family growing up are two such caregiving contexts in which mothers may not have consistently experienced the opportunity to develop optimal RF. Specifically, parental mental illness and substance use may result in attachment trauma (Allen, 2018), the child’s experience of the caregiver as physically or emotionally unavailable to support and regulate them when experiencing distress, further contributing to the child’s interpersonal distress and distrust. In the case of parental mental illness, a caregiver’s emotional lability, irritability, under-regulation, affective distress, or inconsistency can be frightening to the infant who is in need of comfort. In the case of parental substance use, a caregiver’s detachment, intoxication, withdrawal symptoms, and preoccupation with obtaining substances can be similarly distressing to the infant who depends on activating and engaging the caregiver for security and survival. Given that “understanding of minds is hard without the experience of having been understood as a person with a mind” (Fonagy and Target, 2005, p. 334), we might anticipate that individuals whose caregivers were misattuned to their minds and emotions because of mental illness or substance use may grow into adults, have children, and struggle in their parental RF, with the potential for repeating maladaptive intergenerational patterns. This is evident in prior work demonstrating that, for mothers with addictions in particular, those who demonstrated poor RF had experienced significantly more adversity in childhood compared to mothers with moderate to high RF (Håkansson et al., 2018).

In this study, we thus sought to identify how specific risk factors for attachment trauma, namely family history of maternal and paternal mental illness and substance use, may relate to the parental RF of mothers with addictions. Given evidence that attachment representations of maternal and paternal figures evolve independently with the potential for differential effects on developmental outcomes for the child (Dagan and Sagi-Schwartz, 2018; Dagan et al., 2022), we predicted that a maternal history of mental illness and substance use would both be associated with mothers’ RF. Similarly, we predicted that a paternal history of mental illness and substance use would also both be associated with RF. Consistent with prior research (Macfie et al., 2020) and cumulative risk theory (Evans et al., 2013) wherein multiple exposures to adversity are associated with poorer outcomes, we also predicted that mothers in our sample who experienced both a family history of mental illness and substance use would exhibit lower RF compared to mothers who experienced parental mental illness or parental substance use alone. Specifically, (a) mothers with no exposure to either parental mental illness or parental substance use would have greater RF than mothers exposed to either alone and (b) mothers exposed to either parental mental illness or parental substance use alone would have greater RF than mothers exposed to both. Such findings would have the potential to inform intervention efforts aimed at improving the RF of mothers with addictions.

Methods

Participants

In the current study, we used baseline data from 194 mothers recruited across three randomized controlled trials (RCT) testing the efficacy of a parenting intervention designed specifically for mothers in treatment for substance use disorders: Mothering from the Inside Out (Suchman et al., 2008, 2010a,2011, 2017; Lowell et al., 2022). Mothers were recruited from a community-based outpatient substance use treatment agency comprised of several clinics in the greater New Haven, CT area. Mothers were referred to participate in this research by substance use counselors, research staff, or word of mouth.

Mothers were eligible to participate in the study if they were enrolled in outpatient substance use treatment, English-speaking, and caring for at least one child between birth and 60 months of age at time of enrollment. If mothers had more than one child in their care within this age range, mothers chose which child they wanted to focus on when completing the parenting-focused research measures. Exclusion criteria included mothers requiring inpatient hospitalization or detoxification, and mothers experiencing psychosis, suicidality, or significant cognitive impairment. Mothers who met eligibility requirements met individually with a research assistant to complete informed consent procedures.

Baseline data for the first (n = 43), second (n = 68), and third (n = 83) RCTs were merged into a single dataset to increase sample size and statistical power. On average, mothers were approximately 30 years old, had a high school education, and had an average of 2 children. Most mothers were white, unemployed, and were on medication for an opioid use disorder (MOUD). Mothers generally began using substances at a young age, and reported low levels of substance use in the past month. Detailed demographic data can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics by trial.

