Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 01 February 2023
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars In: Evolutionary Psychology 2022 View all 5 articles

Smaller prize, bigger size? Exploring the impact of money on men’s self-reported markers of masculinity

  • 1Department of Economics, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 2Department of Management, University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway

Bodily markers, often self-reported, are frequently used in research to predict a variety of outcomes. The present study examined whether men, at the aggregate level, would overestimate certain bodily markers linked to masculinity, and if so, to what extent. Furthermore, the study explored whether the amount of monetary rewards distributed to male participants would influence the obtained data quality. Men from two participant pools were asked to self-report a series of bodily measures. All self-report measures except weight were consistently found to be above the population mean (height and penis size) or the scale midpoint (athleticism). Additionally, the participant pool that received the lower (vs. higher) monetary reward showed a particularly powerful deviation from the population mean in penis size and were significantly more likely to report their erect and flaccid penis size to be larger than the claimed but not verified world record of 34 cm. These findings indicate that studies relying on men’s self-reported measures of certain body parts should be interpreted with great caution, but that higher monetary rewards seem to improve data quality slightly for such measures.

“Men read maps better than women because only men can understand the concept of an inch equaling a hundred miles.” – Roseanne Barr

1. Introduction

Men’s physical attributes, such as their height, body type, and penis size, have been shown to predict a wide array of phenomena, ranging from body satisfaction, self-view, and feelings of masculinity (Grogan and Richards, 2002; Hall, 2006; Lever et al., 2006) to mating success, income levels, and consumption preferences (Judge and Cable, 2004; Puts, 2010; Otterbring et al., 2020; Richardson et al., 2023). In fact, New Guinean Yupno men even refer to the penis for number 33 in the unique Yupno body count system (Wassmann and Dasen, 1994; Kramer, 2022).

While no clear consensus seems to exist regarding the relationship between men’s penis size and women’s preferences (Štulhofer, 2006; Mautz et al., 2013; Prause et al., 2015), or their sexual satisfaction (Eisenman, 2001), it is arguably fairly well-established that this particular factor constitutes a conspicuous marker of masculinity (Lehman, 1998; Lever et al., 2006; Ostberg, 2010). Indeed, although Freud’s (1925) penis envy concept was meant to psychoanalytically proclaim a female envy for the male reproductive organ, some studies have found symbolic support for penis envy even in men (Hall and Van de Castle, 1965; Melnick, 1997; Gottlieb, 2004; Domhoff, 2013), with many men being concerned about their penis size, supporting the notion that size seems to matter (Lever et al., 2006; Johnston et al., 2014). For example, one study on more than 50,000 heterosexual men and women found that only 55% of men were satisfied with their penis size, although 85% of women expressed satisfaction with their partner’s penis size (Lever et al., 2006). Nevertheless, a more recent study found that 20% of women reported having ended a relationship partly because their partner’s penis size was too small for their personal preferences (Prause et al., 2015). Moreover, Lever et al. (2006) found that almost 9 of 10 men self-reported their penis as either average or large, while only 12% of men reported it to be small. Despite these estimates, 45% of men still wanted their penis to be larger, while only 0.2% preferred a smaller penis.

Beyond penis size, height as well as other bodily markers that signal physical dominance have been shown to be important for men’s reproductive (Pawlowski et al., 2000; Nettle, 2002), occupational (Judge and Cable, 2004; Case and Paxson, 2008), and financial success (Deaton and Arora, 2009; Tyrrell et al., 2016). Accordingly, many men exaggerate their height and athleticism on online dating sites to boost their chances on the mating market (Ellison et al., 2006, 2012; Toma et al., 2008; Toma and Hancock, 2010; Burke and Carman, 2017). One plausible reason for men’s desire to be tall, and for women’s preference for men with an imposing stature and other formidability features, is the link between male stature and status (Jackson and Ervin, 1992; Buss, 2016; Otterbring et al., 2018), which appears to be more than metaphorical, considering that we tend to “look up to” tall individuals, as evidenced from their many benefits in life (Schubert, 2005; Stulp et al., 2013). In other words, just as the “what is beautiful is good”-stereotype (Dion et al., 1972) postulates that physically attractive individuals are evaluated far more favorably even on traits and characteristics that have nothing to do with their looks, people also hold a “height halo,” in which tall people are portrayed and perceived more positively as a function of their “altitude advantage.” Similarly, sometimes weight signals importance (Jostmann et al., 2009), suggesting that men may over-report their weight to signal masculinity, power, and potency (Roberts, 1995; Ambwani and Chmielewski, 2013; Devia et al., 2021).

