Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 09 June 2022
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic A 150 Years’ Celebration of Darwin’s Book on Human Evolution and Sexual Selection: Its Legacy and Future Prospects View all 21 articles

The Evolution of Playfulness, Play and Play-Like Phenomena in Relation to Sexual Selection

  • Department of Experimental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

By conceptualizing Sexual Selection, Darwin showed a way to analyze intra-specific individual differences within an evolutionary perspective. Interestingly, Sexual Selection is often used to investigate the origins of sports, arts, humor, religion and other phenomena that, in several languages, are simply called “play.” Despite their manifested differences, these phenomena rely on shared psychological processes, including playfulness. Further, in such behaviors there is usually considerable individual variability, including sex differences, and positive relationship with mating success. However, Sexual Selection is rarely applied in the study of play, with exception to what is concerned as infant training behavior for adult sex roles. We offer an integrated grounding of playful phenomena aligning evolutionary propositions based on sexual selection, which might stimulate further exploration of playfulness within evolutionary perspective.

Introduction

When Darwin (1871/1981) concluded that Sexual Selection explains individual variation in bodily and psychobehavioral traits that could not be explained by differences in habits or survival, he kickstarted promising scientific fields (Cronin, 1993; Miller, 2001). Within a single class of evolutionary processes, Darwin explained the existence of extravagant and costly anatomical/physiological and psychosocial behavioral features not directly related to survival, sex differences across species, ontogenetic differences pre and post puberty, intra-specific individual differences, and the rapid divergence within closely related species (Cronin, 1993; Miller, 2001; Luoto, 2019). This Darwinian approach shed light on many human traits considered evolutionary puzzles, including aspects of play (Chick, 2001; Burghardt, 2005), humor (Greengross and Miller, 2011), sports (Manning and Taylor, 2001), arts, and religion (Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997; Miller, 2001; Varella et al., 2011, 2017; Barash, 2012).

Here we explore how Sexual Selection can foster comprehension about the evolutionary facets of playfulness, which is usually called “play,” but we suggest it is a broader concept not restricted to children. We argue for the benefits of distinguishing play, playfulness and some of the play-like phenomena and suggest how they might relate to Sexual Selection. Furthermore, we present the convergent evidence pointing to the evolved nature of psychological tendencies toward playfulness, focusing on the distinction between play and gaming.

Definition and Conceptual Distinctions

Depending on epistemological or linguistic differences, “play” can mean either a behavior or a psychological disposition (Sutton-Smith, 2001; Weisfeld and Weisfeld, 2016; Lebed, 2021). Similarly, it can refer to several playful activities. Consequently, “play” and “playfulness” (and sometimes “game”) are used as synonyms. For instance, Huizinga (1938/1980) and Caillois (1958/2001) state that the study of play (or “games” depending on the translation) should include music, theater (role-playing), lottery, philosophy, religion, roller-coaster riding, alcohol drinking, among others. Caillois’ French jeu and Huizinga’s Dutch spel are neither equivalent to English play nor game–neither to the Portuguese brincar nor jogar, respectively (Lebed, 2021). Some languages have only words for a general concept applicable to anything pleasurable, entertaining and autotelic (e.g., Czech, French, German), what we will call “playfulness” henceforward, while other languages make distinctions about different kinds of “play” (e.g., English, Japanese, Portuguese) (Lebed, 2021).

We argue that, beyond linguistic discussions about how narrow or overlapping is their definitions, we suggest using two insights from Evolutionary Psychology: (1) the idea that the human mind has many evolved psychological specializations, (2) the focus on the constellation of underlying psychological capacities being recruited to perform each class of behaviors (Barrett, 2014; Lewis et al., 2017; Pietraszewski and Wertz, 2022). The distinction between the behavioral dimension in the surface and the underlying psychological dimension (cf. Lopes, 2008; Proyer et al., 2021) and the lack of one-to-one correspondence between behavioral and psychological dimensions are crucial. Although many psychological capacities might jointly contribute to each class of behavior, each class of behavior might have their own evolved core capacity/capacities or set of core capacities. For instance, the evolved core capacity for playing might be Playfulness, which is “a propensity to define (or redefine) an activity in an imaginative, non-serious or metaphoric manner so as to enhance intrinsic enjoyment, involvement, and satisfaction” (Glynn and Webster, 1992, p. 85); “the predisposition to frame (or reframe) a situation in such a way as to provide oneself (and possibly others) with amusement, humor, and/or entertainment” (Barnett, 2007, p. 955); “an inclination to pursue activities with the goal of amusement or fun, with an enthusiastic and in-the-moment attitude” (Van Vleet and Feeney, 2015, p. 637); “an individual differences variable that allows people to frame or reframe everyday situations in a way such that they experience them as entertaining, and/or intellectually stimulating, and/or personally interesting” (Proyer, 2017, p. 114).

