Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 22 March 2023
Sec. Psychopathology

Obsessive-compulsive symptoms and dissociative experiences: Suggested underlying mechanisms and implications for science and practice

  • The Consciousness and Psychopathology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be’er Sheva, Israel

A strong and specific link between obsessive-compulsive disorder or symptoms (OCD/S) and a tendency for dissociative experiences (e.g., depersonalization-derealization, absorption and imaginative involvement) cannot be explained by trauma and is poorly understood. The present theoretical formulation proposes five different models conceptualizing the relationship. According to Model 1, dissociative experiences result from OCD/S through inward-focused attention and repetition. According to Model 2, dissociative absorption causally brings about both OCD/S and associated cognitive risk factors, such as thought-action fusion, partly through impoverished sense of agency. The remaining models highlight common underlying causal mechanisms: temporo-parietal abnormalities impairing embodiment and sensory integration (Model 3); sleep alterations causing sleepiness and dreamlike thought or mixed sleep-wake states (Model 4); and a hyperactive, intrusive imagery system with a tendency for pictorial thinking (Model 5). The latter model relates to Maladaptive Daydreaming, a suggested dissociative syndrome with strong ties to the obsessive-compulsive spectrum. These five models point to potential directions for future research, as these theoretical accounts may aid the two fields in interacting with each other, to the benefit of both. Finally, several dissociation-informed paths for further developing clinical intervention in OCD are identified.

1. Introduction

In recent years evidence has accumulated to suggest a strong link between obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) or obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) on one hand (henceforth OCD/S), and a tendency for dissociative experiences on the other hand, including episodes of depersonalization and derealization (sensations of detachment or estrangement from one’s self or the environment, respectively; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), and “absorption and imaginative involvement” (AII; Carlson and Putnam, 1993), (i.e., a tendency for experiencing immersive attention toward an internal or external stimulus, accompanied by obliviousness to surroundings). Dissociative experiences seem to be a transdiagnostic construct relevant to several psychiatric disorders (Soffer-Dudek, 2014; Ellickson-Larew et al., 2020; Buchnik-Daniely et al., 2021; Lynn et al., 2022). It has been suggested that dissociative experiences may interact in specific ways with various disorders, and those specific interactive paths should be studied to advance the understanding of both dissociation and the respective disorders (Soffer-Dudek, 2014). Nevertheless, as dissociative experiences or symptoms have been mostly related to trauma and trauma-related disorders, the dissociation-OCD/S connection is not intuitive for many researchers and clinicians, most of whom naturally belong to only one of the respective fields (either trauma-dissociation or OCD/S). More importantly, the mechanisms governing this association are poorly understood. The present work is a theoretical formulation, aimed to outline suggested underlying mechanisms, and identify pertinent areas that warrant exploration through future empirical research and may be a focus for personalized interventions for high-dissociation individuals with OCD/S. Although most OCD/S researchers see dissociative episodes as an effect of OCD/S, the current formulation suggests that certain dissociative experiences may act as a cause as well; specifically, the present work will try to establish that AII may contribute to OCS as well as to the emergence of established cognitive risk factors for OCD.

The sections below will: (1) briefly define common dissociative experiences and discuss their etiology; (2) describe their strong, specific relationship with OCD/S; (3) suggest five different causal models with specific mechanisms that may underlie that relationship; and finally (4) remark on identified areas for future study and possible interventions. Better insight into the processes that generate the link between OCD/S and dissociative processes will advance clinical psychological science within both respective fields, and may also augment existing psychotherapies for OCD, which have proved to be less successful among clients reporting dissociative experiences (Rufer et al., 2006a; Prasko et al., 2009; Semiz et al., 2014).

2. Common dissociative experiences

Dissociation is defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM, 5th ed.) as “the splitting off of clusters of mental contents from conscious awareness” (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013, p. 820). Dissociative disorders, accordingly, “…are characterized by a disruption of and/or discontinuity in the normal integration of consciousness, memory, identity, emotion, perception, body representation, motor control, and behavior” (p. 291). Alongside extreme or rare forms of dissociation (e.g., dissociative identity disorder; DID, in which one experiences oneself as having multiple identities), several types of experiences that are widely considered to be dissociative are quite common. For example, experiences of depersonalization and/or derealization (DEP/DER), are characterized by feeling detached or estranged from oneself, either one’s own mental processes, body, or actions (e.g., individuals may feel as if they were located outside of their body), or from the world (e.g., objects, actions, or events may be perceived as “dreamlike” or as moving in slow-motion). The prevalence of DEP/DER disorder is estimated at 1–2%, but transient DEP/DER sensations are very common, with lifetime estimations ranging between 26 and 74% (Hunter et al., 2004). A recent, integrative theoretical account of dissociative experiences maintains that whereas early, severe, or complex traumatic stress (e.g., childhood physical or sexual abuse) may result in extreme dissociative disorders (DID, dissociative amnesia), mild-to-moderate stress may result in mild-to-moderate dissociative experiences, such as DEP/DER (Buchnik-Daniely et al., 2021; Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). Although DEP/DER may represent symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) or appear as a transient experience during trauma (a phenomenon labeled peritraumatic dissociation, e.g., Marmar et al., 1996), it is also characteristic of panic attacks or overwhelming anxiety that is not necessarily trauma-driven (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; Soffer-Dudek, 2014). Moreover, DEP/DER experiences increased longitudinally following excessive rumination over a span of several months, in a sample of individuals with depression and anxiety but no PTSD, mixed with a community sample (Vannikov-Lugassi et al., 2021). In daily diary studies spanning non-clinical and clinical samples, state DEP/DER symptoms were predicted by daily stress and anxiety (Stiglmayr et al., 2008; Soffer-Dudek, 2017a). DEP/DER experiences also tend to increase following full or partial sleep deprivation (Giesbrecht et al., 2007; Selvi et al., 2015; van Heugten-van der Kloet et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2017) and to improve in accordance with the improvement of sleep (van der Kloet et al., 2012a). Finally, as a stand-alone disorder, DEP/DER disorder is not especially related to childhood trauma (e.g., Michal et al., 2016). To conclude, etiological factors for DEP/DER experiences are broad, ranging from traumatic to non-traumatic stress and from psychological to physiological effects.

An even more common dissociative experience is an empirically derived subscale of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES; Carlson and Putnam, 1993), labeled AII, considered by many as a personality tendency representing mild or “non-pathological” dissociation (Butler, 2006). AII represents the disintegration of consciousness in everyday behaviors and contexts; it refers to a tendency to be totally immersed in a stimulus, which often will be one’s imagination or daydreaming, which is especially immersive and vivid (Bregman-Hai et al., 2018), but may also be external such as a movie or a book. It is the narrowing of one’s attention to a fragment of experience, to the point where one is oblivious to surroundings (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015), at times failing to notice substantial occurrences, or registering them unconsciously (resulting in dissociative amnesia, a compartmentalization or inaccessibility of autobiographical memories). AII could be argued to represent an inclination for often experiencing moments where a person functions in a less integrative way, with a decreased sense of an overarching observer self (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). Normative dissociation may be manifested, for example, in realizing you are unsure of what you had said to someone, as your mind was “someplace else” during the conversation. Another common dissociative experience is “highway hypnosis,” characterized by driving somewhere yet suddenly realizing that you have no memory for parts of the trip (Williams, 1963). Interestingly, researchers have shown that driving while listening to unrelated auditory content relies on the parallel activation of two separate neural systems, supporting the notion that in certain conditions of normal daily life, our mental functioning is indeed divided (Sasai et al., 2016). Absorbers tend to act on “auto-pilot”; their awareness is focused elsewhere while they are performing routine tasks. This may lead to mistakes (e.g., putting the flour in the freezer instead of in the pantry) or to miscommunication (e.g., answering someone absent-mindedly). Automaticity is a complex concept with many definitions (Moors and De Houwer, 2006); a full review of automaticity is outside the scope of the current work. However, automatic processes are commonly seen as self-regulated actions that run to completion without conscious guidance (Bargh, 1989, 1992, 1997), conscious monitoring (Tzelgov, 1999), or meta-consciousness (Schooler, 2002). The disintegration between awareness or conscious guidance, and performing complex behavior, exemplifies how AII is the common or normative manifestation of the “splitting” of awareness occurring in clinical-level dissociation (Bregman-Hai et al., 2020). Indeed, Butler (2006) has suggested that pathological and “non-pathological” dissociation “…both involve a telescoping of the attentional field to concentrate on a narrow range of experience and the concomitant exclusion of other material (internal or external) from awareness and, to some degree, from accessibility” (p. 45). In line with the idea that AII is characterized by mental disintegration, healthy high AII individuals show decreased neural connectivity (lower EEG coherence) at resting state compared to low AII individuals; The observed decreased coherence in relation to high AII was in the long (frontal-occipital) range, and in the central-parietal short-range pair, both mostly in the left hemisphere (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2019).1 This supports the notion of a stable and meaningful personality trait. Indeed, despite the commonness of such experiences, some individuals tend to have them more than others. Below, it will be established that not only are individual differences in AII relevant to psychopathology, but also, that they are relevant specifically to OCD/S. But before, two important caveats on AII and common dissociation in general should be noted.

First, a closely related construct is the similarly labeled trait of “absorption” as measured by the Tellegen absorption scale (TAS; Tellegen and Atkinson, 1974). However, the constructs are not identical: Tellegen’s absorption involves both a narrowing and an expansion of attention (Tellegen, 1982),2 whereas AII refers mainly to the narrowing of the attentional span, which relegates the neglected—or dissociated—domains to the periphery of consciousness (Putnam, 1997; Leavitt, 2001). Tellegen’s absorption includes the inclination to experience mystical or enlightening experiences and a sense of elation or oneness with nature, all of which are not included in AII. Indeed, AII and the TAS are only moderately to strongly associated, but do not overlap (r = 0.42–0.52; Frischholz et al., 1991; Smyser and Baron, 1993; Levin and Spei, 2004). Moreover, in a factor analysis on N = 841 they were virtually discrete; AII was labeled “attentional dissociation” (Carleton et al., 2010). Finally, AII, but not the TAS, involves impairments specific to one’s sense of self and agency (Bregman-Hai et al., 2020), constructs that will be further discussed below.

Second, it is important to mention that structural dissociation theorists do not consider AII or DEP/DER to represent dissociation (e.g., van der Hart et al., 2004). These experiences are viewed by them as alterations in attention and awareness that should not be labeled “dissociative,” as they do not necessarily stem from trauma and do not represent a structural dissociation of the personality into independently functioning agentic parts. Indeed, AII and DEP/DER do not necessarily stem from trauma, and accordingly, have been suggested as transdiagnostic factors relevant for understanding several psychopathologies, including OCD/S (Soffer-Dudek, 2014); however, this does not automatically imply that they should not be considered as mild forms of dissociation. The definition of dissociation does not include etiology but rather describes specific symptoms or phenomena. The discussion of whether they should be labeled dissociative or not is outside the scope of the current work. In line with most of the literature on dissociation, including the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), The present work will refer to these processes as dissociative. Specifically, DEP/DER disorder appears in the dissociative disorders chapter in the DSM-5 and DEP/DER symptoms have been used in the DSM-5 to define the dissociative subtype of PTSD (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Thus, the current paper will adhere to this terminology. Regarding AII, it is the subscale carrying most of the variance of the DES (Stockdale et al., 2002; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015), which is the most widely used measure in the literature aimed to assess dissociation. AII is also very high in the dissociative disorders, including DEP/DER disorder as well as DID (Ross et al., 1995; Leavitt, 1999, 2001; Simeon et al., 2009) and it is associated with a similar neural correlate of decreased coherence as severe dissociation (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2019). Theoretically, an extensive account on why AII is rightfully considered to represent dissociation has recently been published, referring to its properties of decreased agency, incoherent sense of self, increased automaticity, and temporarily compartmentalized behavior and thought, all of which render it a common and mild manifestation of multiplicity, that may represent a diathesis for the development of more extreme dissociative symptoms (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). The reader is referred to this source for the discussion of whether AII should be considered dissociative.

3. The association of dissociative experiences with obsessive-compulsive symptoms

Although AII is usually referred to as “non-pathological dissociation,” several studies have demonstrated its linear association with psychopathology in general (Leavitt, 2001; Levin and Spei, 2004; Armour et al., 2014; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015; Humpston et al., 2016), and specifically, with OCS (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek, 2017a,2019; Tapancı et al., 2018; Boysan et al., 2022). More generally, dissociative experiences, as measured with the DES as well as with other scales, have been associated with OCD/S in several studies (for a review see Soffer-Dudek, 2014, although additional studies have been published since). Specifically, clinical-sample studies showed that the presence or severity of several OCS in adults or adolescents diagnosed with OCD were associated with dissociative symptoms (Goff et al., 1992; Grabe et al., 1999; Merckelbach and Wessel, 2000; Lochner et al., 2004; Rufer et al., 2006b; Fontenelle et al., 2007; Belli et al., 2012; Tatlı et al., 2018; Meral et al., 2021). In one study, out of 78 patients diagnosed with OCD, 11 (14.1%) were also diagnosed with a comorbid dissociative disorder, and the general mean score of dissociation symptoms in the sample was close to the clinical cutoff score for suspected dissociative disorders (Belli et al., 2012). Similarly, clinical OCD patients are higher in dissociation scales than healthy controls (e.g., Merckelbach and Wessel, 2000; Tapancı et al., 2018; Tatlı et al., 2018). For example, in one study (Tatlı et al., 2018), controls had a mean DES score of 4.87, whereas OCD patients scored a significantly higher 20.58. In another, it was similarly 8.89 vs. 19.36, also a statistically significant difference (Tapancı et al., 2018). Finally, non-clinical or mixed sample studies repeatedly show that OCS and dissociation are associated (Norton et al., 1990; Watson et al., 2004; Zermatten et al., 2006; Aardema and Wu, 2011; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek, 2017a,2019; Vannikov-Lugassi and Soffer-Dudek, 2018a; Ellickson-Larew et al., 2020). Moreover, several studies have demonstrated the specificity of the dissociation-OCD/S link, as it could not be accounted for by general neuroticism, psychopathological comorbidities, depression levels, psychotropic medication, attention deficit symptoms, or mind-wandering (Watson et al., 2004; Rufer et al., 2006a; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek, 2019). There has even been a measure of “obsessional dissociation” developed, merging the two constructs into a single theoretical dimension (Boysan et al., 2018; Yıldırım and Boysan, 2019).

