Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 15 July 2022
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Consciousness Research 2021 View all 6 articles

Direct comparisons between hypnosis and meditation: A mini-review

  • Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto (TN), Italy

Hypnosis and meditation share phenomenological and neurophysiological features, and their comparison is a topic of growing interest in the scientific literature. In this article, we review a classification of these two kinds of non-ordinary states of consciousness, and discuss the studies that directly compare them. Some findings seem to suggest that hypnosis and meditation are distinct phenomena, while others underline their similarities, but experiments that directly contrast them are still scarce and no consensus has been reached yet. While this comparison could give us fundamental insights into central issues concerning the role of attention, metacognition and executive control in the study of consciousness, it is clear that we are still at the early stages of this research.

Introduction

Hypnotic and meditative states are characterized by a series of changes in subjective experiences from the normal waking state, but the literature is still unclear on similarities and differences (Raz and Lifshitz, 2016).

Common characteristics are a state of general wellbeing and relaxation, accompanied by deep concentration, and mental absorption (Lynn et al., 2012). The induction of both states is used for clinical purposes, particularly when dealing with psychological problems such as depression, anxiety and mental stress, or to relieve chronic pain (Zeidan and Grant, 2016).

On the other hand, the procedures for reaching these states have profoundly different historical bases, and in many cases the reported phenomenology presents substantial differences, thus raising a large number of questions about their mutual positioning (Markovic and Thompson, 2016). A key issue that appears central is the understanding of the role played in both states by metacognition, i.e., the ability to represent, monitor, and control ongoing cognitive processes (Lush et al., 2016). While several meditative and specific hypnotic practices seem to train conscious metacognitive skills (Drigas et al., 2022), several hypnotic techniques seem to act more on basic cognition at an unconscious level, inducing a decline in metacognitive abilities (Dienes et al., 2016).

Given these premises, highlighting similarities and differences between these two states seems to be a precondition in the scientific understanding of human consciousness, to disentangle conscious from unconscious processing, as well as to elucidate the role of metacognition. While important theoretical comparisons are present in the literature (e.g., Raz and Lifshitz, 2016), direct experimental comparisons between hypnosis and meditation are an exception. In this article, we briefly start with a definition of hypnotic and meditative states, then discuss the main comparative theories, and then list the few studies that have attempted direct experimental comparisons.

Phenomenology of hypnosis and meditation

In this section, we describe the main phenomenological aspects that separately characterize hypnosis and meditation.

Phenomenology of hypnosis

Hypnosis is defined as a state of consciousness consisting of focused attention, reduced peripheral awareness, and increased responsiveness to suggestion (Elkins et al., 2015). A typical hetero-induced hypnotic session begins with the hypnotist inducing a state of relaxation in the subject, up to a state of true hypnotic trance. In this state, experience and behavior of the hypnotized subject are modeled in accordance with the hypnotic suggestions. Finally, the session ends with a de-induction procedure (for example a countdown) uttered by the hypnotist (Egner and Raz, 2007).

Hypnotic suggestions can induce a wide range of effects, implemented in therapeutic and experimental contexts, ranging from analgesia and other forms of sensory hallucinations to significant modulations of attentional and executive control processes (Terhune et al., 2002; Drigas et al., 2021). There are three main phenomenological components of the hypnotic state: dissociation, absorption and suggestibility (Cardeña and Spiegel, 1991).

Dissociation is the splitting of mental processes from the main body of consciousness, with simultaneous alterations in the sense of self in acting and volition. Subjects deeply immersed in a hypnotic state, when asked to perform a task in response to hypnotic suggestions, perceive a state of alteration in the sense of voluntariness, as if the tasks were performed outside of their own intentionality (Sadler and Woody, 2010).

Absorption, on the other hand, consists of a focused attention that fully engages the mental resources of individuals (Tellegen and Atkinson, 1974). Attention control processes are central to absorption in hypnosis, and therefore a crucial role is hypothesized for executive functions and the frontal lobes (Parris, 2017).

Finally, suggestibility is the ability to model behavior and subjective experience in accordance with hypnotic suggestions. Suggestibility as a trait in the hypnotic context is referred to as hypnotizability, and therefore as the individual's generic ability to experience what is suggested during hypnosis. For this purpose, specific scales have been constructed that allow the comparison between subjects with differences in the hypnotizability trait (Acunzo and Terhune, 2021).