Since the terms most often associated to “playfulness” are “play” and “game,” Tables 1, 2 overview traits used in the literature to, respectively, define them. Definitions of “play” usually focus on behaviors that occur concomitantly to: intrinsic motivation (e.g., “voluntary,” “autotelic,” “fun”), positive emotions (e.g., “joy.” “happiness,” “pleasure”), and lack of stressors (e.g., “relaxed field,” “play world” or “magic circle”). Authors studying human play define it as “imaginative,” while those focusing on non-humans emphasize functionless-like behavioral modifications. Alternatively, definitions of “game” focus on structured rules; conflicts, there are winners and/or losers, and a quantifiable and important/valued outcome. Therefore, a behavior can be considered “play” when is activated the playfulness capacity which generates a playful state of mind. This playfulness state is usually not explicit in definitions of “game,” and some argue it is not even necessary (e.g., Lebed, 2021), but the rules and outcomes are reframing the conflicts to turn them interesting, and hopefully fun. That means that playfulness can exist even in non-joyful and serious activities (cf. Huizinga, 1938/1980; Caillois, 1958/2001; Suits, 1978; Proyer, 2017). Gaming playfulness may be qualitatively different from playing playfulness, but both are playful.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Traits presented in definitions of “play.”

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Traits presented in definitions of “game.”

Thus, playfulness can be combined with other traits, as shown by Sutton-Smith’s (2001) analysis of play(fulness) being depicted as: learning (developmental training), a force or drive (fortune-telling, divine providence, and motivation/necessity), a form of dominance (competitions and games of skill), group identification and belonging (festivals and cultural activities), imagination/creativity (arts and make-believe), self -expression and individualization (hobbies and recreation), and as subversion (jokes and sarcasm). The combination of Playfulness with the tendency for physical activity (i.e., a voluntary movement with energy costs superior to rest levels, Caspersen et al., 1985) might give rise to the class of behavior known as play (cf. Špinka et al., 2001; Burghardt, 2005). Thus, the different subtypes of play (e.g., with object, rough and tumble, roleplaying, rule based) would be further combinations with other psychological capacities (e.g., manipulation, ritualized competition, imagination/representation, normativity). Another example of evolved features in the human mind is artisticality, the autotelic pleasurable capacity for extraordinary ornamentation/aesthetic improvements (Varella et al., 2017; Varella, 2021). The combination between the tendency for physical activity and artisticality gives rise to the different artistic modalities.

Recognizing that behaviors recruit multiple psychological components enables a better understanding of overlapping cases and why many cultures use “play” across different behavioral classes. From a combination of a few psychological invariants one can observe a broad behavioral diversity and overlap of classes. Recruitment of language with playfulness may lead to play on words, jokes, celebrity voice impersonations, or word games, such as crosswords, hangman game, or Wordle. Recruitment of capacities for narrative, exploration, normativity and playfulness may lead to role-playing games (RPGs). Recruitment of language/narrative and artisticality leads to poetry and literary arts. Recruitment of playfulness and artisticality results in colorful domino art, Pictionary, musical jokes and parodies, circus arts, or hobby artists. Recruitment of ritualized competition/cooperation, normativity, self-overcoming and playfulness leads to most sportive games, chase tags, and tabletop games. Recruitment of ritualized competition/cooperation, normativity, self-overcoming and artisticality results in rhythmic gymnastics, figure skating, synchronized swimming, slam dunk contest, keepie-uppie competition, dancing contest, capoeira, or rap battles if language is also recruited.

Furthermore, if we consider that most humans are not serious professional sportspeople, gamers, or artists (Moraes, 2021), but nevertheless recruit the universal capacities underlying those tendencies in their leisure time, hobbies and self-entertainment moments, the contribution of playfulness becomes clear and ubiquitous even in adulthood. About 22% individuals consider free-time a time to fun and about 13% consider it a time to dedicate to one’s own hobby (Mingo and Montecolle, 2014). Playfulness may lead to many pleasurable activities that can or cannot latter become professionalized.