The results delineated above indicate that men may exaggerate their penis size, height, and presumably other factors linked to their physique in self-report situations, partly because men and, to some extent, women seem to equate bigger with better when it comes to male markers of masculinity (Frederick and Haselton, 2007; Mautz et al., 2013; Johnston et al., 2014). Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine whether men at the aggregate level would overestimate their height, weight, athleticism, and penis size in a self-report study, and if so, to what extent. Admittedly, people show a general propensity to present themselves in an ego-boosting way, as self-serving tendencies captured by, for example, the better-than-average effect, the positivity bias, and the optimism bias help defending, maintaining, and enhancing a favorable self-view (Mezulis et al., 2004; Paulhus, 2017; Otterbring and Mitkidis, 2018; Zell et al., 2020). Notwithstanding the almost universal human tendency to exaggerate things in a self-serving way, few studies have compared the magnitude of such exaggerations in the same study across a series of related but still distinct measures. In that sense, the current work contributes to the literature not so much regarding if participants would exaggerate certain male markers of masculinity but rather the magnitude of this exaggeration.

As a second main objective, the study sought to explore whether the amount of monetary rewards could reduce the extent of such potentially exaggerated responses. The latter aim is relevant in light of evidence linking increased monetary compensation to improved data quality under certain circumstances (Litman et al., 2015; Balasubramanian et al., 2017). Given that a growing body of research relies on self-reported online surveys, it is important to understand which implications such data collection techniques have for scholars’ ability to draw reasonable conclusions.

Together, the present research contributes to the literature in two crucial ways. First, our findings reveal that self-report data related to men’s bodily markers of masculinity cannot necessarily be trusted and, as such, should be interpreted with great caution, as men generally exaggerate their size on such measures. Second, however, these exaggerated responses seem to be at least partially contingent on the amount of monetary compensation given to participants, and may, therefore, be meaningfully mitigated by larger (vs. smaller) payments.

2. Methods

The data were collected in connection to another project, which included a large number of measures (approximately 200 items) on consumption preferences, life history traits (e.g., birth order, number of siblings, parental status, number of offspring desired), and subjective life expectancy. Participants with response times quicker than 10 min or incomplete data (n = 167) were excluded prior to analysis because a pretest on the survey items revealed that it took at least 10 min to read the instructions and reply to all items (the study was advertised as taking approximately 30 min to complete). Based on these criteria, the study included 224 Danish men (Mage = 24.95 years, SD = 3.51), with the data collected during the spring semester of 2018 (until early May).

Participants came from two distinct pools (nlow reward = 143; nhigh reward = 81), in which the first pool received a smaller monetary reward for participating (approximately US$5), whereas the second one received a larger monetary reward (approximately US$22). These two pools of participants did not differ in terms of age or several other demographic measures and, as such, can be assumed to represent two relatively matched groups.

The study was approved by the local ethical review board (Project ID: 0220). Participants from the high monetary reward group were sent an email 1 week before the study, with a consent form and information about which kinds of measurements they would be asked to reply to in the study. Three days later, they got a new email stressing the importance of reading the consent form. Subsequently, on the day of the study, participants received an email with a link to Qualtrics. Upon entering the page, participants read and signed the consent form. Next, they were informed that, in order to complete the study, they needed a ruler or a printer to print the ruler that was attached in the email. Participants from the low monetary reward group were sent directly to the consent form and the information there, as these participants were recruited and monitored through a professional marketing survey agency.

All participants provided written informed consent and the study was conducted in accordance with the World Medical Association Code of Ethics (Declaration of Helsinki) for human experimentation. All participants were paid regardless of whether they provided complete data in all tasks, could withdraw from the study at any time without negative consequences, and had the right to contact the principal investigator to seek more information about the study (no participants did). Moreover, as the study requested participants to measure their erect and flaccid penis size, the consent form emphasized that the penis measurements could cause psychological stress, such as discomfort in the measurement situation and worries about not getting erection. Participants were also ensured that the data were collected anonymously, and that their unique replies in no way could be identified in the reporting of the results.

For the measures related to the present article, participants replied to a set of items linked to bodily markers. Specifically, they indicated their athleticism on a scale ranging from 1 (not athletic) to 9 (very athletic); their height in centimeters, their weight in kilograms; and, crucially, their penis size in flaccid and erect states (in centimeters and followed by careful instructions as how the measurements should be done both in the flaccid and erect state). To be able to reliably take all these measurements, participants were sent a ruler and a guide of how to print it out. The ruler was sent in PDF format (scale 1:1) to make sure no alterations could be made with respect to its format. Further, it was specifically mentioned as part of the instructions that the ruler had to be printed in A4 format. Note that the measures were collected through different response formats, which is a fruitful way to mitigate problems associated with common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2003; Gasiorowska et al., 2022).