Measures

Family history

A 90-min structured intake interview was used to identify baseline demographic and psychosocial characteristics of the sample. Mothers provided information about their psychosocial history including their own substance use, psychiatric, legal, and medical history. They also provided information regarding their early development and family history including whether their mother and father experienced mental health or substance use problems. Specifically, mothers provided responses to items derived from the Addiction Severity Index (ASI; McLellan et al., 1992) asking whether any of their biologically-related relatives experienced significant substance use or psychiatric problems that did (or should have) led to treatment. For the current study, we were interested in mothers’ dichotomous yes/no responses to whether their (1) mother experienced mental illness, (2) mother experienced substance use problems, (3) father experienced mental illness, and (4) father experienced substance use problems. The ASI has good reliability and validity in terms of assessing history of first-degree relatives’ substance use and psychiatric problems (Cacciola et al., 1999; Coviello et al., 2004).

Reflective functioning

The Parent Development Interview (PDI; Slade et al., 2003), a widely used instrument for assessing parental reflective functioning, is a semi-structured interview with 19 questions designed to elicit a parent’s verbal narrative about common emotionally-challenging experiences of caring for young children. With permission from the measure’s originator, a shortened 14-item version was used to minimize assessment burden and include positive emotion items (e.g., “Have you ever felt deeply touched or moved as a parent?”). On each item, additional queries were included to encourage the mother to consider her own and her child’s intentions and emotions during these instances and how these mental states might have affected her and her child’s behavior. Interviews were recorded and transcribed verbatim, and mothers’ responses were coded for parental reflective functioning (Slade et al., 2007) by coders who were trained by the measure’s originator, and who were blind with respect to the timepoint at which the interview was conducted and the clinical characteristics and random assignment of the participants. Each response was rated on a 10-point scale ranging from −1 to 9, where −1 indicates active resistance to reflecting about mental states; 1 indicates the absence of recognition of mental states (i.e., events are described solely in terms of behavior); 3 indicates a limited capacity to acknowledge mental states and a lack of understanding how mental states function; 5 indicates ordinary reflective functioning, with mothers demonstrating a rudimentary capacity to understand how mental states work together and influence behavior; scores above 5 indicate an increasingly rich and complex understanding of how mental states arise, interact within and between individuals, and influence relational behavior. In each of the three trials, the mean of all responses was calculated to generate a mean RF score. Inter-rater reliability was established for each trial, with interclass correlations established as ≥0.50 in the first trial (Suchman et al., 2010a,2011), ≥0.77 in the second trial (Suchman et al., 2017), and ≥0.57 in the third trial (Lowell et al., 2022).

Procedure

The current study employed a cross-sectional approach where all baseline data were obtained during the pre-treatment phase of three RCT cohorts testing the efficacy of Mothering from the Inside Out. Baseline assessments consisted of a comprehensive biopsychosocial intake interview, the PDI, and several additional measures that were not used in this study. All procedures were approved by the Yale University Human Investigation Committee.

Data analysis

Two univariate ANOVAs were conducted with mean RF as the dependent variable. Mothers’ family history of mental illness (yes/no) and substance use (yes/no) and an interaction term were entered as the independent variables. ANOVAs were conducted separately for maternal and paternal history. Four post hoc comparisons were conducted for significant interaction effects. The impact of parental substance use within parental mental illness was examined, and the impact of parental mental illness within parental substance use was examined. Effect sizes are presented as partial eta-squared (ηp2), where 0.01 represents a small effect size, 0.06 a medium effect size, and 0.14 a large effect size (Cohen, 1988).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Mothers in the sample generally demonstrated suboptimal reflective functioning (M = 3.15, SD = 0.54) in light of scoring guidelines where a score of 3 reflects the use of mental state language (happy, sad, etc.) without elaboration upon the meaning of the child’s mental states or evidence of linking mental states to behavior (Slade et al., 2003). Analysis of between group differences in demographic characteristics indicated that mothers across the three RCTs did not differ in terms of age, education, employment status, ethnicity, marital status, enrollment in MOUD, and number of children (p’s > 0.11). However, mothers in the first trial reported more involvement with child welfare [F(2,191) = 5.45, p < 0.005], and earlier onset of opioid use [F(2,141) = 6.11, p < 0.003] compared to mothers in the second and third trials, as well as younger target children compared to those in the third trial [F(2,174) = 3.10, p < 0.05]. Additionally, mothers in the third trial reported more cannabis use in the past month compared to mothers in the first trial [F(2,189) = 5.37, p < 0.005].

