Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 27 February 2023
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic How to Enhance Learning Efficiency When Online Learning is Popular and Indispensable: Theory, Research and Practice View all 22 articles

Students’ persistence intention in MOOCs in the psychomotor domain: An extended 3P model of the teaching and learning perspective

Hsi-Hsun Yang
Hsi-Hsun Yang*Jia-Yu LinJia-Yu Lin
  • Department of Digital Media Design, National Yunlin University of Science and Technology, Douliu, Taiwan

This study proposed and tested a model adapted from Biggs’ 3P model that quantifies the behaviors of students who completed MOOCs (Massive Open Online Courses) in order to design intervention measures for low retention rates. Psychomotor domain data from 300 MOOC learners was analyzed in a covariance-based structural equation model (CB-SEM) to analyze the direct and indirect effects of various factors. Results show the basic psychological needs theory (BPNT) in the presage stage significantly positively correlated with engagement in the process stage. Meanwhile, the process stage exhibited a significantly positive correlation with the product stage, representing persistence intention (PI). Furthermore, a full mediation effect was observed among the presage, process, and product stages. The mediating effect demonstrates that higher student engagement leads to more positive exertion on BNPT and PI to complete the course. Moreover, results show bolstering students’ behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement strengthens their PIs.

1. Introduction

The average dropout rate of students who take MOOCs has been reported as high as 90% (Vitiello et al., 2018; Xing and Du, 2019). Such a low completion rate has raised concerns among course providers, educators, and researchers (Cobos and Ruiz-Garcia, 2020; Dai et al., 2020). While most researchers have focused on learner behaviors, cognitive and intentions (Wong et al., 2019; Lambert, 2020; Li and Zhao, 2021; Tao et al., 2022), regardless of courses type, the current study asserts that MOOCs including psychomotor topics should be discussed independently. The difficulty in implementing courses in the psychomotor domain lies in teachers’ inability to directly supervise learners in person. Therefore, this study addressed this gap by focusing on such courses–account for ~10% of all MOOCs. The online learning modes of such courses have evolved along with technology advances. The psychomotor domain has been explored by a few scholars (Yang and Su, 2017; Tien et al., 2020); however, such studies are not supported by extensive research evidence or widely reported. Although this type of MOOC is in the minority of class designs (Shah, 2021). By better understanding the mechanisms of how students in a psychomotor domain class decide to learn, engage with the course, and persist to completion, improvements can be made to course design and by implication student completion and uptake.

Badali et al. (2022) reviewed attempts to add new constructs based on classic theoretical models, such as the technology acceptance model, self-determination theory, expectation confirmation theory, theory of planned behavior, and unified theory of acceptance and use of technology. Such previous work attempted to identify influencing factors of intentions to keep learning among students, pointing out attention should be paid to phased and persistent behaviors among students when learning. The learning process (e.g., timelines) should be incorporated to better identify elements affecting learning behaviors across cause, process, and result. Taking this into consideration, Biggs’ 3P (presage, process, and product stages) model has been modified into a classic model with fixed elements (de la Fuente et al., 2015; Chan and Yeung, 2020; Deng et al., 2020). Kanashiro et al. (2020) surveyed 46 publications related to the 3P model from 2002 to 2017. The authors highlighted that despite criticism for being outdated and oversimplified, the 3P model appears to be the most prominent in higher education by showing that the teaching context and students’ background (presage factors) affect students’ approach to learning (process stage), which can range from surface learning to deep learning (product stage). The classic model gathers all phenomena and covers all influence factors based on timelines. Such factors are not constant and can adapt according to the actual research environment that contributes to examining the causes of a certain teaching or learning phenomenon (Albelbisi et al., 2018; López-García et al., 2019). However, the 3P theoretical model has not been universally adopted for solving the problem of low MOOC completion rates within the psychomotor domain. Therefore, this research gap needs to be addressed, which we discuss next.

In determining a student’s motivation to enroll in a MOOC, the learner’s internal motivation of pleasure and expectation of personal accomplishment should be first quantified (Li et al., 2013; Rump et al., 2017), which also involves satisfying their basic psychological needs (Sun et al., 2019; Ryan and Deci, 2020). The Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT) posits human behaviors are under the influence of incentives of internal motivation. BPNT highlights three basic psychological needs: perceived autonomy, perceived competence, and perceived relatedness, which are the common needs everyone is born with (Deci and Ryan, 1985; Dincer et al., 2019) and have been widely discussed in various online learning contexts by many researchers (Jeno et al., 2019; Hsu, 2021). Satisfying students’ basic psychological needs is considered a prerequisite of classroom engagement (Reeve, 2012), while engagement is regarded as a prerequisite to learning (Guo et al., 2014), which in turn affects the completion rate of students (Appleton et al., 2008). It is worth mentioning that behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement are dynamically interrelated, rather than isolated processes (Fredricks et al., 2004). The rare combination of the 3P theory, BPNT, and engagement is also worth exploring; hence, the second key point of this study. Based on the above premise, this study employed persistence intention (PI), as specified by Jung and Lee (2018), as an indicator of students’ completion of courses. It is valuable to probe the relationships among basic psychological needs, classroom engagement, and student PI.

In this regard, the current study integrated the learning contexts of MOOCs into Biggs’ theoretical model. As such, the presage stage stands for BPNT, the process stage stands for engagement, and the product stage stands for PI. The current study has several objectives. The first objective is to quantify the factors—as well as their mechanisms—that affect students’ behavior to persist in finishing MOOCs during the entire learning process. The second is to qualify how members recognize the presage stage of psychomotor MOOCs while examining how such recognition may affect persistence intention. Furthermore, this research explored the process stage of MOOCs and examined the effects on the links between the presage stage and persistence intention.