3. Results

3.1. Outliers and attrition check

As conservative outlier criteria for height and penis size, we excluded all men who either reported being shorter than the threshold of 147 cm for dwarfism (n = 2; cf. Pritchard, 2021) or who claimed to be taller than the Guinness World Record of 272 cm (n = 1; Silk, 2006) and men who reported that their erect or flaccid penis size was either 0 cm (n = 1) or larger than the claimed world record of 34 cm (Kimmel et al., 2014; Kim, 2016; Zane, 2021) in the erect (n = 12) or flaccid (n = 14) state. In the most extreme case, the self-reported erect penis size (9,000 cm) was 50 times larger than the penis size of an adult elephant, which has the largest penis of any land animal (Giustina, 2005). In total, this resulted in the exclusion of 21 participants’ data, as some of the described cases were multiple outliers. Our data did not include any extreme values on weight when the outliers for penis size and height had been excluded. After excluding 12 additional outliers who had the survey active for an unrealistically long time before sending it in (at least 4 h and at most 5 days), the average survey completion time was approximately 22 min (M = 21.79 min, SD = 16.57).1 Based on these criteria, our final sample comprised 191 participants (Mage = 25.09 years, SD = 3.61), with 121 participants in the low monetary reward group and 70 participants in the high monetary reward group. Attrition due to our exclusion criteria of response time and the above-stated physical attributes was not associated with the amount of payment that participants received, as evidenced by a 2 (monetary reward: low, high) × 2 (participant excluded: now, yes) Chi-square analysis [χ2(1, N = 224) = 0.13, p = 0.71, Cramér’s V = 0.02].

3.2. Main analyses

Similar to other common paradigms, such as coin-flip tasks or die-roll tasks that are frequently used in moral psychology (Gerlach et al., 2019), our main analyses focus on participants’ deviation from a theoretical mean or the scale midpoint, meaning that exaggerations cannot necessarily be detected individually but rather at the group level. As there is no available Danish mean for flaccid penis size, we only present data pertaining to erect penis size in relation to our first study objective. However, when comparing the participant pools to address our second main objective, we report data on flaccid penis size.

Irrespective of athleticism, height, and penis size, participants’ self-reported measures deviated from the available Danish population mean or the scale midpoint on all these physical attributes, albeit only marginally for athleticism. Indeed, participants reported being marginally more athletic than the scale midpoint of 5 (M = 5.21, SD = 1.66; t(190) = 1.79, p = 0.075) and their self-reported height was significantly above than the Danish mean of 180.4 cm (Statistical Yearbook, 2017) for men of similar ages [M = 182.19 cm, SD = 7.66; t(190) = 3.22, p = 0.002].2 This represents a height deviance of approximately 1% compared to the Danish mean and supports earlier investigations in which men have been shown to exaggerate their height (Toma et al., 2008; Bogaert and McCreary, 2011; Pozzebon et al., 2012).

However, the most extreme deviance from the Danish mean (as reported at WordData.info, n.d.) was found for erect penis size, in which participants’ self-reported size (M = 18.02 cm, SD = 3.72) was 21.1% larger than the stated Danish mean of 14.88 cm [t(190) = 11.67, p < 0.001].3 Fisher’s r-to-z transformation revealed that the effect size for the plausible over-reporting in erect penis size relative to the stated Danish population mean (r = 0.645, N = 191) is significantly greater than the meta-analytic effect size for the positivity bias in attributions (r = 0.433, N = 41,538; z = 4.15, p < 0.001), implying that men, on average, exaggerated the size of their erect penis relatively more than people’s general propensity to portray themselves in a self-serving way (Mezulis et al., 2004).

Consistent with former investigations comparing Danes’ self-reported and measured weight (Neermark et al., 2019), we found no significant difference between participants’ self-reported weight and the available Danish mean of 81.9 kg (M = 81.64 kg, SD = 15.64; t(190) = −0.23, p = 0.82), as reported by the State Institute of Public Health (2018). As shown in Table 1, zero-order correlations revealed that erect, but not flaccid, penis size was significantly positively correlated with all other physical attributes, consistent with several previous studies (Siminoski and Bain, 1993; Ponchietti et al., 2001; Lever et al., 2006; Veale et al., 2015).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Zero-order correlations between erect and flaccid penis size, athleticism, weight, and height.

