- 1School of Geography and Environmental Sciences (School of Karst Science), Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China
- 2School of Tourism and Air Service, Guizhou Minzu University, Guiyang, China
- 3School of International Tourism and Culture, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China
- 4School of Geographical Sciences and Remote Sensing, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, China
Introduction: Health information avoidance (HIA) creates serious health risks, particularly for patients with serious health problems such as breast cancer. Although existing research has explained how emotional responses affect HIA from several perspectives, little attention has been paid to how perceived emotional synchrony (PES), as an antecedent, influences HIA behavior, especially in the context of breast cancer patients participating in ritualistic interactive activities. In this study, we constructed a moderated chain mediation model drawn on the Interactive Ritual Chains (IRCs) theory, combined with social cognitive theory to test the relationship between PES and HIA behaviors in cancer patients. At the same time, the important individual characteristic of cancer staging has been overlooked in studies of boundary mechanisms in HIA. We further explored the moderating role of cancer staging.
Methods: We assembled a sample of 302 female patients with breast cancer who participated in ritual interaction activities in five Grade A tertiary hospitals in China. In this study, regression analysis was conducted using SPSS 23.0 and MPlus 8.3 to explore the relationship between PES, positive emotions, coping self-efficacy, and HIA variables to test the hypotheses.
Results: Empirical analyses revealed that PES was negatively correlated with HIA in the context of breast cancer patients participating in ritual interaction activities. Additionally, positive emotions and coping self-efficacy acted as mediators between PES and HIA. Furthermore, positive emotions and coping self-efficacy played a chain-mediation role in the relationship between PES and HIA during ritual interaction activities. Disease stage significantly moderated the strength of these chain-mediated effects, with the chain-mediated influence of positive emotions and coping self-efficacy between PES and HIA being significantly stronger in patients with advanced breast cancer.
Discussion: The study constructed a quantitative conceptual model of how PES influences HIA in cancer patients. Cancer staging was shown to have a moderating effect on this mechanism, which enriches theoretical explanations of HIA behavior. In practice, promoting PES through structured ritual interactions can strengthen emotional connections among breast cancer patients. Developing stage-specific support strategies may facilitate more personalized interventions. Future research should examine the multilevel mechanisms of ritual interaction and the situational role of HIA.
1 Introduction
Breast cancer is a major global health concern (Smit et al., 2019; Wilkinson and Gathani, 2022). According to the recent global cancer burden figures, it is estimated that there were 2.3 million incident breast cancer cases in 2022, making it the most prevalent malignant tumor in the world; it is also a leading cause of cancer-related mortality among women (World Health Organization, 2024). Timely and accurate access to health information is critical for effective diagnosis, treatment, treatment decision-making, and recovery processes in cancer care (Anker et al., 2011; Lu et al., 2024). Nonetheless, a substantial proportion of breast cancer patients make deliberate efforts to avoid cancer-related information that may induce emotional distress or perceived threat. For instance, Melnyk and Shepperd (2012) reported that nearly 25% of women were reluctant to confirm a cancer diagnosis. Furthermore, recent studies indicate that numerous patients actively avoid cancer-related topics (Calderon et al., 2021; Nelissen et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2015). Such health information avoidance (HIA) behaviors have been shown to lead to delayed diagnosis, decreased adherence to treatment regimens, and ultimately poorer health outcomes (Golman et al., 2017; Howell et al., 2020; Link, 2022; Lu et al., 2024; Melnyk and Shepperd, 2012). Understanding the psychological and social mechanisms behind HIA is critical for improving patient engagement and health outcomes in cancer care.
Past research has focused on multiple factors that influence HIA: first, individual differences are manifested in health perceptions (Emanuel et al., 2015; Lipsey and Shepperd, 2019), perceived control (Brown et al., 2017), and emotional motivation (Emanuel et al., 2015; Hertwig and Engel, 2016). Second are social factors, such as social relationships (Howell and Shepperd, 2017; Link and Baumann, 2022; Lipsey and Shepperd, 2019; Price, 2018) and socioeconomic status (Pampel et al., 2010). Finally, information factors relate more to information content (Ganguly and Tasoff, 2017) and information overload (Franconeri et al., 2013; Siebenhaar et al., 2020). A supportive environment and peer interaction provide a unique sense of community and belonging to patients with cancer and meet their perceived emotional needs [e.g., patients' emotional synchrony perception (PES)], while helping patients access cancer treatment information and linking to resources (Ussher et al., 2006). However, research on PES as a factor in the emotional motivation affecting HIA is less frequently conducted, especially in the context of breast cancer. The term PES refers to the idea that people experience similar emotions when participating in rituals, group activities, or synchronized behaviors (Durkheim, 1915; Wlodarczyk et al., 2020; Xiang et al., 2022b). This feeling reflects emotional resonance and a perceived sense of connection and unity among individuals (Collins, 2004; Gabriel et al., 2017; Wood, 2020). PES also produces positive affective outcomes such as increased trust, belonging, and group cohesion (Cheong et al., 2023; Páez et al., 2015; Pizarro et al., 2020). Zlobina and Dávila (2022) emphasize that collective PES experiences and cultural practices have a positive impact on public health behaviors during times of crisis. For emotionally vulnerable populations such as cancer patients, PES may serve as an important emotional buffer that enhances psychological resilience and motivates more open engagement with distressing but necessary health information. Nevertheless, its potential as a protective emotional factor against health information avoidance in oncology contexts has yet to be adequately addressed.
Scholars have explained how emotional responses affect HIA from the following perspectives. Soroya et al. (2021) applied the stimulus–organism–response (SOR) model and introduced an affective layer to explain how information exposure can lead to avoidance behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Zhang and Gao (2021) used the cognition–affect–conation model to study how anxiety affects users' information avoidance and withdrawal on health platforms. In another approach Deline and Kahlor (2019) proposed the Program Risk Information Avoidance (PRIA) model that incorporates cognitive, affective, and sociocultural dimensions. Other studies have drawn on drive theory and the elaboration likelihood model to explore similar avoidance behaviors. However, the above studies have paid little attention to how PES, as an antecedent, influences HIA behavior through emotions and cognition, especially in contexts where patients with breast cancer engage in ritual interaction activities. To address these gaps, the Interactive Ritual Chains (IRCs) theory, proposed by Collins (2004), was introduced to explain the emotional energy of patients with cancer when they engage in ritual interaction activities (Ritchie and Hudson, 2009). According to IRCs, scholars have suggested that participants' shared concerns and emotions trigger reciprocal neurological arousal (Collins, 2004) and have emphasized that higher PES in collective gatherings leads to stronger emotional responses (Páez et al., 2015). The emotional response and regulatory abilities of individuals have been shown to play a crucial role in explaining why people engage in HIA (Yang and Kahlor, 2013; Chasiotis et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2023). Combined with social cognitive theory, perceived emotional and physiological states affect individual self-efficacy (Bandura, 1998), and whether an individual develops HIA behavior is a consequence of changes in self-efficacy (Hua and Howell, 2022). Accordingly, we argue that positive emotions and cognition (e.g., coping self-efficacy) may be associated with HIA behavior, especially in states where participants' emotional energy is mutually aroused. The IRCs theory proposes that social interactions generate emotional energy and cognitive value, which in turn foster shared emotional experiences (Collins, 2004; Yu and Na, 2022). When considered alongside social cognitive theory, the perception of emotions has been shown to influence individual self-efficacy (Bandura, 1998). Positive emotions have been demonstrated to enhance coping abilities and adaptation (Guo and Wang, 2007), while higher levels of self-efficacy have been found to increase patients' willingness to engage with health information (Lee et al., 2008). Fredrickson and Branigan (2005) emphasized that positive emotions form the foundation for building cognitive resources and behavioral capacities. Consequently, positive emotion and self-efficacy may act as mediating factors, linking PES to reduced HIA. This provides a theoretical foundation for exploring this mechanism. However, a chained explanatory mechanism explaining the relationship between PES and HIA in terms of the two constructs of positive affect and coping self-efficacy is lacking.
