- 1Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Facultad de Lenguas y Educación, Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- 2Department of Clinical and Experimental Psychology, University of Huelva, Huelva, Spain
- 3Department of Quantitative Methods, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, Sevilla, Spain
- 4Health Research Institute, University of Canberra, Canberra, ACT, Australia
Background: Curiosity is crucial across various domains, from clinical to educational fields, and holds potential for psychological interventions. Accurate definition and assessment of curiosity are essential for understanding its role and utility. Traditional approaches like factor analysis may not fully capture the construct's nuances.
Objectives: This study aims to reassess the psychometric properties of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II) using Network Analysis.
Methods: A total of 849 Spanish undergraduate students participated in the study. Descriptive analysis, partial-correlation network analysis with gender invariance testing, and Bayesian network model estimation were conducted.
Results: The findings indicate that the CEI-II is best conceptualized as a stable, one-dimensional model, consistent with prior research. The partial-correlation network exhibited moderate density and was invariant in structure, centrality measures, and edge strength across genders, although global strength differed. The Bayesian network identified key pathways for designing interventions based on curiosity.
Conclusions: While the results revealed three distinct item groupings based on centrality measures—challenge-seeking (specific curiosity), novelty-seeking (diversive curiosity), and a combination of both—the empirical evidence supported a stable unidimensional network structure. Items related to specific curiosity showed stronger interconnections, highlighting their importance in fostering curiosity-driven behaviors. These insights suggest that interventions targeting key items may enhance curiosity, and accounting for gender differences could further improve effectiveness.
1 Introduction
A century of research highlights that curiosity has been a topic of interest for psychologists, philosophers and educational scientists (Kidd and Hayden, 2015). This has showed that curiosity is vital across various domains of life. In clinical terms, curiosity predicts reduction in anxiety and depression symptoms (Zainal and Newman, 2022), and improved emotional mood (Lydon-Staley et al., 2020). In the workplace, curiosity predict entrepreneurial and workplace innovation (Celik et al., 2016; Peljko et al., 2016; Chang and Shih, 2019) and it is linked to better overall performance (Kashdan et al., 2020). Socially, it is associated with increased positive affect and interpersonal attractiveness (Kashdan and Roberts, 2004). And lastly, in educational contexts, curiosity contributes to higher grades and lower dropout rates (Wavo, 2004; Singh and Manjaly, 2022; Kashdan and Yuen, 2007). Crucially, sociodemographic factors can influence curiosity; for instance, interest in new experiences may declines with age (Mascherek and Zimprich, 2012; Sakaki et al., 2018). Furthemore, regarding sex-at-birth, women typically display higher curiosity about people, while men tend to be more curious about objects (Su et al., 2009; Graziano et al., 2011).
The strong connection between curiosity and these positive outcomes described above underscores the need to explore methods for enhancing curiosity. To address this, various interventions have been developed and refined over time. In this regard, Schutte and Malouff (2023) conducted a comprehensive review of 41 randomized controlled trials involving 4,496 participants, finding that curiosity-enhancing (i.e., mindfulness training or game play) interventions effectively increased curiosity with a moderate effect size (Hedges' g = 0.57). In methodological terms, the effectiveness of these interventions is commonly assessed through questionnaires, mainly self-reported instruments designed to measure levels of curiosity. Schutte and Malouff (2023) reported that 80% of studies employed these questionnaires, and Gross et al. (2020) found that nearly 90% of educational interventions aimed at enhancing curiosity also relied on them. Thus, self-report questionnaires are extensively used to assess curiosity levels and are considered essential in the field (Johnson et al., 2023).
In this context, over the last decades several questionnaires have been developed (e.g., Kashdan and Roberts, 2004; Aschieri et al., 2020; Naylor, 1981; Reio et al., 2006) (see Grossnickle, 2016 or Wagstaff et al., 2021, for a review on the topic).
However, this extensive interest in defining the concept and operationalizing it in the form of a questionnaire or scale has resulted in numerous—and often inconsistent—definitions and approaches (Kidd and Hayden, 2015; Grossnickle, 2016). But as recently noted by Jovanović and Gavrilov-Jerković (2014), one of the most widely accepted definitions and explanatory models is the one proposed by Kashdan et al. (2004) and Kashdan et al. (2009) from positive psychology. This model conceptualizes curiosity as a personality trait with two key components: “ Stretching” which refers to the desire to explore and seek out new experiences, and “Embracing” which involves a readiness to accept novel and unfamiliar situations, places, and people (Kashdan et al., 2004, 2009). Given that curiosity drives individuals to seek new experiences and adapt to novelty, it has been described as a positive emotional-motivational system that empowers individuals to thrive and fosters a sense of subjective and psychological wellbeing (Kashdan et al., 2004, 2009).
Following this model, Kashdan et al. (2004) and Kashdan et al. (2009) developed the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory (CEI) which has become one of the most extensively used self-report questionnaires in the field (Balgiu, 2018; Setyowati et al., 2020). However, the original CEI had limitations, notably in capturing the full breadth of the construct and the willingness to manage or embrace the tension associated with novelty and uncertainty (Ye et al., 2015). To address these issues, Kashdan et al. (2009) developed the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II), which includes the two subscales: “Stretching,” which measures the motivation to seek knowledge and new experiences, and “Embracing,” which assesses the acceptance of uncertainty and the unpredictable nature of daily life. The refined CEI-II has showed strong reliability and validity across diverse cultural contexts in North America, Europe, Africa, and Asia (e.g., Johnson et al., 2023; Kashdan et al., 2009; Balgiu, 2018; Ye et al., 2015; Tarilonte-Castaño et al., 2023).