2. Hypothesis development

In Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives (Bloom et al., 1956; Andreatta and Dougherty, 2019), educational activities in the psychomotor domain focus more on fostering students’ psychomotor abilities (Harrow, 1972). Psychomotor ability is the ability to control motor actions proceeding a psychological process (including brain activity). Studies have shown that courses in the psychomotor domain can help students learn to make clothes, perform surgical operations, machine materials, cook, and master other psychomotor activities (Andreatta and Dougherty, 2019; Tien et al., 2020).

Studies using Biggs’s (1993) model of students’ learning approaches suggested three stages—the presage-process-product (3P) mode in non-traditional learning environments. They propose interaction among factors forms a dynamic learning model that influences learning outcomes (Lee and Chan, 2018). These three stages represent pre-, learning in, and post-learning (Biggs and Tang, 2003). The presage stage emphasizes that the teachers are central to the learning process, and building the sustainability and legitimacy of teachers requires a combination of personal beliefs and external verification (Maloni et al., 2012). In addition, on the student side, it highlights the importance of the student’s experience and expectations for learning approaches. Furthermore, it requires constant educational institutional support (Barth, 2013). The process stage describes how teaching and students’ experience are integrated, and teaching effectiveness appears to be a critical factor in this stage (Kanashiro et al., 2020), such as classroom engagement (Pilli and Admiraal, 2017). The final stage – product, generally refers to any outcome related to teaching and learning. The assessment of learning outcomes may be complex but can be considered in quantitative and qualitative methods, such as academic achievement, dropout or completion of schooling (Pilli and Admiraal, 2017; Deng et al., 2020).

Taking a MOOC learning experience as the main factor in the presage stage (BNPT), this study found that the basic psychological needs for perceived autonomy can be satisfied by allowing students to browse course units or develop their own learning plans at will. This way, the students are allowed to be in control when studying in a MOOC context. As a result, a learner’s basic psychological need for perceived competence can be met, and such a student can share their knowledge with or seek help from lecturers and other students, addressing their basic psychological need for perceived relatedness (Lan and Hew, 2018). Once those three psychological needs are met, students will have a strong basis for motives that make them more active in learning behavior (Deci and Ryan, 1985; Ryan and Deci, 2017; Dincer et al., 2019). Taking MOOC learning engagement as the main factor in the process stage, this study consists of three interrelated components: behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement (Reeve and Tseng, 2011). The current study found that through the three categories of engagement, all possible types of engagement could be explained from a more comprehensive perspective (Reeve, 2012). Further, according to Núñez and León (2019), if students’ basic psychological needs are satisfied, they will be more active in taking part in learning activities. As a result, those three needs are considered sufficient conditions for learner engagement behaviors. Moreover, Benlahcene et al. (2020) indicated students’ psychological needs for perceived competence and perceived relatedness could be used to predict engagement in the process stage. Therefore, the current research proposed the following hypothesis:

H1: The presage stage (BPNT) positively affects the process stage (engagement).

This study set MOOC learning performance as the critical factor in the product stage (persistence intention). According to Khalil and Ebner (2014), when a MOOC stimulates positive motivations to learn, the proportion of learners who keep learning increases. Among those motivations, the internal motivation of learning is more effective. Hence, cultivating internal motivation was considered an excellent method to stimulate students to complete their courses (Xiong et al., 2015). The quality of internal motivation is mainly influenced by the positive effects of a learner’s basic psychological needs (Sørebø et al., 2009; Han et al., 2017). Assuming these basic needs are satisfied, students’ internal motivations positively correlate with their intentions to complete a MOOC (Han et al., 2017). Therefore, the interaction of these factors determines learning outcomes–referred to as the product. Thus, this research proposed the following hypothesis:

H2: The presage stage (BPNT) positively affects the product stage (persistence intention).

Classroom engagement has been proven to be an effective predictor of persistent learning (Jung and Lee, 2018; Dincer et al., 2019). Furthermore, Oh and Lee (2016) and Yu et al. (2020a,b) pointed out that the dominant contributory factor is a prerequisite to persistent learning. Similarly, Dincer et al. (2019) found cognitive engagement is negatively correlated with dropout rate, implying higher cognitive engagement leads to lower dropout rates. Therefore, this study proposed the following hypothesis:

H3: The process stage (engagement) positively affects the product stage (persistence intention).

Previous research has generally confirmed that perceived autonomy, competence, and relatedness are at the core of learning motivation through persistence intentions (Sun et al., 2019). When BPNT puts members in a good learning motivation environment, it fosters both their engagement and persistence intention. In other words, there is a relationship among the three basic psychological needs, engagement, and persistence intention, in which engagement serves as the link between the three basic psychological needs and persistence intention. The individual’s basic psychological preferences affect both the member’s engagement and persistence intention (Jung and Lee, 2018). Thus, a learner’s engagement affects their persistence intention (Yu et al., 2020a,b). Therefore, we hypothesized that engagement would play a mediating effect between the three basic psychological needs and persistence intention. In addition, based on the Motivation Theories on Learning, engagement can contribute to a learner’s persistence intention by fostering a long-lasting learning motivation (Lerdpornkulrat et al., 2018). Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H4: The process stage (engagement) mediates the association between the presage stage (BPNT) and the product stage (persistence intention).

3. Materials and methods

3.1. Research framework

The current research proposed that the presage stage (including perceived autonomy, perceived competence, and perceived relatedness) positively impacts the process stage (including behavioral, emotional, and cognitive) and the product stage (including persistence intention). Meanwhile, the process stage mediates the association between the presage stage and the product stage (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research framework.