Given previous work demonstrating a relationship between parental RF and sociodemographic factors (Sleed et al., 2018), we explored these relationships prior to our main analyses. We found that mean RF was related to maternal age (r = 0.16, p < 0.03) and education (r = 0.16, p < 0.02). However, when we controlled for these variables in the analyses, the results did not differ; we therefore report results without covariates included in the statistical model.

Maternal history

A univariate ANOVA specifying maternal history (mental illness/substance use) was conducted to examine mothers’ mean RF. This analysis revealed a main effect for maternal mental illness [F(1,190) = 1.68, MSE = 0.28, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.03], a main effect for maternal substance use [F(1,190) = 1.68, MSE = 0.28, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.03], and a significant interaction between history of maternal mental illness and maternal substance use [F(1,190) = 4.24, MSE = 0.28, p = 0.04]. Inclusion of mothers’ age and years of education as covariates did not alter the pattern of results.

Post hoc comparisons revealed that among mothers who reported a family history of maternal mental illness, maternal substance use was not associated with differences in mean RF [F < 1, p > 0.05]. In contrast, among mothers who did not report a history of maternal mental illness, mothers who reported a family history of maternal substance use exhibited significantly lower mean RF than mothers with no family history of maternal substance use [F(1,190) = 10.33, MSE = 0.28, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.05].

Among mothers with a history of maternal substance use, those with a history of maternal mental illness had higher RF than those who had no history of maternal mental illness [F(1,190) = 10.65, MSE = 0.28, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.05]. Among mothers with no family history of maternal substance use, there was no difference between mothers who did or did not report a family history of maternal mental illness [F < 1, p > 0.05]. See Table 2 for descriptive statistics.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for mothers’ mean RF by family history of maternal mental illness and maternal substance use.

Paternal history

A univariate ANOVA specifying paternal history (mental illness/substance use) was conducted to examine mothers’ mean RF. This analysis revealed no significant main effect of paternal mental illness, no significant main effect of paternal substance use, and no significant interaction effect on mothers’ RF [F’s < 1, p’s > 0.05]. Because the interaction effect was not significant, no pairwise comparisons were conducted. Inclusion of mothers’ age and years of education as covariates did not alter the pattern of results. See Table 3 for descriptive statistics.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for mothers’ mean RF by family history of paternal mental illness and paternal substance use.

Discussion

This study utilized baseline data merged across three RCTs testing the efficacy of Mothering from the Inside Out, a parenting intervention designed specifically for mothers in treatment for substance use disorders. A large sample of mothers in outpatient substance use treatment completed the PDI as a measure of RF and provided the opportunity to examine differential relationships involving family history of mental illness and substance use. Overall, our findings replicate and extend prior literature on mothers with addictions suggesting that perceptions of early attachment relationships influence current caregiving (Suchman et al., 2005). Consistent with our first hypothesis, a family history of maternal mental illness and substance use were specifically associated with mothers’ RF, although paternal mental illness and substance use were not. Our cumulative risk hypothesis was not supported given the significant interaction effect where mothers who reported a family history of maternal substance use alone demonstrated lower RF than those who reported both maternal substance use and maternal mental illness. Further, counter to prior work denoting that greater adversity was related to poorer RF (Håkansson et al., 2018), among mothers in our sample who experienced a family history of maternal mental illness, family history of maternal substance use was surprisingly not associated with lower RF. While caution is warranted to not over-interpret these findings, we suggest some possible explanations and clinical implications that will require further investigation.

