Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 02 February 2023
Sec. Psychology of Aging
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Health: Risk, Resilience and Reserve View all 7 articles

Cognitive interventions for memory and psychological well-being in aging and dementias

  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Maynooth University, Co Kildare, Ireland

The human lifespan has expanded drastically in the last few centuries, due to improvements in sanitation, medicine, and nutrition, but with this increase in longevity comes higher rates of cognitive pathology such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia; the latter is estimated to reach more than 75 million people by 2030. Pathology risk is related to measures of executive function, lifestyle factors (e.g., education, occupation, and leisure activities), and cognitive reserve. One way of building cognitive reserve may be to structure the environment to encourage lifelong engagement and learning, and since a pharmacological “cure” for dementia remains elusive, non-pharmacological approaches such as physical activity, social engagement, and cognitive stimulation are becoming increasingly essential to preserving and protecting brain health. Here, we describe our recent research into Reminiscence Therapy (RT) to promote cognitive and psychological function in old age and early dementia. We review the Recall Initiative, which involved brain imaging and behavioral indices of memory pre- and post-RT. We also report results from a pilot study—AIM WARM—in which RT was combined with physical activity, specifically walking, for early-stage dementia. Finally, we outline our future directions for tailored reminiscence interventions in combination with other activities (e.g., yoga and meditation) for different groups, namely early Alzheimer’s disease, Semantic Dementia, and older individuals in the prison system.

1. Introduction

The human lifespan has expanded drastically in the last few centuries, due to improvements in sanitation, medicine, and nutrition (Olshansky et al., 2005). This should not imply that a longer lifespan is accordingly free of disease or disability; rather rates of physical and cognitive pathology are also rising (Livingston et al., 2020). Some age-related cognitive change is normal and occurs in the absence of disease. Older adults can expect slowed processing speed, diffuse attention, and difficulty learning new information (Birren et al., 2006; Craik and Salthouse, 2011). These normal age-related changes are distinct from cognitive pathology, including neurocognitive disorders like mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia (Cabeza et al., 2018).

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and, to a greater extent, Alzheimer’s’ Disease and Related Dementias (ADRD) are classified by changes in cognitive performance, which may impact realms like planning, memory, learning, and attentiveness (Turner et al., 2020). In MCI, these changes are subtle and may not significantly impact activities of daily living or the ability to live independently. A diagnosis of MCI increases the likelihood of converting to dementia, but not all cases are prodromal (Cabeza et al., 2018).

The term dementia refers to a group of progressive changes that are marked by neurodegeneration in specific brain regions. Accordingly, downstream changes can be observed in domains like executive function, perceptual-motor abilities, and personality (Turner et al., 2020). People living with dementia experience significant and persistent changes in cognition, but these are not immediately fatal. Instead, the long-term outcomes are loss of independence and continued declines in cognitive ability. The progressive nature of dementia, combined with improved medical advances for comorbid diagnoses, means that people are living longer with this diagnosis (Livingston et al., 2020). As such, dementia impacts social and economic planning at every level including housing, health, urban and rural planning, transport, policing, and the business environment. Dementia is wide-reaching, estimated to impact more than 80 million people by 2030, and over 150 million worldwide by 2050 (Nichols et al., 2022).

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) represents 60–70% of dementia diagnoses and is subsequently the most common form of dementia (Rizzi et al., 2014). The pathophysiology of AD is, in part, believed to be connected to beta-amyloid peptide (Aβ) deposition throughout the brain (Sepulcre et al., 2018). It has emerged that one of the cornerstone beliefs about the biochemical basis of AD—the presence of Aβ plaques in the brain—may contain fabricated data, potentially undermining decades of research into chemical-based interventions (Lesné et al., 2006). The treatment for AD remains elusive, and available treatments provide a mere stopgap in the form of relief of symptoms from cognitive decline (Srivastava et al., 2021). A discovery to cure dementia is unforeseen and those living with Alzheimer’s are left waiting. A reliable avenue of research may be the impact of lifestyle factors, which have shown promising results.

2. Non-pharmacological approaches

Health and longevity are impacted by innumerable factors ranging from how often someone flosses their teeth to how they spend their leisure time (Scarmeas and Stern, 2003; Brown, 2019). Risk factors that increase the likelihood of dementia include aspects of the general environment (e.g., geography, sun exposure, and air pollution) as well as the habits of individuals (e.g., participation in leisure activities, psychological well-being, and age at retirement).

The latter, lifestyle-based risk factors, encompasses a variety of modifiable variables. In the absence of a cure, this presents an opportunity to examine how lifestyle-based factors may be addressed to influence dementia risk. The risk of developing dementia is shaped by several lifestyle factors, including levels of physical activity, social engagement, mental well-being, and cognitive stimulation (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2010). These elements of daily living can be used to predict the risk of receiving a dementia diagnosis as well as forecasting the rate of decline (Deckers et al., 2019).

Accordingly, this knowledge about risk factors can be used to mitigate dementia diagnoses. Due to the high social and financial costs of dementia, delaying a diagnosis is a viable strategy for burden reduction. By addressing the risks and recognizing the roles of vulnerability and prevention, rates of dementia could drop by 40% (Livingston et al., 2020). Lifestyle factors that decrease the risk of dementia include moderate physical activity, psychological well-being (e.g., no diagnosis of depression), and regular social engagement (Livingston et al., 2020; Sikkes et al., 2021). These are all modifiable factors that can be integrated into interventions using a non-pharmacological approach.

Non-pharmacological approaches are generally easily adopted, have few side effects, and can be combined with pharmacological treatments without interference (Sikkes et al., 2021). As such, these approaches can complement people with complex care needs or those living in institutional settings with a high care burden. Thus, non-pharmacological approaches are feasible for intervention studies and can be used with a variety of populations. In the following section, we describe some non-pharmacological approaches.