Obsessive-compulsive disorder is characterized by intrusive, unwanted, repeated, and uncontrollable thoughts, feelings, impulses, or images, and/or repetitive behaviors, usually aimed at reducing the associated anxiety. Some OCD/S researchers have discussed or shown that OCD/S may bring about dissociative and absorptive states due to immersion in obsessions or engaging total attention in compulsive rituals. In other words, they have viewed dissociation as an outcome of OCD/S. For example, O’Connor and Aardema (2012) have described how during obsessional thinking, individuals experience the world as if “in a bubble,” in an immersive and dissociative consciousness state. Possibly, the narrowing of attention, which is an inherent element in obsessions and in many compulsions (that need to be performed “just right” or memorized) alters the sense of reality and induces DEP/DER. Indeed, in a very relevant analog study, van den Hout et al. (2009) showed how compulsive-like staring brought about DEP/DER in a non-clinical sample. Specifically, they demonstrated how even relatively short intervals (e.g., 30 s) of concentrated staring at a gas stove (participants were asked not to avert their gaze or blink) paradoxically decreased certainty about perception and increased dissociative experiences. This result joined a series of seminal works by van den Hout et al. (2009) showing that compulsions were not only a result of obsessions/anxiety in OCD but also a driving force or causal factor contributing to the maintenance of the inefficient cycle of uncertainty and checking in the disorder. In this respect, dissociation or the disintegration of experience is viewed as an outcome of compulsive acts.

If indeed dissociation is merely an outcome of OCD/S, it should not necessarily be of significant interest to OCD/S clinicians and researchers. Importantly, however, whereas DEP/DER is indeed likely to represent a consequence or by-product of OCD/S, AII may represent a risk factor or augmenting factor in generating or maintaining OCD/S (Soffer-Dudek, 2014, 2019). This proposition is based on the idea that individuals with the tendency to become absorbed, “spaced out,” or dissociated may experience anxiety and confusion following such episodes, which may propel them to perform checking or act out other alleged safety measures. In other words, the narrow attentional spotlight and dissociated/automatic behavior characterizing high AII individuals are not continuously present; occasionally, their awareness returns to an integrated state (Butler, 2006), whereupon they may wonder what just transpired, even regarding their own actions. This uncertainty may cause anxiety, obsessing, and checking in an attempt to substantiate reality (Soffer-Dudek, 2014, 2019). Others have also recently noted that dissociation may be the factor that turns “normal” obsessions into abnormal ones, i.e., into OCD (Boysan et al., 2018), and that dissociative amnesia or lack of confidence in one’s own actions may enhance obsessional doubts (Pozza and Dèttore, 2019). Accordingly, AII demonstrates specificity in predicting OCS, in several ways: (1) AII was the only dissociative factor that predicted a longitudinal increase in OCS (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015); (2) the relationship of AII and OCS cannot be explained by the association of OCS with attention-deficit symptoms or with mind-wandering (Soffer-Dudek, 2019); and finally, and (3) in a daily diary study, with constructs measured as daily states, time-lag analysis indicated that both AII and DEP/DER were elevated on days in which OCS were elevated; however, only AII proved to also be elevated on the previous day (Soffer-Dudek, 2017a). It is important to note that such a design cannot establish initial causality/etiology, but it can shed light on present-day dynamics. To conclude, OCD/S, DEP/DER, and AII may enhance each other reciprocally, meaning that it is important to better understand the role of dissociation in OCD/S.

This importance is also exemplified in several studies showing that high baseline levels of dissociation in individuals with OCD predict poorer treatment response, or in other words, more treatment-resistant OCD (Rufer et al., 2006a; Prasko et al., 2009; Semiz et al., 2014). In one of these studies, an exploration of subscales revealed that the effect stemmed from AII; Specifically, individuals scoring high on AII at baseline were less successful in their cognitive-behavioral therapy for OCD and were more inclined to drop out, even after controlling for psychopathology and medication use (Rufer et al., 2006a). It has been suggested that the reason that highly dissociative individuals with anxiety disorders are less responsive to treatment is because perhaps they detach from negative emotions that arise during exposure therapy sessions, or because of poor attachment following childhood trauma, which hampers the development of a therapeutic alliance (Spitzer et al., 2007; Semiz et al., 2014). Moreover, traumatic-dissociative flashbacks have been suggested to be indistinguishable at times from obsessional phenomena (Lipinski and Pope, 1994; Semiz et al., 2014). However, detachment following negative emotions could be said about any other disorder as well, and thus does not explain the specificity demonstrated for the OCS-dissociation relationship. In addition, it does not explain the specific relation with AII. As for trauma and flashbacks, although the relation of traumatic stress with dissociation is well-documented, the correlation effect size in most samples is medium (r = 0.32 in a meta-analysis; Dalenberg et al., 2012), indicating that the proportion of explained variance is only slightly over 10%, pointing to the need to better understand additional sources of variance in accounting for dissociation. This claim is augmented by the fact that whereas several studies found trauma to be related to OCD/S (Mathews et al., 2008; Lafleur et al., 2011; Berman et al., 2013; Khosravani et al., 2017; Miller and Brock, 2017; Ay and Erbay, 2018), several others did not (Fontenelle et al., 2007; Grabe et al., 2008; Selvi et al., 2012; Visser et al., 2014; Ivarsson et al., 2016). Moreover, in a study exploring both dissociation and trauma as potential predictors of treatment non-response in OCD, only the former was a significant predictor (Semiz et al., 2014). In another study, trauma could not explain the effect of dissociation on OCD (Tatlı et al., 2018). Additional explaining factors that have been mentioned regarding dissociation predicting treatment non-response in OCD are high levels of affect during exposure, inhibiting reality testing, regulation of emotional arousal, and habituation (Rufer et al., 2006a; Semiz et al., 2014). It has also been mentioned that the presence of dissociation could be a marker of more severe obsessive–compulsive symptoms (Goff et al., 1992; Semiz et al., 2014). Yet, most of these mechanisms and explanations are non-specific and vague, leaving a gap in our theoretical understanding of dissociation in OCD.

Additional mechanisms must be responsible for the OCD/S-dissociation link, and the idea presented in this paper is that they are specific to the nature of dissociation as an interruption to an integrated sense of self. Indeed, it is being increasingly acknowledged in the OCD literature that one element characterizing OCD is an incoherent, unstable, or ambivalent sense of self (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Bhar and Kyrios, 2007; Godwin et al., 2020; Aardema et al., 2021; Llorens-Aguilar et al., 2022). For example, a young adult male with OCD stated to the author of this paper that most of the time, he feels as if he is lying, even though he does not intend to lie, thus describing his continuous subjective experience of unfamiliarity and detachment from who he is and his bafflement as to his own identity. This distance from the spontaneous sense of knowing who you are, what you want, or what you mean, is closely related to dissociation and specifically depersonalization experiences, and may be a core feature of OCD/S, as it is closely associated with doubt and uncertainty. Below, specific constructs that may be at play in the OCD/S-dissociation relationship will be highlighted, each denoting a different causal pattern between the respective constructs. Table 1 presents a summary of the specific mechanisms and models suggested in this paper.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of theoretical models presented in this paper, each with a specific suggested mechanism worthy of further study, and a suggested direction of causality between dissociative experiences and obsessive-compulsive symptoms.

3.1. Model 1: The inward-focused attention to detachment model

AII and DEP/DER, two factors of the DES, are consistently strongly related (e.g., Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015). Moreover, when viewing these factors as states, experience-sampling studies show that AII or daydreaming are accompanied by dissociative detachment (Cardeña and Marcusson-Clavertz, 2016; Vannikov-Lugassi and Soffer-Dudek, 2018a). The hypothesized reason for the strong association between AII and DEP/DER across clinical and non-clinical samples is that directing attention inward may bring about DEP/DER (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). One directional finding for example, is that elevated rumination (entailing inward-focusing), longitudinally predicted an increase in DEP/DER from 1 month to the next in a 6-month study, suggesting a long-term cumulative effect, whereas the opposite effect did not exist (Vannikov-Lugassi et al., 2021). In a different study on people diagnosed with psychosis, self-focused attention and DEP/DER were related, and both associated with voice-hearing engagement (Perona-Garcelán et al., 2020). Another relevant clinical sample is maladaptive daydreamers, i.e., individuals characterized by high AII, who become addicted to immersing themselves in rich, narrative fantasies, at the expense of being present in their realities. Their high DEP/DER levels seem to be related to their sense of “unreal-ness” and detachment when withdrawing from their vivid imagination and returning to what they experience as a dreary reality, engendering sensations of discontinuity in the normal sense of being-in-the-world (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). Finally, additional indirect support for the notion that excessive inward-attention brings about DEP/DER is evident in the usefulness of grounding interventions to treat DEP/DER, requiring outward-focusing, to reorient the individual to external reality sensory cues (Neziroglu and Donnelly, 2010). Thus, as portrayed in Figure 1, AII or total immersed attention may cause DEP/DER. In OCD/S, intense absorption in ruminations and obsessions engulfing awareness inwardly may lead to dissociative detachment upon exiting the absorbed state (Soffer-Dudek, 2014). As mentioned above, O’Connor and Aardema (2012) have described the obsessive state as an experience of the world as if in a dissociative “bubble.” More recently, in a linguistic-philosophical exploration of the dynamics of OCD, Soffer-Dudek (2023) asserted that OCD clients repeatedly attempt, in vain, to uncover “true” inner states supposedly underlying their own verbal constructions, only leading them to experience a sense of alienation from themselves.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The inward-focused attention to detachment model, hypothesizing that obsessive and compulsive symptoms lead to excessive inward focused attention and repetition, both of which may cause dissociative detachment experiences. OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or obsessive-compulsive symptoms; AII, absorption and imaginative involvement; DEP/DER, depersonalization/derealization. Yellow boxes represent OCD/S and related phenomena, blue boxes represent dissociative phenomena, and green boxes represent mechanisms involved in their relationship.

Salkovskis (1998) has noted that individuals with OCD/S tend to allocate their attention to, and monitor, processes that would usually be automatic. Thus, as can be seen in Figure 1, OCD/S may be the beginning of the causal chain, generating DEP/DER through AII. Finally, One of the characteristics of OCD/S that maintains the individual’s attention tirelessly focused inward is repetition or perseveration. As mentioned before, van den Hout et al. (2009) have shown how compulsive-like staring brought about DEP/DER in a non-clinical sample. Subsequently, mirror-gazing has been used in several studies as an experimental method of inducing DEP/DER (Caputo, 2010; Brewin et al., 2013; Brewin and Mersaditabari, 2013; Caputo et al., 2021). More generally, different forms of perseveration and repetition may lead to dissociation or breakdown of meaning by blocking the activation of related associations, known as semantic satiation (Pynte, 1991; Sanbonmatsu et al., 2007; van den Hout et al., 2008; Giele et al., 2013, 2014, 2016). In Giele et al. (2016), word repetition was more likely to engender dissociative experiences among individuals with OCD than among healthy participants. The repetitive or compulsive element of these experiences seems to impair normal (automatic) perception, which may explain why rumination, a form of repetitive thinking, was shown to have a directional effect on DEP/DER (Vannikov-Lugassi et al., 2021). It is reasonable to hypothesize that obsessive rumination would have a similar effect, although empirical research is needed to explore this possibility. Figure 1 portrays this idea, whereby OCD/S symptoms inherently carry with them, and perhaps further bring about, both an inward focus of attention and repetition, both of which, in turn, lead to dissociative detachment in the form of DEP/DER.

This model is strengthened by neuropsychological evidence, according to which, event-related potentials in individuals with OCD, stemming from the temporo-parietal junction, seem to point to hyper-focused attention (Mavrogiorgou et al., 2002), and this exact area has been repeatedly implicated in relation to DEP/DER (Simeon et al., 2000; Mantovani et al., 2011), a point which will be further discussed in Model 3. Moreover, functional imaging studies suggest that people with OCD presented with non-affective tasks tend to over-activate regions associated with self-referential processing, including the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex (Rasgon et al., 2017). Engaging excessively in self-referential processing aligns with the idea of their attention being turned inward at the expense of attunement to their environment, perhaps interfering with an automatic or spontaneous sense of self.