3.2. Measures

The first part of the questionnaire includes four demographic questions about age, gender, educational background, and the times of logging into the MOOC platform every week. The second part of the questionnaire has 10 questions about perceived autonomy, competence, and relatedness to basic psychological needs. Further, based on Standage et al. (2005), and Reeve and Sickenius (1994), 15 questions on behavioral, emotional, cognitive, and agentic engagement were included (Skinner et al., 2008, 2009; Mameli and Passini, 2019). An additional three questions about persistence intention in completing MOOCs were also included based on Kalz et al. (2015) to measure students’ views on these seven potential variables when taking psychomotor domain courses on the learning platform. The questionnaire was designed using a seven-point Likert formula to measure the interviewees’ degree of consent to each question.

3.3. Subjects and data collection

Deleting invalid responses resulted in a total of 300 valid questionnaires. Participants all attended MOOC courses offered by the TaiwanLIFE and Ewant learning platforms. These courses included one course in food processing (with a sample size of 44), three courses in cooking (with a sample size of 20, 33, and 29, respectively), three courses in basic and intermediate 2D animation (with a sample size of 69, 41, and 66 respectively), and one course in Chinese calligraphy (with a sample size of 4). Common across these courses is their psychomotor component that requires learners to practice while learning by observing and imitating the behavior of teachers and sharing their finished work made with their own hands. The instructional duration of each course ran from 9–13 weeks. In the middle stage of the courses (weeks 4–6), researchers released online questionnaires through the course’s bulletin board and gave participants 2 weeks to complete. Leaners completed the questionnaires voluntarily with no impact on their grades.

The SPSS version 25 software was employed to perform descriptive statistics on the questionnaire demographic section. Results showed 72 (24.0%) participants were male and 228 (76.0%) female. The largest number of participants, 98, were under the age of 20 (32.7%), followed by 77 (25.7%) aged 21–30, and 53 (17.7%) aged 41–50. In terms of educational background, 141 (46.7%) participants graduated or were enrolled in a high school or vocational high school, followed by 82 (27.3%) who graduated or were enrolled in a university or college. Weekly logging into the MOOC showed 146 (48.7%) learners logged in 1–2 times, 79 (26.3%) learners logged in 3–4 times, and 48 (16.0%) logged in 5–6 times.

3.4. Data analysis

The two-step approach of structural equation modeling (SEM) was suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) to estimate the measurement model and structural analysis. Descriptive analysis was used to describe the profile of the participants, and path analysis was then applied to test the relationships among the independent variables of the presage, process, and product stages, including the mediating effects of the process stage. Structural equation modeling using AMOS version 24 was employed to establish the nature of the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. With the use of the maximum likelihood estimation (MLE), a confirmatory factor analysis of the fit was conducted. Next, a relative path analysis was combined to define the model. All statistical analyses were conducted at a significance level of p ≤ 0.05.

4. Empirical analysis and results

4.1. Confirmatory factor analysis

As shown in Table 1, the composite reliability values of the constructs ranged from 0.864 to 0.948, exceeding the 0.7 value recommended by Nunnally and Bernstein (1994) and indicating that all constructs are internally consistent. All average variances extracted (AVE) ranged from 0.674 to 0.845, exceeding the suggested level of 0.5 (Hair et al., 2010) and demonstrating adequate convergent validity.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Reliability and validity analysis results.

For each construct to be discriminately valid, the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) of a given construct must be larger than its correlations with any other constructs (Hair et al., 2010). As shown in Table 1, all bold numbers in the diagonal direction, representing the square roots of AVE of each construct, are larger than the correlations with other constructs, supporting discriminant validity.

4.2. Model fit

When there are more than 200 samples in SEM, the chi-square value (χ2 = (n − 1) FMIN) may be too large. FMIN refers to the minimum value of the difference between the sample matrix and the expectation matrix. When a chi-square value is too large due to an excessive number of samples, the value of p is likely to be considered unacceptable (Hsu and Yen, 2013). Therefore, based on the suggestions of Bollen and Stine (1992), this study adopted the Bootstrap method to revise the chi-square value from 670.844 to 357.336 and re-estimate it. As shown in Table 2, the obtained major indices of model fit are χ2/DF = 1.412 < 3; RMSEA = 0.045 < 0.08; GFI = 0.950 > 0.9; AGFI = 0.934 > 0.9; NFI = 0.950 > 0.9; TLI (NNFI) = 0.978 > 0.9; CFI = 0.980 > 0.9. All indices showed a good level of model fit. A more detailed illustration is presented in the Discussion section.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model fit indicators.

4.3. Analysis

Path analysis results show the presage stage (β = 0.793, t = 9.193) significantly affected perceived value. The process (β = 0.635, t = 6.043) and presage stages (β = 0.145, t = 1.356) significantly affected persistence intention. These findings support H1 and H3. As shown in Table 3 and Figure 2, the presage stage (β = 0.145, t = 1.356) had no significant impact on persistence intention; hence, H3 is not supported. Lastly, the mediating effect (β = 0.56, t = 9.193) among the presage stage, the process stage, and PI was significantly positive. Therefore, H4 is supported.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Path relationship test table.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Path analysis stage of presage, process, and product.

To sum up, owing to the significant indirect effects (H4) and the insignificant direct effects (H3), this type of mediation can be classified as full mediation as maintained by previous scholars (Baron and Kenny, 1986). In other words, the process stage strongly affects the presage stage and persistence intention. Besides, this study also found the values of R2 of the process stage and persistence intention were 0.628 and 0.570, respectively. Meanwhile, according to the recommended standard from Chin (1998), the value of R2 between 0.33 and 0.67 represents a moderate explanatory power. Therefore, the model in this study exhibits moderate explanatory power.