2.1. Cognitive reserve and engagement

As a pharmacological “cure” for dementia remains unforeseen, it becomes critical that we utilize non-pharmacological approaches to maximize brain health and resiliency. While the causal understanding of dementia pathology is yet unknown, there is evidence for protective effects (Li et al., 2016). Randomized clinical trials (RCT) of physical exercise demonstrate robust protection of cognition (Ahlskog et al., 2011). Complementing this experimental work are epidemiological and correlative studies. Here, research suggests that psychological well-being and social engagement are not only protective of cognition but also can reduce dementia risk altogether (Wilson et al., 2003; Park et al., 2007).

We rely on the knowledge that modifiable lifestyle factors like physical exercise, psychological well-being, and social engagement can preserve and protect brain health. How might these lifestyle factors influence cognition and buffer against pathology? Researchers are only beginning to understand the mechanisms that subserve age-related resilience in cognitive efficiency. Possibilities include improvements in basic cognitive abilities, the development of strategies, and the automatization of selective aspects of a skill or task (Baltes et al., 1999). Cabeza and colleagues suggested that high-performing older adults may be compensating for neural decline by reorganizing neurocognitive networks (Cabeza et al., 2002). This suggestion of “neural plasticity” led to many theories that sought to answer the question: does the aging brain attempt to compensate for cognitive change, and how might the brain achieve this goal?

2.2. Cognitive resources and compensation

The need for compensation can be predicted by lifestyle factors that influence the health and plasticity of the brain. Protective “neural resource enrichment” (e.g., participating in exercise, leisure, and social activities) may preserve or enhance brain function. On the other hand, antagonistic “neural resource depletion” may accelerate age-related declines in performance (e.g., APOE-4 gene, smoking, and obesity; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). Again, we highlight the role of modifiable factors in the aging brain by recognizing that compensation can be helped or harmed by lifestyle factors.

The notion of “brain maintenance” suggests that healthy genetics and lifestyle factors may result in a need for less compensation (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014) see Nyberg et al. (2012) for more on brain maintenance. The resources that are drawn upon during the act of compensation are built and maintained, perhaps throughout the lifespan (Cabeza et al., 2018). By providing opportunities for neural enhancement, the brain can counter age-related decline and other insults. That is, by maintaining resources and building a reserve, the aging brain can effectively buffer decline (Burke et al., 2019).

Though the occurrence of cognitive aging is well-documented, there is less empirical support concerning the mechanisms behind this process and where in the nervous system, it may originate (Salthouse, 2004). Neuroimaging allows for the observation of brain processes by revealing a window into the neuroanatomical substrates of cognitive functions (Festini et al., 2018). Various theories and models (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults; HAROLD; Cabeza et al., 2002); Posterior–Anterior Shift in Aging (PASA; Grady et al., 1994, 2003); and Compensation-Related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis (CRUNCH; Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008) attempt to describe differential processing patterns that are observed in young versus older brains. Perhaps, older adults are compensating for neural decline by reorganizing neurocognitive networks (Grady et al., 1994, 2003; Cabeza et al., 2002; Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008; Festini et al., 2018).

During verbal recall tasks, older adults who demonstrate network activation similar to young adults (e.g., right prefrontal cortex) perform poorly, whereas the highest performing older adults demonstrate unique activation patterns (e.g., bilateral prefrontal cortex; Cabeza et al., 2002). Furthermore, adults demonstrate stronger anterior activation relative to young adults, but weaker posterior (e.g., occipital) activation relative to young adults (Grady et al., 1994, 2003). These patterns of overactivation (which may include the prefrontal cortex as seen in Grady et al., 2003 or bilateral as seen in Cabeza et al., 2002) relative to young adults exist under conditions of low cognitive demand. As cognitive demand conditions increase and tasks become more challenging, older adults “max out” their resources and display under-activation and subsequent decline in performance (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008).

Cognitive changes are typical across the lifespan, though here we focus instead on pathological forms of decline. Age-related declines in function may be addressed via compensation, perhaps in the form of “scaffolding” (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). Compensation refers to the ability of the brain to increase activation or recruit additional resources to cope with heightened demand (Cabeza et al., 2018). The process of compensation may be assisted by scaffolding, which can be internal (e.g., rerouting neural networks or increasing neural activation) and external (e.g., environmental cues and strategies; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). New learning, cardiovascular health, and certain types of cognitive training may enhance the brain’s ability to procure effective scaffolding (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014).

Reserve is a concept that may explain the heterogeneity associated with susceptibility to dementia. It may also be used to describe varying rates of change seen in both the normal and pathological aging process (Schneider and Pichora-Fuller, 2000). Aging is associated with a normal decline in a multitude of cognitive processes and brain functions. Yet some people display relatively minor intraindividual loss of function while others exhibit steep declines in their rate of change (Stern, 2012). So too in pathological aging, the rate of change and trajectory of disease can vary widely.

These pathological trajectories can be described both in terms of interindividual and intraindividual change. For example, the time between a dementia diagnosis and loss of independence can be relatively quick, while others live with the diagnosis for decades (Jagust, 2018). Exhibiting interindividual change, a person may exhibit preservation of function for many years, followed by a precipitous decline (Walsh et al., 2022). Understanding the rate of change and trajectory of decline is critical for maintaining activities of daily living, and thus independence (Hertzog et al., 2008; Walsh et al., 2022). Given a diagnosis of dementia, the ability to retain function presents an important leverage point in the disease. Cognitive reserve may flatten the slope of decline and contribute to preserved independence (Song et al., 2021).

A growing body of literature suggests that age-related decline in cognition can sometimes be reduced through interventions of physical activity, social engagement, and mental well-being (Erickson et al., 2011; Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2016; Allen et al., 2020). These interventions utilize lifestyle factors that are known promoters of cognitive reserve. High levels of cognitive reserve may decrease the risk of dementia by as much as 46%, relative to those with moderate to low levels of cognitive reserve (Stern, 2012). Reserve can be classified as either “brain” [e.g., quantitative measure of neurons or brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)] or “cognitive” (e.g., interindividual differences in performing mental tasks that enable resilience to brain insult, which can be drawn upon during the act of compensation, see the previous section; Stern, 2012). The delineation between brain and cognitive reserve is under debate, with a proposed move toward referring to general “reserve” rather than these separate aspects (Cabeza et al., 2018).