Play and Games as Stemming From Evolved Propensities

The psychological tendency toward play exhibits many criteria of evolved adaptation. It is present in all human cultures (Huizinga, 1938/1980; Gosso and Otta, 2003; Sandseter and Kennair, 2011) and is typical of mammals (Špinka et al., 2001; Burghardt, 2005), especially regarding large-brained mammal orders (Iwaniuk et al., 2001), suggesting play is at least as antique as the first mammals (Late Triassic), but it might have independently evolved in birds with delayed reproduction (Diamond and Bond, 2003; Kaplan, 2020), fish, turtles and octopuses (Kuba et al., 2003; Burghardt, 2005). Playful phenomena are culturally valued (Sutton-Smith, 2001). Play also has high costs (Harcourt, 1991; Greve et al., 2014; Froehle et al., 2019), is observable very early in ontogeny (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 2017; Winther-Lindqvist, 2019), provides physical, cognitive and social benefits (Špinka et al., 2001; Bjorklund and Pellegrini, 2002; Sandseter and Kennair, 2011). It results in specific emotional states (Špinka et al., 2001; Burghardt, 2005; Davis and Panksepp, 2011), has a heritable component (in mice, Walker and Byers, 1991; in humans, Olson et al., 2001), and it is at least partly modular, since its content may change, but it does not disappear under any known disorders, regardless if it is an intellectual, personality, endocrinal or mood disorder (cf. Berenbaum and Hines, 1992; Davis and Panksepp, 2011; Papoudi and Kossyvaki, 2019).

Gaming is considered exclusively human (Breuer et al., 2019), arguably because we usually require some spoken acceptance of explicit rules. Not all animals use violence in competitions, but some of them have non-violent ritualized competitions (Maynard Smith, 1974) that could be called “games” if there was any evidence of mutual agreement. Even rough-and-tumble play and play fighting have some recognizable rules, like controlling own force to avoid harming the play partner, signals of “this is play” (“play face” in primates) and role-reversal (Chick, 2001; Špinka et al., 2001; Burghardt, 2005). Games have also existed for at least 5 or 6 thousand years (Rollefson, 1992), but considering they are universal (Roberts et al., 1959; Chick, 1998; Voogt, 2017), they might have existed for much longer. Indeed, anthropologists have suggested that some archeological artifacts originally considered “works of art” or “religious tools” may be pieces of unknown games (Culin, 1907/2007; Avedon and Sutton-Smith, 1971). Furthermore, gaming is culturally valued (Avedon and Sutton-Smith, 1971; Apostolou et al., 2014; Crist et al., 2016), is associated with physical, cognitive and social benefits (Caillois, 1958/2001; Roberts et al., 1959; Zimmer, 1987; Kaufman et al., 2019). It includes specific mental states (Walther, 2003; McGonical, 2011), and is costly due to self-handicapping rules (Suits, 1978). Gaming develops later than play (Winther-Lindqvist, 2019), and makes individual differences more evident (Caillois, 1958/2001). At least sportive games also have a heritable component (Olson et al., 2001; Tucker and Collins, 2012).

Play as a Result of Natural and Sexual Selection

Different evolutionary mechanisms can explain different ludic phenomena (cf. Liebold et al., 2019). Play behavior is usually seen as a result of Natural Selection, a way for training hard-to-master skills that would require dangerous conditions if play did not existed (Špinka et al., 2001; Bjorklund and Pellegrini, 2002; Burghardt, 2005). Meanwhile, Sexual Selection is used to explain the evolutionary functions of arts (e.g., Varella et al., 2011, 2017), sports (e.g., Lombardo, 2012; Deaner et al., 2016) and even playfulness as an individual difference trait (e.g., Chick, 2001; Moraes et al., 2021). Play’s costs imply play can be a reliable signal of health, and consequently, this might be generalizable to adulthood. The Signal Theory of Playfulness (Chick, 2001) postulates a playful attitude not only signals a healthy condition, but other desirable trait, like non-aggressiveness in males and youthfulness in females. Contrarily, theories about the origins of sports, generaly understood as playful competitions of physical skills, postulates sport play signs fighting and hunting skills in controlled conditions to attract mates (intersexual competition), coalitional allies, status and/or resources (intrasexual competition; see Deaner et al., 2016). Moraes et al. (2021) found that in men other-directed playfulness positively predicted the number of long-term and short-term partners, while in women whimsical playfulness positively predicted the number of short-term relationships. Thus, playfulness is possibly being sexually selected in both sexes because playful adults have more partners. Sexual selection also might explain why the adult play (gaming) is, apparently, human-only and why there are robust cross-cultural sex differences (Roberts et al., 1959; Gray, 2004; Deaner and Smith, 2013; Moraes, 2021).

Sexual Selection shapes characteristics that tend to be costly, sometimes even reducing the chances of survival, are highly variable within the same species, they frequently develop in only one of the sexes and/or after puberty, and are species-specific (Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997; Miller, 2001; De Block and Dewitte, 2007; Lange et al., 2019; Petrie, 2021). Sexual Selection can also be roughly divided into Intersexual Selection, which involves adaptations to choose or be chosen as a mate by an opposite-sex individual, and Intrasexual Selection, which involves adaptations to defeat or intimidate same-sex individuals (Apostolou, 2015; Lange et al., 2019; Petrie, 2021).