Perceived autonomy is most strongly associated with learners’ basic psychological needs (coefficient of 0.895), followed by perceived competence (0.839) and perceived relatedness (0.697). On the other hand, emotional engagement contributes the most to the learners’ engagement (coefficient of 0.942), followed by cognitive engagement (0.884) and agentic engagement (0.591), as shown in Figure 2.

4.4. Mediation analysis

To examine whether the process stage mediates the effect of the presage stage on MOOCs’ persistence intention, this study conducted a mediation analysis using the bootstrapping method proposed by Preacher and Hayes (2008). Bootstrapping has been widely employed in mediation analysis to determine whether the relationship between an independent and a dependent variable is due to the mediating variables—either fully or partially. The effect of each potential variable is presented in Table 2. First, PRE showed both direct (β = 0.318, p < 0.05) and indirect (β = 0.330, p < 0.05) effects on persistence intention. Thus, the total effect of the presage stage on persistence intention was β = 0.648 (p < 0.05), indicating the significance of the indirect effect of the process stage on the relationship between the presage stage and persistence intention. Second, the direct effect size of the presage stage on persistence intention was 0.452. The presage stage had a direct positive effect on the process stage (β = 0.730, p < 0.05). Third, the process stage had a direct positive effect on persistence intention as well (β = 0.318, p < 0.05).

5. Discussion and conclusions

5.1. Index of model fit

The chi-square value is generally used as an index of the absolute fit between the model and data. One example of a statistic that minimizes the impact of sample size on a better model fit is χ2/DF (Wheaton et al., 1977). While there is no consensus on an acceptable ratio for this statistic, the suggested range is from 5.0 (Wheaton et al., 1977) to 2.0 (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007); 3.0 was selected for this study (Kline, 2005). Meanwhile, the chi-square value of this model was 1.579, suggesting a good fit.

Other indices of the absolute fit index include the goodness-of-fit (GFI), adjusted goodness-of-fit (AGFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The GFI and AGFI describe the proportion of variance accounted for by the estimated population covariance, analogous to R2. The closer the GFI and AGFI value is to 1, the larger the value indicates a better fit; otherwise. Generally, researchers suggest that a GFI and AGFI value >0.9 indicates a good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The GFI and AGFI values of the current model are 0.949 and 0.934, respectively, showing the data match the model well. According to its definition, the RMSEA measures the discrepancy due to the approximation per degree of freedom (Steiger, 1989). MacCallum et al. (1996) used 0.01, 0.05, and 0.08 to indicate excellent, good, and mediocre fit, respectively, while the RMSEA of the current model is 0.044—somewhat below the good fit level of 0.05.

Another index used to assess model fit is the incremental fit index, which compares a chi-square for the model with a null model (also called a baseline model or independence model). There are several incremental fit indices, including the comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), and Tucker-Lewis index [TLI, also called the non-normed fit index (NNFI)]. All of these indices exhibited values ranging between ~0 and 1.0. However, the CFI, NFI, and TLI values were between 0.90 and 0.95, which are now considered marginal—above 0.95 is good and below 0.90 is considered a poor fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The CFI, NFI, and TLI of the current model were 0.981, 0.949, and 0.978, respectively. CFI and TLI were more significant than the traditional standard. Though NFI did not reach the high standard level of 0.95, it met the recommended value of Bentler and Bonett (1980), which should be >0.9. Therefore, NFI also met the acceptable fit threshold. In sum, these results show the model is a fair or reasonable representation of the data.

5.2. Academic implications

This study quantified how psychological needs of MOOC learners, in a psychomotor domain, significantly affected engagement and persistence intention. In other words, addressing those psychological needs in the first encounter between learners and the online course is key to learner success. This finding is especially applicable to courses in the psychomotor domain where students are object-oriented, such as courses focused on skills with a physiological component. When students’ autonomy, competence, and relatedness are encouraged, their internal motivations are stimulated. As a result, they participate in learning activities across behaviors, emotions, and cognition (Lan and Hew, 2018; Núñez and León, 2019).

Noteworthily, the influence of Perceived Autonomy (PA), as shown in Figure 2, is greater than Perceived Competence (PC) and Perceived Relatedness (PR). Thus, if a course intends to encourage students to present PA, such as sharing experiences, asking questions, expressing opinions, and creating study-related topics, instructors should supply positive comments to learners, which encourages the positive cycle of autonomous learning. Figure 2 also shows the influence of Emotional Engagement (EE) is greater than Cognitive Engagement (CE) and Behavioral Engagement (BE), demonstrating that BNPT encourages emotional engagement. Therefore, instructors and online instructional videos should provide a sense of emotional communication, such as occasional synchronous online classes to promote emotional interactions with and among learners (Marulanda, 2022). Our results show such activity would be beneficial in improving learner engagement. Furthermore, engagement has a positive effect on persistence intention. Such results are consistent with the previous findings of Oh and Lee (2016), Dincer et al. (2019) and Yus’ et al. (2020a,b). For example, students’ experiencing negative emotions (i.e., due to repeated failures or learning burnout) should be encouraged by instructors connecting the knowledge they learned with the experience they gained.

According to the mediating effect, BNPT directly influences persistence intention through engagement. Students are thus willing to persist in completing courses when the learning environment satisfies their BNPT and their engagement is encouraged. In other words, the three basic psychological needs of learners should be met first. Doing so encourages participation in learning activities, such as viewing instructional videos, participating in discussions, submitting homework, and completing tests. Such learning behaviors also include their attention and efforts. When students respond to their courses with positive emotions, including enjoyment, affection, fun, flow, and immersion, their emotional engagement improves. Many studies support the role of engagement as a mediator on the relationship between basic psychological needs and continuous learning behaviors (Skinner et al., 2008, 2009; Dincer et al., 2019).