In the absence of maternal mental illness, our hypothesis that a family history of maternal substance use would be associated with lower RF stood true. This is consistent with literature suggesting that parents with substance use problems may be withdrawn and neglectful (Kepple, 2018), which does not characterize an environment in which a child’s reflective capacity is fostered (Allen, 2018). Further, maternal substance use might be characterized by psychological unavailability, which Allen (2018, p. 204) notes, “had a greater adverse impact on development than physical neglect or other forms of maltreatment. Thus the subtlest form of maltreatment had the most severe consequences.” Indeed, history of emotional maltreatment in childhood has been similarly linked with maladaptive patterns of parental RF and less supportive caregiving behavior (Condon et al., 2021), consistent with the notion that experiencing one’s own caregiver as emotionally unavailable may play a role in impacting levels of RF in mothers with family history of maternal substance use.

Childhood experiences of maternal substance use and psychological unavailability may thus propagate on to impact the next generation’s parenting through maladaptive cognitions and behaviors (Milligan et al., 2022). Compromised RF capacities impedes one’s ability to make sense of their interpersonal relationships and trauma, both past and present (Fonagy et al., 1994; Berthelot et al., 2015). Furthermore, low parental RF often manifests in failing to see how one’s experiences in childhood have shaped their relationships with their own children (Slade et al., 2007). Thus, the lower RF observed among mothers with substance use problems who also had a family history of maternal substance use may indicate difficulties in reorganizing childhood experiences as adults and lacking the psychological and interpersonal resources to “break the cycle.” This may then manifest repeating patterns characteristic of low RF and lack of emotional availability with their own child (Milligan et al., 2022), including difficulties tolerating child distress and interpreting the child’s intentions as inappropriately hostile (Rutherford et al., 2015), and exhibiting hostility toward the child while struggling to support them (Rosenblum et al., 2008; Zeegers et al., 2017; Condon et al., 2021). Such repetition of past patterns may be even more salient under stress, as high stress and arousal can reactivate representations of attachment trauma (Luyten and Fonagy, 2015), potentially reinforcing both increased substance use (Rutherford and Mayes, 2019) and use of maladaptive pre-mentalizing modes of parental RF (Rutherford et al., 2015).

The paradox in our results is the finding that exposure to maternal substance use alone was associated with lower RF than exposure to maternal substance use in the presence of maternal mental illness. It is possible that participants whose mothers experienced mental illness were more likely to have been identified by healthcare providers, child protective services, or family members as needing additional support, with the ultimate outcome of being removed from the home and placed in more supportive kinship or foster care settings that supported RF development. Based on addiction typology literature (Cloninger et al., 1996), it may also be possible that individuals with type II mothers (i.e., substance use onset < 25 years, more severe addiction, polysubstance use) may have incurred more risk in early childhood associated with the earlier onset of their mothers’ substance use. In contrast, participants with type I parents (i.e., later onset of substance use, secondary to mental illness) may have experienced less risk in infancy and early childhood given that their mothers may have been more psychologically available before the onset of their addictions, therefore providing an early window for RF development.

Alternatively, participants who reported both maternal substance use and mental illness may have developed RF as an adaptive strategy for making sense of their mother’s substance, by understanding it as a means of coping with mental illness, for example. The developmental capacity of RF requires the ability to consider nuanced meaning driving a persons’ behavior, and it is possible that witnessing substance use in the context of maternal mental illness provided organization to the meaning-making process compared to children of mothers without mental illness. The context of maternal mental illness may also encourage an adaptive strategy where children grow attuned to (or even hypervigilant of) their mother’s affective dysregulation and become practiced at reflecting on and anticipating the mental states potentially underlying their mother’s behavior (Miller, 2008). This interpretation is in line with prior findings that children who experienced physical abuse (i.e., trauma accompanied by parental emotion dysregulation, inconsistency, and hostility) were highly adept at detecting subtle emotional cues (Pollak and Sinha, 2002). Similarly heightened emotion recognition and threat detection has been observed in children exposed to parental depression (Lopez-Duran et al., 2013) and substance use (Flykt et al., 2021). Overall, such hypersensitivity may help individuals predict and thereby cope with inconsistent, unpredictable, dysregulated, or confusing caregiving behavior exhibited by mothers experiencing mental illness. Though potentially adaptive in the context of one’s family of origin, it is unclear whether this heightened sensitivity to and reflection about others’ emotions translates to accurate detection of infant cues, and ultimately whether it is adaptive when caring for the next generation. It should also be kept in mind that despite the relatively higher level of RF in this group, mothers nonetheless exhibited below-average RF indicative of having some mentalizing difficulties.

