Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 06 June 2023
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic The Attentional Boost Effect and Related Phenomena: New insights Into the Relation Between Attention and Memory View all 13 articles

Editorial: The attentional boost effect and related phenomena: new insights into the relation between attention and memory

  • 1Department of Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Law, Economics, Politics and Modern Languages, Libera Università Maria Ss. Assunta, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Economy, Mercatorum University, Rome, Italy
  • 4Department of Psychology, Whitman College, Walla Walla, WA, United States

The relation between attention and memory has been unanimously recognized as a key theme of modern cognitive psychology (e.g., Baddeley et al., 1984; Craik et al., 1996). Starting from this consideration, our Research Topic was aimed at putting together a selected set of papers which illustrate the latest advancements in the field. We were particularly interested in the examination of a phenomenon called the Attentional Boost Effect (ABE) but, as we will show below, the contributions extended to the discussion of a wide range of related findings.

The Research Topic includes a comprehensive review of the latest evidence on ABE and an updated version of the Dual-Task Interaction Model (Swallow et al.), previously proposed by Swallow and Jiang. Briefly, the ABE arises when stimuli encoded with to-be-responded items are remembered better than stimuli encoded with to-be-ignored items. This target-induced enhancement generalizes to both visual material (images: Swallow and Jiang, 2010; Sisk and Lee, 2022) and verbal material (Mulligan et al., 2014; Spataro et al., 2015). The Dual-Task Interaction model suggests that the detection of a target item leads to a transient increase in the amount of resources devoted to the perceptual processing of co-occurring stimuli, likely instantiated by the phasic release of norepinephrine from the locus coeruleus. The Dual-Task Interaction Model 2.0 maintains these tenets, but additionally proposes that the perceptual boost occurs whenever the state of the world does not coincide with the state of the neurocognitive system and a response is needed to bring them back into alignment. In addition, to account for emerging results showing that the ABE can extend to the encoding of contextual details (Turker and Swallow, 2019, 2022; Spataro et al., 2020, 2022; Mulligan et al., 2021), the model assumes that the target-related boost may enhance the formation of bound, multi-item representations in the MTL.

Consistent with the notion that subcortical noradrenergic structures play a key role in the ABE, the fMRI study by Moyal et al. found that auditory target detection produced broad physiological and neural effects. These include increases in phasic pupil responses, increases in the activation of the locus coeruleus and the ventral visual cortex, enhancements of the multi-voxel pattern classification of image category in the fusiform gyrus and parahippocampal gyrus, and enhancements in functional connectivity between the ventral visual cortex and the hippocampus. The latter result seems particularly relevant, because it suggests that the ABE-related manipulation benefits working memory maintenance and long-term memory encoding by enhancing communication between perceptual and medial temporal regions at behaviorally relevant times (such as when participants need to respond to target items).

An early finding in the ABE literature was that the memory enhancement following target detection was only significant for images that overlapped in time with the targets; no increase was observed when the images were presented 100 ms before or 100 ms after the targets (Swallow and Jiang, 2011). The study by Shimane et al. used a similar paradigm, in which participants responded to visual Go or No-go cues by pressing a key (the motor task) or by counting (the cognitive task). After each cue, two images were presented (a pre-item and a post-item). Memory for these images was later tested in a surprise recognition task. In line with the Swallow and Jiang (2011) conclusions, the results showed no significant difference in the recognition of pre- and post-items between Go and No-go trials in the motor task. However, in the cognitive task, post-items were better recognized in the Go than in the No-go trials. Moreover, No-go post-items were better memorized in the motor task than in the cognitive task. Jointly, these findings suggest that (a) in some conditions (i.e., with a non-motor task), target presentation may enhance memory for stimuli presented after its disappearance, and (b) covert motor engagement and response inhibition in No-go trials may promote memory encoding for task-irrelevant stimuli.

Another interesting finding in the ABE literature was that the memory enhancement induced by target detection may be reduced for distinctive stimuli that are already subject to heightened attention during an early encoding phase. Interactive effects of this type have been previously reported by Mulligan et al. (2014) for low-frequency words (but see Prull, 2019; for a different conclusion) and by Spataro et al. (2015) for words with rare orthographic features. An exception to this pattern has been reported in this Research Topic by LaPointe et al. They showed that perceptual degradation and target detection had significant, but independent effects on recognition memory, such that the ABE was similar in magnitude for clear and blurry words. Although these results are not necessarily inconsistent with the early-phase elevated attention hypothesis of the ABE, they nonetheless suggest that further research is needed to understand which manipulations are structurally redundant with the ABE.