Scholars have also explained the boundary mechanisms of HIA in terms of situational and subjective factors. Contextual factors include cross-cultural situations, family, and informational environments. Subjective factors are mainly related to demographic characteristics and individual resources such as self-affirmation (Howell and Shepperd, 2017) and time pressure (Guo et al., 2020). However, disease progression stage (cancer staging) is an important individual characteristic that affects the mood and cognition of patients with cancer (Hou et al., 2018; Segrin et al., 2011); this characteristic has, to date, been missing in HIA studies. Therefore, the current study further considered the moderating role of cancer staging by comprehensively analyzing the relationship between the PES and HIA in patients with cancer.
Considering the discussion above, this study mainly aimed to (1) show the promoting influence of PES in patients with breast cancer's interaction on HIA behavior, (2) examine the chain-mediating role of positive emotion and coping self-efficacy, and (3) test the moderating effect of cancer staging. To reach these objectives, this study draws on IRCs theory, combined with social cognitive theory, and uses quantitative data collected from five Grade A tertiary hospitals in China to conduct an empirical study. Current research indicates that this is the first effort to verify the causal implications of PES in the ritual interactions of patients with cancer with HIA, quantitatively. We anticipate that the findings of this study will theoretically enrich research on the interactions and health behaviors of patients with cancer and provide reference points to address patients' HIA in practice.
1.1 The IRCs theory
Originally developed by Collins (1993, 2004), the theory of IRCs conceptualizes human interaction as a sequence of emotionally charged encounters, in which shared emotions and mutual focus are central. According to Collins (2004), a core outcome of successful rituals is emotional energy—a sustained feeling of enthusiasm, confidence, and initiative. Most interactive rituals produce the following outcomes: (1) group solidarity, (2) personal emotional energy, (3) symbols representing the group, and (4) morality. These rituals can take the form of either natural rituals, which are informal and embedded in everyday interactions, or formal rituals, which involve structured, symbolic activities (Collins, 2004).
Although IRC theory does not explain the direction of causality among constructs, it has proven to be of significant value in health research. Scholars have used it to explore how emotional energy generated through social interaction fosters behavioral transformation. For instance, Henckes and Nurok (2015) demonstrate that ritualized emotional exchanges among healthcare professionals foster a sense of belonging and promote adaptive behavior. Similarly, ritual interactions in the daily routine influence clinicians' behavioral decisions (Szymczak, 2016). Recent research has extended this framework to patient contexts. Clarke and Waring (2018) drew on the IRC theory to determine how interactions can help individuals transform negative emotions into positive ones. Their findings indicated that ritual interactions lead to positive results for patients, such as solidarity, belonging, confidence, and positive change. In epidemic settings, Xiang et al. (2022a) explored how ritualized “host–guest” exchanges contributed to emotional regulation and value construction. While prior studies were mainly qualitative, they provide critical conceptual insights into how ritual interactions generate emotional energy and shape behavioral outcomes. These insights provide a solid basis for identifying and operationalizing key constructs such as PES. Based on this foundation, Zlobina and Dávila (2022) conducted empirical research and found that emotional synchrony during collective ritual participation significantly and positively predicts preventive behaviors through cognitive-affective processes. This finding offers direct support for the quantitative measurement of key IRC constructs, such as PES, and their prediction of behavior. This approach enables us to advance beyond descriptive accounts and empirically test the pathways through which ritual participation influences health-related behaviors.
However, the IRCs theory has not been widely used to show how emotional energy generated through ritual interactions may influence health-related behaviors, such as HIA, particularly in the context of breast cancer patients. In this study, we mainly use IRCs theory as a conceptual lens to understand the emotional dynamics that shape PES in patient ritual interactions. Additionally, we integrate this framework with social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1997, 1998) to support the proposed links between PES, coping self-efficacy, and behavioral responses in the context of breast cancer care.
1.2 Patients' PES and HIA behavior
The term PES means how people feel emotionally connected and share feelings when they take part in ritual interactions (Páez et al., 2015; Wlodarczyk et al., 2020). Instead of being a specific emotion, it is better seen as a mental process. The phenomenon represents a strongly shared emotional experience (Durkheim, 1915; Xiang et al., 2022b). PES may trigger collective affective states during group gatherings, shared attention, and when behavioral synchrony occurs (Wlodarczyk et al., 2020). PES reflects the dynamic interactions between event participants and between hosts and guests (Sterchele, 2020; Xiang et al., 2022b; Xie and Li, 2023). Emotional synchrony is a psychological element that provides a sense of connection to others and a sense of belonging to a group (Collins, 2004; Gabriel et al., 2017). This experience typically leads to positive outcomes such as trust, safety, and emotional support (Páez et al., 2015).
On the other hand, avoiding information is described as “denial, blunting, or repression” (Lambert and Loiselle, 2007); it refers to a person's deliberate choice to ignore or distance themselves from the information (Zhao and Liu, 2021). Information avoidance is strongly correlated with negative emotional states such as anxiety, fear, which often arise when individuals feel emotionally overwhelmed or socially isolated (Case et al., 2005). Research on the psychology of emotions has demonstrated that the greatest motivator of behavior is emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Zeelenberg et al., 2008), which motivates health information seeking (Myrick et al., 2016), and reduces HIA. In this context, PES may help patients regulate their negative emotions by fostering emotional safety, reducing feelings of threat, and creating a socially supportive environment. Shared emotional experiences can help individuals feel less isolated and more confident when facing difficult health realities. For example, in group settings such as patient support groups or rituals, emotional synchronization can buffer distress and increase patients' willingness to confront and process health-related information. Deline and Kahlor (2019) pointed out that affective factors can influence information avoidance in risky situations. They also stressed that reducing negative emotions can increase information engagement. Thus, PES can be considered a protective affective factor that may reduce the chance of HIA by promoting emotional openness, psychological resilience, and group-based coping. Based on this reasoning, the following hypothesis is proposed:
H1. PES of patient interactions has a significant negative effect on HIA behavior.
1.3 Mediating effect of positive emotion
Positive emotions are unique immediate responses to personally meaningful and temporary pleasure (Fredrickson, 2001). They are an important driver of individuals' behavioral intentions (Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005) and a core measure of personal health behaviors (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000; Mayne and Bonanno, 2001). IRC theory suggests that PES in group gatherings is associated with emotional responses (Collins, 2004; Durkheim, 1915). Páez et al. (2015) verified that higher PES resulted in more positive emotions.
In this study, we argue that the emotional energy produced during cancer patients' ritual interactions can activate specific emotional responses, such as emotional arousal and empathy. These emotional responses contribute to the emergence of positive emotions (e.g., hope, trust, relief) that are shaped by shared affective experience (Gabriel et al., 2017; Xiang et al., 2022b). Therefore, the related hypotheses are as follows:
H2a. PES of patient interaction has a significant positive effect on patients' positive emotions.
In a positive emotional state, individuals tend to approach and explore new things, and maintain an active connection with the environment. Positive emotions are linked to specific action tendencies, such as generating exploration and grasping new information and experiences (Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005). Yang and Kahlor (2013) indicated that affective factors are correlated with information avoidance behaviors under severe health concerns. A clinical trial has verified that positive emotions have the potential to stimulate more positive information seeking (Yang et al., 2011). Hence, we infer that:
H2b. Positive emotions of patients have a significant negative effect on HIA behavior.
Health sociology scholars have discussed the antecedents and outcome variables of patients' positive emotions. The antecedent variables affecting positive emotions include physical environment (e.g., healthcare setting) and social environment (e.g., hospitableness and social interactions) (Altinay et al., 2023; Batra and Taneja, 2022). The outcome variables included satisfaction, adherence, subjective well-being, and behavioral intentions (Altinay et al., 2023; Legg et al., 2015). This study considers that patients' PES influences their HIA behavior, via positive emotions. According to IRCs theory, behavioral symbols in interaction have a strong cultural function, which can better link participants' emotional resonance and subsequent behaviors (Collins, 2004). As stated by Zhou et al. (2023), positive emotions are a connection mechanism for mental states that mediate social interactions and individual behaviors. Additionally, by combining H2a and H2b, we propose the following:
H2. Positive emotions mediate the negative relationship between the PES of the patient and their HIA behavior.