Phenomenology of meditation

The term meditation refers to a wide variety of contemplative practices, ranging from focused meditation to breath control, visualization or mantra recitations (Matko et al., 2021). These practices engage meditators in repetitive, specific mental trainings aimed at cultivating desired psychological qualities and peculiar states of consciousness. We can divide these practices into three general categories, even though in some protocols they can be used together.

In the first category, specific meditative practices regulate and exercise the meditator's attention toward a specific target (e.g., bodily sensations related to breathing) (Lutz et al., 2008). This concentration is characterized by a perceived state of absorption, with a phenomenological total involvement in a specific experience (e.g., breathing) or task (e.g., visualizing mental images), as opposed to the experience of being continually distracted by extraneous thoughts or stimuli (Pekala, 1991). One special form of focused meditation is mantra recitation, with the repetition of a sound, word or phrase to calm and focus the mind, as in Transcendental Meditation (TM) (Álvarez-Pérez et al., 2022).

The second category includes mindfulness meditation and open monitoring practices, where the attention is opened to present thoughts, sensations, images that come to mind, with the purpose of observing them in a detached and non-judgmental way in the present moment (Raffone and Srinivasan, 2010). Here there is a form of absorption as well, but the mind is not focused on a specific target, instead it operates widely, and the practice is oriented toward the cultivation of meta-awareness (an aspect of metacognition), i.e., the awareness of everything that happens in our experience (Smallwood and Schooler, 2015; Mooneyham and Schooler, 2016).

A third category of meditation practices acts not on the level of individual constructs as in the previous groups, but on the whole self of the practitioner. In this family of practices, sometimes referred to as deconstructive (Dahl et al., 2015), the main activity is to introspectively explore the nature and dynamics of self-related mental processes. This practice unveils a transient or illusory nature of self-representations, which can lead to a progressive deconstruction of the ego's sense and to egolessness (Epstein, 1988).

Theoretical and methodological comparisons between hypnosis and meditation

In this section, we describe the similar and dissimilar phenomenological aspects of the hypnotic and meditative states.

Similarities between hypnosis and meditation

A first overlap between hypnosis and meditation concerns bodily relaxation. Both conditions are states of consciousness achieved by induction procedures that improve relaxation, with corresponding physiological effects. Both hypnosis and meditation show a reduction in sympathetic responses and an increase in parasympathetic tone, although these generalized results are not supported by all the field studies (Tung and Hsieh, 2019; Fernandez et al., 2021).

Secondly, in both states relaxation enhances the development of an effortlessly absorbed state of attention in the present moment. However, it is not yet clear whether the states of hypnotic and meditative absorptions are identical phenomena. In meditative practice, avoiding distractions requires training and effort in novices; in a hypnotic session, individuals are instead absorbed in the suggestions apparently without effort. Some authors underline the psychometric ambiguity of the absorption construct evaluated by the self-assessment questionnaires, suggesting the need to identify a finer level for the definition of different types of absorption (Terhune and Jamieson, 2021).

Lastly, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies that have examined meditation or hypnosis separately show that these states are associated with similar and partially overlapping activation changes in frontal, salience, and default-mode networks, known to be involved in attentional and executive functions (McGeown, 2016). However, these similarities are very difficult to generalize due to the different forms of techniques and tasks included in these two families of practices.

Differences between hypnosis and meditation

A first important difference concerns the methodology: in hypnosis, it is central the hypnotherapist's ability to induce the hypnotic state, whereas in meditation the emphasis is on the autonomous mental practice of the meditator, although both states can be led by real people or audio guides (Häuser et al., 2016; McClintock et al., 2019).

Another difference is that hypnosis depends mainly on the hypnotic suggestibility of the subjects (Oakley and Halligan, 2013), whereas meditative traits can be developed with practice (Kiken et al., 2015). This difference affects experimental designs: while the effects of meditation can be assessed by comparing experienced and novice meditators, the effects of hypnosis are usually assessed by distinguishing between high and low hypnotizable individuals.