Limitations and future directions

Large clinical datasets like the one used in the current study lend valuable information to our understanding of factors affecting high-risk samples and the nuanced differences in RF that exist within such samples. Overall, this secondary data analysis requires replication and extension to further elucidate the links between risk factors for attachment trauma and RF in mothers, as well as fathers and other types of caregivers, with addictions. Although the overall sample was large, some potentially meaningful effects may not have proven significant because of limited statistical power associated with small cell size. The overall mean RF in our sample was also relatively low regardless of family history, and the statistically significant differences found in the current study may not necessarily translate to clinical significance. Given the nature of our secondary data analysis, we were also limited to the information collected as part of the broader RCTs investigating treatment efficacy. While we utilized a well-validated, widely used interview measure of parental RF (the PDI), our assessment of specific risk factors for attachment trauma was limited to mothers’ self-report on ASI items regarding the presence or absence of substance use and psychiatric problems in biologically-related family members. Due to the nature of the ASI, we unfortunately did not have details regarding severity or age of onset of parental mental illness/substance. Parental mental illness was also collapsed into one category which did not allow for subanalyses of the potential impact of different types of parental mental illness (e.g., depression vs. anxiety vs. personality pathology) on mothers’ RF. Future studies should employ a more detailed assessment of risk factors for attachment trauma including family history of mental illness and substance use in addition to other challenges that may interfere with a caregiver’s capacity to hold their child in mind such as intimate partner violence, poverty, and racism, amongst others. Alternatively, future research may investigate these relationships using a more detailed measure of unresolved trauma or hostile-helpless states of mind via the Adult Attachment Interview (Lyons-Ruth et al., 2003; Main et al., 2008). It will also be important to investigate whether our results are generalizable to other samples of mothers with addictions, including mothers who are not currently in substance use treatment, or mothers who are in residential or inpatient treatment. While the majority of mothers in our sample were in treatment for an opioid use disorder (MOUD), it may also be important to see if these results vary by primary drug of abuse. Future research examining the links between attachment trauma and RF may also include a measure of mothers’ other resiliency factors, such as participation in prior mental health treatment or other positive relational experiences (Lieberman et al., 2005; Narayan et al., 2017), that may have influenced their reflective capacities. In light of the findings of the current study, it may also be critical to examine whether family history of maternal mental illness and substance use may moderate mothers’ response to parenting interventions.

Implications

Overall, studying mothers’ experiences within attachment relationships can enhance the field’s understanding of potential antecedents of reflective functioning in the context of addiction. Our findings highlight the importance of disentangling the contributions of different types of childhood adversity or attachment trauma, namely family history of mental illness and substance use, to mothers’ reflective capacities. Critically, a mother’s experience of her caregivers has the potential to influence the care she provides to the next generation. Treatment efforts may potentially be enhanced if we are able to identify underlying factors connecting maternal addiction with RF. Our findings suggest that clinicians working with mothers in recovery from substance use disorders might consider assessing family history of mental illness and substance use and utilizing this information in conceptualization, treatment planning, and clinical approach. Consistent with theoretical and empirical work (Suchman et al., 2004, 2006, 2010b), parenting interventions for mothers with addictions are most successful when there is a focus on enhancing mothers’ RF. Based on the results presented here, this may specifically include helping mothers reflect upon how their early experiences of being cared for by a mother with a mental illness and/or addiction may influence their current caregiving relationships, capacities, and behaviors.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Deidentified clinical baseline dataset collected as part of three randomized controlled trials. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to TM, thomas.mcmahon@yale.edu.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Yale University Human Investigation Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AL was responsible for the conceptualization and first draft of this manuscript, with contributions from TY, EP-C, AZ, AL, CD, and TM on subsequent drafts. TY and TM contributed to statistical analyses and interpretation. EP-C, AZ, CD, and TM contributed to interpretation of the findings and the editorial process. We posthumously acknowledge NS for having completed the three randomized controlled trials that supplied the baseline data included in this manuscript. All authors have approved the final submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01 DA017294, K23 DA014606, K02 DA023504, K23 DA050731, UL 1TR002544, and R01 MH126821).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The views presented in this manuscript are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the funding agencies.