1.4 Mediating effect of coping self-efficacy
Coping self-efficacy is a psychological resource for helping patients adjust to breast cancer (Karademas et al., 2021, 2022), reflecting an individual's subjective judgment of their ability to cope effectively with challenges in life (Bandura, 1982; Chesney et al., 2006). In the context of cancer, coping self-efficacy is a context-specific form of self-efficacy that refers to a patient's perceived ability to cope with the challenges of cancer treatment (Chirico et al., 2017), such as uncertainty associated with the diagnosis of the disease, physical side effects of treatment, postoperative recovery, informational challenges of treatment (Heitzmann et al., 2011). Scholars distinguish three forms of coping self-efficacy: emotion-focused coping, problem-focused coping, and support from family and friends (Chesney et al., 2006), with emotion-focused coping receiving particular attention for its significant impact on emotional adjustment, especially in the context of serious illnesses such as breast cancer.
Coping self-efficacy refers to individuals' belief in their ability to effectively manage challenges, particularly in the context of illness, and it reflects human cognition (Bandura, 1997). According to social cognitive theory, such belief is shaped not only by personal experiences but also by emotional and social interactions (Bandura, 1998). Research has found that PES promotes emotional connectedness, which strengthens self-relevant evaluations such as self-efficacy (Páez et al., 2015). When confronted with stressful events, PES provides emotional support by sharing experiences with others, which may enhance individuals‘ confidence in coping with adversity, thereby increasing their coping self-efficacy (Hua and Howell, 2022; Lee et al., 2008). Therefore, the related hypothesis is as follows:
H3a. PES of patient interaction has a significant positive effect on patient coping self-efficacy.
Related research states that self-efficacy influences individuals to adopt behaviors and improve outcomes (Bandura, 1978, 1997). Coping self-efficacy is a personal resource that may play a crucial role in defensive decision-making, such as avoiding information (Marx-Fleck et al., 2021). Individuals have limited available resources, and when their perceived resources are diminished or insufficient (i.e., “threatened”), they respond to uncertainty and stress through avoidance motivation (Bublatzky et al., 2017). Lee et al. (2008) emphasized that patients' coping self-efficacy is a key factor in determining their use of health information. Insufficient coping self-efficacy may lead to information avoidance when individuals feel highly threatened (Hua and Howell, 2022; Lee et al., 2008; Witte, 1992). Therefore, the related hypothesis is as follows:
H3b. High coping self-efficacy has a significant negative effect on HIA behavior.
Self-efficacy is a personal resource that influences behavior (Abdullah and Wider, 2022; Bandura, 1978). Hence, coping self-efficacy has been identified as an effective mediator of health-promoting behaviors (Geng et al., 2018). For instance, Geng et al. (2018) suggested that good social support for patients with cancer positively influences information seeking indirectly, via coping self-efficacy. We therefore infer that coping self-efficacy is a mediating mechanism linking patients' PES and HIA behaviors. Therefore, by combining hypotheses H3a and H3b, we propose the following:
H3. Coping self-efficacy mediates the negative relationship between patients' PES and HIA behaviors.
1.5 The chain-mediating effect of positive emotions and coping self-efficacy
According to the IRC theory, social interactions activate emotional energy and cognitive value (Collins, 2004; Yu and Na, 2022). In combination with social cognitive theory, perceived emotions affect individual self-efficacy (Bandura, 1998). Guo and Wang (2007) noted that positive emotions can improve individuals' coping levels and promote social adaptation. The greater the patient's self-efficacy, the higher the chance of determining their use of health information services (Lee et al., 2008). In summary, by combining H2 and H3, the following hypothesis is proposed:
H4. Patients' positive emotions and coping self-efficacy chains mediate the relationship between PES and HIA behavior.
1.6 The moderating effect of cancer staging
Cancer staging is not only a medical indicator of disease progression but also has important psychological and behavioral implications. In oncology practice, breast cancer is staged using the internationally recognized TNM system. That categorizes cancer into stages 0, I, II, III, and IV based on tumor size (T), lymph node involvement (N), and presence of distant metastasis (M) (Amin et al., 2017). Generally, a higher stage means more advanced disease and poorer prognosis (Yao et al., 2015). However, in the Chinese clinical and cultural context, patients are often more familiar with simplified categories such as “early,” “middle,” and “advanced” stages, rather than specific TNM stages. To ensure both conceptual clarity and patient comprehension, based on recommendations from oncology professionals, field research (Manne et al., 2001; Segrin et al., 2011; Yao et al., 2015), and the 2022 edition of China's national Breast Cancer Diagnosis and Treatment Guidelines (National Health Commission of the People's Republic of China, 2022), this study adopts the following classification: the early stage includes stage 0, stage I, and part of stage II (IIA and IIB with T2N1M0); the middle stage includes stage IIB (T3N0M0) and stage III; the advanced stage refers to metastatic breast cancer, i.e., stage IV. Existing studies have shown that patients at different stages face significant differences in physical burden, treatment regimens, and expectations regarding life and death, all of which substantially influence their disease perception, emotional responses, and information processing strategies (Segrin et al., 2011).
Uncertainty Management Theory explains how individuals respond to situations filled with uncertainty (Brashers, 2001). It suggests that people do not always seek information when facing health-related threats. Instead, they selectively seek or avoid information often depends on how emotionally prepared they feel. This theory is especially relevant to cancer patients. As the disease progresses, their level of uncertainty often changes throughout the illness. These changes influence how they manage their emotions, also shape how patients choose to engage with health information. Moreover, the Kübler-Ross (1997). Model identifies five sequential psychological phases: denial, followed by anger, bargaining, later depression, finally acceptance. This progression typically occurs after life-altering diagnoses. We observe clear behavioral differences between early and late-stage patients. Those in early stages commonly display denial through two main behaviors: suppressing emotions and steering clear of alarming medical facts. In contrast, patients in advanced stages tend to demonstrate greater acceptance and are usually more willing to express their feelings openly.
Empirical evidence further supports the moderating role of cancer stage in patients' psychological and behavioral responses. Manne et al. (2001) showed that disease stage moderated the link between stress and avoiding health information, patients in early stages were more likely to avoid information. Chen and Zhao (2021) reported that patients with Stages I and II breast cancer tended to suppress their emotions, while stage IV patients showed more positive emotions and signaled a desire for connection with others. Segrin et al. (2011) also highlighted that patients at different stages show distinct emotional states, information processing patterns, and social engagement needs. Furthermore, Graham (2024) showed that disease stage moderated the association between psychological distress and health avoidance. The more advanced the disease, the more likely patients were to view such activities as emotional anchors and opportunities for meaning-making. According to IRCs theory (Collins, 2004), effective social interaction involves four key elements. These include bodily co-presence, a clear group boundary, a shared focus of attention, and emotional synchronization. Cancer patients at different stages respond to these elements in different ways. Based on the above facts, we predicted that the PES generated from ritual interactive activities would be more pronounced in enhancing positive emotions in advanced-stage patients. Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed:
H5. Cancer staging moderates the relationship between PES and positive emotions in patients with cancer.
Cancer staging also affects perceived coping self-efficacy and subsequent health-promoting behaviors in patients with cancer (Hou et al., 2018; Taechaboonsermsak et al., 2005). These differences may influence how they process emotional experiences and engage in health-related behaviors, including HIA. Based on IRCs theory combination with Social Cognitive Theory, this study proposes Hypothesis H4, which explains how PES may reduce HIA through positive emotions and coping self-efficacy. However, the impact of this chain effect may differ depending on cancer staging. Patients in the early stages often pay more attention to clinical treatment and physical recovery. In contrast, those in advanced stages usually care more about making sense of their experience and seeking emotional support (Graham, 2024; Lee et al., 2008). Therefore, patients in more advanced stages may be more sensitive to emotional interactions and more likely to benefit from PES in ways that improve coping self-efficacy and reduce HIA. This analysis can be further characterized as a moderated chain mediation effect. Therefore, this study proposes the following hypothesis:
H6. Cancer staging moderates the chain-mediated effects of patients' PES on HIA behavior via positive emotions and self-efficacy.