5.3. Practical implications

Engagement centers on experience and interaction, exhibited through interactions between learner and learning environment (Lan and Hew, 2018). Teachers can support the three basic psychological needs and assist students in building their confidence. In terms of perceived autonomy, teachers should try to avoid a domineering teaching style in which students are required to complete learning activities within a short period (1–2 weeks; Alamri et al., 2020; Chiu, 2021). Indeed, it is difficult to manage and control homework submissions in the MOOC design considering the high level of learner autonomy. One of the participating MOOC instructors (teaching the animation courses) participating in this study required homework submissions on schedule with missed deadlines resulting in score deductions (Ihantola et al., 2020; Denny et al., 2021). Yet, when a more flexible practice was adopted, where students could submit their homework before the course completion, the submission rate and the quality of homework was higher.

We have observed three different interactions. First, student-content interaction is mainly based on the performance of students. This interaction involves “behavioral engagement” and “cognitive engagement.” From the “behavioral engagement “in the questionnaire, we know that the highest score is “When I watch the intructional martial video, I will concentrate on listening to the content.” A score of 6.21 out of 7, which can reflect the learners’ attitude toward video views, course access, and reading text satisfaction, especially video watching is the most valued and used resource for MOOC learners (Gregori et al., 2018). In addition, from the “cognitive engagement” in the questionnaire, the highest score is “When I study this course, I will try to connect what I have learned with my experience.” A score of 6.21 out of 7. In general, multimedia can stimulate students’ interest and promote course retention and learning.

Second, student–student interaction is mainly about students seeking help, peers’ feedback, and students share for other resources. These behaviors can be seen in one of the courses we investigated, the 2D animation series courses, where students will share notes and experiences. And some students volunteer to assist teachers in answering questions. There is another cooking course, where students are primarily working people. They have the same background and social experience, so the issues discussed can resonate.

Third, student-lecturer interaction is mainly for teachers to provide emotional support and encouragement to students. The specific method is like teachers posting information in the discussion area or answering students’ questions. This interaction involves “Emotional Engagement.” From the “emotional engagement” in the questionnaire, the highest score is “I like to learn new knowledge in this course.” With a score of 6.25 out of 7. Teachers can release more autonomy to students, helping students succeed in learning, building self-confidence, and creating a positive classroom atmosphere.

The question, “I am close to the teaching assistant of this course,” in the questionnaire received the lowest score of 4.92 (of a maximum 7). Given the basic need for perceived relatedness, this finding implies that two to three live broadcasts can be arranged during the courses to compensate for such a lack of close contact. Synchronous online sessions supply instructors an opportunity to exhibit interest in students’ learning and recognize their performance (Phan et al., 2016; Vollet et al., 2017). More attention should be paid to teacher-student interaction in the MOOC context when learning involves the psychomotor domain (Hew, 2014). Finally, in terms of the basic need for perceived competence supported by teachers, each learning task assigned by teachers should be linked to specific training objectives and purposes. Tasks should also adopt peer assessment accompanied by criteria to achieve learning results (Huisman et al., 2018; Gamage et al., 2021).

When designing courses in the psychomotor domain, teachers can consult with the taxonomies of educational objectives proposed by Dave (1970). Taxonomies provide five levels of practice modes for one type of exercise, which allows students to develop a skill at the levels of imitation, manipulation, precision, articulation, and naturalization (Begam and Tholappan, 2018). For instance, in the cooking courses included in this study, the cooking process was video recorded from multiple angles with the recipe displayed for demonstration. Simultaneously, a group of students were arranged to imitate the process and raise questions, helping learners feel telepresence. Furthermore, students were encouraged to comment on the learning experience, take notes and report other related experiences. In this way, learners interacted with each other, increasing engagement and improving the three basic needs. In particular, a sense of belonging from interaction effectively motivates students in the psychomotor MOOC context.

5.4. Limitations and future directions

This study presents some limitations. First, Biggs’ 3P model addresses how teaching and students’ contextual experiences integrate and interdependency to explain students learning outcomes. In addition to behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement, the process stage can add agentic engagement (Benlahcene et al., 2020; Chiu, 2021) to further explore the differences in the antecedents and consequences of different engagements.

Second, the MOOCs in the psychomotor domain in this study come from the same learning platform, so there may be a need for common method variance. It is suggested that future researchers can cover the multiple MOOCs in psychomotor domain of the platform to analyze the 3P model for persistence intention more comprehensively and accurately.

Third, most of the previous engagement research has focused on classroom observations (Jung and Lee, 2018; Dincer et al., 2019; Benlahcene et al., 2020). This study is one of the few that focuses on engagement in the MOOCs context. In the future, it should be possible to use the teaching mode of SPOCs (Small Private Online Courses) to add engagement issues, so that you can observe both the intention of online courses and the behavior in the classroom.

6. Conclusion

The research model presented in this study was adapted from the 3P theoretical model to clarify the behavioral processes that motivate learners to persist in completing a MOOC. This study also employed the CB-SEM method to analyze and examine relationships among BNTP in the presage stage, engagement in the product stage, and persistence intention in the product stage throughout the learning process.

Furthermore, the current study demonstrated that satisfying students’ basic psychological needs improves their engagement. Results have shown that incentives meeting psychological needs for perceived relatedness improve emotional engagement and initiative to engage. In addition, the results of the examination of the mediating effects showed that students’ engagement influenced BNPT and persistence intention. Finally, this study has proven that measures that bolster students’ behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement can promote their persistence intentions.