Thus, it can be concluded that participants likely exaggerated their level of athleticism, presumably as a self-view-bolstering tactic, and that their height and penis size estimates, but not weight, may have been higher than their true scores on these variables, as participants consistently scored significantly above the Danish mean on both height and penis size; especially (and dramatically) so for penis size. Further, if participants exaggerated their height but not their weight, they effectively portrayed themselves as more physically fit.

With respect to the second aim of exploring whether the amount of monetary rewards influenced men’s response pattern on these self-view-relevant attributes, we found no significant difference between the participant pools in athleticism, height, and weight (Fs < 1; see Table 2). However, in terms of penis size, there was a statistically significant difference between the participant pools [F(1, 189) = 7.01 p = 0.009, η2 = 0.04], in which the group receiving the smaller monetary reward self-reported a greater erect penis size than the group receiving the larger monetary reward (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). The same pattern of results was found for flaccid penis size [F(1, 189) = 5.39, p = 0.021, η2 = 0.03], with the group receiving the smaller monetary reward again self-reporting a greater penis size than the group receiving the larger monetary reward (see Table 2). Interestingly, participants in the low payment group (M = 19.59 min, SD = 17.03) had a shorter average response time than participants in the high payment group [M = 25.60 min, SD = 15.11; F(1, 189) = 5.99, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.03]. However, the group-penis size link was not mediated by participants’ response time.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means (and standard deviations) across participant pools for athleticism, height, weight, and erect and flaccid penis size.

3.3. Supplementary analyses

A Pearson’s Chi-square analysis on the entire final sample, including the outliers (N = 224), revealed that participants receiving the smaller monetary reward were significantly more likely (7.7%) to report that their erect penis size was larger than the claimed, but not confirmed, world record of 34 cm (Kimmel et al., 2014; Kim, 2016; Zane, 2021) compared to participants receiving the larger monetary reward (1.2%; χ2(1, N = 224) = 4.25, p = 0.039, Cramér’s V = 0.14]. Similarly, participants receiving the smaller monetary reward were significantly more likely (9.1%) to report that their flaccid penis size was larger than the claimed world record compared to participants receiving the larger monetary reward (1.2%; χ2(1, N = 224) = 5.45, p = 0.020, Cramér’s V = 0.16]. We found the Chi-square analyses informative because a self-reported penis size above 34 cm is arguably easier to categorize as a “true lie”—at the individual level—compared to our main analyses, which only compare the sample mean with the population mean—at the aggregate level.

4. Discussion

The present study shows that men seem to self-report their physical attributes in a self-view-bolstering way, although not for weight, consistent with earlier findings (Neermark et al., 2019). Specifically, at the aggregate level, men reported being marginally more athletic compared to the scale midpoint, claimed to be significantly taller compared to the Danish mean for individuals of similar ages, and stated that their erect penis size was several centimeters longer than the available Danish population mean. The finding that participants do not seem to have over-reported their weight but likely exaggerated their height slightly also implies that they sought to present themselves as more physically fit. Together, these results indicate that, when interested in bodily variables important to men’s self-view and identity, such variables should not be done through self-report; especially not if they concern private bodily measures linked to masculinity (i.e., penis size). Indeed, men deviated substantially more in their reporting of private (vs. publicly visible) body measures, as the overall sample mean in erect penis size was at least 21.1% above the Danish population mean, while only 1% above the Danish mean in height among men of similar ages and roughly equal to the population mean in weight.

Interestingly, giving participants a higher (vs. lower) monetary reward reduced the average self-reported estimate of both erect and flaccid penis size, but had no impact on the more publicly visible measures. To underscore the point that participants in the low monetary reward group provided less accurate self-report estimates, we further found participants in this group to be significantly more likely to report that their erect and flaccid penis size was larger than the claimed world record of 34 cm (Kimmel et al., 2014; Kim, 2016; Zane, 2021). However, the means of erect penis size were still significantly above the available Danish population mean for both the low and high payment groups. As such, even with the higher monetary reward, our results regarding private self-report data do not appear to be trustworthy.