3.2. Model 2: The dissociative experiences to cognitions and symptoms model

It has been previously suggested that AII may play a causal role in enhancing or maintaining OCD/S, especially anxious obsessing and checking compulsions, which are especially related to dissociation (Soffer-Dudek, 2014). Specifically, in the wake of dissociative AII episodes, OCD-related checking may be used to re-orient to the present moment (Soffer-Dudek, 2014, 2019). When ceasing engagement in bouts of AII (e.g., daydreaming, mind-blanking, flow, or other types of immersion), and “waking up” from these moments of decreased self-awareness, individuals may feel a sense of uncertainty regarding their actions in previous moments, and confusion between reality (memories of percepts) and imagination (memories of fantasy). Thus, they may perform checking and repetitive rituals in an attempt to subdue arising anxieties associated with troubling doubts. Indeed, OCD has long been considered a disorder of doubt, and it is well-known that individuals with OCD distrust their cognitive processes (Hermans et al., 2003, 2008; Dar et al., 2022). Thus, as portrayed in Figure 2, confusion between reality and imagination may contribute to OCD/S. Individuals with a natural tendency to often lose their sense of self (i.e., experience many moments of decreased integration–AII), may be more inclined to counteract that feeling and attempt to compensate for it, leading to repeated alternations between moments of decreased and increased awareness to their own thoughts and actions. Thus, a causal influence is hypothesized from AII (in the sensory domain) to the confusion mentioned earlier, which is in the cognitive domain (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The dissociative experiences to cognitions and symptoms model, hypothesizing that dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement along with impaired sense of agency (negative agency) act as sensory-level and experiential-level vulnerability factors for obsessive-compulsive symptoms and the cognitive schemas that facilitate them (e.g., thought-action fusion). AII, absorption and imaginative involvement; SoA, sense of agency; OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Yellow boxes represent OCD/S and related phenomena, blue boxes represent dissociative phenomena, and green boxes represent mechanisms involved in their relationship.

3.2.1. The sense of agency

The understanding of this process may be further enriched by considering the Sense of Agency (SoA), a concept which has been included in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) matrix as a promising construct for dimensional research of mental disorders. SoA is the sense that one is causing a physical or mental act (Gallagher, 2000; Synofzik et al., 2008; Morsella et al., 2011), or in other words, the feeling of controlling one’s own actions and, through them, events in the outside world (Beck et al., 2017). Succinctly conceptualized as the sense that “I did that” (Engbert et al., 2008), it is the intuitive impression that our awareness of willing an action to happen is the trigger which initiates the performance of that action. Despite the intuitiveness of this impression, it should not be taken for granted; neuropsychological evidence suggests that brain activity generating an action temporally precedes awareness of initiating the action (Sato, 2009). This means that our idea of causing an action (i.e., SoA) may not actually be the first cue in the causal chain of performing an action, although this is highly unintuitive. SoA seems to arise in tandem (or even after) the advent of action performance. Thus, there are probably biological, psychological, and environmental factors or cues responsible for engendering SoA. It should come as no surprise, then, that there are incidences in which SoA is impaired. Psychopathological impairments in SoA have been investigated and described mainly regarding Schizophrenia (Frith et al., 2000; Gallagher, 2000, 2015; Frith, 2005; Heinks-Maldonado et al., 2007; Synofzik et al., 2010; Voss et al., 2010; Maeda et al., 2013), however, several studies have demonstrated that attenuated or distorted SoA is also a characteristic of OCD/S (Belayachi and Van der Linden, 2009, 2010; Gentsch et al., 2012; Oren et al., 2016, 2019; Giuliani et al., 2017; Fradkin et al., 2019; Szalai, 2019), which will be elaborated in detail below. SoA is often empirically assessed in an indirect (non-self-report) manner through the measurement of intentional binding. Intentional binding is the tendency of most individuals to experience the result of their action as close in time to the advent of that action. In other words, intentional binding is the temporal binding of intentional actions and their effects (Moore and Obhi, 2012), and this represents SoA as it conveys the connection between the action and its consequence.

Although studies combining the concepts of SoA and dissociation are scarce, theoretically, diminished or impaired agency is a core feature of dissociation. In extreme dissociative manifestations such as DID, one experiences significant parts of one’s behavior and mental activity as stemming from a separate agent, located within one’s body. Switching to that state of a seemingly separate agent is often uncontrollable, which is why the disorder has been conceptualized as an impairment in the unity of agency (Kennett and Matthews, 2003). Notably, the degree to which this disorder is mechanistic vs. agentic has been discussed (Cardeña, 1996; Segall, 1996; van der Hart, 1996). Importantly, however, diminished agency is also characteristic of more common dissociative experiences. In AII, one operates automatically, while awareness is focused elsewhere. Thus, behavior and SoA are decoupled, and SoA may be diminished afterward as well, when emerging from the absorbed state (e.g., asking oneself what had just occurred). In DEP, one feels estranged, or detached from, one’s own body, thoughts, or actions (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). This may represent a disruption of both agency and ownership (Sierra and David, 2011; Ataria, 2015a), or the balance between them (Ataria, 2015b) (agency refers to the sense that I am causing an action; ownership is the sense that I am the one undergoing an experience; Gallagher, 2000). In a non-clinical sample, high AII individuals given a free writing task reported feeling less agency during writing, and indeed, were later less successful than low AII individuals at recognizing words extracted from their own text, supporting the notion that their writing process was characterized by decreased awareness (Bregman-Hai et al., 2020). Recent neurofunctional conceptualizations of SoA rely on connectivity between frontal and parietal areas (Haggard, 2017). As mentioned before, reduced connectivity in these areas has been implicated in AII as well as pathological dissociation (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2019; Carbone et al., 2022).

Sense of agency has been studied mainly by experimental manipulation, but recently a self-report questionnaire has been developed, with the aim of capturing individual differences in baseline SoA (Tapal et al., 2017). Some of the items were worded positively (e.g., “I am in full control of what I do”), whereas others were worded negatively (reversed items, e.g., “While I am in action, I feel like I am a remote controlled robot”). Interestingly, the negatively worded items, describing a distortion in the normal SoA, are extremely similar to items from dissociation scales, describing DEP experiences. Positive and negative SoA items were found to load on separate (yet related) factors. This means that experiencing a distortion in normal SoA (e.g., feeling like a robot) is not equivalent to having low levels of normal SoA (e.g., not endorsing the belief that things that one does are subject only to one’s free will), although these two constructs are related and probably causally interact. Possibly, negative SoA (or DEP) is a sensory subjective experience, whereas positive SoA is a cognitive belief (see Synofzik et al., 2013, for a similar conceptualization of two types of SoA); this suggestion, as well as the causal pattern between them, has not yet been established and should be investigated further. Importantly, negative, rather than positive, SoA was related to OCS (Tapal et al., 2017).

Additional recent studies have found impaired SoA in individuals with OCD/S. For example, this was cleverly demonstrated with an indirect linguistic measure; individuals scoring high on OCS tended to omit agency in their spoken language (Oren et al., 2016). These authors also mention that compulsions directly represent deficient SoA, as compulsive action is not experienced as stemming from free will. Deficient SoA in OCD/S has been demonstrated with additional indirect measures, such as diminished suppression of internal expectations in favor of external perception of the consequences of one’s action (Gentsch et al., 2012), impaired ability to identify one’s gaze as bringing about an auditory stimulus (Giuliani et al., 2017, 2021), increased attribution of thoughts as intruders following an auditory cue (Fradkin et al., 2019), and low levels of intentional binding (Oren et al., 2019). According to Szalai (2019), The SoA impairments characterizing OCD are in line with the presence of neurological soft signs (minor, non-localizable deviations in motor or sensory performance) related to motor coordination and sensory integration, compared to controls (Bolton et al., 1999). Thus, as can be seen in Figure 2, dissociative AII may bring about SoA impairments, which in turn contribute to confusion and eventually OCD/S.

3.2.2. Inflated agency

However, it has also been noted that direct (self-report) measures of SoA may produce opposite results, i.e., provide evidence for enhanced SoA in OCD, when those measures assess illusory sense of control (Oren et al., 2016, 2019; Tapal et al., 2017). Indeed, OCD is characterized by a sense of exaggerated control (Reuven-Magril et al., 2008; Oren et al., 2019). This is well-documented in phenomena such as “thought-action fusion” (TAF; Shafran et al., 1996; Shafran and Rachman, 2004) and “thought-event fusion” (Gwilliam et al., 2004), i.e., the notion that thoughts may bring about consequences similarly to actions or events in the world.

How can we conceptualize this apparent contradiction? The apparent discrepancy between indirect and direct measures of SoA may in fact be readily understood when taking dissociation into account. Specifically, if an individual tends to act in an absorbed, automatic state, i.e., tends to miss out on the “feeling of doing” (Belayachi and Van der Linden, 2010) and on the SoA, it may be more difficult for that individual to reliably differentiate actions from thoughts. AII is characterized by a vivid imagination (this was validated this using a mental rotation task; Bregman-Hai et al., 2018), whereas reality is less vivid (because awareness while acting is lacking); thus, absorbers may find it difficult to tell images and memories apart. In other words, considering dissociation may explain why and how individuals come to develop TAF, which is considered as a risk factor for OCD (Shafran and Rachman, 2004). Indeed, Eddy (2016) also suggested that TAF may stem from deficient SoA.

Figure 2 visually portrays this hypothesized dynamic, whereby dissociative experiences, and reduced agency (negative SoA), which are in the subjective sensory-experiential domain, work together in generating both OCD/S and cognitive risk factors for OCD/S. Specifically, acting in the world in an absorbed state, with diminished SoA, may impair the generation of normal associations between actions and their consequences. Moreover, “magical” associations are learned: one repeatedly experiences events as occurring without the person having an accessible representation of performing them. Thus, over time the individual may create a cognitive schema of the world whereby things just seem to “happen,” although one does not explicitly remember, or merely has a fleeting impression of, causing them—an impression which is difficult to tell apart from imagination. Indeed, TAF is related to schizotypy and fantasy proneness (Muris and Merckelbach, 2003).

One may claim that AII may be reminiscent of “inferential confusion”—a reliance on imagination and a distrust of the senses—which is strongly related to OCS (O’Connor and Robillard, 1995). Indeed, dissociation and AII, OCS, and inferential confusion are inter-related (Aardema and Wu, 2011; Paradisis et al., 2015). However, whereas inferential confusion is considered to be a cognitive reasoning tendency to draw inferences on the basis of unlikely possibilities (O’Connor and Aardema, 2012), AII is presumed to be an automatic state, in which attention is narrowed and poorly regulated, without volition or meta-conscious self-awareness (Butler, 2006), perhaps setting the stage for the advent of inferential confusion. Thus, dissociation or AII may be viewed as a rudimentary, experiential construct, that may lead to the development of cognitive schemas which are usually thought of as the source or beginning of the etiological causal chain, including TAF, thought-event fusion, and inferential confusion (see Figure 2).

3.3. Model 3: The deficient sense of embodiment model

A prominent theory in the field of OCD/S suggests that individuals with obsessive-compulsive tendencies have attenuated access to identifying their own internal states, known as the “Seeking proxies for internal states” (SPIS) model (Liberman and Dar, 2018; Dar et al., 2021). The lack of connection to one’s own bodily clues may be related to a broader deficiency in automatic sensory processing, impaired SoA, and a flawed sense of embodiment. Indeed, individuals with OCD/S more readily incorporate foreign objects into their sense of bodily self (Jalal and Ramachandran, 2017; Jalal et al., 2020). In recent decades there have been several studies addressing abnormal activity in the temporo-parietal-junction as a neural correlate responsible for bodily sensory integration and the sense of residing within one’s own body (Blanke et al., 2002, 2004; Blanke and Mohr, 2005), and indeed, abnormalities in that area are associated with DEP/DER (Simeon et al., 2000; Mantovani et al., 2011), and have been also detected in OCD (McGuire et al., 1994; Mavrogiorgou et al., 2002; Tan et al., 2013). Moreover, as mentioned before, Mavrogiorgou et al. (2002) found that unlike most psychiatric disorders, in OCD an event-related P300 subcomponent generated in the temporo-parietal-junction may indicate hyper-focused attention. This shared neuroanatomical mechanism implicated in DEP/DER and OCD probably points to deficient multisensory integration (Russo et al., 2014; Eddy, 2016). Thus, a hypothesized impairment in a very rudimentary process of binding together different sensory impressions into an integrated sense of a single embodied self may bring about outcomes related to the mind-body connection, including DEP/DER, and body-related OCD-spectrum symptoms and syndromes such as obsessions over specific body parts, body dysmorphic disorder, excoriation (skin-picking) disorder, and trichotillomania. These phenomena may be associated with altered sensory thresholds or atypical interpretation of body signals and sensory input. As can be seen in Figure 3, temporo-parietal junction abnormalities are hypothesized to generate a flaws sense of embodiment, which in turn may cause DEP/DER, diminished SoA, and body-related OCD/S and related spectrum phenomena.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The deficient sense of embodiment model, hypothesizing that a neural mechanism, such as the temporo-parietal junction, is responsible for deficient automatic processing of sensory information, affecting embodiment integration and creating both DEP/DER and body-related OCD/S phenomena. DEP/DER, depersonalization/derealization; SoA, sense of agency; NJREs, not-just-right experiences; OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or obsessive-compulsive symptoms; SPIS, seeking proxies for internal states. OCD/S and related phenomena, blue boxes represent dissociative phenomena, and green boxes represent mechanisms involved in their relationship.