References

Allen, J. G. (2018). Mentalizing in the Development and Treatment of Attachment Trauma. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Berthelot, N., Ensink, K., Bernazzani, O., Normandin, L., Luyten, P., and Fonagy, P. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of attachment in abused and neglected mothers: The role of trauma-specific reflective functioning. Infant Ment. Health J. 36, 200–212. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacciola, J. S., Koppenhaver, J. M., McKay, J. R., and Alterman, A. I. (1999). Test–retest reliability of the lifetime items on the Addiction Severity Index. Psychol. Assess. 11:86. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.11.1.86

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., and Bohman, M. (1996). Type I and type II alcoholism: An update. Alcohol Health and Res. World 20:18.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Condon, E. M., Tobon, A. L., Holland, M. L., Slade, A., Mayes, L., and Sadler, L. S. (2021). Examining mothers’ childhood maltreatment history, parental reflective functioning, and the long-term effects of the Minding the Baby® home visiting intervention. Child Maltreat. 27, 378–388. doi: 10.1177/1077559521999097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coviello, D. M., Alterman, A. I., Cacciola, J. S., Rutherford, M. J., and Zanis, D. A. (2004). The role of family history in addiction severity and treatment response. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 26, 303–13.

Google Scholar

Dagan, O., and Sagi-Schwartz, A. (2018). Early attachment network with mother and father: An unsettled issue. Child Dev. Perspect. 12, 115–121.

Google Scholar

Dagan, O., Schuengel, C., Verhage, M. L., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi-Schwartz, A., Madigan, S., et al. (2022). Configurations of mother-child and father-child attachment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing behavioral problems: An individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2021, 67–94. doi: 10.1002/cad.20450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Li, D., and Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol. Bull. 139:1342.

Google Scholar

Flykt, M. S., Lindblom, J., Belt, R., and Punamäki, R. L. (2021). The role of mother’s prenatal substance use disorder and early parenting on child social cognition at school age. Infant Child Dev. 30:e2221.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 679–700. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Target, M. (2005). Bridging the transmission gap: An end to an important mystery of attachment research? Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 333–343. doi: 10.1080/14616730500269278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Higgitt, A., and Target, M. (1994). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1992 The theory and practice of resilience. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35, 231–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01160.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazso, A. (2021). Managing more than poverty when living with addiction: parents’ emotion and identity work. J. Fam. Issues. 1–22. doi: 10.1177/0192513X211041981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Håkansson, U., Watten, R., Söderström, K., Skårderud, F., and Øie, M. G. (2018). Adverse and adaptive childhood experiences are associated with parental reflective functioning in mothers with substance use disorder. Child Abuse Negl. 81, 259–273. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzis, D., Dawe, S., Harnett, P., and Barlow, J. (2017). Quality of caregiving in mothers with illicit substance use: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Subst. Abuse 11:1178221817694038. doi: 10.1177/1178221817694038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, M. L., Lawrence, H. R., Milletich, R. J., Hollis, B. F., and Henson, J. M. (2015). Modeling risk for child abuse and harsh parenting in families with depressed and substance-abusing parents. Child Abuse Negl. 43, 42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kepple, N. J. (2018). Does parental substance use always engender risk for children? Comparing incidence rate ratios of abusive and neglectful behaviors across substance use behavior patterns. Child Abuse Negl. 76, 44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, A. F., Padrón, E., Van Horn, P., and Harris, W. W. (2005). Angels in the nursery: The intergenerational transmission of benevolent parental influences. Infant Ment. Health J. 26, 504–520.

Google Scholar

Lopez-Duran, N. L., Kuhlman, K. R., George, C., and Kovacs, M. (2013). Facial emotion expression recognition by children at familial risk for depression: High-risk boys are oversensitive to sadness. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 565–574. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowell, A., DeCoste, C., Dalton, R., Dias, H., Martino, S., McMahon, T. J., et al. (2022). Mothering from the Inside Out: Results of a community-based randomized efficacy trial testing a mentalization-based parenting intervention for mothers with addictions [Manuscript submitted for publication], New Haven.