It follows, then, that building cognitive reserve, or enhancing resilience against cognitive insult, is a crucial component of non-pharmacological approaches to promoting cognitive and psychological function in old age and early dementia. Correlational studies between lifestyle variables and cognitive performance show protective benefits (cognitive flexibility, delayed dementia diagnosis, and increased independent living; Jaeggi et al., 2020). Physical activity, social engagement, and cognitive stimulation are all suitable candidates for intervention-based work that reduce the likelihood of dementia and cognitive decline. An important goal for future research is to determine when benefits are and are not produced.

2.3. Cognitive training

Cognitive training studies often demonstrate brain-volumetric increases and increased activation patterns in targeted regions (Lustig et al., 2009). Strategy-based training can demonstrate benefits on the specific task (near-transfer) but lacks benefits on general performance (far-transfer; Lustig et al., 2009). Cognitive performance shows an immediate benefit from training (increased cognition in 86% of the speed of processing group, 74% of the reasoning group, and 26% of the memory group; Gross and Rebok, 2011). However, performance increases on training effects are highly specific. For example, training on memory can increase performance on memory tasks but not speed of processing or reasoning (Gross and Rebok, 2011). Engagement-based interventions can improve cognition and performance while demonstrating far-transfer benefits on a range of tasks.

The engagement model strives to embed the participant in complex environments that are socially enriching (Stine-Morrow et al., 2014). Engaging in social and intellectual activities may buffer age-related declines in cognition even without explicit instruction (Stine-Morrow et al., 2014). One explanation for this benefit is that both social and intellectual challenges require a diverse range of abilities rather than mastery of one skill (e.g., being a Bridge player requires social skills, mathematical abilities, and memory—being a maths prodigy alone will not allow you to master the game). Non-pharmacological interventions that demand cognitive engagement will draw on various skill sets and require cognitive flexibility (Park et al., 2007; Stine-Morrow et al., 2007, 2014; Parisi et al., 2009). The Synapse project is one example of a robust study that examined this association through randomized controlled trials to high- (e.g., learning digital photography) and low- (e.g., trivia or active listening) engagement conditions. Those who were assigned to the high-engagement condition exhibited improvements in neural function and episodic memory. These high-engagement skills may promote the use of distributed networks and promote neural efficiency, thus increasing strategy and scaffolding (McDonough et al., 2015). This may be a mechanism by which various abilities could then contribute to building cognitive reserve (Stine-Morrow et al., 2008).

Group-based interventions foster social interaction, often in the presence of learning a new skill or navigating a novel environment. This approach may boost engagement by requiring the participants to remain socially active while also learning or practicing novel tasks. Some of these studies show benefits for older adults and also may delay dementia (Jaeggi et al., 2014). Perhaps, the building of cognitive reserve is a contributing factor in conferring protective benefits.

Non-pharmacological approaches offer a promising avenue for modifying dementia risk. These approaches often target lifestyle-based factors like moderate physical activity, psychological well-being, and regular social engagement. Interventions which target these factors can demonstrate cognitive protection against dementia, allowing older adults to experience an increased quality of life with greater autonomy and independence. The mechanism for this protection may be explained by the concept of cognitive reserve, which supports a buffer against cognitive insult.

2.4. Reminiscence therapy

Reminiscence Therapy (RT) is now a popular treatment for depression, loneliness, and anxiety in older adults by dealing with past life experiences (Hsieh and Wang, 2003; Scogin et al., 2005; Woods et al., 2005; Chueh and Chang, 2014; Lopes et al., 2016; Syed Elias et al., 2019). Structured RT, where triggers and prompts are provided for discussion in a group setting, seems to increase psychological well-being such as life satisfaction, communication, and self-esteem (Baines et al., 1987; Chao et al., 2006; Yamagami et al., 2007; Preschl et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2015; Allen et al., 2018). Guided reminiscence provides an opportunity for participants to recall stories and anecdotes, while a trained facilitator leads the group and guides the reminiscence topics. Interestingly, RT appears to have a positive effect on global cognition as well in dementia patients who report improved mood, well-being, and cognitive function after 4–6 weeks (Woods et al., 2005; Wang, 2007; Azcurra, 2012; Lopes et al., 2016).

Most notably, RT appears to have therapeutic benefits, particularly for dementia patients (Coll-Planas et al., 2017). Both group-based and individual RT encourage social interaction and cooperation, thus minimizing isolation and depressive symptoms (Coll-Planas et al., 2017; Berntsen and Kirk, 2019). In dementia patients, RT may allow for rehearsal of conversational skills, as well as inspiring a renewed sense of purpose and interest in the world (Baines et al., 1987). Furthermore, RT leads to improved mood, with positive reminiscences evoking generally positive feelings, which may be sustained and augmented with increased sessions (Cappeliez et al., 2008; Coll-Planas et al., 2017). In particular, there is evidence to suggest that triggering reminiscence with music encourages positive associations, thus improving mood and, potentially, cognition (Engelbrecht et al., 2021).

Though the majority of studies support RT as a mood-boosting mechanism primarily, thus impacting cognition indirectly by improving emotional state and increasing motivation and general well-being, there is some evidence to support neurological changes associated with RT. Research demonstrates increased blood flow in the frontal lobe as well as reduced neuroimaging abnormalities associated with RT (Tanaka et al., 2007). Furthermore, reminiscing while listening to music results in greater frontal and central bilateral activation, suggesting that RT, particularly in conjunction with a mediating variable such as music, may have a direct impact on cognition (Engelbrecht et al., 2022). It is possible that the act of reminiscing itself may also be beneficial to global cognition. Consistent, prompted retrieval encourages reactivation and reinforces existing memory traces, as well as allowing for the formation of new associations, particularly when there is a lag between reminiscence sessions (Delaney et al., 2010).

In recent years, research has pointed to the benefits to both mental health and cognitive performance (e.g., memory, attention) of physical exercise and, separately, of actively engaging in guided use of one’s memory, for example through Reminiscence Therapy (RT). These benefits appear to apply to both healthy older adults and those in the early stages of conditions associated with memory problems, such as dementias including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Evidence suggests that physical activity/aerobic exercise (see Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013)— and in particular walking (see O’Mara, 2019)—results in an array of cognitive and psychological benefits which are underpinned by structural and functional changes in memory-related brain structures, particularly the prefrontal cortices and hippocampus.