In summary, the above findings are conducive for educators, teachers, and course designers, who consider providing courses in the psychomotor domain through MOOC platforms to reassess the importance of students’ BNPT and engagement in class and encouraging persistence intentions. Meanwhile, these findings also provide a significant academic reference for online teaching strategies that improve the completion rates of online courses in the psychomotor domain.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants following the local legislation and institutional requirements. This study was carried out following the APA Code of Ethics recommendations with written informed consent from all subjects. The patient/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

H-HY: conceptualization and writing—review and editing. H-HY and J-YL: formal analysis, investigation, and writing original draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alamri, H., Lowell, V., Watson, W., and Watson, S. L. (2020). Using personalized learning as an instructional approach to motivate learners in online higher education: learner self-determination and intrinsic motivation. J. Res. Technol. Educ. 52, 322–352. doi: 10.1080/15391523.2020.1728449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albelbisi, N., Yusop, F. D., and Salleh, U. K. M. (2018). Mapping the factors influencing success of massive open online courses (MOOC) in higher education. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 14, 2995–3012. doi: 10.29333/ejmste/91486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreatta, P. B., and Dougherty, P. J. (2019). “Supporting the development of psychomotor skills,” in Advancing surgical education theory, evidence and practice. eds. D. Nestel, K. Dalrymple, J. T. Paige, and R. Aggarwal (Springer), 183–196.

Google Scholar

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., and Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with school: critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychol. Sch. 45, 369–386. doi: 10.1002/pits.20303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badali, M., Hatami, J., Banihashem, S. K., Rahimi, E., Noroozi, O., and Eslami, Z. (2022). The role of motivation in MOOCs’ retention rates: a systematic literature review. Res. Pract. Technol. Enhanc. Learn. 17, 1–20. doi: 10.1186/s41039-022-00181-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, M. (2013). Many roads lead to sustainability: a process-oriented analysis of change in higher education. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 14, 160–175. doi: 10.1108/14676371311312879

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begam, A. A. A., and Tholappan, A. (2018). Psychomotor domain of Bloom’s taxonomy in teacher education. Shanlax Int. J. Educ. 6, 11–14. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.1299766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benlahcene, A., Kaur, A., and Awang-Hashim, R. (2020). Basic psychological needs satisfaction and student engagement: the importance of novelty satisfaction. J. Appl. Res. Higher Educ. 13, 1290–1304. doi: 10.1108/JARHE-06-2020-0157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol. Bull. 88, 588–606. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1993). From theory to practice: a cognitive systems approach. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 12, 73–85. doi: 10.1080/0729436930120107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J., and Tang, C. (2003). Teaching for quality learning at university (2nd ed.). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Bloom, B. S., Engelhart, M. D., Furst, E. J., Hill, W. H., and Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). “Taxonomy of educational objectives: the classification of end-terms,” in Handbook I: Cognitive domain. (New York: David McKay).

Google Scholar

Bollen, K. A., and Stine, R. A. (1992). Bootstrapping goodness-of-fit measures in structural equation models. Sociol. Methods Res. 21, 205–229. doi: 10.1177/00491241920210020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, C. K., and Yeung, N. C. J. (2020). Students’ ‘approach to develop’ in holistic competency: an adaption of the 3P model. Educ. Psychol. 40, 622–642. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2019.1648767

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, W. W. (1998). “The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling” in Modern methods for business research. ed. G. A. Marcoulides (Mahwah: Erlbaum), 295–358.

Google Scholar

Chiu, T. K. F. (2021). Digital support for student engagement in blended learning based on self-determination theory. Comput. Hum. Behav. 124:106909. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cobos, R., and Ruiz-Garcia, J. C. (2020). Improving learner engagement in MOOCs using a learning intervention system: a research study in engineering education. Comput. Appl. Eng. Educ. 29, 733–749. doi: 10.1002/cae.22316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, H. M., Teo, T., Rappa, N. A., and Huang, F. (2020). Explaining Chinese university students’ continuance learning intention in the MOOC setting: a modified expectation confirmation model perspective. Comput. Educ. 150:103850. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dave, R. H. (1970). Developing and writing behavioural objectives. Arizona: Educational Innovators Press.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., and Cardelle-Elawar, M. (2015). The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: a structural model. Front. Psychol. 6:399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: self-determination in personality. J. Res. Pers. 19, 109–134. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, R., Benckendorff, P., and Gannaway, D. (2020). Linking learner factors, teaching context, and engagement patterns with MOOC learning outcomes. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 36, 688–708. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denny, P., Whalley, J., and Leinonen, J. (2021). “Promoting early engagement with programming assignments using scheduled automated feedback,” in Australasian computing education conference (ACE). SA, Australia. 88–95.

Google Scholar

Dincer, A., Yeşilyurt, S., Noels, K. A., and Vargas Lascano, D. I. (2019). Self-determination and classroom engagement of EFL learners: a mixed-methods study of the self-system model of motivational development. SAGE Open 9, 215824401985391–215824401985315. doi: 10.1177/2158244019853913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., and Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev. Educ. Res. 74, 59–109. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamage, D., Staubitz, T., and Whiting, M. (2021). Peer assessment in MOOCs: systematic literature review. Distance Educ. 42, 268–289. doi: 10.1080/01587919.2021.1911626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregori, E. B., Zhang, J., Galván-Fernández, C., and Fernández-Navarro, F. d. A. (2018). Learner support in MOOCs: identifying variables linked to completion. Comput. Educ. 122, 153–168. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2018.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, P. J., Kim, J., and Rubin, R., (2014). “How video production affects student engagement: an empirical study of MOOC videos,” in Learning@ scale conference. Atlanta, Georgia, USA. 41–50.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Han, S. Y., Yoo, J., Zo, H., and Ciganek, A. P. (2017). Understanding makerspace continuance: a self-determination perspective. Telematics Inform. 34, 184–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2017.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrow, A. J. (1972) A taxonomy of the psychomotor domain: a guide for developing behavioral objectives, New York: David McKay.