Mutual mind-body associations also play a part in maladaptive daydreaming, a condition strongly related to both OCD/S and dissociation (Salomon-Small et al., 2021), which often entails repetitive motor stereotypies, as well as very high rates of skin-picking (Somer et al., 2017); these repetitive movements involve the use of the body to enhance focus on one’s internal mental activity, suggesting that further research should be conducted to understand the relationships between attention, internal states, and imagery, on one hand, and bodily cues, on the other.

Another specific OCD-related phenomenon that may stem from deficient automatic sensory processing is the phenomenon of “Not Just Right” Experiences (NJREs), i.e., sensations that things are not as they should be, which is related to OCD/S (Coles et al., 2003; Belloch et al., 2016; Coles and Ravid, 2016). Compulsions may often stem from the need to “make things right” rather than to avoid harm (Ecker and Gönner, 2008; Belloch et al., 2016; Coles and Ravid, 2016). It has been suggested that performing actions with an attenuated SoA and a diminished “feeling of doing” may be the cause for sensing incompleteness and NJREs (Belayachi and Van der Linden, 2010). However, adding dissociation and embodiment into this equation may deepen our understanding of the phenomenon. Specifically, both NJREs and DEP/DER experiences are characterized by a feeling that something is “off,” i.e., not quite as it should be. For example, a NJRE may be knowing that one has washed one’s hands but still not feeling that they are clean (van Dis and van den Hout, 2016); similarly, a DEP/DER experience may be knowing that one is staring at one’s own reflection in the mirror, but not feeling the expected familiarity. This uncanny impression shared by both experiences may possibly stem from a common neural mechanism, as both DEP/DER and NJREs are experienced in the sensory domain. NJREs have even been conceptualized as “sensory perfectionism” (Coles et al., 2003). Indeed, OCD/S are associated with strong sensory components (Röhlinger et al., 2015; Grimaldi and Stern, 2017) and are characterized by deficient internal motor predictions (Gentsch et al., 2012), suggesting a very basic impairment in the way individuals with OCD/S subjectively experience the world. Thus, DEP/DER and NJREs may stem from a common mechanism, which may be biological, perhaps abnormalities in the temporo-parietal-junction (Eddy, 2016). Figure 3 portrays these hypothesized causalities.

3.4. Model 4: The sleepiness to cognitive deficiencies model

In recent years, several studies have demonstrated that sleep impairments are associated with OCD/S, including a large-scale study on a nationally representative sample (Cox and Olatunji, 2016) and several meta-analyses (e.g., Díaz-Román et al., 2015; Nota et al., 2015). Research has shown that OCS are associated with increased sleep-onset latency, reduced sleep duration, reduced sleep efficiency, and delayed sleep phase (Paterson et al., 2013; Boland and Ross, 2015; Nota et al., 2015). It has been claimed that obsessional rumination at bedtime brings about insomnia, similarly to general anxiety and depression (Boland and Ross, 2015), and that comorbid depressive symptoms may be the true cause for sleep disturbance in OCD (Paterson et al., 2013); however, depression and/or anxiety cannot entirely explain the specific relationship between OCD and disturbed sleep (Timpano et al., 2014; Nota et al., 2015; Cox and Olatunji, 2016). It has been demonstrated that in individuals with OCS, impaired inhibitory functions are related to short sleep duration (Nota et al., 2016), suggesting that perhaps a lack of proper sleep makes it harder to inhibit repetitive thoughts and behaviors; this effect was later claimed to stem from deficient alertness, rather than sleep per se (Kalanthroff et al., 2017; Naftalovich et al., 2020), and indeed, OCD/S worsens during the times of day that are the most non-optimal according to the person’s chronotype (Naftalovich et al., 2021; and also see Cox and Olatunji, 2022). Thus, as can be seen in Figure 4, it is hypothesized that sleep alterations or deficiencies bring about OCD/S through decreased cognitive control. Interestingly, deficiencies in the systems governing alertness and inhibition are the main suspects implicated in DEP/DER disorder as well (Sierra and Berrios, 1998; Sierra et al., 2002).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The sleepiness to cognitive deficiencies model, hypothesizing that sleepiness, mixed sleep-wake states, dreamlike thinking, and deficient alertness, may cause both obsessive-compulsive symptoms and dissociative symptoms, through impairments in cognitive control and psychomotor slowness. SoA, sense of agency; AII, absorption and imaginative involvement; DEP/DER, depersonalization/derealization; OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Yellow boxes represent OCD/S and related phenomena, blue boxes represent dissociative phenomena, and green boxes represent mechanisms involved in their relationship.

More generally, dissociation has also been related to sleep patterns in recent years, specifically, a labile sleep-wake cycle or a tendency for experiencing mixed sleep-wake states (Lynn et al., 2012; van der Kloet et al., 2012b). Dissociation has been conceptualized as sleep elements permeating the waking state (Mahowald and Schenck, 2001; van der Kloet et al., 2012b), and hybrid sleep-wake states are related to automatisms such as sleepwalking episodes, which are considered dissociative (Mahowald and Schenck, 2001). Indeed, sleep and dream patterns characterizing dissociation (discovered by Watson, 2001) may be symmetrically conceptualized as intrusions of wakefulness into sleep (specifically, aroused dreaming patterns or unusual sleep-wake transition phenomena) (Soffer-Dudek and Shahar, 2011; Soffer-Dudek, 2017b). Moreover, dissociative experiences (especially DEP/DER) increased following sleep deprivation in healthy samples (Giesbrecht et al., 2007; Selvi et al., 2015; van Heugten-van der Kloet et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2017), and decreased in parallel to improvements in sleep hygiene in a clinical sample (van der Kloet et al., 2012a). An investigation of four potential mechanisms that may be responsible for the association between rumination and dissociation across two samples, repeatedly revealed the central role of poor sleep quality as the most potent mediator (Vannikov-Lugassi and Soffer-Dudek, 2018b). Thus, as can be seen in Figure 4, sleep abnormalities are also involved in generating DEP/DER. In an opposite causality path, trait dissociation has predictive abilities in the sleep domain; in two healthy high-functioning samples, initial levels of trait AII predicted the extent to which sleepiness increased following sleep deprivation, an effect which remained after recovery sleep as well (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2017). It seemed that high AII individuals experiencing disruption to their sleep cycle had more difficulty in contending with the pull of consciousness toward the sleep state. Thus, AII may represent an impaired ability to regulate, monitor, and control consciousness states, which may be reciprocally related to sleepiness. This is also represented in Figure 4. Notably, sleep deprivation may be one of the conditions in which AII becomes “pathological,” supporting the need for the examination of sleep as an intervening variable when assessing the relation of OCD/S and dissociation.

Interestingly, commission errors, implicated in relation to short sleep in the context of OCD (Nota et al., 2015), are specifically the type of error characterizing dissociators (Giesbrecht et al., 2008). Commission errors in dissociators are probably related to difficulty in differentiating between reality and imagination, as imagination is experienced as vivid and clear (Bregman-Hai et al., 2018). Mixed sleep-wake states probably contribute to this confusion, as reality becomes dream-like. Indeed, dissociation and OCD/S may be related through dysfunctional reality monitoring or reduced confidence in reality (Merckelbach and Wessel, 2000); and this may be brought about by poor sleep.

In a classic, meticulous study of the dream patterns of 361 patients with various neurological impairments or injuries (Solms, 1997), deficits in several anatomical areas were associated with dream reduction or cessation, but certain areas were associated with the opposite: higher frequency of dreaming, higher vividness, dreams experienced as realistic, and dreams pervading waking to the point of confusion between dream and reality. Specifically, these hyper-dreaming effects characterized patients with injuries in the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and basal forebrain (Solms, 1997); accordingly, all those areas have been implicated as neuroanatomical mechanisms involved in OCD as well (e.g., see Sturm et al., 2003; Aouizerate et al., 2004; Modirrousta et al., 2015, respectively). This relates to another possible shared mechanism, namely, excessive imagery, but this will be further discussed in Model 5.

Another sleep-related mechanism which may be responsible for the dissociation-OCD/S relationship may be psychomotor slowness, which has been associated with short sleep duration (Kronholm et al., 2011). Possibly, such slowness may reduce body/brain integrity (discussed in the previous see section “3.3. Model 3: The deficient sense of embodiment model”), and thus may bring about dissociation and sensations of unreality. Indeed, intentional binding or sensory integration is contingent upon short time frames between intention and action (Ismail and Shimada, 2016), and thus psychomotor slowness following short sleep in OCD/S may perhaps diminish SoA and create DEP/DER. 3 Such alterations in the sense of reality and agency may undermine certainty in actions and their consequences (as well as memory and attention; Hermans et al., 2008) and thus aggravate OCD/S. In turn, aggravated OCD/S will further disrupt sleep through insomnia, leading to a vicious cycle. Thus, Figure 4 shows psychomotor slowness as a possible mediator between sleep impairments and OCD/S, through impaired agency.

Finally, it should be noted that dissociative sleep-related automatisms (e.g., sleepwalking) are actions done with very little awareness and SoA, which generates questions regarding moral accountability for these acts (e.g., Broughton et al., 1994; Levy and Bayne, 2004). This may be a useful model for understanding the experience of individuals with OCD/S, who act automatically with little awareness and impaired agency, leading them to ask themselves such questions exactly. Indeed, it has been recently suggested that OCD is associated with a distrust of automaticity (van den Hout et al., 2019).

The various paths through which sleep impairments may explain the OCD/S-dissociation relationship are depicted in Figure 4.

3.5. Model 5: The hyperactive and intrusive imagery system model

Intrusive imagery is a central feature in OCD/S (Rachman, 2007; Lipton et al., 2010; Bouvard et al., 2017). In fact, most OCD obsessions seem to involve images (Rachman, 2007; Speckens et al., 2007), either associated with distressing past events (Speckens et al., 2007) or not associated with memories (Lipton et al., 2010). There are individual differences in peoples’ ability to imagine things clearly, vividly, and immersively, and in the frequency of their tendency to do so (Bregman-Hai et al., 2018; Merckelbach et al., 2022; Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). Possibly, an especially vivid imagination in some people means that internally generated content including vivid images are conjured up with ease and triggered frequently, which may leave them at risk for developing OCD/S (see Figure 5). It could be that people who develop OCD/S are those who tend to engage in pictorial or visual thinking above average. Notably, pictorial thinking cannot include negation; thus, a thought such as “I will never hurt my baby” would paradoxically conjure up an image of the person hurting their baby. The salient or vivid nature of the mental imagery may render it harder to get rid of or control, i.e., more intrusive. This may lead to AII, specifically, attention and awareness becoming engulfed in immersive daydreaming, obliviousness to surroundings, or a detached “zoning out.” Hence, Figure 5 portrays a hyperactive imagery system and intrusions as causal factors in both AII and OCD/S. At the cognitive level, a hyperactive and vivid imagery system would also result in imagined possibilities being more easily confused with reality, i.e., inferential confusion; this means that individuals rely excessively on their imagination at the expense of trusting their sensory input, in turn strengthening OCD/S (O’Connor and Robillard, 1995; Aardema and O’Connor, 2003; Aardema et al., 2005). Obsessions may begin as daydreams or imaginings which are then treated as realistic possibilities, further developing into fixations (Aardema and O’Connor, 2003). Indeed, in a general student sample inferential confusion and AII were strongly related, and both were predictive of OCD/S (Aardema and Wu, 2011). Notably, when including both inferential confusion and AII in a model predicting OCD/S severity in a clinical OCD sample, neither were statistically significant (Pozza et al., 2016), probably due to their largely shared predictive ability, which was not included in either construct’s unique contribution estimate. Thus, the figure includes inferential confusion as another possible mediator.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The hyperactive and intrusive imagery system model, hypothesizing that an excessively active and vivid imagery system, causing intrusive images, underlies both dissociative absorption experiences and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. AII, absorption and imaginative involvement; OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Yellow boxes represent OCD/S and related phenomena, blue boxes represent dissociative phenomena, and green boxes represent mechanisms involved in their relationship.

It is noteworthy that there is a clinical condition in which the core problem is excessive imagery, with vivid, intensely emotional, and narrative fantasy activity, namely, maladaptive daydreaming (Somer, 2002). Maladaptive daydreamers compulsively engage in this activity, which they feel a constant urge to perform. Maladaptive daydreaming is different than OCD/S because it is gratifying in the short run, characterized by reward-seeking—similar to a behavioral addiction—rather than necessarily to threat avoidance. Nevertheless, astoundingly high rates of maladaptive daydreamers have either comorbid OCD/S or OCD-related spectrum disorders, such as skin-picking disorder and body dysmorphic disorder (Somer et al., 2017; Salomon-Small et al., 2021). Related to the latter, research has also shown that intrusive imagery is even more salient in OCD with comorbid body dysmorphic disorder than in OCD alone (Nakata et al., 2007). Notably, maladaptive daydreaming is strongly related to AII and has been suggested to represent a dissociative disorder (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022). An investigation of OCD/S in a sample of over 500 maladaptive daydreamers found that approximately 56% of them were over the cutoff for suspected OCD, and dissociation was a strong mediating factor explaining the relationship of OCD/S and maladaptive daydreaming (Salomon-Small et al., 2021). This supports the idea, portrayed visually in Figure 5, that at least for some individuals, a hyperactive imagery system may underlie both OCD/S and dissociative experiences.