While our results indicate that men may have exaggerated their penis size and, to a lesser extent, their height and athleticism in a self-view-bolstering way, it is important to note that extreme values based on self-report can be the result not only of deliberate exaggerations but also of measurement error. We find a measurement error account unlikely to be the main driver of our results for several reasons. First, regarding penis size, the deviation of more than 20% (upward) from the stated Danish population mean is too extreme to realistically have occurred simply due to measurement error, and a measurement error account should arguably stipulate both under- and over-reporting, which is not congruent with the current results. Second, self-reported penis size has previously been found to correlate positively with social desirability scores (King et al., 2019), suggesting that some men deliberately exaggerate their penis size. Still, our study would have been strengthened by asking participants to also measure other body parts with the ruler that are not commonly connected to masculinity (e.g., their forearms). Such instructions would have allowed us to more explicitly test whether, as we believe, men strategically exaggerate only those bodily cues that are linked to masculinity or, alternatively, whether they over-report all bodily measures, irrespective of their “macho” meaning. It is possible that men, on average, are more inclined to lie about their penis size than their height, weight, or athleticism, considering that the penis is typically concealed and hence easier to lie about without getting caught in everyday interactions, whereas people cannot easily hide their height, weight, and body shape.

In conclusion, our results suggest that private data related to bodily cues of masculinity can only be reliably collected in the lab, where conditions can be fully controlled. Given our findings, scientific studies with self-report data concerning penis size should be interpreted with great caution. However, one remedy to reduce exaggerated response patterns seems to be higher monetary rewards given to participants. Indeed, one study found monetary incentives to be the top priority for online panel participants, and further revealed that data quality can be positively related to monetary compensation (Litman et al., 2015), supporting our argument that increased payments may be important for accessing high-quality data on the private (penis) measures investigated herein. It is possible that participants who received the larger monetary payment, on average, were less inclined to exaggerate the size of their penis because they felt a stronger need to reply (more) honestly. In contrast, those who received the smaller monetary payment may have been more motivated to exaggerate their penis size due to anger for the low payment coupled with the activation of self-threat when receiving questions about male markers of masculinity. Indeed, self-threat has been shown to magnify the self-serving bias (Campbell and Sedikides, 1999) and participants receiving the low monetary reward might have been more prone to engage in (extreme) protest responses—as our Chi-square analyses indicate—due to psychological reactance following the low payment (MacKenzie and Podsakoff, 2012).

Future research could examine, for instance, whether oath scripts or the implementation of interactive survey techniques, with direct feedback to participants when their responses exceed certain probability thresholds, may reduce exaggerated response patterns in studies with self-report measures (Kemper et al., 2020). Before such studies are conducted, the most telling take-away message based on the current results—regarding the aggregate “believability” in men’s self-reported penis size—is perhaps best captured by a quote from the New York Times bestselling author Darynda Jones: “Never trust a man with a penis.”

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the local ethical review board (Project ID: 0220). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JC methodology, investigation, data curation, formal analysis, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. TO conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, and supervision. C-JL writing—review and editing and supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^The nature and significance of the results do not change by including these 12 cases in the main analyses.

2. ^As the population mean regarding height is based on the Danish defense recruitment, with most male conscripts being 17 to 20 years old when their height is measured (Ministry of Defence, 2022), we used the Danish height data from 2012, as conscripts aged 17-20 in 2012 should have a mean age close to our sample mean of 25 years in early 2018 when our study was conducted. Using the population mean of 180.82 cm from the first half of 2018 (Ministry of Defence, 2022) does not change the nature or significance of our results.

3. ^We cannot attest to the validity of the penis size metrics provided on WorldData.info; however, it is reasonable to assume that, if anything, the country-specific means of penis size available on this website should be an over-estimation of the average penis size for men in the stated countries. For example, whereas the Danish mean is 14.88 cm on this site, studies in urology with the penis size measurements taken by medical professionals have found Caucasian men to have an average erect penis size of around 12.9–13.6 cm (Wessells et al., 1996; Veale et al., 2015; King, 2020). This means that our comparison between the average penis size of men in the current study and the Danish average, as available on WorldData.info, should be a conservative rather than liberal test of our theorizing. In fact, our study mean of 18.02 cm for erect penis size is even greater than most former self-report studies on men’s erect penis size, where the mean has been around 15.6–16.8 cm (e.g., Jamison and Gebhard, 1988; Richters et al., 1995; Smith et al., 1998; Bogaert and Hershberger, 1999; King et al., 2019).