Despite its advantages, the CEI-II faces a significant limitation related to its factor structure (Ye et al., 2015). The high correlation between the “Stretching” and “Embracing” subscales raises concerns about their distinctiveness and practical utility. For instance, recent research by Johnson et al. (2023) reported an inter-factor correlation of 1.011, Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023) found an inter-factor correlation of 0.97 and Kashdan et al. (2009) noted a high correlation of 0.85. Although Setyowati et al. (2020) reported a lower inter-factor correlation of 0.43, the majority of studies show high values, and often leads to high cross-loadings in exploratory factor analysis (Ye et al., 2015). Given these challenges, some authors report a one-dimensional solution for the CEI-II. In this regard, Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023) validated the CEI-II in a sample of Spanish university students, demonstrating that the one-dimensional model exhibits strong psychometric properties and high internal consistency. This conclusion aligns with the traditional latent factor model, which posits that items are influenced by a common underlying construct, such as curiosity, and support causal latent variable theory, which suggests that items are related because they stem from the same latent construct (Costantini et al., 2015).
In addition, Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023) found that CEI-II showed a strict invariance between gender even though some studies highlight gender disparities in curiosity. For instance, Su et al. (2009) conducted a meta-analysis showing that men had stronger interests in Realistic (d = 0.84) and Investigative (d = 0.26) areas, including Engineering (d = 1.11) and Science (d = 0.36), aligning with the “Stretching” component of the CEI-II, which focuses on seeking new experiences. In contrast, women showed higher interests in Artistic (d = −0.35), Social (d = −0.68), and Conventional (d = −0.33). Women's stronger inclination toward Artistic, Social, and Conventional interests might reflect a tendency to engage with and accept the emotional and social aspects of new experiences, aligning with the “Embracing” component. These findings may suggest that the instrument is suitable for exploring gender-based differences in curiosity; however, it is possible that the sensitivity of the analysis is insufficient to detect subtle violations of invariance that could arise at a more granular level. This could be better understood using alternative methods such as network analysis.
Therefore, an alternative to the traditional factorial model is network analysis, which could offer valuable insights into this issue. Unlike classical approaches, network analysis views psychological phenomena as networks of interconnected characteristics (Borsboom and Cramer, 2013). Recent developments in network psychometrics propose an alternative view of psychological constructs, conceptualizing them as systems of interconnected components rather than reflections of latent variables. From this perspective, traits such as curiosity are understood as emergent properties of direct interactions among observed behaviors or cognitions (Cramer, 2012; Epskamp and Fried, 2018).
This method could help clarify whether “Stretching” and “Embracing” are distinct or overlapping by visualizing item connections, identifying core and peripheral items. It may refine the scale, adjust subscale boundaries, and assess if a unidimensional model is more suitable. Additionally, recent advancements in network analysis could facilitate comparisons of network structures across different populations, offering a deeper understanding of psychological constructs (van Borkulo et al., 2022). This approach could also assess the invariance of items and their interrelationships across genders. Finally, among network-based analyses, we also find Bayesian Networks (BN). These are Directed Acyclic Graphs (DAG) that, when combined with conditional probabilities, can identify pathways among variables to uncover predictors or even causal-effect relationships (Kitson et al., 2023). This analysis could provide a clearer idea of which nodes are key to increasing curiosity in educational or clinical settings, among others, in order to design more effective interventions (Briganti et al., 2022).
The aim of the present study is to re-evaluate the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II) using network analysis in a sample of Spanish university students, extending previous work by Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023). Prior findings have highlighted challenges in distinguishing the ‘Stretching' and “Embracing” subscales due to high inter-factor correlations, raising concerns about the discriminant validity of the traditional factorial structure. In this context, network analysis offers a valuable alternative to classical latent variable approaches, as it allows for the direct examination of inter-item relationships without assuming underlying common causes.
By estimating both undirected and directed network structures, the study aims to provide a comprehensive representation of the CEI-II's internal organization. This includes evaluating the dimensional structure, the stability of item associations, and the predictability of individual items within the network. Furthermore, the study explores structural invariance across gender, addressing potential item-level differences in network configuration previously suggested in the literature.
Adopting the network psychometric perspective, the CEI-II is conceptualized not as a reflection of latent traits but as a system of interrelated components whose interactions give rise to curiosity-related behaviors. This approach is expected to yield richer insights into the functioning of the scale in this population and to inform the design of future interventions by identifying highly connected or influential items that may serve as leverage points for behavioral change.
2 Methods
2.1 Recruitment and participants
This study is part of the Health Behavior in University (HBU) project, utilizing a cross-sectional survey approach where participants were recruited using stratified random cluster sampling. For more details of the design and methods conducted in research project, see Andrés-Villas et al. (2020).
The current study included 939 students who consented to participate. After excluding those who did not complete the consent form, minors, age outliers within the university population, and participants/questionnaires with missing values, the final study sample consisted of 849 students. Among them, 74.56% identified as female and 25.44% as male, with an age range of 18–26 years (M = 20.66; SD = 2.151).
2.2 Instruments
The Curiosity and Exploration Inventory (CEI-II), created by Kashdan et al. (2009), consists of a 10-item Likert-type scale with five response options ranging from 1 (“Very little or none”) to 5 (“A lot”). A higher score indicates a greater level of curiosity. The Spanish version was developed using a translation and back-translation process conducted by two bilingual translators. This process was supervised by the Spanish team of the WHO Regional Office for Europe's Health Behavior in School-Aged Children Study (Moreno et al., 2020). Semantic equivalence between the original and back-translated versions was ensured through discussion and revision until all discrepancies were resolved (Hambleton and de Jong, 2003). Under traditional factorial analysis approach this adaptation has showed excellent psychometric properties in terms of reliability and validity in the present sample (Tarilonte-Castaño et al., 2023).
2.3 Statistical Analysis
All the analyses were performed using R version 4.2.3 (R Core Team, 2018).
2.3.1 Descriptive analysis of the items by gender
First, descriptive analyses were conducted to determine the mean and standard deviation of the CEI-II and its items by gender, as well as their statistically significant differences. Due to the multiple pairwise comparisons, a False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) was applied to prevent an increase in Type I error. These analyses were performed using the psych (version 2.2.3), effectsize (version 0.8.9), and dplyr (version 1.1.4) R packages.