More playful individuals are preferred as mates (Chick et al., 2012; Proyer and Wagner, 2015), and playfulness is also positively correlated with physical fitness (Proyer et al., 2018), relationship satisfaction (Brauer et al., 2021) and is subject to assortative mating (Chick et al., 2020), which can explain its heritability. Additionally, players of sportive games have more sexual partners (Faurie et al., 2004). However, there is almost no studies about the generalizability to other games and a few exceptions found mixed results (Lange and Schwab, 2019; Moraes, 2021). If neither Natural Selection, nor Intersexual Selection were alone responsible for the evolution of gaming, Intrasexual Selection might also play some role (cf. De Block and Dewitte, 2007). Thus, gaming may be a way to compete for resources that will indirectly increase the fitness, such as status or coalition allies (De Block and Dewitte, 2007, 2009; Balish et al., 2016; Winegard et al., 2018). Importantly, these selective forces may act simultaneously in different degrees. De Block and Dewitte (2009) argue there are so many sportive games because sports act as honest signals of good genes, which must be informative (provide evolutionary relevant information), accurate (hard to falsify, reliable) and transparent (spectators should be able to decode the sign). But there is often a trade-off among these three qualities and different sports target different proportions. Furthermore, there might be redundant and multiple signals display within ornamental signaling (Valentova et al., 2017; Pereira et al., 2019). Additionally, sports that lost their signal value to new ones can continue through exaptation, i.e., acquiring new functions, like when fans try to earn money through bets or try to impress people displaying their knowledge about game-related history and statistics, but the original (distal) function of the sport was to signal players’ physical skills.

Some attempts to apply an evolutionary perspective in the study of non-sportive games include Gray’s (2004) analysis, showing that men play casino games more often than women as a byproduct of male general higher risk-taking and dependency on resource holding to attract mates. Some researchers study digital games from an evolutionary framework (e.g., Mendenhall et al., 2010; Lange et al., 2019), but it is often not clear if they talk about digital gaming or digital pretense play. We have no knowledge of attempts to explain the adaptive values of other games through evolutionary mechanisms, but it would surely be interesting. For instance, card games rely on deception and social manipulation (Altice, 2014) and, consequently, they may be the products of sexual selection processes combining playfulness, cheating and cheater-detection behavioral and mind reading (cf. Varella, 2018).

Discussion

An evolutionary framework has greatly contributed to the study of play, but it has been focused on “repeated, incompletely functional behavior differing from more serious versions structurally, contextually, or ontogenetically, and initiated voluntarily when the animal is in a relaxed or low-stress setting” (Burghardt, 2005, p. 51). Though it captures what is usually thought when someone says “play,” it does not capture all the playful experiences one can get, nor capture the proper psychological level in which evolution operates (Barrett, 2014). Phenomena as “game,” “sport,” “music-playing,” “role-playing,” and “fortune-telling” are called “play” in some languages and not in others (Huizinga, 1938/1980). This might reflect a phylogenetic continuity or that similar combinations of psychological mechanisms are being used. We suggest that all of them use in some level the psychological capacity of playfulness. They may have evolved through Natural Selection as mechanisms to actively search for ways of increasing one’s own fitness, but also Sexual Selection acted on how they are used to create complex phenomena as gaming and arts (Liebold et al., 2019).

We argue that there is enough robust evidence to support the existence of these mechanisms (Caillois, 1958/2001; Scott and Godbey, 1992; Walther, 2003; Conway, 2010; Argento et al., 2017; Zosh et al., 2018; Lebed, 2021). What is needed is a good theory to avoid the “theory crisis” (Eronen and Bringmann, 2021). Sexual Selection might be this theory. However, researchers should accept some basic principles which make studies richer and less prone to appear contradictory, and avoid misunderstandings (Varella et al., 2013). Some of these principles are already used in other areas, like bio-musicology (Fitch, 2015): recognize that these behavioral phenomena are composed of many interacting psychological components; look for cross-cultural and inter-specific homologies and analogies; avoid elitism and focus on ecological validity; consider the Tinbergen’s questions–mechanism, ontogeny, function, phylogeny, plus their updates (e.g., subjective experience, Burghardt, 2005; and sociocultural history as medial explanation between proximal and distal ones, Varella et al., 2012).

Importantly, Darwin’s (1871/1981) theory has since been expanded and updated. Different mechanisms of Sexual Selection have been proposed (cf. Puts, 2010; Shuker and Kvarnemo, 2021). For instance, men may play sports more often because women prefer sportive men (Faurie et al., 2004), because sports work as behavioral armaments in intrasexual competition (Lombardo, 2012) or as displays for potential parents-in-law (Apostolou, 2017), besides other bio-socio-cultural functions. Psychological traits can function as both armaments and ornaments (Berglund et al., 1996).