References

Ambwani, S., and Chmielewski, J. F. (2013). Weighing the evidence: social desirability, eating disorder symptomatology, and accuracy of self-reported body weight among men and women. Sex Roles 68, 474–483. doi: 10.1007/s11199-012-0244-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramanian, P., Bennett, V. M., and Pierce, L. (2017). The wages of dishonesty: the supply of cheating under high-powered incentives. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 137, 428–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2017.03.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogaert, A. F., and Hershberger, S. (1999). The relation between sexual orientation and penile size. Arch. Sex. Behav. 28, 213–221. doi: 10.1023/A:1018780108597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogaert, A. F., and McCreary, D. R. (2011). Masculinity and the distortion of self-reported height in men. Sex Roles 65, 548–556. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0003-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, M. A., and Carman, K. G. (2017). You can be too thin (but not too tall): social desirability bias in self-reports of weight and height. Econ. Human Biology 27, 198–222. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2017.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (2016). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic books.

Google Scholar

Campbell, W. K., and Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 3, 23–43. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Case, A., and Paxson, C. (2008). Stature and status: height, ability, and labor market outcomes. J. Polit. Econ. 116, 499–532. doi: 10.1086/589524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deaton, A., and Arora, R. (2009). Life at the top: the benefits of height. Econ. Human Biology 7, 133–136. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devia, C., Flórez, K. R., Costa, S. A., and Huang, T. T. K. (2021). Prevalence of self-reported obesity among diverse Latino adult populations in new York City, 2013–2017. Obes. Sci. Pract. 7, 379–391. doi: 10.1002/osp4.490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dion, K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domhoff, G. W. (2013). Finding meaning in dreams: A quantitative approach. Germany: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Eisenman, R. (2001). Penis size: survey of female perceptions of sexual satisfaction. BMC Womens Health 1:1. doi: 10.1186/1472-6874-1-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison, N. B., Hancock, J. T., and Toma, C. L. (2012). Profile as promise: A framework for conceptualizing veracity in online dating self-presentations. New Media Soc. 14, 45–62. doi: 10.1177/1461444811410395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison, N., Heino, R., and Gibbs, J. (2006). Managing impressions online: self-presentation processes in the online dating environment. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 11, 415–441. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00020.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., and Haselton, M. G. (2007). Why is muscularity sexy? Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 1167–1183. doi: 10.1177/0146167207303022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1925). “Some psychical consequences of the anatomical distinction between the sexes” in The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud. ed. J. Strachey, vol. 19 (London: Hogarth Press), 248–258.

Google Scholar

Gasiorowska, A., Folwarczny, M., and Otterbring, T. (2022). Anxious and status signaling: examining the link between attachment style and status consumption and the mediating role of materialistic values. Personal. Individ. Differ. 190:111503. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K., and Hertwig, R. (2019). The truth about lies: A meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychol. Bull. 145, 1–44. doi: 10.1037/bul0000174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giustina, A. (2005). Sex world records, 152.

Google Scholar

Gottlieb, R. M. (2004). Refusing the cure: Sophocles’s Philoctetes and the clinical problems of self-injurious spite, shame and forgiveness. Int. J. Psychoanal. 85, 669–690. doi: 10.1516/A750-YQQL-NB4C-LLGC

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grogan, S., and Richards, H. (2002). Body image: focus groups with boys and men. Men Masculinities 4, 219–232. doi: 10.1177/1097184X02004003001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, S. S. (2006). Size matters: How height affects the health, happiness, and success of boys--and the men they become. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Google Scholar

Hall, C., and Van de Castle, R. L. (1965). An empirical investigation of the castration complex in dreams. J. Pers. 33, 20–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1965.tb01368.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, L. A., and Ervin, K. S. (1992). Height stereotypes of women and men: the liabilities of shortness for both sexes. J. Soc. Psychol. 132, 433–445. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1992.9924723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamison, P. L., and Gebhard, P. H. (1988). Penis size increase between flaccid and erect states: an analysis of the Kinsey data. J. Sex Res. 24, 177–183. doi: 10.1080/00224498809551408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, L., McLellan, T., and McKinlay, A. (2014). (perceived) size really does matter: male dissatisfaction with penis size. Psychol. Men Masculinity 15, 225–228. doi: 10.1037/a0033264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D., and Schubert, T. W. (2009). Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1169–1174. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02426.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, T. A., and Cable, D. M. (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace success and income: preliminary test of a theoretical model. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 428–441. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemper, N. P., Popp, J. S., and Nayga, R. M. (2020). A query theory account of a discrete choice experiment under oath. Eur. Rev. Agric. Econ. 47, 1133–1172. doi: 10.1093/erae/jbz026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, W. W. (2016). “History and cultural perspective” in Penile augmentation, eds. Park, N., Kim, S., and Moon, D. (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 11–25. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-46753-4_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmel, M., Milrod, C., and Kennedy, A. (Eds.). (2014). Cultural encyclopedia of the penis. United States: Rowman & Littlefield.