This study constructed a moderated chain-mediated effects model (Figure 1) to comprehensively explore the mechanisms and boundaries of the role of PES on HIA behavior in patients with breast cancer.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Participants and procedures
The study sample was drawn from five Grade A tertiary hospitals in China—three in Guizhou, one in Hebei, and one in Tianjin. The selected hospitals organized a series of patient interaction ritual activities irregularly, in addition to structured treatment. The main interactive activities include (i) ritual activities for traditional Chinese festivals, such as the Dragon Boat Festival; (ii) award ceremonies, such as the presentation of a “diploma” to a patient who has completed chemotherapy; (iii) outdoor rituals, such as patients' salon, anti-cancer star contests in “pink ribbon month”; and (iv) ceremonial interactions on special days, such as Women's Day and patients' birthdays. These interactions adhere to humanistic care and provide ritual and embodied experiences to the patients. These cases were therefore suitable for our study and reflected our research aims. The participants were all patients with breast cancer who were admitted to the above hospitals and who participated in interactive activities. After being informed of the study's goal and agreeing to take part in the study, the patients were asked to complete and submit the questionnaire. The survey data collected in this study has been strictly anonymized, and no identifying information is included.
The survey was conducted over four periods. First, on June 21, 2023, research team members participated in the activities of the Breast Surgery Department of a tertiary hospital in Guizhou as volunteers, celebrating the Dragon Boat Festival with doctors and patients with breast cancer and conducting traditional rituals, such as wrapping zongzi and sewing scented sachets (Figure 2). In this survey, 18 questionnaires were distributed, and 18 were returned. Second, on October 15, 2023, research team members again voluntarily participated in an outdoor ceremony organized by a tertiary hospital in Guizhou—the Breast Cancer Patient Salon, and the Anti-Cancer Star Competition at a Healing Garden (Figure 3). In this survey, 43 questionnaires were distributed and 42 were returned. Third, on October 28, 2023, team members participated in an outdoor ceremony for Pink Ribbon Month, organized by a tertiary hospital in Guizhou. In this survey, 65 questionnaires were distributed and 60 were returned. Finally, we contacted three eligible hospitals in Tianjin, Hebei, and Guizhou following the Pink Ribbon Interactive Rituals that were held in the three hospitals, where 225 questionnaires were distributed and 217 were returned. In total, 337 questionnaires were retrieved. After excluding invalid questionnaires with too short a response time, consecutively choosing the same answer, and obvious patterns or errors in the answers, a total of 302 valid questionnaires were included in the analysis, an effective recovery rate of 89.61%. Owing to the specificity of breast cancer as a disease, all the valid samples were female patients with breast cancer, and the statistical information of the samples is shown in Table 1.
2.2 Measurements
All the measurement scales used in this study were in English, and have previously been published elsewhere. The PES scale was adopted from Páez et al., 2015. The Positive Emotion scale was derived from the scale (Diener et al., 2009). Coping self-efficacy was measured using the 12-item CBI-B (Heitzmann et al., 2011), which is a brief, validated scale with four factors and strong reliability (α = 0.84–0.88). The CBI-B is widely used in cancer research due to its efficiency and psychometric strength. The HIA scale was adapted from Howell and Shepperd (2017). The HIA scale used in this study was adapted from the original Information Avoidance Scale (IAS), developed by Howell and Shepperd (2016, 2017). Past studies have shown that reverse-worded items may confuse respondents or cause bias, especially in clinical groups or individuals under cognitive strain (Suárez-Álvarez et al., 2018; Weijters et al., 2013). To improve clarity and reliability, we excluded reverse-scored items and selected four IAS items to measure individuals' tendency to avoid health-related information. Patient demographic information was used as a control variable based on related research (Chen and Zhao, 2021; Ireland et al., 2019; Kübler-Ross, 1997).
First, we created a preliminary translation, invited experts in the field to review it, and discussed the content of translation, thus initially determining the Chinese measurement items. Two breast surgery experts were then invited to revise and adjust the questionnaire to include medically relevant questions. According to relevant studies (Perz et al., 2011; Segrin et al., 2011; Yao et al., 2015) and doctors' recommendations, cancer is classified into early, mid, and advanced stages. All items were measured on a five-point Likert scale, except for the control variables. A scale from “1” to “5” indicates “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” respectively.
3 Data analysis and results
3.1 Homology bias test
In this study, we used Harman's one-way method, which employs principal component analysis without rotation to detect common method bias. The results of the exploratory factor analysis of the 26 question items showed that there were five factors with eigenvalues >1, cumulatively explaining 68.97% of the total variance; the first factor had a methodological explanatory rate of 16.90%, much <50%, indicating that the common methodological bias of this study was within the acceptable range and would not have a serious impact on the results of the study.
3.2 Reliability and validity test
As shown in Table 2, the Cronbach's alpha coefficients for the four variables in this study and the combined reliability CRs were greater than the commonly used criterion of 0.7. This indicates that the scales have good internal consistency and high reliability. Cluster validity can be tested based on standardized factor loadings and mean extracted variance of the variables (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). The standardized factor loadings for each of the question items in the four variables ranged from 0.56 to 0.88, all greater than the critical value of 0.5, and their average variances extracted (AVEs) were all greater than or equal to the recommended value of 0.5. This suggests that the variable scales have good convergent validity.
Using Mplus 8.3, a validated factor analysis was conducted to evaluate the four main variables' discriminant validity. The four-factor model, displayed in Table 3, showed a good fit (χ2 = 663.68, df = 293, χ2/df = 2.27, <3; RMSEA = 0.065, < 0.08; SRMR = 0.042, < 0.05; TLI = 0.922, CFI = 0.930, both >0.9), indicating that the sample data of the present study and the four-factor model has a good matching effect, and the four-factor model outperforms alternative models, showing a better fit. Additionally, the square root of the mean extracted variance of each variable was higher than the other variables' correlation coefficient (Table 4). This demonstrates that the four key variables are different constructs with good discriminant validity in terms of connotations and measurements.
3.3 Correlation analysis of the variables
Table 4 indicates a significant positive correlation between PES, positive emotions, and coping self-efficacy (r = 0.66, p < 0.01; r = 0.50, p < 0.01). Positive emotions were significantly and positively correlated with coping self-efficacy (r = 0.59, p < 0.01). Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation between PES, positive emotion, coping self-efficacy, and HIA behavior (r = −0.56, p < 0.01; r = −0.63, p < 0.01; r = −0.62, p < 0.01). The variables' correlation coefficients' signs and significance support the assumptions of the theoretical model and offer initial evidence for further hypothesis testing.
3.3.1 Collinearity diagnostics by cancer stage
We checked for multicollinearity to ensure the stability of the moderated mediation model. We first ran linear regressions. PES and positive emotion were used to predict coping self-efficacy (CE). Both showed VIF values of 1. This suggested that there was no multicollinearity. Next, we dummy-coded the cancer stage variable. We then created interaction terms between PES and each dummy variable. These interaction terms were used to predict CE. All interactions were significant. None showed signs of serious multicollinearity. To further reduce potential bias, we mean-centered PES. We then regenerated the interaction terms. The results again showed no clear multicollinearity. This supported the model's robustness and interpretability.
3.4 Hypothesis testing
Hypothetical models were constructed using Mplus 8.3, to perform a path analysis and test the relationship between the variables. The best fit was found between the observed data and the hypothetical model, as indicated in Table 5. The results of path analysis using hypothetical model are shown in Figure 4.
With a standardized path coefficient of −0.224 (p < 0.01), PES was found to have a significant negative predictive effect on HIA. The standardized path coefficient of PES affecting positive emotions is 0.693 (p < 0.01), indicating that PES significantly and positively predicted positive emotions. Positive emotions, in turn, had a significant negative effect on HIA, as evidenced by the standardized path coefficient of −0.328 (p < 0.01) for positive emotions' impact on HIA. PES significantly positively predicted coping self-efficacy, as evidenced by the standardized path coefficient of PES affecting coping self-efficacy of 0.193 (p < 0.01). Coping self-efficacy had a significant negative predictive influence on HIA, with a standardized path coefficient of −0.430 (p < 0.01) for HIA. Moreover, positive emotions significantly positively predicted coping self-efficacy, as evidenced by the standardized path coefficient of positive emotions affecting coping self-efficacy of 0.417 (p < 0.01). Thus, H1, H2a, H2b, H3a, and H3b were approved.