As discussed above, the contributions included in this Research Topic cover a wide range of phenomena that are not limited to the ABE. Glicksohn et al., for example, demonstrated that the encoding of visual objects benefited from the association with unusual sounds, and that reactivating these sounds strengthened the entire multisensory representation, resulting in better memory for contextual details (such as the objects' locations). Muhmenthaler and Meier showed that objects presented during switch trials (i.e., trials in which participants had to switch between different classification tasks) were recognized worse than those presented during repeat trials, that the effect was still robust after a 1-week delay and that it was mainly due to recollection processes. Yu et al. used a three-phase sequential paradigm and found that the recognition of semantically-encoded words reduced the incidental encoding of new “foil” words, as compared to the recognition of orthographically-encoded words. They suggested that the detrimental effect occurred because semantic tasks relied primarily on recollection processes, whereas non-semantic tasks relied more strongly on familiarity processes. Lastly, a three-phase sequential paradigm was also adopted by Zhao et al., who reported that objects encoded with self-referential cues were recognized better than objects encoded with other-referential cues.

In conclusion, while we are still far from having a comprehensive understanding of the cognitive and neural underpinnings of the complex interactions between attention and memory, the studies briefly summarized in this editorial represent a promising starting point that should motivate enduring research efforts.

Author contributions

PS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M., and Thomson, N. (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. J. Exper. Psychol. 113, 518–540. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., and Anderson, N. D. (1996). The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. J. Exper. Psychol. 125, 159–180. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, N. W., Spataro, P., and Picklesimer, M. (2014). The attentional boost effect with verbal materials. J. Exper. Psychol. 40, 1049–1063. doi: 10.1037/a0036163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, N. W., Spataro, P., Rossi-Arnaud, C., and Wall, A. R. (2021). The attentional boost effect and source memory. J. Exper. Psychol. 48, 1725–1737. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prull, M. W. (2019). The attentional boost effect for words in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 34, 405–417. doi: 10.1037/pag0000337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sisk, C. A., and Lee, V. G. (2022). Concurrent target detection is associated with better memory for object exemplars. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 29, 159–168. doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-01983-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spataro, P., Mulligan, N. W., and Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2015). Limits to the attentional boost effect: The moderating influence of orthographic distinctiveness. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 987–992. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0767-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spataro, P., Mulligan, N. W., Saraulli, D., and Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2022). The attentional boost effect facilitates the encoding of contextual details: New evidence with verbal materials and a modified recognition task. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 84, 1489–1500. doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02509-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spataro, P., Saraulli, D., Cestari, V., Mulligan, N. W., Santirocchi, A., Borowiecki, O., et al. (2020). The attentional boost effect enhances the recognition of bound features in short-term memory. Memory 28, 926–937. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1801752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swallow, K. M., and Jiang, Y. V. (2010). The attentional boost effect: Transient increases in attention to one task enhance performance in a second task. Cognition 115, 118–132. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swallow, K. M., and Jiang, Y. V. (2011). The role of timing in the attentional boost effect. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 389–404. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0045-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turker, H. B., and Swallow, K. M. (2019). Attending to behaviorally relevant moments enhances incidental relational memory. Memory Cogn. 47, 1–16. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0846-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turker, H. B., and Swallow, K. M. (2022). Diffusion Decision Modeling of Retrieval Following the Temporal Selection of Behaviorally Relevant Moments. Comput. Brain Behav. 5, 302–325. doi: 10.1007/s42113-022-00148-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention, memory, cognition, attentional boost effect, recognition

Citation: Rossi-Arnaud C, Saraulli D, Spataro P and Prull M (2023) Editorial: The attentional boost effect and related phenomena: new insights into the relation between attention and memory. Front. Psychol. 14:1226596. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1226596

Received: 21 May 2023; Accepted: 24 May 2023;
Published: 06 June 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Bernhard Hommel, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Germany

Copyright © 2023 Rossi-Arnaud, Saraulli, Spataro and Prull. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pietro Spataro, pietro.spataro@unimercatorum.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.