Google Scholar

King, B. M. (2020). Average-size erect penis: fiction, fact, and the need for counseling. J. Sex Marital Ther. 47, 80–89. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2020.1787279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, B. M., Duncan, L. M., Clinkenbeard, K. M., Rutland, M. B., and Ryan, K. M. (2019). Social desirability and young men’s self-reports of penis size. J. Sex Marital Ther. 45, 452–455. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2018.1533905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, P. (2022). Iconic mathematics: math designed to suit the mind. Front. Psychol. 13:362. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.890362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehman, P. (1998). In an imperfect world, men with small penises are unforgiven: the representation of the penis/phallus in American films of the 1990s. Men Masculinities 1, 123–137. doi: 10.1177/1097184X98001002001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lever, J., Frederick, D. A., and Peplau, L. A. (2006). Does size matter? Men’s and women’s views on penis size across the lifespan. Psychol. Men Masculinity 7, 129–143. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.7.3.129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litman, L., Robinson, J., and Rosenzweig, C. (2015). The relationship between motivation, monetary compensation, and data quality among US-and India-based workers on mechanical Turk. Behav. Res. Methods 47, 519–528. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0483-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKenzie, S. B., and Podsakoff, P. M. (2012). Common method bias in marketing: causes, mechanisms, and procedural remedies. J. Retail. 88, 542–555. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2012.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mautz, B. S., Wong, B. B., Peters, R. A., and Jennions, M. D. (2013). Penis size interacts with body shape and height to influence male attractiveness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 6925–6930. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219361110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnick, B. A. (1997). Metaphor and the theory of libidinal development. Int. J. Psychoanal. 78, 997–1015.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., and Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychol. Bull. 130, 711–747. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministry of Defence. (2022). Statistical information: Outcomes, average height, and BMI (body mass index). Available at: https://www.forpers.dk/globalassets/fps/dokumenter/2022/-statistik-forsvarets-dag-2021-2022-.pdf (Accessed October 21, 2022).

Google Scholar

Neermark, S., Holst, C., Bisgaard, T., Bay-Nielsen, M., Becker, U., and Tolstrup, J. S. (2019). Validation and calibration of self-reported height and weight in the Danish health examination survey. Eur. J. Pub. Health 29, 291–296. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cky187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nettle, D. (2002). Height and reproductive success in a cohort of British men. Hum. Nat. 13, 473–491. doi: 10.1007/s12110-002-1004-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostberg, J. (2010). Thou shalt sport a banana in thy pocket: gendered body size ideals in advertising and popular culture. Mark. Theory 10, 45–73. doi: 10.1177/1470593109355255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otterbring, T., and Mitkidis, P. (2018). Commentary: folk-economic beliefs: an evolutionary cognitive model. Front. Psychol. 9:1120. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otterbring, T., Ringler, C., Sirianni, N. J., and Gustafsson, A. (2018). The Abercrombie & Fitch effect: the impact of physical dominance on male customers’ status-signaling consumption. J. Mark. Res. 55, 69–79. doi: 10.1509/jmr.15.0247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otterbring, T., Sundie, J., Li, Y. J., and Hill, S. (2020). Evolutionary psychological consumer research: bold, bright, but better with behavior. J. Bus. Res. 120, 473–484. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulhus, D. L. (2017). Socially desirable responding on self-reports. Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences, 1–5. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1349-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pawlowski, B., Dunbar, R. I., and Lipowicz, A. (2000). Evolutionary fitness: tall men have more reproductive success. Nature 403:156. doi: 10.1038/35003107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponchietti, R., Mondaini, N., Bonafè, M., Di Loro, F., Biscioni, S., and Masieri, L. (2001). Penile length and circumference: a study on 3,300 young Italian males. Eur. Urol. 39, 183–186. doi: 10.1159/000052434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozzebon, J. A., Visser, B. A., and Bogaert, A. F. (2012). Do you think you’re sexy, tall, and thin? The prediction of self-rated attractiveness, height, and weight. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 42, 2671–2700. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00957.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prause, N., Park, J., Leung, S., and Miller, G. (2015). Women’s preferences for penis size: A new research method using selection among 3D models. PLoS One 10:e0133079. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pritchard, E. (2021). Incongruous encounters: the problem of accessing accessible spaces for people with dwarfism. Disability & Society 36, 541–560. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2020.1755236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 157–175. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, D. C., Devlin, J., Hogan, J. S., and Thompson, C. (2023). Small penises and fast cars: Evidence for a psychological link. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/uy7ph

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richters, J., Gerofi, J., and Donovan, B. (1995). Are condoms the right size (s)? A method for self-measurement of the. Venereology 8, 77–81.