A growing literature has begun to suggest that such reminiscence activities can lead to psychological and/or cognitive gains for healthy, community-dwelling older individuals as well as those living with dementias. Other studies suggest that the benefits of RT can be enhanced by combining it with other interventions such as exercise or creative engagement. This, primarily, is what makes RT so unique as an intervention; combining RT with other activities allows for a more tailored, bespoke intervention that is better suited to the individual. Moreover, these interventions are non-invasive, stimulating, and enjoyable, impacting and often improving quality of life (Berntsen and Kirk, 2019).

2.5. Exercise

In the older population, the task of finding solutions to cognitive decline is still a major challenge. One powerful potential avenue is exercise, which has shown promising effects on cognitive abilities and general quality of life in older adults (Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013). Neuroimaging has also demonstrated exercise’s impact on functional brain plasticity (Cui et al., 2018). Increasingly frequently, public health guidelines advocate exercise as an essential tool for the prevention and/or moderation of the number of dementia cases worldwide (Erickson et al., 2011; Gajewski and Falkenstein, 2016).

By definition, exercise is “a type of physical activity consisting of planned, structured, and repetitive bodily movement done to improve and/or maintain one or more components of physical fitness” (Caspersen et al., 1985).

Unfortunately, the decrease in human physical capacities is often associated with the process of aging and tends to go hand-in-hand with a decline in an individual’s activity levels. Remarkably, exercise appears to offer a protective function, or at least helps to preserve global cognition and human functional capacity such as muscle mass or cardiorespiratory function. More precisely, aerobic and resistance exercises are shown to have strong effects on general health, such as improved endocrine function or better cognitive abilities (Valenzuela et al., 2019). In contrast with aerobic activities, only a small quantity of resistance exercise is actually needed to show benefits on cognition for older adults, and particularly on memory (Sun et al., 2021). This effect was explained by the essential value that resistance training can have on an individual’s quality of life for people living with dementia (Erickson et al., 2011; Barha et al., 2017).

In fact, various studies have shown that the combination of multiple non-pharmacological activities, rather than one, has stronger outcomes for preventing global cognitive/executive functional decline in people living with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). There has also been a sharp increase in literature promoting the benefits of multicomponent exercise interventions in helping the global well-being and cognition of older adults (Paramos-de-Carvalho et al., 2021).

The World Health Organization (WHO), in October 2022, highlighted the importance of the older population committing to at least 150–300 min of moderate-intensity aerobic exercise every week or, alternatively, 75–150 min of vigorous aerobic exercise of muscle strengthening activities (including the major muscle groups) for a minimum of three times per week. However, there remains no real consensus on the minimum quantity needed to show benefits on cognition, reflecting the ideas that for the older population, “doing some physical activity is better than doing none” (Bull et al., 2020), and that “the shorter and more frequent the better” (Sanders et al., 2019).

Globally, exercise is one of the most reliable interventions demonstrated to have real effect on the prevention/treatment of cognitive decline/dementia in older adults. However, it is still ambiguous which type of exercise is the most reliable and, more importantly, which exercise is the best for each population. However, a recent review paper has suggested that low/moderate levels of aerobic exercise over medium to long durations (e.g., walking, cycling) may be more beneficial for memory for young and older adults in the pre-encoding phase, while comparable levels of anaerobic/resistance exercise may be more effective post-encoding, and less so for older adults (Loprinzi et al., 2021). This distinction warrants further exploration. Aerobic fitness has been identified as a preventative measure with regard to natural hippocampal degradation; higher aerobic fitness is associated with larger hippocampal volume and improved memory in older adults (Erickson et al., 2011). This may be a result of the similarities between aerobic exercise and successful encoding of memories—both require repetition. It is likely that aerobic exercise carried out in conjunction with RT may be particularly beneficial, given the effect of each on the hippocampus.

The findings of our projects (see below) will focus on understanding and measuring the impact of different types of exercises. By doing so, we hope to address one of the major health and social concerns of the 21st century.

3. Interventions

3.1. Recall

Next, we review the Recall Initiative, which involved brain imaging and behavioral indices of memory pre- and post-RT. In 2019, we established a 2-year project, the RECALL Initiative, funded by the IRC New Horizons funding scheme. We focused on the potential benefits of reminiscence therapy on memory and well-being for healthy older adults living in the community, and people in the early stages of dementia. The initial study incorporated pre- and post-reminiscence testing of processes including memory, attention, and executive function, as well as psychological variables—anxiety, depression, quality of life, and social engagement. A small sub-group also took part in functional MRI brain scanning at Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience to test for structural and functional brain changes as a result of the intervention. A Philips Intera Achieva 3.0 T MR system (Best, The Netherlands) was used to acquire the MRI data. Preprocessing was performed using fmriprep1 and fMRI statistical analyses were performed with SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping; Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom).2 During the group sessions, we recorded audio (with permission) of the stories and memories that our participants recalled, with these recordings hosted in a designated section of the Digital Repository of Ireland.3

Groups in Maynooth, Swords, Tallaght, and Dublin city center all took part in 6 weeks of reminiscence. Weekly groups recalled stories on their earlier years about personal experiences and major national events, with the help of photos, such as the visit of John F Kennedy in 1963 and the Pope in 1979. While only modest improvements were observed in these groups, this is likely due to the fact that they were a cognitively very healthy group at the outset and the fact that numbers were low in each group, reducing statistical power (Allen et al., 2020, 2021a). To examine potential benefits for cognitive decline, a modified version of this intervention was administered to a small group of older adults living with dementia, with a weekly group meeting in Peamount Healthcare’s Dementia Unit. Again, measures of cognition and psychological health were taken before and after 6 weeks of reminiscence, with some indications of improvements in certain aspects of memory (Allen et al., 2021b). An additional patient experiencing Transient Epileptic Amnesia (TEA) also participated in the intervention, and again some modest improvements were observed. Functional imaging data are currently being analyzed (Viard et al., in preparation). The classical autobiographical memory network (including the precuneus, medial frontal and lateral temporal gyri, and hippocampi) was activated for pre- and post-visits separately. Direct comparisons showed greater activation for post-visit in left superior medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, and anterior hippocampus, compared to pre-visit (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A small sub-group also took part in functional MRI brain scanning at Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience to test for structural and functional brain changes as a result of the intervention. Functional imaging data are currently being analyzed (Viard et al., in preparation). Initial results suggest the classical autobiographical memory network (including the precuneus, medial frontal and lateral temporal gyri, and hippocampi) was activated for pre- and post-visits separately. Direct comparisons showed greater activation for post-visit in left superior medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, and anterior hippocampus, compared to pre-visit.