An important theoretical framework that highlights the differences between the cognitive mechanisms underlying these two states is the theory of Higher Order Thoughts (HOT; Rosenthal, 2005). In this theory, a mental state can be defined as conscious when a person is aware of living that mental state. In this sense, Dienes et al. (2020) proposed a differentiation between HOT cognitive control and COLD control, that is cognitive control in the absence of accurate metacognitive processes. This form of COLD control during hypnosis can be seen in the alteration of the sense of agency, defined as the experience of being the initiator of an intentional action. From this perspective, COLD hypnotic control is interpretable as a lack of awareness of intentions.

On the other end, in another set of studies in clinical settings and with inductions involving attention and imagery (Drigas et al., 2022), hypnosis appears to have an impact on metacognitive skills and wellbeing, thus opening up to the counterintuitive possibility that unconscious processes can act on metacognitive development.

Related to this, a crucial point of differentiation between hypnosis and meditation concerns the experience of dissociation, as the hypnosis literature agrees that a sense of involuntary action and dissociated volition is experienced during these states (Sadler and Woody, 2010). This appears to be very different from meditative states, where the emphasis is instead on increasing and integrating the meditator's sense of presence into one's experiences. Therefore, some researchers consider hypnosis a form of strategically self-induced deception, while meditation as a form of self-induced intuition (Dienes et al., 2016).

A final point of differentiation concerns the effect of these two states on the experience of being a self. While in hypnosis people report having some sort of hidden observer who was witnessing the suggested execution (e.g., motor response) from a third person perspective, in most meditative states practitioners are initially involved in an effort to observe the flow of their thoughts without being involved in it. As the practice progresses, meditators report a weakening of the first person perspective, to the point, in advanced meditative states, of a sense of dissolution of the boundaries of the self (Epstein, 1988).

Direct experimental contrasts between hypnosis and meditation

Here, we review the studies that directly contrast hypnosis and meditation in experimental settings, arranging them into four different groups.

Contrasting hypnosis and transcendental meditation

The first group includes four studies that compared hypnosis (self or hetero-induced) and TM. Walrath and Hamilton (1975) found no differences in heart rate, respiratory rate reduction, and skin resistance between two groups of experienced meditators who either performed a TM session or performed a self-hypnosis session.

Similar lack of physiological differences were found in Morse et al. (1977), in which participants were monitored during alertness, TM, hypnosis (with only relaxation or with analgesia) and relaxation while awake. Psychophysiological measurements included respiratory rate, heart rate, blood pressure, skin resistance, electroencephalography (EEG), and muscle activity. The results showed differences only with respect to alertness, while there were no significant differences between the states of relaxation, with the exception of muscle activity, which was deeper in meditation. Experientially, participants reported relaxation in hypnosis and meditation as being equally more effective than pure relaxation.

In a third study, Barmark and Gaunitz (1979) compared the effects of TM and audio-recorded hypnosis. As in the two previous studies, physiological data showed no significant differences between hypnosis and TM, particularly in heart rate and skin temperature. A slower respiratory rate was detected during TM. Participants reported that during hypnosis compared to alertness there was greater vividness in mental images and a heightened sense of concentration, along with less attention to environmental stimuli and respiratory sensations. Instead, during TM meditators reported that their bodies became lighter and warmer, and as if time went by faster. Benson et al. (1978) found that high hypnotizable subjects lowered anxiety and systolic blood pressure both in TM and self-hypnosis compared to lows.

Contrasting hypnosis with attention meditation and open monitoring

A second group of studies compared hypnosis with attention and open monitoring meditation. In Nuys (1973), participants performed focused meditation exercises, attention assignments, and hypnotic susceptibility assessments. The results indicated that good concentration is a necessary condition for hypnotic susceptibility, but not sufficient, as some participants who did show good concentration were not suggestible. Spanos et al. (1978, 1980) replicated these results, reporting that hypnotizability is related to a low intrusion rate of distracting thoughts, and therefore to the absorption and vividness of mental images. Heide et al. (1980) showed that highly hypnotizable individuals presented most substantial decrements in anxiety after a 1-week meditation treatment compared to lows, while a brief training in meditation did not modify hypnotic responsivity.

Brown et al. (1983) conducted a study to investigate the phenomenological differences during self-hypnosis, daydreaming, and mindfulness meditation performed during retreats. While self-hypnosis involved more self-referential thinking, memory changes and intense emotions, daydreaming emphasized the presence of spontaneous mental images. Meditation initially involved a difficulty in managing distractions during practice, but with experience, a greater awareness of bodily processes was learned, facing changes in the perception of time and sense of self, with mental processes appearing to slow down and with a vivid awareness, which took on an impersonal quality.