Maladaptive daydreamers report higher sense of presence in their daydreams (Bigelsen et al., 2016), suggesting that they may have a low threshold for experiencing percepts and sensations as vivid or real. Anomalies in imagination or an overly active vivid imagination is also associated with magical thinking, paranoid ideation, and even the psychosis spectrum (Rasmussen and Parnas, 2015; Zsila et al., 2019; Merckelbach et al., 2022; Rasmussen et al., 2022). In the neuropsychological realm, the temporal lobes are known to be involved in generating percepts and have been suggested as a mechanism that may relate psychosis and creativity (Flaherty, 2011); perhaps an overly active temporal lobe may stand at the bottom of both excessive imagery and magical thinking. Indeed, the temporal lobes have been associated with paranormal beliefs, anomalous perceptual experiences, and magical thinking (Persinger, 1984; Persinger and Valliant, 1985; Persinger and Makarec, 1987; Bell et al., 2007; Thalbourne and Maltby, 2008). Specifically, the inhibition of left lateral temporal activation successfully suppressed magical thinking, assessed in that study as the tendency to report meaningful information based on random noise (Bell et al., 2007). One study also showed that anomalous experiences (e.g., sensory experiences, sense of presence) can be induced in some people by applying weak magnetic fields from outside the skull in the temporo-parietal area (Persinger and Healey, 2002), implicating the same area already discussed in Model 3 when it comes to unusual embodied sensory experiences. Interestingly, there is evidence of the involvement of temporal lobe abnormalities in OCD, based on consistently higher rates of OCD/S in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, compared to either idiopathic generalized epilepsy or non-epileptic controls (e.g., Isaacs et al., 2004; Monaco et al., 2005; Ertekin et al., 2009). Temporal lobe epilepsy is also associated with increased vivid nightmares (Silvestri and Bromfield, 2004). Thus, temporal lobe abnormalities are included in Figure 5 as a possible causal factor generating the excessive imagery.

4. Discussion

In the present work, five different theoretical causal models were presented, explaining the strong and specific relationship of dissociative experiences and OCD/S, none of which rely on general constructs such as trauma or detaching from negative emotion–constructs that are usually considered as the immediate etiological factors of dissociation, but are not specific to OCD/S. The five models do not negate one another but may be complementary, in other words, may all be true; they are presented separately for the sake of theoretical clarity. However, dissociation and OCD/S may reciprocally affect each other as well as stem from several common mechanisms.

Indeed, although the models were presented as separate, they are closely related. For example, both Models 2 (SoA) and 3 (Embodiment) focus on impaired binding or integration of sensory and/or motor systems; and both Models 4 (Sleepiness) and 5 (Imagery) imply enhanced salience of visual thought during waking. All five models probably work together in contributing to the anatomical-functional-psychological infrastructure generating and/or maintaining OCD/S. For example, attention overly focused inward (Model 1) may reciprocally affect and be affected by, decreased sensory integration (Models 2, 3), and that internal attentional spotlight may be directed toward salient, uncontrolled images (Models 4, 5), which further demand their share of attention, leaving the individual in a vicious cycle of fixation on fantastical possibilities.

More research is needed concerning self-concept clarity, identity, and automaticity in OCD/S. Additionally, AII, SoA, embodiment, sleep-wake boundaries, and imagination activity all seem to be worthwhile avenues for further investigation regarding OCD/S etiological and maintaining factors. In particular, there is a need for experimental work on dissociation and OCD/S, exploring the possibility of dissociative experiences as a causal factor. For example, experiments could explore whether manipulating individuals into an immersed and absorbed state while they perform a task with little awareness may bring about anxiety and uncertainty regarding their automatic actions. Also, experience-sampling and prospective-longitudinal designs are scarce in the OCD/S field; those could shed light on the day-to-day temporal dynamics of the OCD/S–dissociation relationship, as well as the long-term effects of personality. Specifically, large-scale longitudinal developmental studies may reveal whether experiential inclinations such as trait AII may, over time, engender cognitive schema risk factors for OCD/S, such as TAF.

The present work builds upon previous etiological knowledge accumulated on OCD/S, such as inferential confusion, TAF, and the SPIS model. However, considering dissociation adds important insights with incremental value for understanding OCD/S. For example, adding an experiential and sensory perspective helps shed light on the development of cognitive risk factors such as inferential confusion and TAF. Notably, the SPIS model suggests that TAF stems from lack of access to one’s feelings and motivations, and therefore trying to compensate by assigning excessive importance to one’s own thoughts and images (Dar et al., 2021). The current theoretical formulation postulates that considering dissociation is crucial to understanding how it may be that a person may feel diminished access to their own feelings and motivations, and offers a somewhat more specific explanation for TAF, which is characterized by magical thinking. Again, it does not contradict or negate the explanation suggested previously but enriches its nuances.

High-dissociation individuals are known to gain less from ordinary treatment for OCD (Rufer et al., 2006a; Prasko et al., 2009; Semiz et al., 2014), which is why the current formulations may be important in inspiring the development of clinical interventions for such individuals. The models suggested in this work harbor practical clinical significance in several domains. First, future studies could examine whether individuals with OCD may benefit from the use of grounding techniques, usually associated with treating dissociation, to focus their attention outward and ground their sense of bodily self when getting caught up in obsessions or negative emotion. This was briefly mentioned as a useful aid in two independent case reports of clients with OCD (Proescher, 2010; Cheli et al., 2020). Second, the idea that AII may contribute to OCD/S (Models 2 and 4) means that understanding dissociative-absorptive qualities in OCD may impact interventions. For example, one study aimed to enhance attentional abilities in OCD patients by training them at selective attention, but that did not decrease their symptoms (Haug et al., 2013). The present conceptualization, according to which individuals with OCD have a tendency for an absorptive, narrow attentional spotlight, in the form of AII, explains why training them to attend selectively to a single stimulus was unlikely to alleviate their symptoms. In contrast, the training of patients with OCD for attentional control should perhaps focus on the development of the ability to integrate mental states and multitask. Moreover, empathically addressing clients’ sensory experiences of increased automaticity, decreased agency, and resulting reality-fantasy confusion may help them feel better understood by the therapist, which may facilitate their motivation to correct their cognitive oversights.

Third, future studies should explore whether alterations in automatic processing and integration of bodily cues (Model 3), perhaps due to temporo-parietal abnormalities, may also be of importance as a possible focus of intervention. Such a neuropsychological intervention has been attempted in the field of dissociation (Mantovani et al., 2011). Fourth, the idea that sleepiness and mixed sleep-wake states may be involved in the etiology or maintenance of OCD/S (Model 4) means that at the beginning of treatment, an OCD client should be assessed not only with dissociation scales, but also pertaining to sleep quality and sleep-wake boundary phenomena. Sleep-wake boundaries are important to consider, rather than merely focusing on traditional sleep quality measures (Soffer-Dudek, 2017b). For example, a client may off-handedly report that they have no problems falling asleep and indeed, report that they drink coffee every evening and still fall asleep within minutes. This may suggest an inclination for experiencing mixed states, that may manifest in both nighttime aroused dreaming and daytime dissociative experiences, that may possibly exacerbate their OCD symptoms. While this suggestion is speculative and in need of further study, it should be considered as it is quite easy to implement sleep hygiene interventions in addition to treatment as usual. Finally, a hyperactive imagery system (Model 5) may suggest the existence of comorbid maladaptive daydreaming. If the client experiences elaborate, narrative, absorbing fantasies, which they engage in compulsively, treatment should address that, for example by monitoring the imagination activity and its triggers (Herscu et al., 2023). Maladaptive daydreaming often involves an idealized version of the self (Somer, 2002) and experimenting with different personalities (Soffer-Dudek and Somer, 2022), meaning that addressing self-image, self-coherency, or other underlying self-related issues may also be relevant.

The present work has several limitations. Specifically, knowledge of the neuropsychological underpinnings of dissociative experiences is quite scant and preliminary, which makes it difficult to identify the functional source of dissociative impairments in OCD/S. Second, the definitions of dissociative experiences may be at times quite hazy and controversial (e.g., see van der Hart et al., 2004), with points of overlap with other existing constructs such as negative SoA, for DEP/DER, and deficits in attention, for AII (e.g., see Carleton et al., 2010, who defined AII as “attentional dissociation”). Importantly, though, attention deficit and mind-wandering have been shown to be separate factors from AII, and the latter had significant incremental value over them in predicting OCD/S (Soffer-Dudek, 2019). Still, overlapping or ambiguous definitions hinder scientific clarity. Finally, a lack of specific intervention protocols for mild dissociative states limits the ability to tailor treatments differently for individuals with OCD who are high in dissociation. Nevertheless, the use of grounding techniques and the implementation of sleep hygiene seem to be promising avenues.

Hopefully, the present work may encourage further empirical research into the dissociative aspects of OCD/S, as well as establish dissociation as a transdiagnostic factor relevant to additional psychopathologies rather than trauma alone. These novelties carry theoretical significance for the understanding of a link between clinical symptoms which is currently poorly understood and under-researched. Such explorations will enrich both respective fields.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

NS-D was solely responsible for the literature review, conceptualization, writing, reviewing and editing, and any other work associated with this manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding for this research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation (grant No. 1444/22).

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Prof. Gideon Anholt–an OCD researcher-clinician (among other things), for his continuing support and encouragement over the years, as the author—a dissociation researcher-clinician—found herself leaning toward OCD research. He has helped the author think more clearly about causality in the OCD-dissociation relationship and has commented on previous versions of some of the ideas presented in this work.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

AII, absorption and imaginative involvement; DEP/DER, depersonalization and/or derealization; DID, dissociative identity disorder; DSM-5, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition; NJREs, not-just-right experiences; OCD/S, obsessive-compulsive disorder or symptoms; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; SoA, sense of agency; SPIS, seeking proxies for internal states; TAF, thought-action fusion.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Decreased coherence or connectivity also seems to be a neural marker of dissociation in general, either in healthy participants (Carbone et al., 2022), or in clinical samples including PTSD (Tursich et al., 2015) and DID or dissociative disorders (Hopper et al., 2002; Farina et al., 2014). It is also an established correlate of hypnosis (van der Kruijs et al., 2014), a state which is conceptually close to AII.
  2. ^ Tellegen (1982). Content categories: Absorption items (revised). University of Minnesota, Unpublished manuscript.
  3. ^ Note that slowness following short sleep is probably independent of the phenomenon of “obsessional slowness” (Rachman, 1974; Veale, 1993), although obsessional slowness might also be hypothesized to bring about diminished SOA and dissociation.

References

Aardema, F., and O’Connor, K. (2003). Seeing white bears that are not there: inference processes in obsessions. J. Cogn. Psychother. 17, 23–37. doi: 10.1891/jcop.17.1.23.58270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aardema, F., O’Connor, K. P., Emmelkamp, P. M., Marchand, A., and Todorov, C. (2005). Inferential confusion in obsessive–compulsive disorder: the inferential confusion questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 293–308. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aardema, F., Radomsky, A. S., Moulding, R., Wong, S. F., Bourguignon, L., and Giraldo-O’Meara, M. (2021). Development and validation of the multidimensional version of the fear of self questionnaire: corrupted, culpable and malformed feared possible selves in obsessive-compulsive and body-dysmorphic symptoms. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 28, 1160–1180. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aardema, F., and Wu, K. D. (2011). Imaginative, dissociative, and schizotypal processes in obsessive-compulsive symptoms. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 74–81. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: APA. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aouizerate, B., Guehl, D., Cuny, E., Rougier, A., Bioulac, B., Tignol, J., et al. (2004). Pathophysiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder: a necessary link between phenomenology, neuropsychology, imagery and physiology. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 195–221. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armour, C., Contractor, A. A., Palmieri, P. A., and Elhai, J. D. (2014). Assessing latent level associations between PTSD and dissociative factors: is depersonalization and derealization related to PTSD factors more so than alternative dissociative factors? Psychol. Inj. Law 7, 131–142. doi: 10.1007/s12207-014-9196-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ataria, Y. (2015a). Sense of ownership and sense of agency during trauma. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 14, 199–212. doi: 10.1007/s11097-013-9334-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ataria, Y. (2015b). Dissociation during trauma: the ownership-agency tradeoff model. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 14, 1037–1053. doi: 10.1007/s11097-014-9392-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ay, R., and Erbay, L. G. (2018). Relationship between childhood trauma and suicide probability in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 261, 132–136. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A. (1989). “Conditional automaticity: varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition,” in Unintended thought, eds J. S. Uleman and J. A. Bargh (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 3–51. doi: 10.2307/1423027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. Am. J. Psychol. 105, 181–199