The social interaction element of the intervention appears to be a key mechanism driving any changes observed; by encouraging healthy older adults and people living with dementia to engage in a weekly, semi-structured, peer-to-peer group activity, social as well as cognitive function are engaged, leading to a greater likelihood of engagement, reduced dropout, and a more positive overall experience for participants.

3.2. AIM-WARM

In our Age-Inclusive Maynooth: Walk and Recall Memories (AIM-WARM) work, which was funded by an Irish Research Council New Foundations grant 2020, we investigated the potential impact of combining the exercise of walking and reminiscence therapy, in early Alzheimer’s disease groups. We measured the potential benefits of this combination on autobiographical memory, cognitive abilities, and psychological well-being in a small-sample pilot study (n = 2).

Performance on cognitive and psychological measures was taken pre- and post-intervention, on paper, and in individual sessions with each participant.

Cognitive measures were assessed with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA, Nasreddine et al., 2005) and the Episodic Autobiographical Memory Interview (EAMI; Irish et al., 2008). Psychological well-being data were measured with the help of the Control, Autonomy, Self-Realization, and Pleasure Quality of Life Questionnaire (CASP-19; Netuveli et al., 2006); the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985); and lastly the Holden Communication Scale (Strøm et al., 2016).

We again employed the Patient and Public Involvement (PPI) approach, which was established in our previous work and will continue to be employed in each of our future projects. Details of this approach can be found here.4 Four PPI consultants/advisors (two people living with dementia and two carers) were recruited through the Dementia Research Advisory Team, from the Alzheimer’s Society of Ireland (two women, two men), and were meticulously embedded throughout the project—from the design to interpretation/dissemination of the results. Their contributions strongly improved the content of the intervention and taught us the most effective, applicable, and convenient method to employ in this study.

With assistance from the geography department, we planned a geographical tour including specific historical sites as visual cues, selecting those that may be most familiar to the community participants (i.e., local church). Prior to the experiment, care was taken to complete a Walkability Audit Tool for Roads and Streets, to make sure that the routes were safe. Additionally and as mentioned, people with dementia and carers also offered unique insights and viewpoints to improve our research project, including for these walking-routes. Lastly, our selection criteria included people who were familiar with, or who used to live in Maynooth Town, for more engaged reminiscence sessions. The intervention group completed hourly, twice-weekly sessions for 2 weeks, walking and reminiscence-based interventions. During the intervention, we stopped to discuss each historical site/personal memories, and recorded the conversations for later anonymized upload to the Digital Repository of Ireland (DRI) archive.

Autobiographical Memory was one of the main focuses within the study. In the intervention group, after the completion of four reminiscence-plus-walking sessions, no significant main effect was found, despite an interesting increase in semantic memory (i.e., the name of a friend), precisely during the period of middle adulthood (Episodic Autobiographical Memory Interview, EAMI; Irish et al., 2008). This supports the reminiscence peak between 10 and 30 years of age. In parallel, this also accompanies the Multiple Trace Theory, which supports the possible distinction between semantic and episodic memory (Roche et al., in preparation; data available on Open Science at doi 10.17605/OSF.IO/Q9VAY). Combining reminiscence with walking may represent a viable means to enhance the benefits of these interventions. However, as this is a pilot study, findings should be interpreted cautiously, with further work needed.

Our next project, entitled “Tailored Reminiscence Interventions for Aging and Dementias in Community Settings (TRIADICS),” continues work on tailored, group-based interventions, using larger samples and different populations of interest. This will enable further interpretation and to understand better the potential benefits of these combined interventions.

4. Discussion

4.1. Future directions

Results from the AIM-WARM initiative will shape the future of our next project, TRIADICS. This project, funded by IRC Coalesce, will assess the benefits to cognition and psychological health using tailored, co-created interventions. These may include exercise, meditation, breathwork, and group reminiscence meetings for older adults and people living with dementias, including Alzheimer’s disease and Semantic Dementia.

We will continue to call on a Patient and Public Involvement (PPI) approach to co-create these tailored therapeutic reminiscence interventions with representatives from each of the four groups of interest: community-dwelling older adults, people living with Alzheimer’s disease, people living with Semantic Dementia, and older people living in prisons. This project will also utilize functional MRI to obtain neuroanatomical data to provide insight into relevant brain mechanisms and structures that may be engaged in reminiscence therapy and throughout this intervention.

Using an iterative process, these Reminiscence Therapy interventions are being refined with the intent to deploy this service on a larger, public scale and disseminate this approach to the broader public. The project will develop, refine, and pilot-test a set of tailored interventions and then validate them through a series of pre-post mixed-methods studies. Subsequently, this regime can be rolled-out in larger-scale initiatives via links with Age-Friendly Ireland (AFI) and Meath County Council, the national contact point for age-friendly communities in Ireland. Finally, recalled memories and stories elicited during the program will be recorded, archived, anonymized, and made available to the public as part of our existing, bespoke section of the Digital Repository of Ireland (DRI).