Google Scholar

Luyten, P., and Fonagy, P. (2015). The neurobiology of mentalizing. Pers. Disord. 6, 366–379. doi: 10.1037/per0000117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons-Ruth, K., Yellin, C., Melnick, S., and Atwood, G. (2003). Childhood experiences of trauma and loss have different relations to maternal unresolved and hostile-helpless states of mind on the AAI. Attach. Hum. Dev. 5, 330–352. doi: 10.1080/14616730310001633410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macfie, J., Zvara, B. J., Stuart, G. L., Kurdziel-Adams, G., Kors, S. B., Fortner, K. B., et al. (2020). Pregnant women’s history of childhood maltreatment and current opioid use: The mediating role of reflective functioning. Addict. Behav. 102:106134. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Main, M., Hesse, E., and Goldwyn, R. (2008). “Studying differences in language usage in recounting attachment history: An introduction to the AAI,” in Clinical applications of the Adult Attachment Interview, eds H. Steele and M. Steele (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 31–68.

Google Scholar

McLellan, A. T., Kushner, H., Metzger, D., Peters, R., Smith, I., Grissom, G., et al. (1992). The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index. J Subst Abuse Treat. 9, 199–213.

Google Scholar

Miller, A. (2008). The Drama of the Gifted Child: The Search for the True Self. New York, NY: Basic books.

Google Scholar

Milligan, K., Rodrigues, E. R., Daari-Herman, L., and Urbanoski, K. A. (2022). Parental reflective function in substance use disorder: Individual differences and intervention potential. Curr. Addict. Rep. 9, 59–66. doi: 10.1007/s40429-021-00391-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreland, A., Newman, C., Crum, K. I., and Are, F. (2021). Types of child maltreatment and child welfare involvement among opioid-using mothers involved in substance use treatment. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 126:106021. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2021.106021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narayan, A. J., Ippen, C. G., Harris, W. W., and Lieberman, A. F. (2017). Assessing angels in the nursery: A pilot study of childhood memories of benevolent caregiving as protective influences. Infant Ment. Health J. 38, 461–474. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pajulo, M., Suchman, N., Kalland, M., and Mayes, L. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of residential treatment for substance abusing pregnant and parenting women: Focus on maternal reflective functioning and mother-child relationship. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 448–465. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paltrow, L. M., and Flavin, J. (2013). Arrests of and forced interventions on pregnant women in the United States, 1973–2005: Implications for women’s legal status and public health. J. Health Polit. Policy Law 38, 299–343. doi: 10.1215/03616878-1966324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollak, S. D., and Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev. Psychol. 38:784.

Google Scholar

Porreca, A., Biringen, Z., Parolin, M., Saunders, H., Ballarotto, G., and Simonelli, A. (2018). Emotional Availability, Neuropsychological Functioning, and Psychopathology: The Context of Parental Substance Use Disorder. Biomed Res. Int. 2018:5359037. doi: 10.1155/2018/5359037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romanowicz, M., Voort, J. L. V., Shekunov, J., Oesterle, T. S., Thusius, N. J., Rummans, T. A., et al. (2019). The effects of parental opioid use on the parent–child relationship and children’s developmental and behavioral outcomes: A systematic review of published reports. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 13:5. doi: 10.1186/s13034-019-0266-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenblum, K. L., McDonough, S. C., Sameroff, A. J., and Muzik, M. (2008). Reflection in thought and action: Maternal parenting reflectivity predicts mind-minded comments and interactive behavior. Infant Ment. Health J. 29, 362–376. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, H. J. V., and Mayes, L. C. (2019). Parenting stress: A novel mechanism of addiction vulnerability. Neurobiol. Stress 11:100172. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, H. J. V., Booth, C. R., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., and Mayes, L. C. (2015). Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behav. Dev. 40, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, S. A., Pires, A. P., Guerreiro, C., and Cardoso, A. (2013). Balancing motherhood and drug addiction: The transition to parenthood of addicted mothers. J. Health Psychol. 18, 359–367. doi: 10.1177/1359105312443399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 269–281.