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A. (1997). “The automaticity of everyday life,” in The Automaticity of Everyday life: Advances in Social Cognition, ed. R. S. Wyer (London: Psychology Press) doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, B., Di Costa, S., and Haggard, P. (2017). Having control over the external world increases the implicit sense of agency. Cognition 162, 54–60 doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belayachi, S., and Van der Linden, M. (2009). Level of agency in sub-clinical checking. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 293–299 doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belayachi, S., and Van der Linden, M. (2010). Feeling of doing in obsessive–compulsive checking. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 534–546 doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70249-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, V., Reddy, V., Halligan, P., Kirov, G., and Ellis, H. (2007). Relative suppression of magical thinking: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cortex 43, 551–557. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belli, H., Ural, C., Vardar, M. K., Yesılyurt, S., and Oncu, F. (2012). Dissociative symptoms and dissociative disorder comorbidity in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 975–980 doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belloch, A., Fornés, G., Carrasco, A., López-Solá, C., Alonso, P., and Menchón, J. M. (2016). Incompleteness and not just right experiences in the explanation of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 236, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, N. C., Wheaton, M. G., and Abramowitz, J. S. (2013). Childhood trauma and thought action fusion: a multi-method examination. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 2, 43–47 doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhar, S. S., and Kyrios, M. (2007). An investigation of self-ambivalence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1845–1857 doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigelsen, J., Lehrfeld, J. M., Jopp, D. S. and Somer, E. (2016). Maladaptive daydreaming: evidence for an under-researched mental health disorder. Conscious. Cogn. 42, 254–266 doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., and Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain 127, 243–258 doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., and Mohr, C. (2005). Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 184–199 doi: 10.1038/419269a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., and Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature 419, 269–270 doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2015.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boland, E. M., and Ross, R. J. (2015). Recent advances in the study of sleep in the anxiety disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 38, 761–776 doi: 10.1155/1999/639045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolton, D., Gibb, W., Lees, A., Raven, P., Gray, J. A., Chen, E., and Shafran, R. (1999). Neurological soft signs in obsessive-compulsive disorder: standardised assessment and comparison with schizophrenia. Behav. Neurol. 11, 197–204 doi: 10.1080/16506073.2016.1262894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouvard, M., Fournet, N., Denis, A., Sixdenier, A., and Clark, D. (2017). Intrusive thoughts in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical participants: a comparison using the international Intrusive thought Interview Schedule. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 46, 287–299. doi: 10.1007/s11126-017-9555-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boysan, M., Yildirim, A., Besiroglu, L., Kefeli, M. C., and Kagan, M. (2018). Development and preliminary psychometric properties of an instrument for the measurement of obsessional dissociative experiences: the van obsessional dissociation questionnaire (VOD-Q). Psychiatr. Q. 89, 549–568. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00681-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boysan, M., Yıldırım, A., and Ökmen, A. C. (2022). Relationships between probabilistic inferences, meta-cognitions, obsessional beliefs, dissociative experiences and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a mixture structural equation modeling approach. Curr. Psychol. 41, 1494–1503. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bregman-Hai, N., Abitbul-Gordon, S., Deutsch, I., Garbi, D., Shelef, L., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Leave everything to the imagination: cognitive functioning of individuals high in dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement. J. Res. Pers. 76, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bregman-Hai, N., Kessler, Y., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2020). Who wrote that? automaticity and reduced sense of agency in individuals prone to dissociative absorption. Conscious. Cogn. 78:102861. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewin, C. R., Ma, B. Y., and Colson, J. (2013). Effects of experimentally induced dissociation on attention and memory. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 315–323. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewin, C. R., and Mersaditabari, N. (2013). Experimentally-induced dissociation impairs visual memory. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 1189–1194.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Broughton, R., Billings, R., Cartwright, R., Doucette, D., Edmeads, J., Edwardh, M., et al. (1994). Homicidal somnambulism: a case report. Sleep 17, 253–264. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchnik-Daniely, Y., Vannikov-Lugassi, M., Shalev, H., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). The path to dissociative experiences: a direct comparison of different etiological models. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 28, 1091–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, L. (2006). Normative dissociation. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 29, 45–62. doi: 10.1068/p6466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caputo, G. B. (2010). Strange-face-in-the-mirror illusion. Perception 39, 1007–1008. doi: 10.1177/0276236620969632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caputo, G. B., Lynn, S. J., and Houran, J. (2021). Mirror- and eye-gazing: an integrative review of induced altered and anomalous experiences. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 40, 418–457. doi: 10.1159/000519563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbone, G. A., Imperatori, C., Bersani, F. S., Massullo, C., Orlando, E. M., and Farina, B. (2022). Dissociative-traumatic dimension and triple network: an EEG functional connectivity study in a sample of university students. Psychopathology 55, 28–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cardeña, E. (1996). False dichotomies and true metaphors: a comment on Segall’s “Metaphors of agency”. Dissociation 9, 162–163. doi: 10.1037/cns0000080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardeña, E., and Marcusson-Clavertz, D. (2016). The relation of hypnotizability and dissociation to everyday mentation: an experience-sampling study. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 3, 61–79. doi: 10.1002/da.20656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carleton, R. N., Abrams, M. P., and Asmundson, G. J. G. (2010). The attentional resource allocation scale (ARAS): psychometric properties of a composite measure for dissociation and absorption. Depress. Anxiety 27, 775–786. doi: 10.1037/t86316-000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. B., and Putnam, F. W. (1993). An update on the dissociative experiences scale. Dissociation 6, 16–27. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheli, S., MacBeth, A., Popolo, R., and Dimaggio, G. (2020). The intertwined path of perfectionism and self-criticism in a client with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. J. Clin. Psychol. 76, 2055–2066. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00044-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M. E., Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., and Rheaume, J. (2003). “Not just right experiences”: perfectionism, obsessive-compulsive features and general psychopathology. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 681–700. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M. E., and Ravid, A. (2016). Clinical presentation of not-just right experiences (NJREs) in individuals with OCD: characteristics and response to treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 87, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. C., and Olatunji, B. O. (2016). Sleep disturbance and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: results from the national comorbidity survey replication. J. Psychiatr. Res. 75, 41–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. C., and Olatunji, B. O. (2022). Delayed circadian rhythms and insomnia symptoms in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Affect. Disorders 318, 94–102. doi: 10.1037/a0027447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalenberg, C. J., Brand, B. L., Gleaves, D. H., Dorahy, M. J., Loewenstein, R. J., Cardeña, E., et al. (2012). Evaluation of the evidence for the trauma and fantasy models of dissociation. Psychol. Bull. 138, 550–588. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dar, R., Lazarov, A., and Liberman, N. (2021). Seeking proxies for internal states (SPIS): towards a novel model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 147:103987. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722001908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dar, R., Sarna, N., Yardeni, G., and Lazarov, A. (2022). Are people with obsessive-compulsive disorder under-confident in their memory and perception? a review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 52, 2404–2412. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Román, A., Perestelo-Pérez, L., and Buela-Casal, G. (2015). Sleep in obsessive–compulsive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 16, 1049–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ecker, W., and Gönner, S. (2008). Incompleteness and harm avoidance in OCD symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 895–904. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eddy, C. M. (2016). The junction between self and other? temporo-parietal dysfunction in neuropsychiatry. Neuropsychologia 89, 465–477. doi: 10.1037/cns0000218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellickson-Larew, S., Stasik-O’Brien, S. M., Stanton, K., and Watson, D. (2020). Dissociation as a multidimensional transdiagnostic symptom. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 7, 126–150. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engbert, K., Wohlschläger, A., and Haggard, P. (2008). Who is causing what? the sense of agency is relational and efferent triggered. Cognition 107, 693–704. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ertekin, B. A., Kulaksızoğlu, I. B., Ertekin, E., Gürses, C., Bebek, N., Gökyiğit, A., et al. (2009). A comparative study of obsessive–compulsive disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy and idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 14, 634–639. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013-0461-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, B., Speranza, A. M., Dittoni, S., Gnoni, V., Trentini, C., Vergano, C. M., et al. (2014). Memories of attachment hamper EEG cortical connectivity in dissociative patients. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 264, 449–458. doi: 10.1177/070674371105600303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W. (2011). Brain illness and creativity: mechanisms and treatment risks. Can. J. Psychiatry 56, 132–143. doi: 10.1007/s11126-007-9043-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontenelle, L. F., Domingues, A. M., Souza, W. F., Mendlowicz, M. V., de Menezes, G. B., Figueira, I. L., et al. (2007). History of trauma and dissociative symptoms among patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and social anxiety disorder. Psychiatr. Q. 78, 241–250. doi: 10.1177/2167702618797102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fradkin, I., Eitam, B., Strauss, A. Y., and Huppert, J. D. (2019). Thoughts as unexpected intruders: context, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and the sense of agency over Thoughts. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7, 162–180.

Google Scholar

Frischholz, E. J., Braun, B. G., Sachs, R. G., Schwartz, D. R., Lewis, J., Shaeffer, D., et al. (1991). Construct validity of the dissociative experiences scale (DES): i. the relationship between the DES and other self-report measures of DES. Dissociation 4, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D. (2005). The self in action: lessons from delusions of control. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 752–770. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00052-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 357–363. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 14–21. doi: 10.1007/s13164-014-0222-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (2015). Relations between agency and ownership in the case of schizophrenic thought insertion and delusions of control. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 6, 865–879. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentsch, A., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Endrass, T., and Kathmann, N. (2012). Dysfunctional forward model mechanisms and aberrant sense of agency in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 652–659. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giele, C. L., van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., and Dek, E. C. (2014). Paradoxical effects of compulsive perseveration: sentence repetition causes semantic uncertainty. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 3, 35–38. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.727006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giele, C. L., van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Dek, E. C., Hoogers, E. E., and de Wit, K. (2013). Ironic effects of compulsive perseveration. Memory 21, 417–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giele, C. L., van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Dek, E. C., Toffolo, M. B., and Cath, D. C. (2016). Perseveration induces dissociative uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 52, 1–10. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.5.617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giesbrecht, T., Lynn, S. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., and Merckelbach, H. (2008). Cognitive processes in dissociation: an analysis of core theoretical assumptions. Psychol. Bull. 134, 617–647. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giesbrecht, T., Smeets, T., Leppink, J., Jelicic, M., and Merckelbach, H. (2007). Acute dissociation after 1 night of sleep loss. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliani, M., Martoni, R. M., Crespi, S. A., O’Neill, J., Erzegovesi, S., de’Sperati, C., et al. (2021). Did I do that? cognitive flexibility and self-agency in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 304:114170. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2017.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliani, M., Martoni, R. M., Gregori Grgič, R., Crespi, S. A., Cavallini, M. C., and de’Sperati, C. (2017). Who has done it? exploring gaze agency in obsessive-compulsive checkers. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 11:39. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godwin, T. L., Godwin, H. J., and Simonds, L. M. (2020). What is the relationship between fear of self, self-ambivalence, and obsessive–compulsive symptomatology? a systematic literature review. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 27, 887–901. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199205000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goff, D. C., Olin, J. A., Jenike, M. A., Baer, L., and Buttolph, M. L. (1992). Dissociative symptoms in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 180, 332–337. doi: 10.1159/000029105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabe, H. J., Goldschmidt, F., Lehmkuhl, L., Gansicke, M., Spitzer, C., and Freyberger, H. J. (1999). Dissociative symptoms in obsessive-compulsive dimensions. Psychopathology 32, 319–324. doi: 10.1159/000112029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabe, H. J., Ruhrmann, S., Spitzer, C., Josepeit, J., Ettelt, S., Buhtz, F., et al. (2008). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychopathology 41, 129–134.

Google Scholar

Grimaldi, S. J., and Stern, E. R. (2017). Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Phenomenology, Pathophysiology, and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Guidano, V., and Liotti, G. (1983). Cognitive Processes and Emotional Disorders. New York, NY: Guildford Press. doi: 10.1002/cpp.402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gwilliam, P., Wells, A., and Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). Dose meta-cognition or responsibility predict obsessive–compulsive symptoms: a test of the metacognitive model. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 11, 137–144. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 196–207.

