Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 13 February 2023
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploring Human Subjectivity View all 6 articles

Editorial: Exploring human subjectivity

  • 1Centro Universitario de la Ciénega, Universidad de Guadalajara, Guadalajara, Mexico
  • 2Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED), Madrid, Spain
  • 3Facultat de Psicologia, Ciències de l'Educació i l'Esport Blanquerna, Universitat Ramon Llull, Barcelona, Spain

Editorial on the Research Topic
Exploring human subjectivity

In their eagerness to systematize knowledge, the social sciences have come up against a prima facie insurmountable obstacle throughout their history: subjectivity. From Wittgenstein's problem of private language (Wittgenstein, 1953; Kripke, 2004) to the impossibility of interpersonal comparisons of utility (Lemieux, 2022), to the controversy over the measurement of happiness (Gardiner et al., 2020), or the debate over the very possibility of measuring psychological phenomena (Michell, 1999), the specter of subjectivity is always present when it comes to transcending the gulf between one individuality and another.

It can be said, therefore, that (inter)subjectivity is the frontier of the social sciences, which makes it an apt subject for this issue of Frontiers.

The texts in this issue reflect the breadth and scope of the problem of subjectivity in science, in its empirical, methodological and theoretical aspects. Beginning with the latter, the theoretical article of the issue addresses subjectivity from one of its facets, the study of consciousness. In recent decades the materialist assumptions underlying positivist scientific practice have been sharply questioned for their inability to explain consciousness (the “hard problem,” Melloni et al., 2021) without appealing to ontological dualism. From these questionings has emerged the alternative of the ontological primacy of consciousness; that is, that the ultimate foundation of reality is consciousness (and, hence, subjectivity), from which matter is derived (Bitbol, 2008). This position frames the article “Is Consciousness First in Virtual Reality?”, which takes virtual reality (VR) as a paradigm for conceiving reality as a product of consciousness via perception and explores the requirements and implications of this assumption (Slater and Sanchez-Vives).

Subjectivity is present at the methodological level as the challenge of individualizing and measuring “objectively” (i.e., independent of perception) the subjective aspects of the human condition, from personality traits to experiences or meanings. The authors of “Rigorous idiography: Exploring subjective and idiographic data with rigorous methods—The method of derangements” relate this contrast between subjectivity and objectivity to two others common in the psychological literature: between the idiographic (information about a single individual) and the nomothetic (information that allows the individual to be compared with peers on a given dimension), and between the qualitative and the quantitative (Evans et al.). The aim of his article is to bridge the gap between these poles by proposing an elegant but statistically rigorous method for testing whether purely subjective, idiographic and qualitative data contain reliable and objective information: the method of derangements.

Another way to bridge this gap has been the use of mixed techniques, among which the Repertory Grid technique (Osterberg-Kaufmann, 2022) stands out. The brief report “Self-concept 6 months after traumatic brain injury and its relationship with emotional functioning” shows that this technique is able to demonstrate changes in the self-concept of patients who have suffered traumatic brain injuries, correlating them with life satisfaction, anxiety and quality of life (Mascialino et al.).

The influence of subjectivity on behavior is analyzed in “Going green is exhausting for dark personalities but beneficial for the light ones: An experience sampling study that examines the subjectivity of pro-environmental behavior,” an article that makes use of ecological momentary assessment, which allows for the collection of informants' experiences as they emerge naturally from the context (Kesenheimer and Greitemeyer). The authors show that people with pro-environmental attitudes and/or “light” personality (faith in humanity, humanism and kantism) not only behave in a pro-environmental way in daily life but also perceive pro-environmental behaviors as less burdensome and more advantageous than people with “dark” personality (Machiavellianism, sadism, narcissism or psychopathy).

The last text of the monograph goes into the field of subjectivity par excellence: psychotherapy. The aim of “Identification of dynamic patterns of personal positions in a patient diagnosed with borderline personality disorder and the therapist during change episodes of the psychotherapy” is to study the processes of subjective transformation of a patient diagnosed with borderline personality disorder (Mellado et al.). To this end, the authors identify key episodes of change (defined as transformations in the patient's subjective theories of self) and, within these, the self-states that both the patient and the therapist go through (as judged by the dialogic positions they adopt vis-à-vis the interlocutor). In this way, they present evidence for the existence of dynamic interactive patterns between patient and therapist positions and their impact on the latter's improvement.

As can be seen, the five articles in this issue address the problem of subjectivity from different angles, in various fields and with different methodologies, so we are sure that they will be of interest to researchers who dare to tread the tempestuous sea of subjectivity in science.

Author contributions

EO wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bitbol, M. (2008). Is consciousness primary? NeuroQuantology 6, 53–72. doi: 10.14704/nq.2008.6.1.157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, G., Lee, D., Baranski, E., Funder, D., and Members of the International Situations Project (2020). Happiness around the world: a combined etic-emic approach across 63 countries. PLoS ONE 15, e0242718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kripke, S. A. (2004). Wittgenstein on Rules and Private Language: An Elementary Exposition (Reprinted. ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

Lemieux, P. (2022). Interpersonal Comparisons of Utility. Econlib. Available online at: https://www.econlib.org/interpersonal-comparisons-of-utility/ (accessed January 15, 2023).

Melloni, L., Mudrik, L., Pitts, M., and Koch, C. (2021). Making the hard problem of consciousness easier. Science 372, 911–912. doi: 10.1126/science.abj3259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michell, J. (1999). Measurement in Psychology: A Critical History of a Methodological Concept. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511490040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osterberg-Kaufmann, N. (2022). Innovating empirical research on legitimacy: repertory grid analysis. Front. Polit. Sci. 4, 832250. doi: 10.3389/fpos.2022.832250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations: The German Text, with a Revised English Translation. Oxford: Blackwell.

Keywords: subjectivity, linguistics, computing, artificial intelligence, machine learning, natural language processing, communality, neuropsychology

Citation: Ortiz EL, Saúl LA and Botella L (2023) Editorial: Exploring human subjectivity. Front. Psychol. 14:1146775. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146775

Received: 17 January 2023; Accepted: 31 January 2023;
Published: 13 February 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Ezequiel A. Di Paolo, IKERBASQUE Basque Foundation for Science, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Ortiz, Saúl and Botella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Esteban Laso Ortiz, yes esteban.laso@academicos.udg.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.