More recently, Semmens-Wheeler and Dienes (2012) in the theoretical framework of HOT theory, underlined a methodological issue: the measurement of the subjective perception of intrusive thoughts is a self-monitoring (meta-awareness) activity, and it is therefore possible that highly hypnotizable individuals are simply not aware of the distracting intrusive thoughts. In this sense, the authors proposed the term “cold absorption” in the context of hypnosis, as opposed to “hot absorption” in experienced meditators. The authors compared the hypnotizability scores of experienced meditators against a database of 500 subjects, finding that the meditators were less suggestible than the average of all other subjects. They therefore hypothesized that meditation and hypnosis are opposite with regard to the role of meta-awareness. This hypothesis was verified in a survey by Lush et al. (2016), in which they investigated the subjective times of awareness of an intention to move, a judgment considered to be of a metacognitive type. They found that more easily hypnotized people are less capable of metacognitive judgment, and therefore attribute the initiation of the intention to move later than experienced meditators, whereas the practice of meditation leads to accurate judgments. Furthermore, a cross-sectional study (Grover et al., 2018) showed that hypnotizability and mindfulness facets where negatively correlated.

Contrasting hypnosis and meditation in the perception of pain

A third group of studies compared hypnosis and meditation in the context of pain sensation, mostly for clinical treatments. In a recent review, De Benedittis (2021) underlines how both hypnosis and meditation attenuate pain, but with both similarities and differences in the multiple neurocognitive mechanisms involved. Both phenomena involve the frontal modulation of pain-related areas, but their role in hypnosis seems to depend on the type of suggestion given, while in meditation depends on the level of practice.

Swain and Trevena (2014) compared the effects of brief mindfulness and hypnosis sessions on resistance to pain caused by a hand placed in cold water at 0 C (Cold Pressor Task, CPT). Both interventions showed their efficacy compared to control in two different modalities: no difference was found on between DVD presentations and in person procedures. Participants, however, reported lower subjective pain scores after hypnosis compared to mindfulness.

Recently, Grover et al. (2021) replicated the previous study, finding no differences in CPT outcomes after a single recorded session of hypnosis or mindfulness meditation. Both conditions, however, modulated changes in self-reported pain perception, but while hypnosis induced a reduction in pain intensity and unpleasant elements of pain, mindfulness only correlated with a reduction in pain intensity.

Williams et al. (2022) evaluated the effectiveness of mindfulness meditation and hypnosis vs. an active control condition (educational training) in a randomized study of U.S. military veterans suffering from chronic pain and depression. The results showed no significant differences immediately after the treatments: however, in the follow-up evaluations at 3 and 6 months, the groups that practiced hypnosis and meditation showed a decrease in the intensity of pain and depression.

Contrasting hypnosis and meditation in electroencephalographic studies

A fourth group of studies concerns hypnosis and meditation comparisons mainly with the use of EEG. Halsband et al. (2009) measured EEG activity during hypnosis and meditation of a single highly hypnotizable subject expert in Vajrayana practice, a form of Tibetan meditation that aims to achieve a state of enhanced cognition and emotions (Amihai and Kozhevnikov, 2014). They report significant differences between the two states in the alpha 1 and theta 2 frequency bands. High amplitudes in the alpha frequency bands were greater under hypnosis in the central and temporal positions, while the alpha frequency in meditation was more pronounced in the frontal positions than in the control. Greater activity in the theta band two was observed only under hypnosis in both hemispheres. While the authors admit that it is difficult to draw conclusions from a study with a single subject and with these variances, it is interesting to report that the two states do not show identical brain activations.

Another one-participant study compared the EEG correlates of one form of TM (Sidhi) with those caused by audio-recorded hypnosis in a man with moderate hypnotic responsiveness (Pekala and Creegan, 2020). The participant showed significant phenomenological differences between the two states, assessed by the Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory, combined with electrophysiological correlates. A greater alpha and beta activity was found during TM than in hypnosis, with a greater beta in the left prefrontal cortex, and increased global delta activity during hypnosis.