Google Scholar

Haug, E. T., Havnen, A., Hansen, B., Bless, J., and Kvale, G. (2013). Attention training with dichotic listening in OCD patients using an iPhone/iPod app. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 10 3(Suppl. 1), 45–47. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinks-Maldonado, T. H., Mathalon, D. H., Houde, J. F., Gray, M., Faustman, W. O., and Ford, J. M. (2007). Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 286–296. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermans, D., Engelen, U., Grouwels, L., Joos, E., Lemmens, J., and Pieters, G. (2008). Cognitive confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder: distrusting perception, attention and memory. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 98–113. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00015-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermans, D., Martens, K., De Cort, K., Pieters, G., and Eelen, P. (2003). Reality monitoring and metacognitive beliefs related to cognitive confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 383–401. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herscu, O., Somer, E., Federman, A., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2023). Mindfulness meditation and self-monitoring reduced maladaptive daydreaming symptoms: a randomized controlled trial of a brief self-guided web-based program. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1300/J229v03n01_06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopper, A., Ciorciari, J., Johnson, G., Spensley, J., Sergejew, A., and Stough, C. (2002). EEG coherence and dissociative identity disorder: comparing EEG coherence in DID hosts, alters, controls and acted alters. J. Trauma Dissociation 3, 75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humpston, C. S., Walsh, E., Oakley, D. A., Mehta, M. A., Bell, V., and Deeley, Q. (2016). The relationship between different types of dissociation and psychosis-like experiences in a non-clinical sample. Conscious. Cogn. 41, 83–92. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0701-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, E. C. M., Sierra, M., and David, A. S. (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealization. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 39, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2004.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacs, K. L., Philbeck, J. W., Barr, W. B., Devinsky, O., and Alper, K. (2004). Obsessive–compulsive symptoms in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 5, 569–574. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ismail, M. A. F., and Shimada, S. (2016). ‘Robot’ hand illusion under delayed visual feedback: relationship between the senses of ownership and agency. PLoS One 11:e0159619. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0563-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivarsson, T., Saavedra, F., Granqvist, P., and Broberg, A. G. (2016). Traumatic and adverse attachment childhood experiences are not characteristic of OCD but of depression in adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 47, 270–280. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jalal, B., McNally, R. J., Elias, J. A., Potluri, S., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2020). “Fake it till you make it”! contaminating rubber hands (“Multisensory stimulation therapy”) to treat obsessive-compulsive disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:414. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2017.1279638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jalal, B., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2017). “I feel your disgust and relief”: can the action understanding system (mirror neuron system) be recruited to induce disgust and relief from contamination vicariously, in individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder symptoms? Neurocase 23, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(94)90014-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipinski, J. F., and Pope, H. G. Jr. (1994). Do “flashbacks” represent obsessional imagery? Compr. Psychiatry 35, 245–247. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalanthroff, E., Linkovski, O., Weinbach, N., Pascucci, O., Anholt, G., and Simpson, H. B. (2017). What underlies the effect of sleep disruption? the role of alertness in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 57, 212–213. doi: 10.1353/ppp.2004.0020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennett, J., and Matthews, S. (2003). The unity and disunity of agency. Philos. Psychiatry Psychol. 10, 305–312. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khosravani, V., Kamali, Z., Jamaati Ardakani, R., and Samimi Ardestani, M. (2017). The relation of childhood trauma to suicide ideation in patients suffering from obsessive-compulsive disorder with lifetime suicide attempts. Psychiatry Res. 255, 139–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00899.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kronholm, E., Sallinen, M., Era, P., Suutama, T., Sulkava, R., and Partonen, T. (2011). Psychomotor slowness is associated with self-reported sleep duration among the general population. J. Sleep Res. 20, 288–297. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafleur, D. L., Petty, C., Mancuso, E., McCarthy, K., Biederman, J., Faro, A., et al. (2011). Traumatic events and obsessive compulsive disorder in children and adolescents: is there a link? J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 513–519. doi: 10.1023/A:1026275916184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavitt, F. (1999). Dissociative experiences scale taxon and measurement of dissociative pathology: does the taxon add to an understanding of dissociation and its associated pathologies? J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 6, 427–440. doi: 10.1002/jclp.1110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavitt, F. (2001). MMPI profile characteristics of women with varying levels of normal dissociation. J. Clin. Psychol. 57, 1469–1477. doi: 10.1177/1073191103256377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, R., and Spei, E. (2004). Relationship of purported measures of pathological and nonpathological dissociation to self-reported psychological distress and fantasy immersion. Assessment 11, 160–168. doi: 10.1080/09540260400003909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, N., and Bayne, T. (2004). Doing without deliberation: automatism, automaticity, and moral accountability. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 16, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, N., and Dar, R. (2018). Obsessive-compulsive tendencies are related to seeking proxies for internal states in everyday life. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 61, 164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipton, M., Brewin, C. R., Linke, S., and Halperin, J. (2010). Distinguishing features of intrusive images in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 24, 816–822. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llorens-Aguilar, S., Arnáez, S., Aardema, F., and García-Soriano, G. (2022). The relationship between obsessions and the self: feared and actual self-descriptions in a clinical obsessive–compulsive disorder sample. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 29, 642–651. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lochner, C., Seedat, S., Hemmings, S. M. J., Kinnear, C. J., Corfield, V. A., Niehaus, D. J. H., et al. (2004). Dissociative experiences in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania: clinical and genetic findings. Compr. Psychiatry 45, 384–391. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, S. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Merckelbach, H., Giesbrecht, T., and van der Kloet, D. (2012). Dissociation and dissociative disorders: challenging conventional wisdom. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 48–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-102424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, S. J., Polizzi, C., Merckelbach, H., Chiu, C.-D., Maxwell, R., van Heugten, D., et al. (2022). Dissociation and dissociative disorders reconsidered: beyond sociocognitive and trauma models toward a transtheoretical framework. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 18, 259–289. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeda, T., Takahata, K., Muramatsu, T., Okimura, T., Koreki, A., Iwashita, S., et al. (2013). Reduced sense of agency in chronic schizophrenia with predominant negative symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 209, 386–392.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mahowald, M. W., and Schenck, C. H. (2001). Evolving concepts of human state dissociation. Arch. Ital. Biol. 139, 269–300. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantovani, A., Simeon, D., Urban, N., Bulow, P., Allart, A., and Lisanby, S. (2011). Temporo-parietal junction stimulation in the treatment of depersonalization disorder. Psychiatry Res. 186, 138–140. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.7.94

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marmar, C. R., Weiss, D. S., Metzler, T. J., and Delucchi, K. (1996). Characteristics of emergency services personnel related to peritraumatic dissociation during critical incident exposure. Am. J. Psychiatry 153(Suppl.), 94–102. doi: 10.1002/da.20316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, C. A., Kaur, N., and Stein, M. B. (2008). Childhood trauma and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Depress. Anxiety 25, 742–751. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(02)00055-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavrogiorgou, P., Juckel, G., Frodl, T., Gallinat, J., Hauke, W., Zaudig, M., et al. (2002). P300 subcomponents in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 36, 399–406. doi: 10.1192/bjp.164.4.459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, P., Bench, C., Frith, C., Marks, I., Frackowiak, R., and Dolan, R. (1994). Functional anatomy of obsessive–compulsive phenomena. Br. J. Psychiatry 164, 459–468. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01959-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meral, Y., Boysan, M., Sandıkçı, T., Çalışkan, Y., Haşimoğlu, A., Doğangün, B., et al. (2021). Relationships between dissociation, obsessive beliefs, and self-esteem in juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder: a case-controlled clinical study. Curr. Psychol. [Epub head of print]. doi: 10.1037/cns0000272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., and Lynn, S. J. (2022). Empirical research on fantasy proneness and its correlates 2000–2018: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 9, 2–26. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200012000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merckelbach, H., and Wessel, I. (2000). Memory for actions and dissociation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 188, 846–848. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0908-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michal, M., Adler, J., Wiltink, J., Reiner, I., Tschan, R., Wölfling, K., et al. (2016). A case series of 223 patients with depersonalization-derealization syndrome. BMC Psychiatry 16:203. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M. L., and Brock, R. L. (2017). The effect of trauma on the severity of obsessive-compulsive spectrum symptoms: a meta-analysis. J. Anxiety Disord. 47, 29–44. doi: 10.1002/da.22363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modirrousta, M., Shams, E., Katz, C., Mansouri, B., Moussavi, Z., Sareen, J., et al. (2015). The efficacy of deep repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the medial prefrontal cortex in obsessive compulsive disorder: results from an open-label study. Depress. Anxiety 32, 445–450. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monaco, F., Cavanna, A., Magli, E., Barbagli, D., Collimedaglia, L., Cantello, R., et al. (2005). Obsessionality, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 7, 491–496. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. W., and Obhi, S. S. (2012). Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 546–561. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A., and De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: a theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132, 297–326. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2010.504727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morsella, E., Berger, C. C., and Krieger, S. C. (2011). Cognitive and neural components of the phenomenology of agency. Neurocase 17, 209–230. doi: 10.1348/014466503321903616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muris, P., and Merckelbach, H. (2003). Thought-action fusion and schizotypy in undergraduate students. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 42, 211–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naftalovich, H., Anholt, G. E., Keren, R., Ben Arush, O., and Kalanthroff, E. (2021). Waxing and waning: the roles of chronotype and time of day in predicting symptom fluctuations in obsessive-compulsive disorder using a daily-monitoring design. J. Psychiatric Res. 143, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naftalovich, H., Tauber, N., and Kalanthroff, E. (2020). But first, coffee: the roles of arousal and inhibition in the resistance of compulsive cleansing in individuals with high contamination fears. J. Anxiety Disorders 76:102316. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900021052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakata, A. C. G., Diniz, J. B., Torres, A. R., De Mathis, M. A., Fossaluza, V., Braganças, C. A., et al. (2007). Level of insight and clinical features of obsessive-compulsive disorder with and without body dysmorphic disorder. CNS Spectrums 12, 295–303.

Google Scholar

Neziroglu, F., and Donnelly, K. (2010). Overcoming Depersonalization Disorder: a Mindfulness and Acceptance Guide to Conquering Feelings of Numbness and Unreality. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199005)46:3<273::AID-JCLP2270460304>3.0.CO;2-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, G. R., Ross, C. A., and Novotny, M. F. (1990). Factors that predict scores on the dissociative experiences scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 46, 273–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nota, J. A., Schubert, J. R., and Coles, M. E. (2016). Sleep disruption is related to poor response inhibition in individuals with obsessive-compulsive and repetitive negative thought symptoms. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 50, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nota, J. A., Sharkey, K. M., and Coles, M. E. (2015). Sleep, arousal, and circadian rhythms in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51, 100–107.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O’Connor, K., and Aardema, F. (2012). Living in a bubble: dissociation, relational consciousness, and obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Conscious. Stud. 19, 216–246. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00042-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, K. P., and Robillard, S. (1995). Inference processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder: some clinical observations. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 887–896. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oren, E., Eitam, B., and Dar, R. (2019). Intentional binding and obsessive-compulsive tendencies: a dissociation between indirect and direct measures of the sense of agency. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 20, 59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oren, E., Friedmann, N., and Dar, R. (2016). Things happen: individuals with high obsessive–compulsive tendencies omit agency in their spoken language. Conscious. Cogn. 42, 125–134. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradisis, S. M., Aardema, F., and Wu, K. D. (2015). Schizotypal, dissociative, and imaginative processes in a clinical OCD sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 71, 606–624. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, J. L., Reynolds, A. C., Ferguson, S. A., and Dawson, D. (2013). Sleep and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Sleep Med. Rev. 17, 465–474. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perona-Garcelán, S., López-Jiménez, A. M., Bellido-Zanin, G., Senín-Calderón, C., Ruiz-Veguilla, M., and Rodríguez-Testal, J. F. (2020). The relationship with the voices as a dialogical experience: the role of self-focused attention and dissociation. J. Clin. Psychol. 76, 549–558. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.59.2.583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persinger, M. A. (1984). Propensity to report paranormal experiences is correlated with temporal lobe signs. Percept. Mot. Skills 59, 583–586. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200208000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persinger, M. A., and Healey, F. (2002). Experimental facilitation of the sensed presence: possible intercalation between the hemispheres induced by complex magnetic fields. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 190, 533–541. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1987.9711068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persinger, M. A., and Makarec, K. (1987). Temporal lobe epileptic signs and correlative behaviors displayed by normal populations. J. Gen. Psychol. 114, 179–195. doi: 10.2466/pms.1985.60.3.903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persinger, M. A., and Valliant, P. M. (1985). Temporal lobe signs and reports of subjective paranormal experiences in a normal population: a replication. Percept. Mot. Skills 60, 903–909. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S212983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozza, A., and Dèttore, D. (2019). “Was it real or did i imagine it?” perfectionistic beliefs are associated with dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 12, 603–607.

Google Scholar

Pozza, A., Torniai, S., and Dèttore, D. (2016). Inferential confusion moderates the effects of dissociative experiences on OCD symptoms severity in a clinical sample with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 13, 108–114.

Google Scholar

Prasko, J., Raszka, M., Adamcova, K., Grambal, A., Koprivova, J., Kudrnovská, H., et al. (2009). Predicting the therapeutic response to cognitive behavioural therapy in patients with pharmacoresistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 30, 615–623. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2010.10401744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proescher, E. J. (2010). Hypnotically facilitated exposure response prevention therapy for an OIF veteran with OCD. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 53, 19–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Putnam, F. (1997). Dissociation in Children and Adolescents. New York, NY: Guilford. doi: 10.1007/BF01074284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pynte, J. (1991). The locus of semantic satiation in category membership decision and acceptability judgment. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 20, 315–335. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachman, S. (2007). Unwanted intrusive images in obsessive compulsive disorders. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 402–410. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(74)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachman, S. J. (1974). Primary obsessional slowness. Behav. Res. Ther. 12, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasgon, A., Lee, W. H., Leibu, E., Laird, A., Glahn, D., Goodman, W., et al. (2017). Neural correlates of affective and non-affective cognition in obsessive compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Eur. Psychiatry 46, 25–32. doi: 10.1111/acps.12323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, A. R., and Parnas, J. (2015). Pathologies of imagination in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 131, 157–161. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.808009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, A. R., Raballo, A., Preti, A., Sæbye, D., and Parnas, J. (2022). Anomalies of imagination, self-disorders, and schizophrenia spectrum psychopathology: a network analysis. Front. Psychiatry 12:808009. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuven-Magril, O., Dar, R., and Liberman, N. (2008). Illusion of control and behavioral control attempts in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 334–341.