2.3.2 Community detection
To evaluate the number, organization, and robustness of the network's dimensional structure, a bootstrap-based Exploratory Graph Analysis (EGA) was performed (Golino et al., 2002). In this approach, variable communities—representing distinct dimensions—are identified by detecting clusters of nodes that exhibit strong mutual associations through connecting edges. These clusters reflect coherent groups of variables that tend to function together. Items were assigned to a given community only if their stability coefficients exceeded 0.70, ensuring reliability in their classification. Items with lower stability were excluded to prevent potential disruption of the dimension's structural integrity (Christensen and Golino, 2021). The R package used for this analysis was EGAnet (version 2.0.2).
2.3.3 Network structure estimation
To estimate the structural configuration of the CEI-II item network, a partial-correlation approach was employed within the framework of Gaussian Graphical Models (GGMs) (Epskamp and Fried, 2018; Epskamp et al., 2018). In this model, edges between nodes represent conditional dependencies that persist after controlling for the influence of all other variables. To enhance interpretability and reduce spurious connections, the network was regularized using the Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO), which systematically eliminates weak associations by shrinking them to zero (Friedman et al., 2008; Van Borkulo et al., 2014). This yields a more parsimonious and robust representation of the network. The reliability of the estimated edge weights was evaluated through the computation of confidence intervals, typically at the 95% level (Epskamp and Fried, 2018; Hevey, 2018). All analyses were conducted using the R packages qgraph (version 1.9.5), for network estimation, and mgm (version 1.2.14), to characterize its predictability power.
2.3.4 Estimation of centrality measures
Centrality indices quantify the importance of nodes within a network, reflecting their relative influence in the model. In this study, four centrality measures were computed: strength, closeness, betweenness, and expected influence (Epskamp and Fried, 2018; Epskamp et al., 2018; Opsahl et al., 2010). Strength represents the sum of edge weights connected to a node, closeness captures the inverse of the total distance from one node to all others, and betweenness measures how frequently a node lies on the shortest paths between other nodes (Epskamp et al., 2018; Opsahl et al., 2010).
To evaluate the reliability of these indices, a Correlation Stability (CS) coefficient was calculated using subset bootstrapping with 1,000 samples. This metric indicates the maximum proportion of observations which can be removed while still maintaining a correlation of 0.70 or higher between the original centrality indices and those of the subsets, with 95% certainty (Epskamp and Fried, 2018). A CS coefficient above 0.25 was considered acceptable, with values above 0.50 indicating good stability (Hevey, 2018; Glück et al., 2017). All analyses were conducted in R using the packages qgraph (version 1.9.5), for centrality estimation, and bootnet (version 1.5.6), for assessing the stability of centrality metrics.
2.3.5 Analysis of invariance in network structure
To assess whether the network structure was invariant across gender groups, a Network Comparison Test (NCT) was conducted (van Borkulo et al., 2022). It is important to note that this test assesses differences in structural associations (i.e., partial correlations between items), not measurement relations as defined in latent variable models. Therefore, the results are interpreted in terms of structural rather than measurement invariance. This procedure evaluates potential differences between groups at four distinct levels: (1) overall network structure, based on the assumption that both networks share the same configuration; (2) global strength, referring to the total level of connectivity across the network; (3) individual edge strength, which examines whether the magnitude of specific connections between nodes differs between male and female networks; and (4) centrality measures, which were compared based on their Correlation Stability (CS) coefficients. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure was applied to correct p-values for multiple testing in the analysis of edge differences. The R package used for this analysis was NetworkComparisonTest (version 2.2.2).
2.3.6 Bayesian network estimation
The development of the Bayesian Network (BN) followed a two-step procedure. The first step involved estimating the underlying Directed Acyclic Graph (DAG), and the second focused on fitting and validating the BN model using the study dataset. DAG estimation was performed using the PC-Stable algorithm without applying any constraints (Scutari, 2010). The algorithm starts with a complete graph and removes edges based on conditional independence tests. V-structures are identified, and directional edges are added if no cycles exist, based on correlation data, which limits causal conclusions (Li et al., 2019; Spirtes and Glymour, 1991). To enhance the robustness of the resulting structure, 200 bootstrap samples were generated, and only edges with a selection frequency above 0.85 and a directionality >0.5 were retained in the final graph (Briganti et al., 2022).
In the second step, the dataset was subjected to a 10-fold cross-validation procedure to fit and validate the BN model. In each iteration, 90% of the data was used for model training and the remaining 10% for testing, repeating this process until all fold combinations had been evaluated.
The implementation of this procedure was carried out using the bnlearn R package (version 4.9) for DAG estimation, dismo (version 1.3-14) for generating the k-fold partitions, and caret (version 6.0.94) for evaluating the predictive performance of the model.
2.3.7 Predictive performance
Model performance in terms of predictive accuracy and error was evaluated using two metrics: the coefficient of determination (R2) and the root-mean-square error (RMSE). These measures were applied to both the partial-correlation networks and the Bayesian Network (BN) model, providing insights into both absolute and relative accuracy. In the partial-correlation framework, a node's predictability was estimated by treating all other nodes in the network as potential predictors (Haslbeck and Fried, 2017). In contrast, within the BN approach, the predictability of a target node (child) was assessed based solely on its direct parent nodes, as defined by the directional connections identified during DAG construction.
Predictive accuracy and error for each node were calculated as the average values across all iterations of the cross-validation procedure implemented in the second phase of the BN modeling. In both modeling approaches, predictability was visually represented in the network diagrams using node borders, with variations in appearance indicating different levels of explained variance. Performance metrics were obtained using the mgm package (version 1.2.14) for the GGM and the caret package (version 6.0.94) for the BN model.
For additional detail on the use of these metrics in network analysis and Bayesian networks, see Scutari (2010), Borsboom et al. (2021), and Haslbeck et al. (2021).
3 Results
3.1 Descriptive statistics
Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations by gender for the 10 items and the overall scale score. Statistically significant differences (p < 0.05) were identified in every item except for items 1 and 10. Females scored higher on items 2 and 8 but lower on the rest. Regarding the total score, significant differences were found, with males scoring higher than females, although the effect size was small.