Utilizing the PROCESS macro (Model 6), repeated bootstrap sampling was performed. The mediating role of positive emotions and coping self-efficacy on the relationship between PES and HIA was tested using 5,000 sampling repeats and a 95% confidence interval (CI). The outcomes are displayed in Table 6. Positive emotion had a significant mediating role between PES and HIA, the indirect impact value of positive emotion between PES on HIA was −0.227, and the 95% CI (−0.344, −0.101) did not contain 0; therefore, H2 is supported. Coping self-efficacy had a significant mediation effect between PES and HIA, as evidenced by the indirect effect value of −0.083 of coping self-efficacy between PES on HIA and the absence of 0 in the 95% CI (−0.157, −0.023). H3 is therefore supported. Positive emotions and coping self-efficacy had a significant chain-mediating effect, the value of this effect was −0.124, and the 95% CI (−0.202, −0.060) did not contain 0. H4 is therefore supported.
In addition to the indirect effect values, Table 6 also reports the model fit statistics (R2 and F values) for each mediation path. The explained variance (R2) of the three models ranges from 0.469 to 0.545, and all F-tests reached a significant level (p < 0.01), indicating that the models fit well and have strong explanatory power.
To ensure that cancer staging was a suitable moderator, we first tested for group homogeneity. One-way ANOVA and post hoc LSD tests were conducted for PES and CE (Coping Self-Efficacy). The results showed no significant group differences for PES (F = 0.418, p = 0.740) or CE (F = 0.706, p = 0.549). Pairwise comparisons also showed no significant differences between any two groups (all p > 0.05). These findings indicate that the variables were evenly distributed across cancer stages. This satisfies the statistical assumption required for moderation analysis. As Hayes (2018) points out, a moderator does not need to have a significant main effect. Its value lies in how it changes the strength or direction of the predictor–outcome relationship. Based on this, we proceeded to test cancer staging as a moderator in the model.
PROCESS macro (Model 83) was used to test the moderating impact of cancer staging. The results were as follows: First, positive emotion was significantly impacted by the interaction term between PES and cancer staging (β = 0.131, p < 0.05), indicating that the relationship between PES and positive emotion was significantly moderated by cancer staging. A moderating effect analysis was conducted to further explain the relationship between the moderating effects (Figure 5). The results showed that when the stage of disease progression was in a high subgroup (tending to advanced breast cancer), the positive effect of PES on positive emotion was stronger [β = 0.791, 95% CI = (0.674, 0.9090), p < 0.05]; for patients in the low subgroup of disease progression stage (tending toward early breast cancer), the positive effect of PES on positive emotion was weaker, but still reached the significance level [β=0.576, 95% CI = (0.452, 0.699), p < 0.05]. This indicates that the positive effect of PES on positive emotions is enhanced when the stage of disease progression tends to advance, supporting H5.
Second, in the chain mediation of patients' PES through positive emotions and coping self-efficacy with HIA, the product of the path coefficients between the interaction and mediating variables was −0.024 (p < 0.05), suggesting that the chain mediation effect was moderated by cancer staging. In Table 7, when the cancer staging was in the low subgroup (trending toward early stage), the mediation effect value of patients' PES through positive emotions and coping self-efficacy to HIA was [β = −0.189, 95% CI = (−0.294, −0.082), p < 0.05], with a significant chain-mediated effect; When the cancer staging was in a high subgroup (trending toward advanced stage), the mediation effect value of patients' PES through positive emotions and coping self-efficacy to HIA was [β = −0.259, 95% CI = (−0.388, −0.1160), p < 0.05], with a significant chained mediation effect; at both the high and low levels, the indirect effect differed significantly, with a difference of −0.071 [95% CI = (−0.147, −0.013), p < 0.05]. This indicates that the chain-mediated effects of positive emotions and coping self-efficacy between PES and HIA are significantly strengthened among patients with advanced-stage breast cancer. In addition to the full chain mediation, we also examined two simpler mediation paths (Table 8). These were: PES → positive emotion → HIA, and PES → coping self-efficacy → HIA. In Table 8, we tested whether cancer staging moderated these two indirect effects. For the first path (PES → PE → HIA), the indirect effect was significant in both early-stage and advanced-stage groups. However, it was stronger in the advanced-stage group [β = −0.401, 95% CI = (−0.516, −0.276)] than in the early-stage group [β = −0.292, 95% CI = (−0.390, −0.191)]. The index of moderated mediation was −0.067 [95% CI = (−0.131, −0.012)], which confirms that cancer staging significantly influenced this mediation pathway. For the second path (PES → CE → HIA), a similar trend was found. The indirect effect was −0.335 [95% CI = (−0.462, −0.204)] in the advanced-stage group, and β = −0.189 [95% CI = (−0.302, −0.094)] in the early-stage group. The index of moderated mediation was −0.089 [95% CI: (−0.157, −0.011)], which again indicates a significant moderation effect. These results demonstrate that cancer staging affects the full chain of mediation and shapes the strength of each individual mediation path. In both cases, patients at more advanced stages experienced stronger indirect effects. In summary, being in different disease stages significantly moderated the magnitude of the chain-mediated effect, supporting H6.
4 Discussion
4.1 Theoretical implications
The theoretical implications of this study are as follows. First, this quantitative study proposes a conceptual model to explain the reduction of HIA from the perspective of PES in cancer patients during ritual interactions, utilizing the theoretical frameworks of IRCs and social cognition. Existing studies have argued that interpersonal interactions between patients are of significant value (Birkelund and Larsen, 2013), and that peer patients are the best information providers, because of their similar physical experiences (Kulik et al., 2000). Previous studies have rarely explored the mechanisms influencing HIA by considering the PES factors in patient interactions. This study argues that applying the IRCs theory to the enhancement of positive emotions and coping self-efficacy by emotional energy in patients' ritual interactions is also useful in explaining how and why patients with cancer reduce HIA.
Second, we explored a chain mediation mechanism linking PES to HIA through two mediators: positive emotions and coping self-efficacy. The results suggest that PES exerts an indirect effect on HIA through these two factors, in which positive emotions and coping self-efficacy act as separate or linked mediators. Previous research has typically focused on positive emotions and coping self-efficacy as the most direct antecedents (Chasiotis et al., 2021; Hua and Howell, 2022), ignoring the mechanisms by which the two are transmitted between PES and HIA. In contrast, this study integrates them into a unified chain mediation model, offering a more comprehensive understanding of their sequential roles. We conducted internal model comparisons among three competing models: a chain mediation model including both mediators in sequence, and two alternative models each including only one mediator. The chain model demonstrated superior model fit (e.g., CFI = 0.930, RMSEA = 0.065; see Table 5) and a higher R2 for HIA (R2 = 0.545; see Table 6), indicating stronger explanatory power in predicting patients' HIA behavior. Drawing on this chain structure, we identified four possible pathways linking PES and HIA. These findings expand our understanding of how PES works during ritual interactions. They also offer a stronger theoretical foundation for future research on patients' shared emotional experiences.
Third, this study emphasizes the importance of cancer staging as an individual difference that influences emotional and behavioral responses. Considering that disease progression affects patient emotions (Chen and Zhao, 2021; Kübler-Ross, 1997), this study verified the moderating effect of cancer staging on the mediator model, illustrating the interaction between cancer staging and PES on positive emotions and HIA behavior to varying degrees, thereby enriching the theoretical results. By integrating cancer staging into the model, this study extends current theories of health information avoidance. This dynamic perspective deepens our understanding of PES and offers a more nuanced framework for examining patient behavior in cancer care contexts.
4.2 Practical implications
First, PES in peer interactions among breast cancer patients should be actively promoted. Consistent with what Zlobina and Dávila (2022) emphasized, collective PES experiences can lead to positive health behaviors. This study confirms that PES generated in ritual interactions significantly influences patients' HIA behaviors. Given that the majority of breast cancer patients are women—who are generally more receptive to emotionally focused supportive communication (Kunkel and Burleson, 1999), empathy-centered support programs are especially valuable. Hospitals, community organizations, and NGOs should co-develop structured ritual-based group activities (e.g., healing circles, commemorative events, shared creative activities like planting or painting) to promote emotional sharing and trust. Furthermore, clinical staff and patient navigators can play an active role in inviting new patients to these groups through digital platforms, posters in clinics, and direct referrals from physicians.
Second, ritual interaction activities are an effective method of clinical intervention. Consistent with previous studies, ritual activities play a positive role in cancer treatment (Santos et al., 2018; Buchbinder et al., 2009). Therefore, the following ritual interactive activities could be considered for integration into clinical intervention practices: celebrations of traditional Chinese festivals; award ceremonies, such as presenting a “diploma” to patients who have completed chemotherapy; outdoor rituals like patient salons and “anti-cancer star” contests during Pink Ribbon Month; and ceremonial interactions on special occasions such as Women's Day and patients' birthdays.