Google Scholar

Ross, C. A., Ellason, J. W., and Anderson, G. (1995). A factor analysis of the dissociative experiences scale (DES) in dissociative identity disorder. Dissociation 8, 229–235. doi: 10.1159/000089225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rufer, M., Held, D., Cremer, J., Fricke, S., Moritz, S., Peter, H., et al. (2006a). Dissociation as a predictor of cognitive behavior therapy outcome in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychother. Psychosom. 75, 40–46. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0620-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rufer, M., Fricke, S., Held, D., Cremer, J., and Hand, I. (2006b). Dissociation and symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a replication study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256, 146–150. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, M., Naro, A., Mastroeni, C., Morgante, F., Terranova, C., Muscatello, M. R., et al. (2014). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a “sensory-motor” problem? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röhlinger, J., Wulf, F., Fieker, M., and Moritz, S. (2015). Sensory properties of obsessive thoughts in OCD and the relationship to psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 230, 592–596.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Salkovskis, P. M. (1998). “Psychological approaches to the understanding of obsessional problems,” in Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder, eds R. P. Swinson, M. M. Antony, S. Rachman, and M. A. Richter (New York, NY: Guilford Press). doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomon-Small, G., Somer, E., Harel-Schwarzmann, M., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). Maladaptive daydreaming and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a confirmatory and exploratory investigation of shared mechanisms. J. Psychiatr. Res. 136, 343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Posavac, S. S., Vanous, S., Ho, E. H., and Fazio, R. H. (2007). The deautomatization of accessible attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43, 365–378. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1613200113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasai, S., Boly, M., Mensen, A., and Tononi, G. (2016). Functional split brain in a driving/listening paradigm. PNAS 113, 14444–14449. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, A. (2009). Both motor prediction and conceptual congruency between preview and action-effect contribute to explicit judgment of agency. Cognition 110, 74–83. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01949-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schooler, J. W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 339–344.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Segall, S. R. (1996). Metaphors of agency and mechanism in dissociation. Dissociation 9, 154–159. doi: 10.3109/13651501.2011.617458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selvi, Y., Besiroglu, L., Aydin, A., Gulec, M., Atli, A., Boysan, M., et al. (2012). Relations between childhood traumatic experiences, dissociation, and cognitive models in obsessive compulsive disorder. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 16, 53–59. doi: 10.5152/npa.2015.7116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selvi, Y., Kiliç, S., Aydin, A., and Güzel Özdemir, P. (2015). The effects of sleep deprivation on dissociation and profiles of mood, and its association with biochemical changes. Arch. Neuropsychiatry 52, 83–88. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0787-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semiz, U. B., Inanc, L., and Bezgin, C. H. (2014). Are trauma and dissociation related to treatment resistance in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder? Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 49, 1287–1296. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafran, R., and Rachman, S. (2004). Thought-action fusion: a review. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 35, 87–107. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(96)00018-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., and Rachman, S. (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 10, 379–391. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00015-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierra, M., and Berrios, G. E. (1998). Depersonalization: neurobiological perspectives. Biol. Psychiatry 44, 898–908. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierra, M., and David, A. S. (2011). Depersonalization: a selective impairment of self-awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 99–108. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierra, M., Senior, C., Dalton, J., McDonough, M., Bond, A., Phillips, M. L., et al. (2002). Autonomic response in depersonalization disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 833–838. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvestri, R., and Bromfield, E. (2004). Recurrent nightmares and disorders of arousal in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Res. Bull. 63, 369–376. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181aaef6b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simeon, D., Giesbrecht, T., Knutelska, M., Smith, R. J., and Smith, L. M. (2009). Alexithymia, absorption, and cognitive failures in depersonalization disorder: a comparison to posttraumatic stress disorder and healthy volunteers. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 197, 492–498. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Hazlett, E. A., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Hollander, E., and Buchsbaum, M. S. (2000). Feeling unreal: a PET study of depersonalization disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 1782–1788.

Google Scholar

Smyser, C. H., and Baron, D. A. (1993). Hypnotizability, absorption, and subscales of the dissociative experiences scale in a nonclinical population. Dissociation 6, 42–46. doi: 10.1037/cns0000023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N. (2014). Dissociation and dissociative mechanisms in panic disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and depression: a review and heuristic framework. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 1, 243–270.

Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017a). Daily elevations in dissociative absorption and depersonalization in a non-clinical sample are related to daily stress and psychopathological symptoms. Psychiatry 80, 265–278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017b). Arousal in nocturnal consciousness: how dream- and sleep- experiences may inform us of poor sleep quality, stress, and psychopathology. Front. Psychol. 8:733. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N. (2019). Dissociative absorption, mind-wandering, and attention deficit symptoms: associations with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 58, 51–69. doi: 10.4324/9781003326588-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N. (2023). “Psychoanalysis and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a wittgensteinian-existential perspective,” in Psychoanalytic Investigations in Philosophy: an Interdisciplinary Exploration of Current Existential Challenges, ed. D. Lemberger (Milton Park: Routledge). doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N., Lassri, D., Soffer-Dudek, N., and Shahar, G. (2015). Dissociative absorption: an empirically unique, clinically relevant, dissociative factor. Conscious. Cogn. 36, 338–351. doi: 10.1037/a0022941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N., and Shahar, G. (2011). Daily stress interacts with trait dissociation to predict sleep-related experiences in young adults. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 719–729. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N., Shelef, L., Oz, I., Levkovsky, A., Erlich, I., and Gordon, S. (2017). Absorbed in sleep: dissociative absorption as a predictor of sleepiness following sleep deprivation in two high-functioning samples. Conscious. Cogn. 48, 161–170. doi: 10.4324/9781003057314-41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N., and Somer, E. (2022). “Maladaptive daydreaming is a dissociative disorder: supporting evidence and theory,” in Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: Past, Present, Future, 2nd Edn, eds M. J. Dorahy, S. N. Gold, and J. A. O’Neil (Milton Park: Taylor & Francis). doi: 10.1111/jopy.12391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffer-Dudek, N., Todder, D., Shelef, L., Deutsch, I., and Gordon, S. (2019). A neural correlate for common trait dissociation: decreased EEG connectivity is related to dissociative absorption. J. Pers. 87, 295–309. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M. (1997). The Neuropsychology of Dreams: a Clinico-Anatomical Study. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. doi: 10.1023/A:1020597026919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somer, E. (2002). Maladaptive daydreaming: a qualitative inquiry. J. Contemp. Psychother. 32, 195–210. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., and Ross, C. A. (2017). The comorbidity of daydreaming disorder (maladaptive daydreaming). J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 205, 525–530. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speckens, A. E. M., Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., and Cuthbert, B. (2007). Imagery special issue: intrusive images and memories of earlier adverse events in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 411–422. doi: 10.1080/00048670701449146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Freyberger, H. J., and Grabe, H. J. (2007). Dissociation predicts symptom-related treatment outcome in short-term inpatient psychotherapy. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 41, 682–687.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Stiglmayr, C. E., Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Bretz, J., Behm, R., Mohse, M., Lammers, C. H., et al. (2008). Dissociative symptoms are positively related to stress in borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 117, 139–147. doi: 10.1177/1073191102009001011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stockdale, G. D., Gridley, B. E., Balogh, D. W., and Holtgraves, T. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of single- and multiple-factor competing models of the dissociative experiences scale in a nonclinical sample. Assessment 9, 94–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturm, V., Lenartz, D., Koulousakis, A., Treuer, H., Herholz, K., Klein, J. C., et al. (2003). The nucleus accumbens: a target for deep brain stimulation in obsessive–compulsive- and anxiety-disorders. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 26, 293–299. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synofzik, M., Their, P., Leube, D. T., Schlotterbeck, P., and Lindner, A. (2010). Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one’s actions. Brain 133, 262–271. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., and Newen, A. (2008). I move, therefore i am: a new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 411–424. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., and Voss, M. (2013). The experience of agency: an interplay between prediction and postdiction. Front. Psychol. 4:127. doi: 10.1007/s13164-017-0371-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szalai, J. (2019). The sense of agency in OCD. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 10, 363–380. doi: 10.1007/s12264-013-1370-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L., Fan, Q., You, C., Wang, J., Dong, Z., Wang, X., et al. (2013). Structural changes in the gray matter of unmedicated patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neurosci. Bull. 29, 642–864. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapal, A., Oren, E., Dar, R., and Eitam, B. (2017). The sense of agency scale: a measure of consciously perceived control over one’s mind, body, and the immediate environment. Front. Psychol. 8:1552. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapancı, Z., Yıldırım, A., and Boysan, M. (2018). Neurological soft signs, dissociation and alexithymia in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and healthy subjects. Psychiatry Res. 260, 90–97. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2018.16.2.161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatlı, M., Cetinkaya, O., and Maner, F. (2018). Evaluation of relationship between obsessive-compulsive disorder and dissociative experiences. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 16, 161–167. doi: 10.1037/h0036681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tellegen, A., and Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-absorbing experiences (“absorption”): a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 83, 268–277. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thalbourne, M. A., and Maltby, J. (2008). Transliminality, thin boundaries, unusual experiences, and temporal lobe lability. Pers. Individ. Differ. 44, 1617–1623. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timpano, K. R., Carbonella, J. Y., Bernert, R. A., and Schmidt, N. B. (2014). Obsessive compulsive symptoms and sleep difficulties: exploring the unique relationship between insomnia and obsessions. J. Psychiatr. Res. 57, 101–107. doi: 10.1111/acps.12387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tursich, M., Ros, T., Frewen, P. A., Kluetsch, R. C., Calhoun, V. D., and Lanius, R. A. (2015). Distinct intrinsic network connectivity patterns of post-traumatic stress disorder symptom clusters. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 132, 29–38.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tzelgov, J. (1999). Automaticity and processing without awareness. Psyche 5, 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., de Boer, C., du Bois, A., and Dek, E. (2008). Perseverative and compulsive-like staring causes uncertainty about perception. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1300–1304. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Smeets, M., Dek, E. C. P., Turksma, K., and Saric, R. (2009). Uncertainty about perception and dissociation after compulsive-like staring: time course of effects. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 535–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hout, M. A., Van Dis, E. A. M., Van Woudenberg, C., and Van de Groep, I. H. (2019). OCD-like checking in the lab: a meta-analysis and improvement of an experimental paradigm. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 20, 39–49.

Google Scholar

van der Hart, O. (1996). Discussion of: “metaphors of agency and mechanisms in dissociation”. Dissociation 9, 164–166. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2004.01480.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Hart, O., Nijenhuis, E., Steele, K., and Brown, D. (2004). Trauma-related dissociation: conceptual clarity lost and found. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 38, 906–914. doi: 10.1037/a0024781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kloet, D., Giesbrecht, T., Lynn, S. J., Merckelbach, H., and de Zutter, A. (2012a). Sleep normalization and decrease in dissociative experiences: evaluation in an inpatient sample. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 140–150. doi: 10.1177/1745691612437597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kloet, D., Merckelbach, H., Giesbrecht, T., and Lynn, S. J. (2012b). Fragmented sleep, fragmented mind: the role of sleep in dissociative symptoms. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 159–175. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-302905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kruijs, S. J. M., Bodde, N. M. G., Carrette, E., Lazeron, R. H. C., Vonck, K. E. J., Boon, P. A. J. M., et al. (2014). Neurophysiological correlates of dissociative symptoms. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 85, 174–179.

Google Scholar

van Dis, E. A. M., and van den Hout, M. A. (2016). Not just right experiences as ironic result of perseverative checking. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 13, 100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Heugten-van der Kloet, D., Giesbrecht, T., and Merckelbach, H. (2015). Sleep loss increases dissociation and affects memory for emotional stimuli. J. Behav Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 47, 9–17. doi: 10.1111/papt.12279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vannikov-Lugassi, M., Shalev, H., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). From brooding to detachment: rumination longitudinally predicts an increase in depersonalization and derealization. Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 94, 321–338. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vannikov-Lugassi, M., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018a). No time like the present: thinking about the past and the future is related to state dissociation among individuals with high levels of psychopathological symptoms. Front. Psychol. 9:2465.

Google Scholar

Vannikov-Lugassi, M., and Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018b). Rumination and dissociation: the mediating role of poor sleep quality and pre-sleep cognitions. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 5, 185–211. doi: 10.1037/cns0000128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veale, D. (1993). Classification and treatment of obsessional slowness. Br. J. Psychiatry 162, 198–203. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.2.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, H. A., van Minnen, A., van Megen, H., Eikelenboom, M., Hoogendoorn, A. W., Kaarsemaker, M., et al. (2014). The relationship between adverse childhood experiences and symptom severity, chronicity, and comorbidity in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 1034–1039. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., and Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain 133, 3104–3112. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: individual differences in sleep-related experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. J. Abnorm Psychol. 110, 526–535. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.4.526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Wu, K. D., and Cutshall, C. (2004). Symptom subtypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder and their relation to dissociation. J. Anxiety Disord. 18, 435–458. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(03)00029-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, G. W. (1963). Highway hypnosis: an hypothesis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 11, 143–151. doi: 10.1080/00207146308409239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yıldırım, A., and Boysan, M. (2019). A theoretical integration within obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and dissociative spectrums: obsessional dissociation. Sleep Hypn. 21, 23–37. doi: 10.5350/Sleep.Hypn.2019.21.0169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zermatten, A., van der Linden, M., Larøi, F., and Ceschi, G. (2006). Reality monitoring and motor memory in checking-prone individuals. J. Anxiety Disord. 20, 580–596. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsila, Á, Urbán, R., McCutcheon, L. E., and Demetrovics, Z. (2019). A path analytic review of the association between psychiatric symptoms and celebrity worship: the mediating role of maladaptive daydreaming and desire for fame. Pers. Individ. Differ. 151:109511. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obsessive-compulsive disorder, dissociation, absorption, agency, self, embodiment, attention, imagery

Citation: Soffer-Dudek N (2023) Obsessive-compulsive symptoms and dissociative experiences: Suggested underlying mechanisms and implications for science and practice. Front. Psychol. 14:1132800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1132800

Received: 27 December 2022; Accepted: 03 March 2023;
Published: 22 March 2023.

Edited by:

Pilar Flores, University of Almería, Spain

Reviewed by:

Ana Sánchez-Kuhn, University of Almería, Spain
Santiago Mora, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Copyright © 2023 Soffer-Dudek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nirit Soffer-Dudek, soffern@bgu.ac.il

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.