Here we take this sesquicentennial celebration as an opportunity to invite the researchers to get inspired by Darwin’s pioneering and courageous move of looking into intra-specific differences and searching the mechanisms that drive them, in order to disentangle some of the most intriguing aspects of human life: How do playful phenomena (games, sports, arts, religion, humor, flirting, etc.) differ among each other? And how they can contribute to individual survival and reproductive success? Future studies should consider the overlapping among playful phenomena, their multiple levels, cultural meaning, interspecies similarities, organism’s developmental stage and both Natural and Sexual Selection.

Author Contributions

YM: idealization and writing of the manuscript. JV: supervision and revision of the manuscript. MV: manuscript writing and revisions. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

YM was supported by scholarship granted by the CAPES (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior), no. 88887.374660/2019-00. MV was supported by the scholarship granted by CAPES (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior), number PNPD 33002010037P0–MEC/CAPES.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank to the editor, the reviewer for the insightful feedback and our colleagues in the Evolution, Behavior and Sexuality laboratory for the theoretical discussions.

References

Altice, N. (2014). The Playing Card Platform. Analog Game Studies, I(IV). Available online at: https://analoggamestudies.org/2014/11/the-playing-card-platform/ (accessed May 5, 2022).

Google Scholar

Apostolou, M. (2015). The athlete and the spectator inside the man: a cross-cultural investigation of the evolutionary origins of athletic behavior. Cross Cult. Res. 49, 151–173. doi: 10.1177/1069397114536516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apostolou, M. (2017). Sexual Selection in Homo sapiens: Parental Control over Mating and the Opportunity Cost of Free Mate Choice, Sexual Selection in Homo sapiens. Berlin: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-58999-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apostolou, M., Frantzides, N., and Pavlidou, A. (2014). Men competing, men watching: exploring watching-pattern contingencies in sports. Int. J. Sport Commun. 7, 462–476. doi: 10.1123/ijsc.2014-0033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argento, A., Mill, D., Carmichael, V., Mettler, M., and Heath, N. L. (2017). Gamers and video games users: What’s the difference? J. Interpers. Relat. Intergroup Relat. Identity 10, 58–68.

Google Scholar

Avedon, E. M., and Sutton-Smith, B. (1971). The Study of Games. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Balish, S. M., Deaner, R. O., Rainham, D., and Blanchard, C. (2016). Sex differences in sport remain when accounting for countries’ gender inequality. Cross Cult. Res. 50, 395–414. doi: 10.1177/1069397116665815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barash, D. P. (2012). Homo mysterious: Evolutionary Puzzles of Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Barnett, L. A. (2007). The nature of playfulness in young adults. Pers. Individ. Dif. 43, 949–958. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, H. C. (2014). The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve. Oxford: Oxford University.

Google Scholar

Berenbaum, S. A., and Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychol. Sci. 3, 203–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00028.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berglund, A., Bisazza, A., and Pilastro, A. (1996). Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 58, 385–399. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01442.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F., and Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The Origins of Human Nature. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Brauer, K., Sendatzki, R., Scherrer, T., Chick, G., and Proyer, R. T. (2021). Revisiting adult playfulness and relationship satisfaction: APIM analyses of middle-aged and older couples. Int. J. Appl. Posit. Psychol. 1–29. doi: 10.1007/s41042-021-00058-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breuer, J., Pietschmann, D., Liebold, B., and Lange, B. P. (eds) (2019). Evolutionary Psychology and Digital Games: Digital Hunter-Gatherers. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3229.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caillois, R. (1958/2001). Man, Play and Games. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., and Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 100, 126–131.

Google Scholar

Chick, G. (1998). Games in culture revisited: a replication and extension of Roberts, Arth, and Bush (1959). Cross Cult. Res. 32, 185–206. doi: 10.1177/106939719803200204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chick, G. (2001). “What is play for? Sexual selection and the evolution of play,” in Theory in Context and Out: Play & Culture Studies, ed. S. Reifel (Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing), 3–25.

Google Scholar

Chick, G., Proyer, R., Purrington, A., and Yarnal, C. (2020). Do birds of a playful feather flock together? Playfulness and assortative mating. Am. J. Play 12, 178–215.

Google Scholar

Chick, G., Yarnal, C., and Purrington, A. (2012). Play and mate preference: testing the signal theory of adult playfulness. Am. J. Play 4, 407–440.

Google Scholar

Conway, S. (2010). Hyper-Ludicity, Contra-Ludicity, and the Digital Game. Eludamos 4, 135–147.

Google Scholar

Crist, W., Voogt, A., and Dunn-Vaturi, A.-E. (2016). Facilitating Interaction : board games as social lubricants in the ancient near east. Oxf. J. Archaeol. 35, 179–196. doi: 10.1111/ojoa.12084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronin, H. (1993). The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual selection from Darwin to today. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Culin, S. (1907/2007). Games of the North American Indians (Reprinted Edition). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871/1981). The Decent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, Biology and Human Affairs. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Davis, K. L., and Panksepp, J. (2011). The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1946–1958. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Block, A., and Dewitte, S. (2007). Mating games: cultural evolution and sexual selection. Biol. Philos. 22, 475–491.