5. Conclusion

Brain disease remains a significant societal challenge for the 21st century. The annual cost of brain disease in the EU is estimated at approximately €800 billion, with dementia accounting for over €100 billion of this figure (€105,163,000; (Gustavsson et al., 2011)). This substantial economic cost is mirrored by the catastrophic personal impact dementias have on sufferers and their families/carers, with AD alone affecting some 35 million people worldwide (Ising et al., 2015); this figure is expected to treble by 2050. Any interventions which can effect even a modest reduction in the prevalence or impact of dementia and normal age-related memory decline will lead to substantive personal, social, and financial benefits to society at large, and will reduce strain on already laboring healthcare systems and overworked medical professionals. Lifestyle-based interventions which may bolster resilience and cognitive reserve, while also addressing a need for community engagement and social contact, represent an encouraging avenue for future research, and may present an affordable, enjoyable, and effective means to preserve cognitive function and health into later life in both normality and pathology. Driven by the ethos of PPI, projects such as the Recall Initiative, AIM WARM, and TRIADICS represent our first steps in this direction, and will hopefully lead to novel, creative, and widely applicable interventions for brain health.

Author contributions

CD, CP, MC, and RR contributed equally to the writing of the manuscript. CD and CP also collected data and carried out PPI workshops for projects reported above for the AIM WARM and TRIADICS projects. RR was PI for all of the above-reported projects. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by three awards from the Irish Research Council (IRC), specifically IRC New Foundations 2020 (Ref: IRC/NF/2019/ROCHE), IRC New Horizons 2016 (Ref: REPRO/2016/76), and IRC Coalesce 2021 (REF: COALESCE/2021/101) schemes. CD and MC are supported by Coalesce funding, and CP was funded under the New Foundations scheme.

Acknowledgments

This work was completed in collaboration with the Alzheimer’s Society of Ireland. Sincere thanks to Sean Commins (MU), Ronan Foley (MU), Ciara O’Reilly (ASI), and the Dementia Research Advisory Team for their valuable input.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Ahlskog, J. E., Geda, Y. E., Graff-Radford, N. R., and Petersen, R. C. (2011). Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin. Proc. 86, 876–884. doi: 10.4065/mcp.2011.0252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, A. P., Doyle, C., Commins, S., and Roche, R. A. P. (2018). Autobiographical memory, the ageing brain and mechanisms of psychological interventions. Ageing Res. Rev. 42, 100–111. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2017.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, AP, Doyle, C, Doyle, CM, Monaghan, C, Fitzpatrick, N, and Roche, R.A.P. (2021a). What we talk about when we talk about the past: discursive psychological analysis of autobiographical reminiscence in older Irish adults in Psychobiographical Illustrations on Meaning and Identity in Sociocultural Contexts. (eds.) C-H Mayer, P. J. P. Fouche, and R. Niekerkvan (Palgrave Mcmillan)

Google Scholar

Allen, A. P., Doyle, C., and Roche, R. A. P. (2020). The impact of reminiscence on autobiographical memory, cognition and psychological well-being in healthy older adults. Eur. J. Psychol. 16, 317–330. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i2.2097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, A. P., Tully, M. L., O’Neill, D., and Roche, R. A. P. (2021b). Reminiscence groupwork and autobiographical memory as part of meaningful activities. Group 30, 77–87. doi: 10.1921/gpwk.v30i1.1548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azcurra, D. J. L. S. (2012). A reminiscence program intervention to improve the quality of life of long-term care residents with Alzheimer’s disease. A randomized controlled trial. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 34, 422–433. doi: 10.1016/j.rbp.2012.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baines, S., Saxby, P., and Ehlert, K. (1987). Reality orientation and reminiscence therapy: a controlled cross-over study of elderly confused people. Br. J. Psychiatry 151, 222–231. doi: 10.1192/bjp.151.2.222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., and Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 471–507. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barha, C. K., Davis, J. C., Falck, R. S., Nagamatsu, L. S., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2017). Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front Neuroendocrinol. 46, 71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Kirk, M. (2019). “Watering Memory Trees,” Cerebrum: The Dana Forum on Brain Science, Vol. 2019.

Google Scholar

Birren, J. E., Schaie, K. W., Abeles, R. P., Gatz, M., and Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 6th Edn Burlington, MC, USA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Brown, G. C. (2019). The endotoxin hypothesis of neurodegeneration. J. Neuroinflammation 16, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12974-019-1564-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, F. C., Al-Ansari, S. S., Biddle, S., Borodulin, K., Buman, M. P., Cardon, G., et al. (2020). World Health Organization 2020 guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Br. J. Sports Med. 54, 1451–1462. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2020-102955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, S. N., Mormino, E. C., Rogalski, E. J., Kawas, C. H., Willis, R. J., and Park, D. C. (2019). What are the later life contributions to reserve, resilience, and compensation? Neurobiol. Aging 83, 140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage 17, 1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappeliez, P., Guindon, M., and Robitaille, A. (2008). Functions of reminiscence and emotional regulation among older adults. J. Ageing Stud. 22, 266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2007.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., and Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. (Washington, D.C.: 1974), 100, 126–131.

Google Scholar

Chao, S.-Y., Liu, H.-Y., Wu, C.-Y., Jin, S.-F., Chu, T.-L., Huang, T.-S., et al. (2006). The effects of group reminiscence therapy on depression, self esteem, and life satisfaction of elderly nursing home residents. J. Nurs. Res. 14, 36–45. doi: 10.1097/01.JNR.0000387560.03823.c7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chueh, K.-H., and Chang, T.-Y. (2014). Effectiveness of group reminiscence therapy for depressive symptoms in male veterans: 6-month follow-up. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 377–383. doi: 10.1002/gps.4013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coll-Planas, L., Watchman, K., Doménech, S., McGillivray, D., O'Donnell, H., and Tolson, D. (2017). Developing evidence for football (soccer) reminiscence interventions within long-term care: a co-operative approach applied in Scotland and Spain. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 18, 355–360. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2017.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., and Salthouse, T. A. (2011). The Handbook of Aging and Cognition Psychology press.