Google Scholar

Roberts, R. J. (1995). Can self-reported data accurately describe the prevalence of overweight? Public Health 109, 275–284. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3506(95)80205-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 1–21. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

State Institute of Public Health, Denmark. (2018). Anthropometry (weight, height, BMI, fat percentage, and waist-to-hip ratio). Available at: https://www.sdu.dk/da/sif/forskning/projekter/kram/resultater (Accessed October 21, 2022).

Google Scholar

Statistical Yearbook. (2017). Conscription result with conscripts’ BMI and height, Denmark. Available at: https://www.dst.dk/pukora/epub/upload/22259/headword/dk/29.pdf (Accessed October 21, 2022).

Google Scholar

Silk, J. (2006). Our place in the multiverse. Nature 443, 145–146. doi: 10.1038/443145a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siminoski, K., and Bain, J. (1993). The relationships among height, penile length, and foot size. Annals of Sex Res. 6, 231–235. doi: 10.1177/107906329300600305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. M., Jolley, D., Hocking, J., Benton, K., and Gerofi, J. (1998). Does penis size influence condom slippage and breakage? Int. J. STD AIDS 9, 444–447. doi: 10.1258/0956462981922593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Štulhofer, A. (2006). How (un) important is penis size for women with heterosexual experience? Arch. Sex. Behav. 35, 5–6. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-8989-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stulp, G., Buunk, A. P., Verhulst, S., and Pollet, T. V. (2013). Tall claims? Sense and nonsense about the importance of height of US presidents. Leadersh. Q. 24, 159–171. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toma, C. L., and Hancock, J. T. (2010). Looks and lies: the role of physical attractiveness in online dating self-presentation and deception. Commun. Res. 37, 335–351. doi: 10.1177/0093650209356437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toma, C. L., Hancock, J. T., and Ellison, N. B. (2008). Separating fact from fiction: an examination of deceptive self-presentation in online dating profiles. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1023–1036. doi: 10.1177/0146167208318067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyrrell, J., Jones, S. E., Beaumont, R., Astley, C. M., Lovell, R., Yaghootkar, H., et al. (2016). Height, body mass index, and socioeconomic status: mendelian randomisation study in UK biobank. BMJ 352:582. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veale, D., Miles, S., Bramley, S., Muir, G., and Hodsoll, J. (2015). Am I normal? A systematic review and construction of nomograms for flaccid and erect penis length and circumference in up to 15 521 men. BJU Int. 115, 978–986. doi: 10.1111/bju.13010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassmann, J., and Dasen, P. R. (1994). Yupno number system and counting. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 25, 78–94. doi: 10.1177/0022022194251005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessells, H., Lue, T. F., and McAninch, J. W. (1996). Penile length in the flaccid and erect states: guidelines for penile augmentation. J. Urol. 156, 995–997. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5347(01)65682-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WordData.info. (n.d.). Average penis size by country: Worldwise comparison. Accessed at: https://www.worlddata.info/average-penissize.php (Accessed November 22, 2022).

Google Scholar

Zane, Z. (2021). Jonah Falcon, the man with the world’s largest penis, answers 20 questions about the world’s largest penis. Men’s Health, July 29, 2021. Accessed at: https://www.menshealth.com/sex-women/a37135564/jonah-falcon-biggest-penis-interview/ (Accessed November 22, 2022).

Google Scholar

Zell, E., Strickhouser, J. E., Sedikides, C., and Alicke, M. D. (2020). The better-than-average effect in comparative self-evaluation: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146, 118–149. doi: 10.1037/bul0000218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: penis size, monetary rewards, self-report, data quality, masculinity, bodily cues, online surveys

Citation: Christensen JD, Otterbring T and Lagerkvist C-J (2023) Smaller prize, bigger size? Exploring the impact of money on men’s self-reported markers of masculinity. Front. Psychol. 14:1105423. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1105423

Received: 22 November 2022; Accepted: 11 January 2023;
Published: 01 February 2023.

Edited by:

Peter Karl Jonason, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Francis McAndrew, Knox College, United States
Rebecca Burch, State University of New York at Oswego, United States

Copyright © 2023 Christensen, Otterbring and Lagerkvist. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jacob Dalgaard Christensen, ✉ jacob.dalgaard.christensen@slu.se; Tobias Otterbring, ✉ tobias.otterbring@uia.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.