3.2 Network analysis
3.2.1 Bootstrap exploratory graph analysis
The bootstrap exploratory graph analysis identified a one-community solution with 100% probability. Furthermore, all items loaded onto this single community with 100% stability, as supported by the 95% confidence intervals of the edges—indicating both strong interpretability and high accuracy of the network structure (Figure 1). Regarding the centrality measures, the strength, closeness, and betweenness of the obtained network were found to be stable, with CS-coefficients of 0.75 (min = 0.673; max = 1), 0.595 (min = 0.517; max = 0.673), and 0.439 (min = 0.362; max = 0.517), respectively. Therefore, the network structure can be considered unidimensional, and its edges and centrality indices are fully reliable.
Figure 2 displays the partial-correlation network structure of the CEI-II, where almost all identified relationships are represented in blue due to positive correlations among the nodes, except for the edge between nodes 5 and 10, represented in red due to their inverse relationship. The network density was 55.56% (25 non-zero edges out of 45 possible edges) and showed an average weight of 0.164, with a minimum of −0.11 and a maximum of 0.34. The most robust connections were identified between item 5 and items 3 and 4, both with weights >0.3, while the least robust connection, in absolute values, was between item 5 and item 7, with a value lower than 0.1.

Figure 2. Partial-correlation network showing relationships between items of CEI-II. R2 is represented with orange borders.
Regarding centrality measures (Figure 3), item 5 exhibits the highest strength value, closely followed by item 9. In terms of betweenness, item 3 has the highest value, followed by items 5, 8, and 9, which hold most of the indirect paths among nodes in the network. For closeness, the highest score was obtained by item 9, followed by items 3 and 5. Across all indicators, item 1 had the lowest score, making it the least central node in the network. Expected influence scores were very similar to strength due to the low number of negative connections identified among nodes.

Figure 3. Standardized centrality measures of the network (betweenness, closeness, strength and expected influence).
3.2.2 Network structure invariance between males and females
The Network Comparison Test (NCT) revealed that the female network exhibited lower global strength (global strength = 4.157) than the male network (global strength = 4.57), with the difference reaching statistical significance (S = 0.412, p = 0.045), indicating higher overall connectivity in the male network. In contrast, the difference in network structure (M = 0.203, p = 0.21) was not statistically significant, suggesting comparable structural configurations across gender. Additionally, no significant differences were observed in centrality measures (p > 0.5 for all comparisons, Figure 4) or edge strength (Figure 5) after applying the Benjamini-Hochberg correction, with a significance threshold set at 0.05.

Figure 5. Networks of female [(A) panel; N = 633] and male participants [(B) panel; N = 216]. R2 is represented with orange borders.
3.3 Bayesian network
3.3.1 DAG estimation
The BN was used to determine the likely direction of edges within the network. The final BN consisted of a DAG containing 16 directed edges. In this structure, item 5 served as the sole parent node, initiating the network. In contrast, items 1 and 7 functioned exclusively as child nodes, while the remaining items played a dual role, acting both as parent and child nodes within the network (see Figure 6).
3.3.2 Models predictability
The predictability and error for each item in the network were estimated using partial-correlation models for the total of the sample, females and males separately (see Table 2). In every model, item 9 was the one with the highest explained variance and lower RMSE, while item 1 showed the lowest explained variance and highest RMSE. Furthermore, item 1 showed the greatest discrepancy by gender, with a 0.16 in R2 among them, being the female group the one with worse predictability.
Regarding the predictability of the BN model, it showed the highest explained variance for item 7, which receives the most influence from the rest of the network, while the lowest error was for item 4. On the other hand, item 1 exhibits the worst predictability, while item 10 showed the largest error in prediction (see Table 2).
4 Discussion
The aim of the present study was to apply psychometric network analysis to investigate the network model of the Spanish version of the CEI-II, including potential differences between females and males, and to analyze the explanatory power and accuracy of the models. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to apply network analysis to the CEI-II.
4.1 Factor structure and network perspective: complementary approaches
Regarding the factor and network structure, from a quantitative standpoint, the results are best understood as supporting a stable unidimensional model. This finding aligns with the factor structure identified in the analysis conducted by Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023), providing converging evidence for the model. This model is a more parsimonious solution than the one obtained in the original study (Kashdan et al., 2009), demonstrating that these communities are consistent with the latent factors of factor models (Golino and Epskamp, 2017).
This unidimensionality raises questions about the suitability of applying network analysis vs. traditional factorial models. While factor analysis assumes that items reflect an underlying latent variable, network analysis conceptualizes items as dynamic entities that interact with each other. In this sense, the observed unidimensional structure could indicate that the CEI-II items are strongly interconnected and form a cohesive system, justifying the application of network analysis to examine the relationships between items beyond a traditional factorial structure. This finding suggests that curiosity, as measured by the CEI-II, can be examined both as a latent construct and as a system of interacting items, depending on the analytical framework. The network approach emphasizes item-level dynamics and potential intervention targets in relation to theoretical models, supporting their construction and redefinition. As such, these models can be viewed as a complementary analytical perspective to the factor analysis reported by Tarilonte-Castaño et al. (2023), enabling different levels of granularity and facilitating more precise and effective interventions or evaluations.
4.2 Item-level patterns
At the item level, in terms of betweenness (i.e., the extent to which an item lies on the shortest paths between other items), closeness (i.e., the measure of how quickly an item can access other items in the network), and strength (i.e., the sum of the weights of the items connected to an item), three groups of items can be visually identified, attending to the aforementioned metrics. However, it should be noted that this interpretative perspective, at item level, was established post-hoc and does not reflect distinct empirical dimensions. The robust unidimensional solution remains, with the groupings offering a conceptual guide for more targeted interventions based on item content and centrality measures.