Google Scholar

Cui, M. Y., Lin, Y., Sheng, J. Y., Zhang, X., and Cui, R. J. (2018). Exercise intervention associated with cognitive improvement in Alzheimer’s disease. Neural Plast. 9234105. doi: 10.1155/2018/9234105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deckers, K., Nooyens, A., van Boxtel, M., Verhey, F., Verschuren, M., and Köhler, S. (2019). Gender and educational differences in the association between lifestyle and cognitive decline over 10 years: the Doetinchem cohort study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 70, S31–S41. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delaney, P. F., Verkoeijen, P. P., and Spirgel, A. (2010). Spacing and testing effects: A deeply critical, lengthy, and at times discursive review of the literature. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 53, 63–147. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53003-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelbrecht, R., Bhar, S., and Ciorciari, J. (2022). Music-assisted reminiscence therapy with older adults: feasibility, Acceptibility, and outcomes. Music. Ther. Perspect. doi: 10.1093/mtp/miac021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelbrecht, R., Bhar, S., and Ciorciari, J. (2021). Planting the SEED: A model to describe the functions of music in reminiscence therapy. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 44:101441. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2021.101441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Festini, S. B., Zahodne, L., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2018). “Theoretical perspectives on age differences in brain activation: HAROLD, PASA, CRUNCH—How do they STAC up?” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology.

Google Scholar

Gajewski, P. D., and Falkenstein, M. (2016). Physical activity and neurocognitive functioning in aging - a condensed updated review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act: official journal of the European Group for Research into Elderly and Physical Activity, 13:1. doi: 10.1186/s11556-016-0161-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Pinilla, F., and Hillman, C. (2013). “The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities,” in Comprehensive Physiology. ed. R. Terjung. 1st ed (Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley), 403–428.

Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., Maisog, J. M., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mentis, M. J., Salerno, J. A., et al. (1994). Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. J. Neurosci. 14, 1450–1462. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01450.1994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., Beig, S., Keightley, M. L., Burian, H., and Black, S. E. (2003). Evidence from functional neuroimaging of a compensatory prefrontal network in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 23, 986–993. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00986.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, A. L., and Rebok, G. W. (2011). Memory training and strategy use in older adults: results from the ACTIVE study. Psychol. Aging 26, 503–517. doi: 10.1037/a0022687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jacobi, F., Allgulander, C., Alonso, J., Beghi, E., et al. (2011). Cost of disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 718–779. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.08.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., and Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 9, 1–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01034.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, H.-F., and Wang, J.-J. (2003). Effect of reminiscence therapy on depression in older adults: A systematic review. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 40, 335–345. doi: 10.1016/S0020-7489(02)00101-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irish, M., Lawlor, B. A., O’Mara, S. M., and Coen, R. F. (2008). Assessment of Behavioural markers of autonoetic consciousness during episodic autobiographical memory retrieval: A preliminary analysis. Behav. Neurol. 19, 3–6. doi: 10.1155/2008/691925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ising, C., Stanley, M., and Holtzman, D. (2015). Current thinking on the mechanistic basis of Alzheimer’s and implications for drug development. Clin. Pharmacol. Therap. 98, 469–471. doi: 10.1002/cpt.200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Parlett-Pelleriti, C. M., Moon, S. M., Evans, M., Kritzmacher, A., et al. (2020). Investigating the effects of spacing on working memory training outcome: A randomized, controlled, multisite trial in older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 75, 1181–1192. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., and Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Mem. Cogn. 42, 464–480. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0364-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagust, W. (2018). Imaging the evolution and pathophysiology of Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 687–700. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0067-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesné, S., Koh, M. T., Kotilinek, L., Kayed, R., Glabe, C. G., Yang, A., et al. (2006). A specific amyloid-β protein assembly in the brain impairs memory. Nature 440, 352–357. doi: 10.1038/nature04533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J.-Q., Tan, L., Wang, H.-F., Tan, M.-S., Tan, L., Xu, W., et al. (2016). Risk factors for predicting progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 87, 476–484. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-310095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, T. S., Afonso, R. M. L. B. M., and Ribeiro, Ó. M. (2016). A quasi-experimental study of a reminiscence program focused on autobiographical memory in institutionalized older adults with cognitive impairment. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 66, 183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2016.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loprinzi, P. D., Day, S., Hendry, R., Hoffman, S., Love, A., Marable, S., et al. (2021). The effects of acute exercise on short-and long-term memory: Considerations for the timing of exercise and phases of memory. Eur. J. Psychol. 17, 85.

Google Scholar

Lustig, C., Shah, P., Seidler, R., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). Aging, training, and the brain: A review and future directions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 504–522. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9119-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonough, I. M., Haber, S., Bischof, G. N., and Park, D. C. (2015). The synapse project: engagement in mentally challenging activities enhances neural efficiency. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 33, 865–882. doi: 10.3233/RNN-150533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netuveli, G., Wiggins, R. D., Hildon, Z., Montgomery, S. M., and Blane, D. (2006). Quality of life at older ages: evidence from the English longitudinal study of aging (wave 1). J. Epidemiol. Community Health 60, 357–363. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.040071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, E., Steinmetz, J. D., Vollset, S. E., Fukutaki, K., Chalek, J., Abd-Allah, F., et al. (2022). Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Public Health 7, e105–e125. doi: 10.1016/s2468-2667(21)00249-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Mara, S. (2019). In Praise of Walking: The New Science of How We Walk and Why It’s Good for Us. London: Random House.

Google Scholar

Olshansky, S. J., Passaro, D. J., Hershow, R. C., Layden, J., Carnes, B. A., Brody, J., et al. (2005). A potential decline in life expectancy in the United States in the 21st century. N. Engl. J. Med. 352, 1138–1145. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsr043743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paramos-de-Carvalho, D., Jacinto, A., and Saúde, L. (2021). The right time for senescence. Elife 10:e72449.