Google Scholar

De Block, A., and Dewitte, S. (2009). Darwinism and the Cultural Evolution of Sports. Perspect. Biol. Med. 52, 1–16. doi: 10.1353/pbm.0.0063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deaner, R. O., Balish, S. M., and Lombardo, M. P. (2016). Sex differences in sports interest and motivation: an evolutionary perspective. Evol. Behav. Sci. 10, 73–97. doi: 10.1037/ebs0000049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deaner, R. O., and Smith, B. A. (2013). Sex Differences in Sports Across 50 Societies. Cross Cult. Res. 47, 268–309. doi: 10.1177/1069397112463687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, J., and Bond, A. B. (2003). A comparative analysis of social play in birds. Behaviour 140:4536079.

Google Scholar

Eberle, S. G. (2014). The elements of play toward a philosophy and a definition of play. Am. J. Play 6, 214–233.

Google Scholar

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (2017). “Behavior development (ontogeny),” in Human Ethology, ed. I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt (New York, NY: Routledge), 548–605.

Google Scholar

Eronen, M. I., and Bringmann, L. F. (2021). The theory crisis in psychology: how to move forward. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 779–788. doi: 10.1177/1745691620970586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faurie, C., Pontier, D., and Raymond, M. (2004). Student athletes claim to have more sexual partners than other students. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00064-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140091. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froehle, A. W., Wells, G. K., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, A. Z., Lew-Levy, S., and Crittenden, A. N. (2019). Physical activity and time budgets of Hadza forager children: implications for self-provisioning and the ontogeny of the sexual division of labor. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 31:e23209. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glynn, M. A., and Webster, J. (1992). The adult playfulness scale: an initial assessment. Psychol. Rep. 71, 83–103. doi: 10.2466/pr0.71.5.83-103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosso, Y., and Otta, E. (2003). “Em uma aldeia Parakanã,” in Brincadeira e Cultura: Viajando pelo Brasil que Brinca, eds A. M. A. Carvalho, C. M. C. Magalhães, F. A. R. Pontes, and I. D. Bichara (São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo), 33–76.

Google Scholar

Gray, P. (2004). Evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives on gambling. J. Gambl. Stud. 20, 347–371. doi: 10.1007/s10899-004-4579-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, P. (2009). Play as a foundation for hunter-gatherer social existence. Am. J. Play 1, 476–522. doi: 10.1300/J082v41n02_07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greengross, G., and Miller, G. (2011). Humor ability reveals intelligence, predicts mating success, and is higher in males. Intelligence 39, 188–192. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greve, W., Thomsen, T., and Dehio, C. (2014). Does playing pay? The fitness-effect of free play during childhood. Evol. Psychol. 12, 434–447. doi: 10.1177/147470491401200210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harcourt, R. (1991). Survivorship costs of play in the South American fur seal. (Arctocephalus australis). Anim. Behav. 42, 509–511.

Google Scholar

Huizinga, J. (1938/1980). Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play-Element in Culture. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N., Nelson, J. E., and Pellis, S. M. (2001). Do big-brained animals play more? Comparative analyses of play and relative brain size in mammals. J. Comp. Psychol. 115, 29–41. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juul, J. (2010). The game, the player, the world: looking for a heart of gameness. Plurais 1, 248–270.

Google Scholar

Kaplan, G. (2020). Play behaviour, not tool using, relates to brain mass in a sample of birds. Sci. Rep. 10:20437. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76572-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, D., Ma, M., Sauvé, L., Renaud, L., and Duplàa, E. (2019). Benefits of digital gameplay for older adults: Does game type make a difference? Int. J. Aging Res. 2:43. doi: 10.28933/ijoar-2019-07-2805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuba, M., Meisel, D. V., Byrne, R. A., Griebel, U., and Mather, J. A. (2003). Looking at play in Octopus vulgaris. Berl. Paläontol. Abh. 3, 163–169.

Google Scholar

Lange, B. P., Breuer, J., Liebold, B., and Pietschmann, D. (2019). “Why an evolutionary psychological approach to digital games,” in Evolutionary Psychology and Digital Games: Digital Hunter-Gatherers, eds J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold, and B. P. Lange (New York, NY: Routledge), 1–14.

Google Scholar

Lange, B. P., and Schwab, F. (2019). “Game on: sex differences in the production and consumption of video games,” in Evolutionary Psychology and Digital Games: Digital Hunter-Gatherers, eds J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold, and B. P. Lange (New York, NY: Routledge), 193–204.