Google Scholar

Hew, K. F. (2014). Promoting engagement in online courses: what strategies can we learn from three highly rated MOOCS. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 47, 320–341. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, L. (2021). Learning tourism and hospitality subjects with massive open online courses (MOOCs): a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. J. Hosp. Leis. Sport Tour. Educ. 29:100276. doi: 10.1016/j.jhlste.2020.100276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, M. C., and Yen, H. P. (2013). The research of behavioral tendencies model of those high school students who spectator the NBA game of Jeremy Lin. Int. J. Lisrel 6, 24–56. (in Chinese)

Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huisman, B., Admiraal, W., Pilli, O., van de Ven, M., and Saab, N. (2018). Peer assessment in MOOCs: the relationship between peer reviewers’ ability and authors’ essay performance. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 49, 101–110. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ihantola, P., Fronza, I., Mikkonen, T., Noponen, M., and Hellas, A. (2020), “Deadlines and MOOCs: how do students behave in MOOCs with and without deadlines,” in 2020 IEEE Frontiers in education conference (FIE). Uppsala, Sweden. 1–9.

Google Scholar

Jeno, L. M., Adachi, P. J., Grytnes, J. A., Vandvik, V., and Deci, E. L. (2019). The effects of m-learning on motivation, achievement and well-being: a self-determination theory approach. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 50, 669–683. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, Y., and Lee, J. (2018). Learning engagement and persistence in massive open online courses (MOOCS). Comput. Educ. 122, 9–22. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2018.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalz, M., Kreijns, K., Walhout, J., Castaño-Munoz, J., Espasa, A., and Tovar, E. (2015). Setting-up a European cross-provider data collection on open online courses. Int. Rev. Res. Open Distrib. Learn. 16, 62–77. doi: 10.19173/irrodl.v16i6.2150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanashiro, P., Iizuka, E. S., Sousa, C., and Dias, S. E. F. (2020). Sustainability in management education: a Biggs’ 3P model application. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 21, 671–684. doi: 10.1108/IJSHE-05-2019-0176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, H., and Ebner, M. (2014). “MOOCs completion rates and possible methods to improve retention-a literature review,” in World conference on educational multimedia, hypermedia and telecommunications. Tampere, Finland. 1236–1244.

Google Scholar

Kline, T. J. B. (2005). Psychological testing: a practical approach to design and evaluation. CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Lambert, S. R. (2020). Do MOOCs contribute to student equity and social inclusion? A systematic review 2014–18. Comput. Educ. 145:103693. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103693

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, M., and Hew, K. F. (2018). “The validation of the MOOC learner engagement and motivation scale,” in EdMedia: World conference on educational media and technology. Amsterdam, Netherlands. 1625–1636.

Google Scholar

Lee, W. W. S., and Chan, C. K. K. (2018). Relationships among epistemic beliefs, perception of learning environment, study approaches and academic performance: a longitudinal exploration with 3P model. Asia Pac. Educ. Res. 27, 267–276. doi: 10.1007/s40299-018-0384-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerdpornkulrat, T., Koul, R., and Poondej, C. (2018). Relationship between perceptions of classroom climate and institutional goal structures and student motivation, engagement and intention to persist in college. J. Furth. High. Educ. 42, 102–115. doi: 10.1080/0309877X.2016.1206855

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Hsieh, P. A., and Rai, A. (2013). Motivational differences across post acceptance information system usage behaviors: an investigation in the business intelligence systems context. Inf. Syst. Res. 24, 659–682. doi: 10.1287/isre.1120.0456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., and Zhao, M. (2021). A study on the influencing factors of continued intention to use MOOCs: UTAUT model and CCC moderating effect. Front. Psychol. 12:528259. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.528259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-García, A., Miralles-Martínez, P., and Maquilón, J. (2019). Design, application and effectiveness of an innovative augmented reality teaching proposal through 3P model. Appl. Sci. 9:5426. doi: 10.3390/app9245426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol. Methods 1, 130–149. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloni, M. J., Smith, S. D., and Napshin, S. (2012). A methodology for building faculty support for the United Nations principles for responsible management education. J. Manag. Educ. 36, 312–336. doi: 10.1177/1052562911430310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mameli, C., and Passini, S. (2019). Development and validation of an enlarged version of the student agentic engagement scale. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 37, 450–463. doi: 10.1177/07342829187578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marulanda, J. F. F. (2022). Student satisfaction pilot experience with synchronous classroom live streaming styles during the COVID-19 pandemic. IEEE Revista Iberoamericana de Tecnologias del Aprendizaje 17, 301–306. doi: 10.1109/RITA.2022.3191285

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Núñez, J. L., and León, J. (2019). Determinants of classroom engagement: a prospective test based on self-determination theory. Teachers Teach. 25, 147–159. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2018.1542297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C., and Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.), Mcgraw-Hill: New York.