Google Scholar

Parisi, J. M., Stine-Morrow, E. A., Noh, S. R., and Morrow, D. G. (2009). Predispositional engagement, activity engagement, and cognition among older adults. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 16, 485–504. doi: 10.1080/13825580902866653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Gutchess, A. H., Meade, M. L., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2007). Improving cognitive function in older adults: nontraditional approaches. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 62, 45–52. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preschl, B., Maercker, A., and Wagner, B. (2011). The working alliance in a randomized controlled trial comparing online with face-to-face cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. BMC Psychiatry 11, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 177–182. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 65B, 405–415. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 355–370. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzi, L., Rosset, I., and Roriz-Cruz, M. (2014). Global epidemiology of dementia: Alzheimer’s and vascular types. BioMed. Res. Int. 2014:e908915. doi: 10.1155/2014/908915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 13, 140–144. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00293.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, L. M., Hortobagyi, T., la Bastide-van Gemert, S., van der Zee, E. A., and van Heuvelen, M. J. (2019). Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS one 14:e0210036.

Google Scholar

Scarmeas, N., and Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 625–633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B. A., and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000). “Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive aging research,” in The handbook of aging and cognition. eds. F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 155–219.

Google Scholar

Scogin, F., Welsh, D., Hanson, A., Stump, J., and Coates, A. (2005). Evidence-based psychotherapies for depression in older adults. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 12, 222–237. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpi033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepulcre, J., Grothe, M. J., d’Oleire Uquillas, F., Ortiz-Terán, L., Diez, I., Yang, H.-S., et al. (2018). Neurogenetic contributions to amyloid beta and tau spreading in the human cortex. Nat. Med. 24, 1910–1918. doi: 10.1038/s41591-018-0206-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sikkes, S. A. M., Tang, Y., Jutten, R. J., Wesselman, L. M. P., Turkstra, L. S., Brodaty, H., et al. (2021). Toward a theory-based specification of non-pharmacological treatments in aging and dementia: focused reviews and methodological recommendations. Alzheimers Dement. 17, 255–270. doi: 10.1002/alz.12188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., Stern, Y., and Gu, Y. (2021). Modifiable lifestyle factors and cognitive reserve: A systemic review of current evidence. Ageing Res. Rev. 74:101551. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srivastava, S., Ahmad, R., and Khare, S. K. (2021). Alzheimer’s disease and its treatment by different approaches: A review. Eur. J. Med. Chem. 216:113320. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmech.2021.113320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A., Parisi, J. M., Morrow, D. G., Greene, J., and Park, D. C. (2007). An engagement model of cognitive optimization through adulthood. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 62, 62–69. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A., Parisi, J. M., Morrow, D. G., and Park, D. C. (2008). The effects of an engaged lifestyle on cognitive vitality: A field experiment. Psychol. Aging 23, 778–786. doi: 10.1037/a0014341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A., Payne, B. R., Roberts, B. W., Kramer, A. F., Morrow, D. G., Payne, L., et al. (2014). Training versus engagement as paths to cognitive enrichment with aging. Psychol. Aging 29, 891–906. doi: 10.1037/a0038244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strøm, B. S., Engedal, K., Saltyte Benth, J., and Grov, E. K. (2016). Psychometric evaluation of the Holden Communication Scale (HCS) for persons with dementia. BMJ Open 6:e013447. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, H., Soh, K. G., Roslan, S., Wazir, M. R. W. N., and Soh, K. L. (2021). Does mental fatigue affect skilled performance in athletes? A systematic review. PloS one 16:e0258307. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syed Elias, S. M., Petriwskyj, A., Scott, T., and Neville, C. (2019). Spiritual reminiscence therapy for older people with loneliness, anxiety and depression living in a residential aged care facility, Malaysia: A qualitative approach. Australas. J. Ageing 38, E25–E30. doi: 10.1111/ajag.12598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, K., Yamada, Y., Kobayashi, Y., Sonohara, K., Machida, A., Nakai, R., et al. (2007). Improved cognitive function, mood and brain blood flow in single photon emission computed tomography following individual reminiscence therapy in an elderly patient with Alzheimer's disease. Geriatr Gerontol Int 7, 305–309. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2007.00418.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. S., Stubbs, T., Davies, D. A., and Albensi, B. C. (2020). Potential new approaches for diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Front. Neurol. 11:496. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, P. L., Castillo-García, A., Morales, J. S., Izquierdo, M., Serra-Rexach, J. A., Santos-Lozano, A., et al. (2019). Physical Exercise in the Oldest Old. Compr. Physiol. 9, 1281–1304. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c190002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, C. E., Yang, Y. C., Oi, K., Aiello, A., Belsky, D., Harris, K. M., et al. (2022). Age profiles of cognitive decline and dementia in late life in the aging, demographics, and memory study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 77, 1880–1891. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbac038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J.-J. (2007). Group reminiscence therapy for cognitive and affective function of demented elderly in Taiwan. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 22, 1235–1240. doi: 10.1002/gps.1821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Barnes, L. L., and Bennett, D. A. (2003). Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 634–642. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.634.14572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, B., Spector, A. E., Jones, C. A., Orrell, M., and Davies, S. P. (2005). Reminiscence therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 3, 3–11. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001120.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamagami, T., Oosawa, M., Ito, S., and Yamaguchi, H. (2007). Effect of activity reminiscence therapy as brain-activating rehabilitation for elderly people with and without dementia. Psychogeriatrics 7, 69–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8301.2007.00189.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S.-J., Hwu, Y.-J., Wu, P.-I., and Chang, C.-W. (2015). The effects of reminiscence therapy on depression, self-esteem and life satisfaction on institutionalized older adults: a meta-analysis. J. Nurs. Healthcare Res. 11, 33–42. doi: 10.6225/JNHR.11.1.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: memory, aging, dementia, reminiscence therapy, cognitive reserve, lifestyle, non-pharmacological interventions

Citation: Dinius CJ, Pocknell CE, Caffrey MP and Roche RAP (2023) Cognitive interventions for memory and psychological well-being in aging and dementias. Front. Psychol. 14:1070012. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1070012

Received: 14 October 2022; Accepted: 17 January 2023;
Published: 02 February 2023.

Edited by:

Caoimhe Hannigan, National College of Ireland, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Fahad Naveed Ahmad, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada
José Cuenca-Alfaro, Peruvian Institute of Neurosciences (IPN), Peru

Copyright © 2023 Dinius, Pocknell, Caffrey and Roche. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Richard A. P. Roche, ✉ richard.roche@mu.ie

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.