Google Scholar

Lebed, F. (2021). Play and spiel are not the same: anti-wittgensteinian arguments and consideration of game as a kind of human play. Games Cult. 16, 743–761. doi: 10.1177/1555412020973104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lever, J. (1978). Sex differences in the complexity of children’s play and games. Am. Sociol. Rev. 43, 471–483. doi: 10.2307/2094773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, D. M., Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., and Buss, D. M. (2017). Evolutionary psychology: a how-to guide. Am. Psychol. 72, 353. doi: 10.1037/a0040409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebold, B., Koban, K., and Ohler, P. (2019). “Evolution of Play,” in Evolutionary Psychology and Digital Games: Digital Hunter-Gatherers, 2, eds J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold, and B. P. Lange (New York, NY: Routledge). doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06097-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombardo, M. P. (2012). On the evolution of sport. Evol. Psychol. 10, 1–28. doi: 10.1177/147470491201000101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, C. (2008). Ludicity: A Theoretical Horizon for Understanding the Concepts of Game, Game-Playing and Play. Available online at: https://ria.ua.pt/bitstream/10773/7906/1/ECCBL_Lopes,completedtext_ECCBL2_.pdf (accessed April 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

Luoto, S. (2019). An updated theoretical framework for human sexual selection: from ecology, genetics, and life history to extended phenotypes. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 5, 48–102. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0103-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., and Taylor, R. P. (2001). Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00063-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maynard Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. J. Theor. Biol. 47, 209–221. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(74)90110-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGonical, J. (2011). Reality is Broken: Why Games Make us Better and How they Can Change the World. New York, NY: The Penguin press.

Google Scholar

Mendenhall, Z., Nepomuceno, M. V., and Saad, G. (2010). “Exploring video games from an evolutionary psychological perspective,” in Encyclopedia of E-Business Development and Management in the Global Economy, ed. I. Lee (Hershey: Business Science Reference), 734–742.

Google Scholar

Miller, G. F. (2001). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York, NY: Anchor Books. doi: 10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.1196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mingo, I., and Montecolle, S. (2014). Subjective and objective aspects of free time: the Italian case. J. Happiness Stud. 15, 425–441. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9429-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, F. S., and Stadzisz, P. C. (2017). “Jogo Digital: definição do termo,” in Proceedings of the XVI Simpósio Brasileiro de Jogos e Entretenimento Digital, ed. B. Campagnolo de Paula (Curitiba: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Paraná), 296–299.

Google Scholar

Moraes, Y. L. (2021). Jogos Como Competições por Status e Parceiros: Uma Análise Evolucionista de por que Adultos Jogam. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo. doi: 10.11606/D.47.2021.tde-12082021-222310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moraes, Y. L., Varella, M. A. C., Silva, C. S. A., and Valentova, J. V. (2021). Adult playful individuals have more long- and short-term relationships. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3:E24. doi: 10.1017/ehs.2021.19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, J. M., Vernon, P. A., Harris, J. A., and Jang, K. L. (2001). The heritability of attitudes: a study of twins. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 845–860.

Google Scholar

Papoudi, D., and Kossyvaki, L. (2019). “Play and children with autism: insights from research and implications for practice,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Play: Developmental and Disciplinary Perspectives, eds P. K. Smith and J. L. Roopnarine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 563–579.

Google Scholar

Pereira, K. J., Varella, M. A. C., Kleisner, K., Pavlovič, O., and Valentova, J. V. (2019). Positive association between facial and vocal femininity/masculinity in women but not in men. Behav. Process. 164, 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, M. (2021). Evolution by sexual selection. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9:786868. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.786868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietraszewski, D., and Wertz, A. E. (2022). Why evolutionary psychology should abandon modularity. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17, 465–490. doi: 10.1177/1745691621997113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T. (2017). A new structural model for the study of adult playfulness: assessment and exploration of an understudied individual differences variable. Pers. Individ. Dif. 108, 113–122. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., Gander, F., Bertenshaw, E. J., and Brauer, K. (2018). The positive relationships of playfulness with indicators of health, activity, and physical fitness. Front. Psychol. 9:1440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., Gander, F., Brauer, K., and Chick, G. (2021). Can playfulness be stimulated? A randomised placebo-controlled online playfulness intervention study on effects on trait playfulness, well-being, and depression. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 13, 129–151. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., and Wagner, L. (2015). Playfulness in adults revisited: the signal theory in German speakers. Am. J. Play 7, 201–227.