Third, the cancer stage should be considered when implementing health interventions. This study found that the cancer staging was a particularly important moderating variable, consistent with the findings of Chen and Zhao (2021), patients in Stages I and II were more likely to suppress their emotions. Therefore, it is important for clinicians to implement personalized interventions based on the cancer stage to better promote health information behaviors. For early-stage patients, small-scale expressive interaction activities guided by clinical nurses are organized to help build a sense of safety and alleviate information-related anxiety. For mid-stage patients, patient support meetings are held to stimulate their initiative and enhance self-efficacy. For late-stage patients, carefully designed commemorative rituals affirm their perseverance in treatment and promote the construction of life meaning.
4.3 Limitations and future studies
This study has the following limitations. First, we made no distinctions between geographical differences. The research found that there are differences in the form and effect of patient interaction activities and emotional energy in large hospitals in China's eastern, central, and western cities. In the future, it will be possible to differentiate them according to geographic regions and to compare the differences in the formation mechanism of patients' emotional energy on HIA behavior. Second, this study did not consider the status of the participants. According to IRCs theory, under the status rituals, leaders in the central position or high status of participants have the highest emotional energy, while followers in the periphery of the status of the follower obtain less emotional energy (Collins, 2004). Third, this study explored the mechanism of influence between PES and HIA in patients' ritual interactions, mainly at the individual level, without considering the different forms of ritual interaction. In reality, this should be a complex process with multiple levels and factors. In the future, we can continue to explore other mediating variables that have not been the focus of this study, from the organizational and geographical levels, to continuously improve the theoretical findings from multiple perspectives. Finally, although this study focuses on the maladaptive aspects of HIA, it is important to acknowledge that HIA may also serve as an adaptive coping strategy under specific emotional or contextual conditions. Future studies should pay attention to the situational and adaptive dimensions of HIA, especially regarding how patients balance information engagement with psychological self-protection.
5 Conclusions
Using IRCs and social cognitive theories as a framework, this study proposes a first-stage moderated chain mediation model to investigate the effects of PES on patients' ritual interaction with HIA, with positive emotions and coping self-efficacy as the mediators and cancer staging as the moderator. Our research suggests that PES in the ritual interactions of patients with breast cancer was negatively related to HIA via positive emotions and coping self-efficacy. Our findings highlight the importance of adopting an interactional ritual perspective in understanding patient interactions. Guided by the IRCs theory, the physical presence of other patients with cancer while participating in ritualistic interactive activities allows them to feel emotional energy and positive attitudes, which contribute to their health information behaviors.
Furthermore, our findings show that cancer staging is a regulatory element that moderates HIA reactions to patient PES. Clinicians should implement personalized interventions based on cancer staging to better promote health information behaviors and provide more humane care for patients with breast cancer to stimulate emotional energy, thereby reducing the occurrence of HIA.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of Guizhou Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author contributions
JC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YY: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FD: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the Graduate Student Research Fund Program of Guizhou (Grant number 2024YJSKYJJ156), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant numbers 41861029 and 72462010), the Science and Technology Support Project of Guizhou Science and Technology Department (Grant number 2022-254), and Guiyang Science and Technology Project (Grant number 2022-4-11).
Acknowledgments
The authors sincerely thank all the participants who generously supported this study.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Abbreviations
AVEs, average variances extracted; HIA, health information avoidance; IRC, Interactive Ritual Chains (theory); PES, patients' emotional synchrony perception; CI, confidence interval.
References
Abdullah, M. R., and Wider, W. (2022). The moderating effect of self-efficacy on supervisory support and organizational citizenship behavior. Front. Psychol. 13:961270. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.961270
Altinay, L., Alrawadieh, Z., Tulucu, F., and Arici, H. E. (2023). Reprint of: the effect of hospitableness on positive emotions, experience, and well-being of hospital patients. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 112:103517. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2023.103517
Amin, M. B., Greene, F. L., Edge, S. B., Compton, C. C., Gershenwald, J. E., Brookland, R. K., et al. (2017). The eighth edition AJCC cancer staging manual: continuing to build a bridge from a population-based to a more “personalized” approach to cancer staging. CA Cancer J. Clin. 67, 93–99. doi: 10.3322/caac.21388
Anker, A. E., Reinhart, A. M., and Feeley, T. H. (2011). Health information seeking: a review of measures and methods. Patient Educ. Couns. 82, 346–354. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.12.008
Bandura, A. (1978). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Adv. Behav. Res. Ther. 1, 139–161. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(78)90002-4
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am. Psychol. 37, 122–147. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122
Bandura, A. (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol. Health 13, 623–649. doi: 10.1080/08870449808407422
Batra, M., and Taneja, U. (2022). The role of servicescape on emotions and satisfaction of patients in Indian hospitals. J. Health Manag. 24, 433–439. doi: 10.1177/09720634221109088
Birkelund, R., and Larsen, L. S. (2013). Patient–patient interaction – caring and sharing. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 27, 608–615. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2012.01072.x
Brashers, D. E. (2001). Communication and uncertainty management. J. Commun. 51, 477–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2001.tb02892.x
Brown, A. J., Thaker, P. H., Sun, C. C., Urbauer, D. L., Bruera, E., Bodurka, D. C., and Ramondetta, L. M. (2017). Nothing left to chance? The impact of locus of control on physical and mental quality of life in terminal cancer patients. Support. Care Cancer 25, 1985–1991. doi: 10.1007/s00520-017-3605-z
Bublatzky, F., Alpers, G. W., and Pittig, A. (2017). From avoidance to approach: the influence of threat-of-shock on reward-based decision making. Behav. Res. Ther. 96, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.01.003
Buchbinder, M., Longhofer, J., and McCue, K. (2009). Family routines and rituals when a parent has cancer. Fam. Syst. Health 27, 213–227. doi: 10.1037/a0017005
Calderon, C., Gomez, D., Carmona-Bayonas, A., Hernandez, R., Ghanem, I., Gil Raga, M., et al. (2021). Social support, coping strategies and sociodemographic factors in women with breast cancer. Clin. Transl. Oncol. 23, 1955–1960. doi: 10.1007/s12094-021-02592-y
Case, D. O., Andrews, J. E., Johnson, J. D., and Allard, S. L. (2005). Avoiding versus seeking: the relationship of information seeking to avoidance, blunting, coping, dissonance, and related concepts. J. Med. Libr. Assoc. 93, 353–362.
Chasiotis, A., Wedderhoff, O., Rosman, T., and Mayer, A.-K. (2021). The role of approach and avoidance motivation and emotion regulation in coping via health information seeking. Curr. Psychol. 40, 5235–5244. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00488-3
Chen, M., and Zhao, L. (2021). Mapping breast cancer survivors' psychosocial coping along disease trajectory: a language approach. J. Health Psychol. 26, 2563–2576. doi: 10.1177/1359105320919893
Cheong, J. H., Molani, Z., Sadhukha, S., and Chang, L. J. (2023). Synchronized affect in shared experiences strengthens social connection. Commun. Biol. 6:1099. doi: 10.1038/s42003-023-05461-2
Chesney, M. A., Neilands, T. B., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M., and Folkman, S. (2006). A validity and reliability study of the coping self-efficacy scale. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 421–437. doi: 10.1348/135910705X53155
Chirico, A., Lucidi, F., Merluzzi, T., Alivernini, F., De laurentiis, M., Botti, G., and Giordano, A. (2017). A meta-analytic review of the relationship of cancer coping self-efficacy with distress and quality of life. Oncotarget 8, 36800–36811. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.15758
Clarke, J. M., and Waring, J. (2018). The transformative role of interaction rituals within therapeutic communities. Sociol. Health Illn. 40, 1277–1293. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12773
Collins, R. (1993). Emotional energy as the common denominator of rational action. Ration. Soc. 5, 203–230. doi: 10.1177/1043463193005002005
Deline, M. B., and Kahlor, L. A. (2019). Planned risk information avoidance: a proposed theoretical model. Commun. Theory 29, 272–294. doi: 10.1093/ct/qty035
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., and Oishi, S. (2009). “New measures of well-being,” in Assessing Well-Being, Ed. E. Diener (Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands), 247–266.