Google Scholar

Oh, Y., and Lee, S. M. (2016). The effects of online interactions on the relationship between learning-related anxiety and intention to persist among e-learning students with visual impairment. Int. Rev. Res. Open Distrib. Learn. 17, 89–107. doi: 10.19173/irrodl.v17i6.2581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, T., McNeil, S. G., and Robin, B. R. (2016). Students’ patterns of engagement and course performance in a massive open online course. Comput. Educ. 95, 36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2015.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilli, O., and Admiraal, W. F. (2017). Students' learning outcomes in massive open online courses (MOOCs): some suggestions for course design. J. High. Educ. 7, 46–71. Available at: https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/pub/yuksekogretim/issue/41141/497309

Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J. (2012). “A self-determination theory perspective on student engagement” in Handbook of research on student engagement. eds. L. S. Christenson, A. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York, NY: Springer), 149–172.

Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Sickenius, B. (1994). Development and validation of a brief measure of the three psychological needs underlying intrinsic motivation: the Afs scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 54, 506–515. doi: 10.1177/001316449405400202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Tseng, C.-M. (2011). Agency as a fourth aspect of students’ engagement during learning activities. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rump, M., Esdar, W., and Wild, E. (2017). Individual differences in the effects of academic motivation on higher education students’ intention to drop out. Eur. J. Higher Educ. 7, 341–355. doi: 10.1080/21568235.2017.1357481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61:101860. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, D. (2021). By the numbers: MOOCs in 2021, Class central. Available at: https://www.classcentral.com/report/mooc-stats-2021/

Google Scholar

Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., and Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: part of a larger motivational dynamic? J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 765–781. doi: 10.1037/a0012840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., and Furrer, C. J. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: conceptualization and assessment of children’s behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 69, 493–525. doi: 10.1177/0013164408323233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sørebø, Ø., Halvari, H., Gulli, V. F., and Kristiansen, R. (2009). The role of self-determination theory in explaining teachers’ motivation to continue to use e-learning technology. Comput. Educ. 53, 1177–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2009.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standage, M., Duda, J. L., and Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A test of self-determination theory in school physical education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 75, 411–433. doi: 10.1348/000709904X22359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiger, J. H. (1989). EzPATH: a supplementary module for SYSTAT and SYGRAPH. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT.

Google Scholar

Sun, Y., Ni, L., Zhao, Y., Shen, X. L., and Wang, N. (2019). Understanding students’ engagement in MOOCs: an integration of self-determination theory and theory of relationship quality. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 50, 3156–3174. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Tao, D., Fu, P., Wang, Y., Zhang, T., and Qu, X. (2022). Key characteristics in designing massive open online courses (MOOCs) for user acceptance: an application of the extended technology acceptance model. Interact. Learn. Environ. 30, 882–895. doi: 10.1080/10494820.2019.1695214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tien, L. C., Lin, S. Y., Yin, H., and Chang, J. C. (2020). The impact of a flipped classroom on the creativity of students in a cake decorating art Club. Front. Psychol. 11:533187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.533187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitiello, M., Walk, S., Helic, D., Chang, V., and Guetl, C. (2018). User behavioral patterns and early dropouts detection: improved users profiling through analysis of successive offering of MOOC. J. Univ. Comput. Sci. 24, 1131–1150.

Google Scholar

Vollet, J. W., Kindermann, T. A., and Skinner, E. A. (2017). In peer matters, teachers matter: peer group influences on students’ engagement depend on teacher involvement. J. Educ. Psychol. 109, 635–652. doi: 10.1037/edu0000172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, B., Muthen, B., Alwin, D. F., and Summers, G. (1977). Assessing reliability and sability in panel models. Sociol. Methodol. 8, 84–136. doi: 10.2307/270754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, J., Baars, M., Davis, D., Van Der Zee, T., Houben, G. J., and Paas, F. (2019). Supporting self-regulated learning in online learning environments and MOOCs: a systematic review. Int. J. Hum.–Comput. Interact. 35, 356–373. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2018.1543084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, W., and Du, D. (2019). Dropout prediction in MOOCs: using deep learning for personalized intervention. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 57, 547–570. doi: 10.1177/0735633118757015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, Y., Li, H., Kornhaber, M. L., Suen, H. K., Pursel, B., and Goins, D. D. (2015). Examining the relations among student motivation, engagement, and retention in a MOOC: a structural equation modeling approach. Global Educ. Rev. 2, 23–33.

Google Scholar

Yang, H. H., and Su, C. H. (2017). Learner behaviour in a MOOC practice-oriented course: in empirical study integrating TAM and TPB. Int. Rev. Res. Open Distrib. Learn. 18, 35–63. doi: 10.19173/irrodl.v18i5.2991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J., Huang, C., Han, Z., He, T., and Li, M. (2020a). Investigating the influence of interaction on learning persistence in online settings: moderation or mediation of academic emotions? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2320. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J., Huang, C., Wang, X., and Tu, Y. (2020b). “Exploring the relationships among interaction, emotional engagement and learning persistence in online learning environments,” International symposium on educational technology. Bangkok, Thailand. 293–297.

Google Scholar

Keywords: psychomotor domain, MOOCs, 3P model of teaching and learning, structural equation modeling, persistence intention

Citation: Yang H-H and Lin J-Y (2023) Students’ persistence intention in MOOCs in the psychomotor domain: An extended 3P model of the teaching and learning perspective. Front. Psychol. 14:1094138. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1094138

Received: 09 November 2022; Accepted: 02 February 2023;
Published: 27 February 2023.

Edited by:

Chang Liu, The University of Texas at Austin, United States

Reviewed by:

Hariharan N. Krishnasamy, Universiti Utara Malaysia, Malaysia
Shih-Chih Chen, National Kaohsiung University of Science and Technology, Taiwan

Copyright © 2023 Yang and Lin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hsi-Hsun Yang, ✉ jimmy@yuntech.edu.tw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.