Google Scholar

Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 157–175. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, J. M., Arth, M. J., and Bush, R. R. (1959). Games in Culture. Am. Anthropol. 61, 597–605. doi: 10.1525/aa.1959.61.4.02a00050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rollefson, G. O. (1992). A Neolithic Game Board from ?Ain Ghazal, Jordan. Bull. Am. Schools Orient. Res. 286, 1–5. doi: 10.2307/1357113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salen, K., and Zimmerman, E. (2004). Rules of Play: Game Design Fundamentals. London: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Sandseter, E. B. H., and Kennair, L. E. O. (2011). Children’s risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the Anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evol. Psychol. 9, 257–284. doi: 10.1177/147470491100900212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, D., and Godbey, G. C. (1992). An analysis of adult play groups: social versus serious participation in contract bridge. Leis. Sci. 14, 47–67. doi: 10.1080/01490409209513156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuker, D. M., and Kvarnemo, C. (2021). The definition of sexual selection. Behav. Ecol. 32, 781–794. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arab055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Špinka, M., Newberry, R. C., and Bekoff, M. (2001). Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. Q. Rev. Biol. 76, 141–168. doi: 10.1086/393866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suits, B. (1978). The Grasshopper: Games, Life and Utopia. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Google Scholar

Sutton-Smith, B. (2001). The Ambiguity of Play, 2nd Edn. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

Google Scholar

Tucker, R., and Collins, M. (2012). What makes champions? A review of the relative contribution of genes and training to sporting success. Br. J. Sports Med. 46, 555–561. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentova, J. V., Varella, M. A., Havlíček, J., and Kleisner, K. (2017). Positive association between vocal and facial attractiveness in women but not in men: a cross-cultural study. Behav. Process. 135, 95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vleet, M., and Feeney, B. C. (2015). Play behavior and playfulness in adulthood. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 9, 630–643. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C. (2021). Evolved features of artistic motivation: analyzing a brazilian database spanning three decades. Front. Psychol. 5663. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.769915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C. (2018). The biology and evolution of the three psychological tendencies to anthropomorphize biology and evolution. Front. Psychol. 9:1839. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C., dos Santos, I. B. C., Ferreira, J. H. B. P., and Bussab, V. S. R. (2013). Misunderstandings in applying evolution to human mind and behavior and its causes: a systematic review. EvoS J. 5, 81–107.

Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C., Souza, A. A. L., and Ferreira, J. H. B. P. (2011). Evolutionary aesthetics and sexual selection in the evolution of rock art aesthetics. Rock Art Res. 28, 153–186.

Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C., Souza, A. A. L., and Ferreira, J. H. B. P. (2012). Considering both proximal and distal explanations for (rock) art production and appreciation as fruitful. Rock Art Res. 29, 227–229.

Google Scholar

Varella, M. A. C., Valentova, J. V., and Fernández, A. M. (2017). “Evolution of artistic and aesthetic propensities through female competitive ornamentation,” in The Oxford Handbook of Women and Competition, ed. M. L. Fisher (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 757–784.

Google Scholar

Voogt, A. (2017). Strategic games in society: the geography of adult play. Int. J. Play 6, 308–318. doi: 10.1080/21594937.2017.1382986

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C., and Byers, J. A. (1991). Heritability of locomotor play in house mice, Mus domesticus. Anim. Behav. 42, 891–897. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80141-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther, B. K. (2003). Playing and gaming: reflections and classifications. Game Stud. 3, 1–20. Available online at: http://www.gamestudies.org/0301/walther/ (accessed April 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Weisfeld, G., and Weisfeld, C. C. (2016). Is play an emotion? Some ethological observations. Hum. Ethol. Bull. 31, 4–29. doi: 10.22330/heb/311/004-029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winegard, B., Winegard, B., and Geary, D. C. (2018). The status competition model of cultural production. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 4, 351–371. doi: 10.1007/s40806-018-0147-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winther-Lindqvist, D. (2019). “Playing games with rules in early child care and beyond,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Play: Developmental and Disciplinary Perspectives, eds P. K. Smith and J. L. Roopnarine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 222–239.

Google Scholar

Zahavi, A., and Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Zimmer, L. J. (1987). Playing at being men. Oceania 58, 22–37. doi: 10.1002/j.1834-4461.1987.tb02234.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zosh, J. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Hopkins, E. J., Jensen, H., Liu, C., Neale, D., et al. (2018). Accessing the inaccessible: redefining play as a spectrum. Front. Psychol. 9:1124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: play, gaming, playfulness, sexual selection, mental mechanisms, human evolution, ludicity, evolutionary psychology

Citation: Moraes YL, Valentova JV and Varella MAC (2022) The Evolution of Playfulness, Play and Play-Like Phenomena in Relation to Sexual Selection. Front. Psychol. 13:925842. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.925842

Received: 22 April 2022; Accepted: 09 May 2022;
Published: 09 June 2022.

Edited by:

Karlijn Massar, Maastricht University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Kay Brauer, Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Moraes, Valentova and Varella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yago Luksevicius Moraes, yagolmoraes@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.