Durkheim, E. (1915). The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life (J. W. Swain, Trans.). New York, NY: Macmillan. (Original work published 1912).
Emanuel, A. S., Kiviniemi, M. T., Howell, J. L., Hay, J. L., Waters, E. A., Orom, H., and Shepperd, J. A. (2015). Avoiding cancer risk information. Soc. Sci. Med. 147, 113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.10.058
Folkman, S., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. Am. Psychol. 55, 647–654. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.6.647
Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104
Franconeri, S. L., Alvarez, G. A., and Cavanagh, P. (2013). Flexible cognitive resources: competitive content maps for attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.010
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218
Fredrickson, B. L., and Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cogn. Emot. 19, 313–332. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238
Gabriel, S., Valenti, J., Naragon-Gainey, K., and Young, A. F. (2017). The psychological importance of collective assembly: development and validation of the tendency for effervescent assembly measure (TEAM). Psychol. Assess. 29, 1349–1362. doi: 10.1037/pas0000434
Ganguly, A., and Tasoff, J. (2017). Fantasy and dread: the demand for information and the consumption utility of the future. Manag. Sci. 63, 4037–4060. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2550
Geng, Z., Ogbolu, Y., Wang, J., Hinds, P. S., Qian, H., and Yuan, C. (2018). Gauging the effects of self-efficacy, social support, and coping style on self-management behaviors in Chinese cancer survivors. Cancer Nurs. 41, E1–E10. doi: 10.1097/NCC.0000000000000571
Golman, R., Hagmann, D., and Loewenstein, G. (2017). Information avoidance. J. Econ. Lit. 55, 96–135. doi: 10.1257/jel.20151245
Graham, J. (2024). Breast cancer: the psychological impact of diagnosis, treatment, and remission. Cureus 16:e70814. doi: 10.7759/cureus.70814
Guo, Y., Lu, Z., Kuang, H., and Wang, C. (2020). Information avoidance behavior on social network sites: information irrelevance, overload, and the moderating role of time pressure. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 52:102067. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102067
Guo, X., and Wang, Z. (2007). Concept, function and meaning of positive emotion. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 15, 810–815.
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 2nd ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Heitzmann, C. A., Merluzzi, T. V., Jean-Pierre, P., Roscoe, J. A., Kirsh, K. L., and Passik, S. D. (2011). Assessing self-efficacy for coping with cancer: development and psychometric analysis of the brief version of the cancer behavior inventory (CBI-B). Psychooncology 20, 302–312. doi: 10.1002/pon.1735
Henckes, N., and Nurok, M. (2015). “The first pulse you take is your own” – but don't forget your colleagues: emotion teamwork in pre-hospital emergency medical services. Sociol. Health Illn. 37, 1023–1038. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12261
Hertwig, R., and Engel, C. (2016). Homo ignorans: deliberately choosing not to know. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 359–372. doi: 10.1177/1745691616635594
Hou, W.-H., Huang, Y.-J., Lee, Y., Chen, C.-T., Lin, G.-H., and Hsieh, C.-L. (2018). Validation of the integrated model of health literacy in patients with breast cancer. Cancer Nurs. 41, 498–505. doi: 10.1097/NCC.0000000000000540
Howell, J. L., Lipsey, N. P., and Shepperd, J. A. (2020). “Health information avoidance,” in The Wiley Encyclopedia of Health Psychology, 1st ed., Eds. K. Sweeny, M. L. Robbins, and L. M. Cohen (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 279–286.
Howell, J. L., and Shepperd, J. A. (2016). Establishing an information avoidance scale. Psychological Assessment. 28, 1695–1708. doi: 10.1037/pas0000315
Howell, J. L., and Shepperd, J. A. (2017). Social exclusion, self-affirmation, and health information avoidance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 68, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.05.005
Hua, J., and Howell, J. L. (2022). Coping self-efficacy influences health information avoidance. J. Health Psychol. 27, 713–725. doi: 10.1177/1359105320965664 .
Ireland, A. V., Finnegan-John, J., Hubbard, G., Scanlon, K., and Kyle, R. G. (2019). Walking groups for women with breast cancer: mobilising therapeutic assemblages of walk, talk and place. Soc. Sci. Med. 231, 38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.016
Karademas, E. C., Roziner, I., Mazzocco, K., Pat-Horenczyk, R., Sousa, B., Oliveira-Maia, A. J., et al. (2022). The mutual determination of self-efficacy to cope with cancer and cancer-related coping over time: a prospective study in women with breast cancer. Psychol. Health 38:1635–1648. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2022.2038157
Karademas, E. C., Simos, P., Pat-Horenczyk, R., Roziner, I., Mazzocco, K., Sousa, B., et al. (2021). Cognitive, emotional, and behavioral mediators of the impact of coping self-efficacy on adaptation to breast cancer: an international prospective study. Psychooncology 30, 1555–1562. doi: 10.1002/pon.5730
Kulik, J. A., Shelby, D., and Cooper, R. N. (2000). The effects of fellow patients on the emotional well-being and satisfaction with care of postoperative cosmetic surgery patients. Plast. Reconstr. Surg. 106, 1407–1414; discussion 1415–1416. doi: 10.1097/00006534-200011000-00033
Kunkel, A. W., and Burleson, B. R. (1999). Assessing explanations for sex differences in emotional support: a test of the different cultures and skill specialization accounts. Hum. Commun. Res. 25, 307–340. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1999.tb00448.x
Lambert, S. D., and Loiselle, C. G. (2007). Health information-seeking behavior. Qual. Health Res. 17, 1006–1019. doi: 10.1177/1049732307305199
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. Am. Psychol. 46, 819–834. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.46.8.819
Lee, S. Y., Hwang, H., Hawkins, R., and Pingree, S. (2008). Interplay of negative emotion and health self-efficacy on the use of health information and its outcomes. Commun. Res. 35, 358–381. doi: 10.1177/0093650208315962
Legg, A. M., Andrews, S. E., Ho, H., Ghane, A., Tabuenca, A., and Sweeny, K. (2015). Patients' anxiety and hope: predictors and adherence intentions in an acute care context. Health Expect. 18, 3034–3043. doi: 10.1111/hex.12288
Link, E. (2022). “Health information avoidance,” in The International Encyclopedia of Health Communication, 1st ed., Eds. E. Y. Ho, C. L. Bylund, and J. C. M. Van Weert (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 1–6.
Link, E., and Baumann, E. (2022). Explaining cancer information avoidance: comparing people with and without cancer experience in the family. Psychooncology 31, 442–449. doi: 10.1002/pon.5826
Lipsey, N. P., and Shepperd, J. A. (2019). Powerful audiences are linked to health information avoidance: results from two surveys. Soc. Sci. Med. 225, 51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.01.046
Lu, Q., Link, E., Baumann, E., and Schulz, P. J. (2024). Linking patient-centered communication with cancer information avoidance: the mediating roles of patient trust and literacy. Patient Educ. Couns. 123:108230. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2024.108230
Manne, S., Glassman, M., and Du Hamel, K. (2001). Intrusion, avoidance, and psychological distress among individuals with cancer. Psychosom. Med. 63, 658–667. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00019
Marx-Fleck, S., Junker, N. M., Artinger, F., and van Dick, R. (2021). Defensive decision making: operationalization and the relevance of psychological safety and job insecurity from a conservation of resources perspective. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 94, 616–644. doi: 10.1111/joop.12353
Mayne, T. J., and Bonanno, G. A. (eds.), (2001). Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Melnyk, D., and Shepperd, J. A. (2012). Avoiding risk information about breast cancer. Ann. Behav. Med. 44, 216–224. doi: 10.1007/s12160-012-9382-5
Myrick, J. G., Willoughby, J. F., and Verghese, R. S. (2016). How and why young adults do and do not search for health information: cognitive and affective factors. Health Educ. J. 75, 208–219. doi: 10.1177/0017896915571764
National Health Commission of the People's Republic of China (2022). Breast cancer diagnosis and treatment guidelines (2022 edition). Chin. J. Ration. Drug Use Explor. 19, 1–26.