First, there are items related with challenge-seeking such as Item 5 (“I view challenging situations as an opportunity to grow and learn”), Item 9 (“I frequently seek out opportunities to challenge myself and grow as a person”), Item 3 (“I am at my best when doing something that is complex or challenging”) and Item 8 (“I prefer jobs that are excitingly unpredictable”). This group of items may align with the “Embracing” dimension of the CEI-II, as outlined by Kashdan et al. (2009).
From an epistemic view, the aforementioned items may comprise the so-called “specific curiosity” (Berlyne, 1960). Specific curiosity can be defined as exploration that leads to a detailed investigation of new stimuli to obtain new information (Schutte and Malouff, 2023; Berlyne, 1960; Litman and Spielberger, 2003). This type of curiosity, built on complexity theories (see Dubey and Griffiths, 2020 for a review), is deliberate, goal-oriented and closely linked to problem-solving, helping to resolve knowledge gaps by acquiring substantive facts (Grossnickle, 2016; Hagtvedt et al., 2019; Harrison et al., 2011; Le Cunff, 2024). Thus, individuals with high specific curiosity are prone to seek out situations and events that are notably ambiguous or uncertain (Kashdan et al., 2009). Ultimately, it has been demonstrated that this process can foster creativity (Hagtvedt et al., 2019).
Second, there are items related with novelty-seeking dimension of curiosity such as Item 7 (“I am always looking for experiences that challenge how I think about myself and the world”), Item 4 (“Everywhere I go, I am out looking for new things or experiences”), and Item 10 (“I am the kind of person who embraces unfamiliar people, events, and places”). This group of items may align with the “Stretching” dimension of CEI-II, as outlined by Kashdan et al. (2009).
The aforementioned items may comprise the so-called “diversive curiosity” (Berlyne, 1960). Diversive curiosity is commonly described as the tendency to seek stimulation regardless of source or content (Berlyne, 1960; Litman and Spielberger, 2003). This type of curiosity, build on novelty theories (see Dubey and Griffiths, 2020 for a review), it is spontaneous, driven by the desire of boredom, novelty or stimulation (Schutte and Malouff, 2023; Kashdan et al., 2009; Litman and Spielberger, 2003; Le Cunff, 2024; Whitecross and Smithson, 2023). Individuals with high diversive curiosity actively are ready to grow and expand, rather than remaining in familiar situation (Chang and Shih, 2019). This type of curiosity can lead to high distractibility, with individuals frequently switching between topics and activities due to sudden interest in something new and unrelated (Whitecross and Smithson, 2023); and ultimately can lead to unlearned situations (Dubey and Griffiths, 2020).
Third, we find items that combine specific curiosity and diversive curiosity such as Item 6 (“I like to do things that are a little frightening”) Item 2 (“I am the type of person who really enjoys the uncertainty of everyday life”) and Item 1 (“I actively seek as much information as I can in new situations”).
Crucially, their weaker centrality measures—such as lower betweenness, closeness and strength—can be interpreted as peripheral items in the network (Hevey, 2018), rather than being key drivers of curiosity. It should be noted for the reader that Item 1 (“I actively seek as much information as I can in new situations”) is the most disconnected in the network. These groupings should be understood as descriptive patterns based on item centrality within a unified curiosity network, not as distinct latent dimensions (Borsboom and Cramer, 2013; Robinaugh et al., 2016). This perspective is compatible with the unidimensional structure identified through EGA (Christensen and Golino, 2021; Golino and Epskamp, 2017). Thus, these groupings are not psychometrically validated subtypes, but rather a conceptual framework that may guide future research and interventions (Fried and Cramer, 2017).
4.3 Gender differences in network structure
In the analysis from a gender perspective, item-level results from the Network Comparison Test (NCT) revealed no significant differences in the overall network structure, centrality measures, or edge strength between males and females. However, a significant difference in global strength was found, with the male network showing greater overall connectivity than the female network. Differences in network structure or global strength between gender groups are interpreted as variations in the organization of item-level associations, consistent with the network psychometric perspective. These findings do not imply violations of measurement invariance in the classical psychometric sense but rather suggest potentially distinct patterns of inter-item activation between groups.
One possible explanation is that males are generally less risk-averse and more tolerant of uncertainty in decision-making, which may result in more feedback loops and interconnected nodes within their curiosity network (Robichaud et al., 2003; Harris and Jenkins, 2006). However, it is important to note that uncertainty is context-dependent, and various situational factors can influence how risk aversion manifests across genders (Schubert et al., 1999). Additionally, research suggests that males tend to be more motivated by goal-directed tasks and exhibit greater exploratory behavior in novel situations compared to females (Sarin and Wieland, 2016). In a recent study, 73.7% of males chose more challenging tasks when given limited options, compared to only 27.5% of females, reflecting higher engagement in challenge-seeking behavior (Bieberstein et al., 2020).
As suggested by Borsboom and Cramer (2013), network analysis allows for interpreting observed variables (e.g., items) not merely as expressions of an underlying latent factor but as components within a causal system characterized by continuous mutual influence. This approach has significant implications for psychological interventions, indicating that targeting central items can enhance therapeutic outcomes.
4.4 Bayesian network, item predictability, and error analysis
In this regard the Bayesian Network (BN) analysis reveals important directional insights. While these structural patterns do not imply causality, the results revealed that viewing challenges as opportunities for growth (Item 5) may function as a “parent” node and occupies a central position within the network structure. Item 1 and Item 7, which involve actively seeking information and new stimulus are categorized as “child” nodes and may play a more peripheral role within the network. The remaining items illustrate a network where various items mutually reinforce one with another.
Given the above, items associated with specific curiosity demonstrate stronger connections and may act as key drivers. In contrast, the remaining items appear more peripheral, indicating that not all dimensions of curiosity are equally integrated into the CEI-II. These items might be particularly useful in interventions aimed at improving curiosity. This needs to be empirically tested, however, as we did not conduct formal redundancy diagnostics. Thus, any considerations about item refinement remain preliminary and should be addressed in future studies using dedicated methods. Interestingly, Schutte and Malouff (Schutte and Malouff, 2023) found in their meta-analysis that interventions targeting general curiosity (n = 2) showed larger effect sizes compared to those focused on specific curiosity (n = 39). Since more interventions focus on specific curiosity, these findings could provide valuable insights for a broad range of studies.