Recently, intracranial EEG was used in three patients with no meditative or hypnotic experience (Bauer et al., 2022). The day after the surgery, patients listened to three different audios guiding to mind-wandering, mindfulness meditation, and an imaginative hypnotic state. The pre-recorded hypnotic procedure consisted of bringing attention to bodily sensations and then imagining visiting a pleasant place. The authors found non-specific and diffuse amplitude modulations in the three conditions. Connectivity analysis revealed common patterns in the three conditions, predominant in the low frequencies (delta, theta, and alpha). The connectivity patterns that were unique to the three conditions predominated in the gamma band, and one-third of the correlations in these models were negative.

Conclusions and future directions

In summary, several theoretical models and some experiments identify points of overlap and points of difference between the hypnotic state and the meditative state. In particular, hypnosis appears to be a form of attention focalization supported by an external expert in suggestion methodologies, with prominent imaginative elements and with a dissociation of executive control. Meditative states induce a state of absorption and concentration, but these are typically self-induced, and are forced through numerous practice sessions, which over time can lead to a progressive integration into executive control and—in the long run—to a decrease in the differentiation between the self and the external world.

The large number of meditative practices and the many possible hypnotic inductions open to a combinatorial explosion of interesting experimental contrasts, many of which have not been yet performed. The experiments that directly investigated these similarities and differences are currently only preliminary, although they begin to show phenomenological differences involving metacognition, absorption and executive control, while psychophysiological and EEG studies are too few to draw any kind of meaningful conclusion.

To better understand these states of consciousness and their relationships with ordinary states, it is necessary to increase research efforts, both from the point of view of theoretical models and the collection of data that make a direct comparison between hypnosis and meditation.

Author contributions

GP and ND contributed equally to the literature search, literature analysis, and writing of the manuscript. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Acunzo, D. J., and Terhune, D. B. (2021). A critical review of standardized measures of hypnotic suggestibility. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 69, 50–71. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2021.1833209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Álvarez-Pérez, Y., Rivero-Santana, A., Perestelo-Pérez, L., Duarte-Díaz, A., Ramos-García, V., Toledo-Chávarri, A., et al. (2022). Effectiveness of mantra-based meditation on mental health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 3380. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19063380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amihai, I., and Kozhevnikov, M. (2014). Arousal vs. relaxation: a comparison of the neurophysiological and cognitive correlates of Vajrayana and Theravada meditative practices. PLoS ONE 9, e102990. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barmark, S. M., and Gaunitz, S. C. (1979). Transcendental meditation and heterohypnosis as altered states of consciousness. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 27, 227–239. doi: 10.1080/00207147908407564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, P. R., Sabourdy, C., Chatard, B., Rheims, S., Lachaux, J.-P., Vidal, J. R., et al. (2022). Neural dynamics of mindfulness meditation and hypnosis explored with intracranial EEG: a feasibility study. Neurosci. Lett. 766, 136345. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, H., Frankel, F. H., Apfel, R., Daniels, M. D., Schniewind, H. E., Nemiah, J. C., et al. (1978). Treatment of anxiety: a comparison of the usefulness of self-hypnosis and a meditational relaxation technique. Psychother. Psychosom. 30, 229–242. doi: 10.1159/000287304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, D., Forte, M., Rich, P., and Epstein, G. (1983). Phenomenological differences among self hypnosis, mindfulness meditation, and imaging. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 2, 291–309. doi: 10.2190/JWC1-AXLY-6R0L-G2NK

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardeña, E., and Spiegel, D. (1991). Suggestibility, Absorption, and Dissociation: An Integrative Model of Hypnosis, ed J. F. Schumaker (New York: Taylor & Frances/Routledge), 93–107.

Google Scholar

Dahl, C. J., Lutz, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2015). Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 515–523. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Benedittis, G. (2021). Neural mechanisms of hypnosis and meditation-induced analgesia: a narrative review. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 69, 363–382. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2021.1917294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dienes, Z., Lush, P., Palfi, B., Roseboom, W., Scott, R., Parris, B., et al. (2020). Phenomenological Control as Cold Control. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Dienes, Z., Lush, P., Semmens-Wheeler, R., Parkinson, J., Scott, R., and Naish, P. (2016). “Hypnosis as self-deception; meditation as self-insight,” in Hypnosis and Meditation: Toward an Integrative Science of Conscious Planes, eds A. Raz, and M. Lifshitz (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 107–128.