Nelissen, S., Beullens, K., Lemal, M., and Van den Bulck, J. (2015). Fear of cancer is associated with cancer information seeking, scanning and avoiding: a cross-sectional study among cancer diagnosed and non-diagnosed individuals. Health Inf. Libr. J. 32, 107–119. doi: 10.1111/hir.12100
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., and Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings: a model of collective effervescence. Front. Psychol. 6:710. doi: 10.1037/t41745-000
Pampel, F. C., Krueger, P. M., and Denney, J. T. (2010). Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 36, 349–370. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102529
Perz, J., Ussher, J. M., Butow, P., and Wain, G. (2011). Gender differences in cancer carer psychological distress: an analysis of moderators and mediators. Eur. J. Cancer Care 20, 610–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2354.2011.01257.x
Pizarro, J. J., Basabe, N., Amutio, A., Telletxea, S., Harizmendi, M., and Van Gordon, W. (2020). The mediating role of shared flow and perceived emotional synchrony on compassion for others in a mindful-dancing program. Mindfulness 11, 125–139. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01200-z
Price, D. M. (2018). Evidence for Information Avoidance as a Barrier to HIV Testing: A Two Study Test of Theory and Intervention. Doctoral Dissertation. Storrs: University of Connecticut.
Ritchie, J. R. B., and Hudson, S. (2009). Understanding and meeting the challenges of consumer/tourist experience research. Int. J. Tour. Res. 11, 111–126. doi: 10.1002/jtr.721
Santos, S., Crespo, C., Canavarro, M. C., and Kazak, A. E. (2018). Family rituals when children have cancer: a qualitative study. J. Fam. Psychol. 32, 643–653. doi: 10.1037/fam0000419
Segrin, C., Badger, T. A., and Figueredo, A. J. (2011). Stage of disease progression moderates the association between social support and depression in prostate cancer survivors. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 29, 552–560. doi: 10.1080/07347332.2011.599362
Siebenhaar, K. U., Köther, A. K., and Alpers, G. W. (2020). Dealing with the COVID-19 infodemic: distress by information, information avoidance, and compliance with preventive measures. Front. Psychol. 11:567905. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567905
Smit, A., Coetzee, B. J., Roomaney, R., Bradshaw, M., and Swartz, L. (2019). Women's stories of living with breast cancer: a systematic review and meta-synthesis of qualitative evidence. Soc. Sci. Med. 222, 231–245. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.01.020
Soroya, S. H., Farooq, A., Mahmood, K., Isoaho, J., and Zara, S. (2021). From information seeking to information avoidance: understanding the health information behavior during a global health crisis. Inf. Process. Manag. 58:102440. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2020.102440
Sterchele, D. (2020). Memorable tourism experiences and their consequences: an interaction ritual (IR) theory approach. Ann. Tour. Res. 81:102847. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2019.102847
Suárez-Álvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L., García-Cueto, E., Cuesta, M., and Muñiz, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: a questionable practice. Psicothema 30, 149–158. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.33
Szymczak, J. E. (2016). Infections and interaction rituals in the organisation: clinician accounts of speaking up or remaining silent in the face of threats to patient safety. Sociol. Health Illn. 38, 325–339. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12371
Taechaboonsermsak, P., Kaewkungwal, J., Singhasivanon, P., Fungladda, W., and Wilailak, S. (2005). Causal relationship between health promoting behavior and quality of life in cervical cancer patients undergoing radiotherapy. Southeast Asian J. Trop. Med. Public Health 36, 1568–1575.
Ussher, J., Kirsten, L., Butow, P., and Sandoval, M. (2006). What do cancer support groups provide which other supportive relationships do not? The experience of peer support groups for people with cancer. Soc. Sci. Med. 62, 2565–2576. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.10.034
Weijters, B., Baumgartner, H., and Schillewaert, N. (2013). Reversed item bias: an integrative model. Psychol. Methods 18, 320–334. doi: 10.1037/a0032121
Wilkinson, L., and Gathani, T. (2022). Understanding breast cancer as a global health concern. Br. J. Radiol. 95:20211033. doi: 10.1259/bjr.20211033
Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Commun. Monogr. 59, 329–349. doi: 10.1080/03637759209376276
Wlodarczyk, A., Zumeta, L., Pizarro, J. J., Bouchat, P., Hatibovic, F., Basabe, N., and Rimé, B. (2020). Perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings: validation of a short scale and proposition of an integrative measure. Front. Psychol. 11:1721. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01721
Wood, E. H. (2020). I remember how we all felt: perceived emotional synchrony through tourist memory sharing. J. Travel Res. 59, 1339–1352. doi: 10.1177/0047287519888290
World Health Organization. (2024). Breast cancer. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/breast-cancer (Accessed June 23, 2025).
Xiang, K., Huang, W.-J., Gao, F., and Lai, Q. (2022a). COVID-19 prevention in hotels: ritualized host-guest interactions. Ann. Tour. Res. 93:103376. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2022.103376
Xiang, K., Zhang, H., and Qiao, G. (2022b). How to create interactive rituals: key factors and strategic configurations for host-guest interaction in boutique hotels. J. Hosp. Tour. Manag. 52, 428–438. doi: 10.1016/j.jhtm.2022.08.002
Xie, J., and Li, X. (2023). From theatrical elements to festival attendees' perceived emotional synchrony: the perspective of interaction ritual chain theory. J. Hosp. Tour. Manag. 56, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jhtm.2023.06.016
Yang, M. Z., King, W. C., Oettingen, G., and Sheeran, P. (2023). Evidence that mental contrasting reduces health information avoidance. Ann. Behav. Med. 57, 687–692. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaad031
Yang, Z. J., and Kahlor, L. (2013). What, me worry? The role of affect in information seeking and avoidance. Sci. Commun. 35, 189–212. doi: 10.1177/1075547012441873
Yang, Z. J., McComas, K. A., Gay, G., Leonard, J. P., Dannenberg, A. J., and Dillon, H. (2011). Information seeking related to clinical trial enrollment. Commun. Res. 38, 856–882. doi: 10.1177/0093650210380411
Yao, F., Zhang, C., Du, W., Liu, C., and Xu, Y. (2015). Identification of gene-expression signatures and protein markers for breast cancer grading and staging. PLoS ONE 10:e0138213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138213
Yu, X. Q., Luo, Q., Kahn, C., O'Connell, D. L., and Houssami, N. (2015). Temporal trends show improved breast cancer survival in Australia but widening urban-rural differences. Breast 24, 524–527. doi: 10.1016/j.breast.2015.03.006
Yu, Z., and Na, M. (2022). Experiential value of volunteer tourism: the perspective of interaction ritual chains. Tour. Stud. 22, 348–372. doi: 10.1177/14687976221115230
Zeelenberg, M., Nelissen, R. M. A., Breugelmans, S. M., and Pieters, R. (2008). On emotion specificity in decision making: why feeling is for doing. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 3, 18–27. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500000139
Zhang, N., and Gao, B. J. (2021). Research on the formation and influencing factors of the negative use behavior of online health information services for the elderly under the CAC paradigm. Libr. Inf. Serv. 65, 96–104.
Zhao, S., and Liu, Y. (2021). The more insufficient, the more avoidance? Cognitive and affective factors that relate to information behaviours in acute risks. Front. Psychol. 12:730068. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.730068
Zhou, G., Liu, Y., Hu, J., and Cao, X. (2023). The effect of tourist-to-tourist interaction on tourists' behavior: the mediating effects of positive emotions and memorable tourism experiences. J. Hosp. Tour. Manag. 55, 161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jhtm.2023.03.005
Keywords: perceived emotional synchrony, positive emotions, coping self-efficacy, health information avoidance, breast cancer, a moderated chain mediation model
Citation: Chen J, Yang Y, Du F and Li J (2025) When breast cancer patients participate in ritual interactive activities: the mechanism of perceived emotional synchrony on health information avoidance. Front. Psychol. 16:1566773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1566773
Received: 25 January 2025; Accepted: 16 July 2025;
Published: 31 July 2025.
Edited by:
Federica Mauro, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyReviewed by:
María Cantero-García, Universidad a Distancia de Madrid, SpainQiaofei Wu, National University of Singapore, Singapore
Copyright © 2025 Chen, Yang, Du and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yang Yang, c2NvdHRnem51QHllYWgubmV0