Google Scholar

Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., and Kaplan, M. (2003). The Parent Development Interview - Revised [Unpublished manuscript]. New York, NY: City University of New York.

Google Scholar

Slade, A., Bernbach, E., Grienenberger, J., Levy, D., and Locker, A. (2007). Addendum to Fonagy, Target, Steele, & Steele reflective functioning scoring manual for use with the Parent Development Interview (Unpublished Manuscript ed.). New York, NY: The City University of New York.

Google Scholar

Sleed, M., Slade, A., and Fonagy, P. (2018). Reflective Functioning on the Parent Development Interview: Validity and reliability in relation to socio-demographic factors. Attach. Hum. Dev. 22, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1555603</p<

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C. L., McMahon, T. J., Dalton, R., Mayes, L. C., and Borelli, J. (2017). Mothering From the Inside Out: Results of a second randomized clinical trial testing a mentalization-based intervention for mothers in addiction treatment. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 617–636. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C., Castiglioni, N., McMahon, T. J., Rounsaville, B., and Mayes, L. (2010a). The Mothers and Toddlers Program, an attachment-based parenting intervention for substance using women: Post-treatment results from a randomized clinical pilot. Attach. Hum. Dev. 12, 483–504. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2010.501983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C., Leigh, D., and Borelli, J. (2010b). Reflective functioning in mothers with drug use disorders: Implications for dyadic interactions with infants and toddlers. Attach. Hum. Dev. 12, 567–585. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2010.501988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C., McMahon, T. J., Rounsaville, B., and Mayes, L. (2011). The Mothers and Toddlers Program, an attachment-based parenting intervention for substance-using women: Results at 6-week follow-up in a randomized clinical pilot. Infant Ment. Health J. 32, 427–449. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., McMahon, T. J., Slade, A., and Luthar, S. S. (2005). How early bonding, depression, illicit drug use, and perceived support work together to influence drug-dependent mothers’ caregiving. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 75, 431–445. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.75.3.431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N., DeCoste, C., Castiglioni, N., Legow, N., and Mayes, L. (2008). The Mothers and Toddlers Program: Preliminary findings from an attachment-based parenting intervention for substance-abusing mothers. Psychoanal. Psychol. 25:499. doi: 10.1037/0736-9735.25.3.499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N., Mayes, L., Conti, J., Slade, A., and Rounsaville, B. (2004). Rethinking parenting interventions for drug-dependent mothers: From behavior management to fostering emotional bonds. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 27, 179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2004.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N., Pajulo, M., DeCoste, C., and Mayes, L. (2006). Parenting interventions for drug-dependent mothers and their young children: The case for an attachment-based approach. Fam. Relat. 55, 211–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00371.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taplin, S., and Mattick, R. P. (2015). The nature and extent of child protection involvement among heroin-using mothers in treatment: High rates of reports, removals at birth and children in care. Drug Alcohol Rev. 34, 31–37. doi: 10.1111/dar.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G.-J. J. M., and Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant–parent attachment. Psychol. Bull. 143, 1245–1272. doi: 10.1037/bul0000114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: maternal addiction, reflective functioning, family history, attachment trauma, early adversity

Citation: Lowell AF, Yatziv T, Peacock-Chambers E, Zayde A, DeCoste C, Suchman N and McMahon TJ (2022) Reflective functioning in mothers with addictions: Differential relationships involving family history of mental illness and substance use. Front. Psychol. 13:911069. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.911069

Received: 01 April 2022; Accepted: 05 August 2022;
Published: 13 October 2022.

Edited by:

Mirjam Kalland, University of Helsinki, Finland

Reviewed by:

Michelle Dow Keawphalouk Narangajavana, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States
Alessio Porreca, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Lowell, Yatziv, Peacock-Chambers, Zayde, DeCoste, Suchman and McMahon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amanda F. Lowell, amanda.lowell@yale.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.