Google Scholar

Drigas, A., Mitsea, E., and Skianis, C. (2021). The role of clinical hypnosis and VR in special education. Int. J. Recent Contribut. Eng. Sci. IT 9, 4–17. doi: 10.3991/ijes.v9i4.26147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drigas, A., Mitsea, E., and Skianis, C. (2022). Clinical hypnosis & VR, subconscious restructuring- brain rewiring & the entanglement with the 8 pillars of metacognition X 8 layers of consciousness X 8 intelligences. Int. J. Online Eng. 18, 78–95. doi: 10.3991/ijoe.v18i01.26859

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Raz, A. (2007). “Cognitive control processes and hypnosis,” in Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective, ed G. A. Jamieson (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 29–50. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2007.10401595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elkins, G. R., Barabasz, A. F., Council, J. R., and Spiegel, D. (2015). Advancing research and practice: the revised APA Division 30 definition of hypnosis. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 57, 378–385. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2015.1011465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, M. (1988). The deconstruction of the self: ego and “egolessness” in Buddhist insight meditation. J. Transpers. Psychol. 20, 61–69.

Google Scholar

Fernandez, A., Urwicz, L., Vuilleumier, P., and Berna, C. (2021). Impact of hypnosis on psychophysiological measures: a scoping literature review. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 64, 36–52. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2021.1873099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grover, M. P., Jensen, M. P., Patterson, D. R., Gertz, K. J., and Day, M. A. (2018). The association between mindfulness and hypnotizability: clinical and theoretical implications. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 61, 4–17. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2017.1419458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grover, M. P., Jensen, M. P., Ward, L. C., Ehde, D. M., Mattingley, J. B., Thorn, B. E., et al. (2021). An experimental investigation of the effects and mechanisms of mindfulness meditation versus self-hypnosis versus an attention control on cold pressor outcomes. Mindfulness 12, 923–935. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01556-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halsband, U., Mueller, S., Hinterberger, T., and Strickner, S. (2009). Plasticity changes in the brain in hypnosis and meditation. Contemp. Hypn. 26, 194–215. doi: 10.1002/ch.386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Häuser, W., Hagl, M., Schmierer, A., and Hansen, E. (2016). The efficacy, safety and applications of medical hypnosis: a systematic review of meta-analyses. Deutsch. Ärztebl. Int. 113, 289–296. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2016.0289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heide, F. J., Wadlington, W. L., and Lundy, R. M. (1980). Hypnotic responsivity as a predictor of outcome in meditation. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 28, 358–366. doi: 10.1080/00207148008409864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., and Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Pers. Indiv. Diff. 81, 41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lush, P., Naish, P., and Dienes, Z. (2016). Metacognition of intentions in mindfulness and hypnosis. Neurosci. Consci. 2016, niw007. doi: 10.1093/nc/niw007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, S., Malaktaris, A., Maxwell, R., Mellinger, D. I., and van der Kloet, D. (2012). Do hypnosis and mindfulness practices inhabit a common domain? Implications for research, clinical practice, and forensic science. J. Mind Body Regul. 2, 12–26.