Furthermore, the analysis of predictability and error indicates that Item 9 emerges as the most predictable item, showcasing the highest explained variance and the lowest error. This indicates that it is a central component of the CEI-II. In contrast, Item 1 shows the lowest explained variance and the highest error, particularly within the female sample, suggesting its predictability is influenced by gender differences in information-seeking behavior. Item 7, a “child” node, exhibits the highest explained variance in the BN model, possibly due to receiving the most connections. Lastly, Item 10, shows the largest prediction error, suggesting it may be affected by external factors not adequately captured in the CEI-II network structure.
4.5 Limitations and future directions
Nevertheless, there are several limitations that should be recognized. First, our analyses suggest that the network structure is invariant across genders, supporting the generalizability of the overall pattern of relationships. Differences in edge strengths or prevalence estimates could exist when compared with other university student populations, non-university sectors, or in cultures with different gender distributions. Therefore, the gender imbalance could influence certain aspects of the results. Second, the cross-sectional design of the study prevents us from establishing causal relationships among the items, underscoring the need for longitudinal studies and experimental designs to test the obtained models. Third, the network structure occasionally displays a lack of stability, which may lead to inconsistent findings. Fourth, focusing exclusively on the CEI-II may limit our understanding of curiosity as a multidimensional construct; examining other related (and unrelated) constructs could yield additional evidence of validity. Further studies should consider including motivation types and behaviors related to them (e.g., leisure activities) to integrate this construct into a more complex system that is closer to reality. And lastly, we did not conduct formal redundancy diagnostics, so any considerations regarding item overlap or refinement remain preliminary. Future research should address this using dedicated methods (Christensen et al., 2023).
5 Conclusion
The findings indicate that the CEI-II is best conceptualized as a stable, one-dimensional model, which aligns with previous research. From an interpretative perspective, the results reveal three distinct groups of items regarding their centrality measures: challenge-seeking (specific curiosity), novelty-seeking (diversive curiosity), and a combination of both. Items related with specific curiosity show stronger interconnections within the network, suggesting they play a pivotal role in fostering curiosity-driven behaviors. This suggests that targeting these key items in interventions may significantly enhance individuals' curiosity. In this regard, to design more effective interventions aimed at fostering curiosity, it might be useful to consider the relevance of different items. Specifically, interventions may prioritize Items 5 and 9, which are identified as central to the network, while Items 1 and 10 appear comparatively less relevant. Additionally, it is essential to address the differences in responses between males and females for more tailored interventions.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Research Ethics Committee of Huelva Centers (CEI) of the Junta de Andalucía (0846-N-19/P1027/19). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
SN-L: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PP-M: Writing – review & editing, Supervision. CS: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. DD-M: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the EPIT 2020 project (University of Huelva, Spain) under Grant UHU-6272020, as well as by funding from the EPIT 20242025 academic year.
Acknowledgments
We thank Montserrat Andrés-Villas for making this research project possible and for her continuous support.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Abbreviations
BN, Bayesian Network; CEI-II, Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II; CS, Correlation Stability; DAG, Directed Acyclic Graph; EGA, Exploratory Graph Analysis; FDR, False Discovery Rate; GGMs, Gaussian Graphical Models; HBU, Health Behavior in University; LASSO, Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator; NCT, Network Comparison Test; RMSE, Root-mean-square error; WHO, World Health Organization.
References
Andrés-Villas, M., Díaz-Milanés, D., Remesal-Cobreros, R., Vélez-Toral, M., and Pérez-Moreno, P. J. (2020). Dimensions of leisure and perceived health in young university students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 17, 1–14. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17238750
Aschieri, F., Durosini, I., and Smith, J. D. (2020). Self-curiosity: definition and measurement. Self Identity 19, 105–115. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2018.1543728
Balgiu, B. A. (2018). Psychometric properties of Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II) in the case of a sample of Romanian students. Rev Psihol. 64, 102–114. Available online at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2018-31154-003
Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. 57,449–518.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Available online at: http://content.apa.org/books/11164-000 (Accessed October 05, 2024).