Google Scholar

Markovic, J., and Thompson, E. (2016). Hypnosis and Meditation: A Neurophenomenological Comparison. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.31231/osf.io/eh237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matko, K., Ott, U., and Sedlmeier, P. (2021). What do meditators do when they meditate? Proposing a novel basis for future meditation research. Mindfulness 12, 1791–1811. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01641-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClintock, A. S., McCarrick, S. M., Garland, E. L., Zeidan, F., and Zgierska, A. E. (2019). Brief mindfulness-based interventions for acute and chronic pain: a systematic review. J. Alternat. Complement. Med. 25, 265–278. doi: 10.1089/acm.2018.0351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGeown, W. J. (2016). Hypnosis, Hypnotic Suggestibility, and Meditation: An Integrative Review of the Associated Brain Regions and Networks. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Mooneyham, B. W., and Schooler, J. W. (2016). “Mind wandering and meta-awareness in hypnosis and meditation,” in Hypnosis and Meditation: Towards an Integrative Science of Conscious Planes, eds A. Raz, and M. Lifshitz (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 221–240.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Morse, D. R., Martin, J. S., Furst, M. L., and Dubin, L. L. (1977). A physiological and subjective evaluation of meditation, hypnosis, and relaxation. Psychosom. Med. 39, 304–324. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197709000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuys, D. V. (1973). Meditation, attention, and hypnotic susceptibility: a correlational study. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 21, 59–69. doi: 10.1080/00207147308409306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakley, D. A., and Halligan, P. W. (2013). Hypnotic suggestion: opportunities for cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 565–576. doi: 10.1038/nrn3538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B. A. (2017). The role of frontal executive functions in hypnosis and hypnotic suggestibility. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 4, 211–229. doi: 10.1037/cns0000106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekala, R. J. (1991). “The phenomenology of consciousness inventory,” in Quantifying Consciousness, eds C. E. Izard, and J. L. Singer (New York, NY: Springer), 127–143. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-0629-8_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekala, R. J., and Creegan, K. (2020). States of consciousness, the qEEG, and noetic snapshots of the brain/mind interface: a case study of hypnosis and sidhi meditation. OBM Integr. Complement. Med. 5, 019. doi: 10.21926/obm.icm.2002019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffone, A., and Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cogn. Process. 11, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0354-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, A., and Lifshitz, M. (2016). Hypnosis and Meditation: Towards an Integrative Science of Conscious Planes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Rosenthal, D. (2005). Consciousness and Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Sadler, P., and Woody, E. (2010). “Dissociation in hypnosis: theoretical frameworks and psychotherapeutic implications,” in Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis, eds S. J. Lynn, J. W. Rhue, and I. Kirsch (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 151–178. doi: 10.2307/j.ctv1chs5qj.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semmens-Wheeler, R., and Dienes, Z. (2012). The contrasting role of higher order awareness in hypnosis and meditation. J. Mind Body Regulat. 2, 43–57.

Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 66, 487–518. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanos, N. P., Rivers, S. M., and Gottlieb, J. (1978). Hypnotic responsivity, meditation, and laterality of eye movements. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 87, 566. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.5.566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanos, N. P., Stam, H. J., Rivers, S. M., and Radtke, H. L. (1980). Meditation, expectation and performance on indices of nonanalytic attending. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 28, 244–251. doi: 10.1080/00207148008409849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, N. R., and Trevena, J. (2014). A comparison of therapist-present or therapist-free delivery of very brief mindfulness and hypnosis for acute experimental pain. New Zeal. J. Psychol. 43, 22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tellegen, A., and Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 83, 268. doi: 10.1037/h0036681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terhune, D. B., Cleeremans, A., Raz, A., and Lynn, S. J. (2017). Hypnosis and top-down regulation of consciousness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 81, 59–74.

Google Scholar

Terhune, D. B., and Jamieson, G. A. (2021). Hallucinations and the meaning and structure of absorption. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2108467118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2108467118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tung, Y.-H., and Hsieh, J.-C. (2019). The impacts of mindfulness on heart rate variability: a brief review. Int. J. Pharma Med. Biol. Sci. 8, 132–137. doi: 10.18178/ijpmbs.8.4.132-137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walrath, L. C., and Hamilton, D. W. (1975). Autonomic correlates of meditation and hypnosis. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 17, 190–197. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1975.10403739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, R. M., Day, M. A., Ehde, D. M., Turner, A. P., Ciol, M. A., Gertz, K. J., et al. (2022). Effects of hypnosis vs mindfulness meditation vs education on chronic pain intensity and secondary outcomes in veterans: a randomized clinical trial. Pain. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidan, F., and Grant, J. A. (2016). Meditative and Hypnotic Analgesia: Different Directions, Same Road? Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Keywords: mindfulness, attention, absorption, contemplation, metacognition, cognitive control, consciousness

Citation: Penazzi G and De Pisapia N (2022) Direct comparisons between hypnosis and meditation: A mini-review. Front. Psychol. 13:958185. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.958185

Received: 31 May 2022; Accepted: 27 June 2022;
Published: 15 July 2022.

Edited by:

Luca Simione, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council (ISTC-CNR), Italy

Reviewed by:

Athanasios Drigas, National Centre of Scientific Research Demokritos, Greece

Copyright © 2022 Penazzi and De Pisapia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicola De Pisapia, nicola.depisapia@unitn.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.