Bieberstein, F., von Jaussi, S., and Vogel, C. (2020). Challenge-seeking and the gender wage gap: a lab-in-the-field experiment with cleaning personnel. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 175, 251–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.03.037
Borsboom, D., and Cramer, A. O. J. (2013). Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 91–121. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608
Borsboom, D., Deserno, M. K., Rhemtulla, M., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., McNally, R. J., et al. (2021). Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science. Nat. Rev. Methods Prim. 1:58. doi: 10.1038/s43586-021-00055-w
Briganti, G., Scutari, M., and McNally, R. J. A. (2022). Tutorial on Bayesian networks for psychopathology researchers. Psychol. Methods. 28, 947–961. doi: 10.1037/met0000479
Celik, P., Storme, M., Davila, A., and Myszkowski, N. (2016). Work-related curiosity positively predicts worker innovation. J. Manag. Dev. 35, 1184–1194. doi: 10.1108/JMD-01-2016-0013
Chang, Y.-Y., and Shih, H.-Y. (2019). Work curiosity: a new lens for understanding employee creativity. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 29:100672. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2018.10.005
Christensen, A. P., Garrido, L. E., and Golino, H. (2023). Unique variable analysis: a network psychometrics method to detect local dependence. Multivari. Behav. Res. 58, 1165–1182. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2023.2194606
Christensen, A. P., and Golino, H. (2021). Estimating the stability of psychological dimensions via bootstrap exploratory graph analysis: a Monte Carlo Simulation and Tutorial. Psychology 3, 479–500. doi: 10.3390/psych3030032
Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., Perugini, M., Mõttus, R., Waldorp, L. J., et al. (2015). State of the aRt personality research: a tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R. J. Res. Pers. 54, 13–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.003
Cramer, A. O. J. (2012). Why the item “23 +1” is not in a depression questionnaire: validity from a network perspective. Meas. Interdiscip. Res. Perspect. 10, 50–54. doi: 10.1080/15366367.2012.681973
Dubey, R., and Griffiths, T. L. (2020). Reconciling novelty and complexity through a rational analysis of curiosity. Psychol. Rev. 127, 455–476. doi: 10.1037/rev0000175
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., and Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav. Res. Methods. 50, 195–212. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1
Epskamp, S., and Fried, E. I. (2018). A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol. Methods. 23, 617–634. doi: 10.1037/met0000167
Fried, E. I., and Cramer, A. O. J. (2017). Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 999–1020. doi: 10.1177/1745691617705892
Friedman, J., Hastie, T., and Tibshirani, R. (2008). Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics 9, 432–441. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxm045
Glück, T. M., Knefel, M., and Lueger-Schuster, B. (2017). A network analysis of anger, shame, proposed ICD-11 post-traumatic stress disorder, and different types of childhood trauma in foster care settings in a sample of adult survivors. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 8, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1372543
Golino, H., Shi, D., Christensen, A. P., Garrido, L. E., Nieto, M. D., Sadana, R., et al. (2002). Investigating the performance of exploratory graph analysis and traditional techniques to identify the number of latent factors: a simulation and tutorial. Psychol. Methods 25, 292–320. doi: 10.1037/met0000255
Golino, H. F., and Epskamp, S. (2017). Exploratory graph analysis: a new approach for estimating the number of dimensions in psychological research. PLoS ONE 12:e0174035. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174035
Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., and Woodcock, A. (2011). Exploring and measuring differences in person-thing orientations. Pers. Individ. Dif. 51, 28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.004
Gross, M. E., Zedelius, C. M., and Schooler, J. W. (2020). Cultivating an understanding of curiosity as a seed for creativity. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 35, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.07.015
Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Disentangling curiosity: dimensionality, definitions, and distinctions from interest in educational contexts. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 28, 23–60. doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9294-y
Hagtvedt, L. P., Dossinger, K., Harrison, S. H., and Huang, L. (2019). Curiosity made the cat more creative: Specific curiosity as a driver of creativity. Organ Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 150, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.10.007
Hambleton, R. K., and de Jong, J. H. (2003). Advances in translating and adapting educational and psychological tests. Lang. Test. 20, 127–134. doi: 10.1191/0265532203lt247xx
Harris, C. R., and Jenkins, M. (2006). Gender differences in risk assessment: why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgm. Decis. Mak. 1, 48–63. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500000346
Harrison, S. H., Sluss, D. M., and Ashforth, B. E. (2011). Curiosity adapted the cat: the role of trait curiosity in newcomer adaptation. J. Appl. Psychol. 96, 211–220. doi: 10.1037/a0021647
Haslbeck, J. M. B., Ryan, O., Robinaugh, D. J., Waldorp, L. J., and Borsboom, D. (2021). Modeling psychopathology: from data models to formal theories. Psychol. Methods. 27, 930–957. doi: 10.1037/met0000303
Haslbeck, J. M. B. B., and Fried, E. I. (2017). How predictable are symptoms in psychopathological networks? A reanalysis of 18 published datasets. Psychol. Med. 47, 2767–2776. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001258
Hevey, D. (2018). Network analysis: a brief overview and tutorial. Heal. Psychol. Behav. Med. 6, 301–328. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2018.1521283
Johnson, N. E., Nerima, D., Kahura, N., and Osborn, T. L. (2023). Psychometric properties of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II among Kenyan adolescents. Front. Sociol. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2023.1189915
Jovanović, V., and Gavrilov-Jerković, V. (2014). The good, the bad (and the ugly): the role of curiosity in subjective well-being and risky behaviors among adolescents. Scand. J. Psychol. 55, 38–44. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12084
Kashdan, T. B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar, D., et al. (2009). The curiosity and exploration inventory-II: development, factor structure, and psychometrics. J. Res. Pers. 43, 987–998. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.011
Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., Disabato, D. J., McKnight, P. E., Kelso, K., Naughton, C., et al. (2020). Curiosity has comprehensive benefits in the workplace: developing and validating a multidimensional workplace curiosity scale in United States and German employees. Pers. Individ. Dif. 155:109717. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109717
Kashdan, T. B., and Roberts, J. E. (2004). Trait and state curiosity in the genesis of intimacy: differentiation from related constructs. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 792–816. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.6.792.54800
Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P., and Fincham, F. D. (2004). Curiosity and exploration: facilitating positive subjective experiences and personal growth opportunities. J. Pers. Assess. 82, 291–305. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8203_05
Kashdan, T. B., and Yuen, M. (2007). Whether highly curious students thrive academically depends on perceptions about the school learning environment: a study of Hong Kong adolescents. Motiv. Emot. 31, 260–270. doi: 10.1007/s11031-007-9074-9
Kidd, C., and Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88, 449–460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010
Kitson, N. K., Constantinou, A. C., Guo, Z., Liu, Y., and Chobtham, K. (2023). A survey of Bayesian Network structure learning. Artif. Intell. Rev. 56, 8721–8814. doi: 10.1007/s10462-022-10351-w
Le Cunff, A. L. (2024). Systematic curiosity as an integrative tool for human flourishing: a conceptual review and framework. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 58, 1876–1894. doi: 10.1007/s12124-024-09856-6
Li, H., Cabeli, V., Sella, N., and Isambert, H. (2019). “Constraint-based causal structure learning with consistent separating sets,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, eds. H. Wallach, H. Larochelle, A. Beygelzimer, F. d'Alché-Buc, E. Fox, R. Garnett (Curran Associates, Inc.). Available online at: https://proceedings.neurips.cc/paper_files/paper/2019/file/e6872f5bbe75073f8c7cfb93de7f6f3a-Paper.pdf (Accessed October 05, 2024).
Litman, J. A., and Spielberger, C. D. (2003). Measuring epistemic curiosity and its diversive and specific components. J. Pers. Assess. 80, 75–86. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8001_16
Lydon-Staley, D. M., Zurn, P., and Bassett, D. S. (2020). Within-person variability in curiosity during daily life and associations with well-being. J. Pers. 88, 625–41. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12515
Mascherek, A., and Zimprich, D. (2012). Stability and change in typical intellectual engagement in old age across 5 years. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 67B, 309–316. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr101
Moreno, C., Ramos, P., Rivera, F., Jiménez-Iglesias, A., García-Moya, I., Sánchez-Queija, I., et al. (2020). Informe técnico de los resultados obtenidos por el Estudio Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) 2018 en España. Madrid: Spanish Ministry of Health.
Naylor, F. D. A. (1981). state-trait curiosity inventory. Aust. Psychol. 16, 172–183. doi: 10.1080/00050068108255893
Opsahl, T., Agneessens, F., and Skvoretz, J. (2010). Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Soc. Netw. 32, 245–251. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2010.03.006
Peljko, Ž., Jeraj, M., Sǎvoiu, G., and Marič, M. (2016). An empirical study of the relationship between entrepreneurial curiosity and innovativeness. Organizacija 49, 172–182. doi: 10.1515/orga-2016-0016
R Core Team (2018). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 292–320.
Reio, T Y, Petrosko, J., Wiswell, A., and Thongsukmag, J. (2006). The measurement and conceptualization of curiosity. J. Genet. Psychol. 167, 117–135. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.167.2.117-135
Robichaud, M., Dugas, M. J., and Conway, M. (2003). Gender differences in worry and associated cognitive-behavioral variables. J. Anxiety Disord. 17, 501–516. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00237-2
Robinaugh, D. J., Millner, A. J., and McNally, R. J. (2016). Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 747–757. doi: 10.1037/abn0000181
Sakaki, M., Yagi, A., and Murayama, K. (2018). Curiosity in old age: a possible key to achieving adaptive aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 88, 106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.007
Sarin, R., and Wieland, A. (2016). Risk aversion for decisions under uncertainty: are there gender differences? J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 60, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2015.10.007
Schubert, R., Brown, M., Gysler, M., and Brachinger, H. W. (1999). Financial decision-making: are women really more risk averse? Am. Econ. Rev. 89, 381–385. doi: 10.1257/aer.89.2.381
Schutte, N. S., and Malouff, J. M. (2023). A meta-analytic investigation of the impact of curiosity-enhancing interventions. Curr. Psychol. 42, 20374–20384. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03107-w
Scutari, M. (2010). Learning Bayesian networks with the bnlearn R Package. J. Stat. Softw. 35, 1–22. doi: 10.18637/jss.v035.i03
Setyowati, A., Chung, M.- H, Yusuf, A., and Haksama, S. (2020). Psychometric of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II in Indonesia. J. Public Health Res. 9:1745. doi: 10.4081/jphr.2020.1745
Singh, A., and Manjaly, J. A. (2022). Using curiosity to improve learning outcomes in schools. SAGE Open. 12:69392. doi: 10.1177/21582440211069392
Spirtes, P., and Glymour, C. (1991). An algorithm for fast recovery of sparse causal graphs. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 9, 62–72. doi: 10.1177/089443939100900106
Su, R., Rounds, J., and Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: a meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychol. Bull. 135, 859–884. doi: 10.1037/a0017364
Tarilonte-Castaño, I., Díaz-Milanés, D., Andrés-Villas, M., Morales-Domínguez, Z., and Pérez-Moreno, P. J. (2023). Validation of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II in Spanish University Students. Healthcare 11:1128. doi: 10.3390/healthcare11081128
Van Borkulo, C. D., Borsboom, D., Epskamp, S., Blanken, T. F., Boschloo, L., Schoevers, R. A., et al. (2014). A new method for constructing networks from binary data. Sci. Rep. 4, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/srep05918
van Borkulo, C. D., van Bork, R., Boschloo, L., Kossakowski, J. J., Tio, P., Schoevers, R. A., et al. (2022). Comparing network structures on three aspects: a permutation test. Psychol. Methods. 28, 1273–1285. doi: 10.1037/met0000476
Wagstaff, M. F., Flores, G. L., Ahmed, R., and Villanueva, S. (2021). Measures of curiosity: a literature review. Hum Resour Dev Q. 32, 363–389. doi: 10.1002/hrdq.21417
Wavo, E.-Y.-T. (2004). Honesty, cooperation and curiosity and achievement of some schools on Nanjing (China). IFE Psychol. 12:23653. doi: 10.4314/ifep.v12i2.23653
Whitecross, W. M., and Smithson, M. (2023). Curiously different: Interest-curiosity and deprivation-curiosity may have distinct benefits and drawbacks. Pers. Individ. Dif. 213:112310. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2023.112310
Ye, S., Ng, T. K., Yim, K. H., and Wang, J. (2015). Validation of the curiosity and exploration inventory-II (CEI-II) among Chinese university students in Hong Kong. J. Pers. Assess. 97, 403–410. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1013546
Keywords: curiosity, creativity, CEI-II, psychometric properties, network analysis, Bayesian network, Spain
Citation: Navas-León S, Pérez-Moreno PJ, Santin Vilarino C and Diaz-Milanes D (2025) Revalidation of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II) using network analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1514959. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1514959
Received: 21 October 2024; Accepted: 15 July 2025;
Published: 30 July 2025.
Edited by:
Snezana Smederevac, University of Novi Sad, SerbiaReviewed by:
Ana-Maria Cazan, Transilvania University of Braşov, RomaniaRiet Van Bork, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
Copyright © 2025 Navas-León, Pérez-Moreno, Santin Vilarino and Diaz-Milanes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Carmen Santin Vilarino, c2FudGluQGRwc2kudWh1LmVz; Diego Diaz-Milanes, ZGRpYXpAdWxveW9sYS5lcw==