Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 September 2025

Sec. Perception Science

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1600306

Simulated intra-individual skin color changes and their impact on facial attractiveness across Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian faces

  • 1MIRAI Technology Institute, Shiseido Co., Ltd., Yokohama, Japan
  • 2Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Informatics, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan

Introduction: Understanding the influence of facial features on attractiveness is important for the development of cosmetics that improve attractiveness. Although it is known that changes in skin color affect attractiveness, few studies have investigated whether changes in skin color within individuals affect attractiveness, or whether these effects vary across geographic and cultural contexts. Therefore, through online surveys, we aimed to investigate changes in visual attractiveness due to intra-individual changes in skin color, using images of Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian faces.

Method: Ten patterns of skin color change were applied to the average faces of Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian men and women in their 20s. Using online surveys,1,200 Japanese, 1,518 Chinese, and 1,200 Italian participants were asked to rate the attractiveness of the facial images on a 5-point Likert scale.

Results: Changes in attractiveness associated with changes in skin color were observed in all surveys. Furthermore, we found geographic, cultural, and sex differences in changes in attractiveness associated with changes in skin color.

Conclusion: The results indicate that intra-individual changes in skin color can alter facial attractiveness. Furthermore, changes in attractiveness in response to skin color changes may vary according to facial sex and geographic and cultural backgrounds.

1 Introduction

Individuals use cosmetics to improve their appearance and attractiveness. Makeup can correct skin brightness and color (Kim et al., 2023). Skin color, a major factor that impacts attractiveness (Fink et al., 2006; Fink et al., 2012), is affected by melanin and hemoglobin levels. Skin color can change owing to increased melanin from sun exposure, oxygenation and deoxygenation of the blood, and stress (Mohiuddin, 2019; Stephen et al., 2009; Watanabe and Kageyama, 2017). Moreover, these changes can occur in a short period of time, from a few hours to a few days (Clydesdale et al., 2001; Hönigsmann et al., 1986; Leary et al., 1992). If changes in an individual’s skin color has a negative impact on attractiveness, cosmetics are an effective method to manage skin color (Kim et al., 2023). Furthermore, the impact of skin color change on attractiveness may vary according to geographic and cultural backgrounds (Caisey et al., 2006; Yamamoto et al., 2002). Understanding these variations is crucial for the development of global cosmetic products.

Various studies have investigated the relationship between skin color and facial attractiveness. Increased facial redness in Caucasian male faces increased attractiveness through improved health (Thorstenson et al., 2017). Meanwhile, dark skin was reportedly more attractive than bright skin on the faces of Caucasian females (Fink et al., 2001). Apparent health has been reported to influence facial attractiveness (Jones et al., 2016). The association between attractiveness and apparent health has been examined not only in Western but also in Japanese faces (Rhodes et al., 2007). Skin redness indicates increased blood perfusion and oxygenation and increases apparent health (Stephen et al., 2009). Melanin and carotenoids influence skin brightness and yellowness; increased yellowness and decreased brightness in Caucasian faces are known to increase apparent health (Stephen et al., 2012). There was a positive correlation between facial yellowness and health in Caucasian females (Henderson et al., 2016). Skin color can evoke the state of an individual’s blood circulation and carotenoids absorbed from fruits and vegetable consumption, which can alter one’s perceived health (Stephen et al., 2009; Stephen et al., 2012). Moreover, perceived health is correlated with attractiveness, and skin color is considered a factor that influence attractiveness (Jones et al., 2004). Facial symmetry and averageness also affect attractiveness (Grammer and Thornhill, 1994; Jones et al., 2001). However, skin color is more susceptible to change compared with facial symmetry and averageness, which may make it a useful indicator of health status and attractiveness (Little et al., 2011).

In addition to apparent health, the relationship between skin color and attractiveness is influenced by geographic and cultural backgrounds. In Asian countries, skin brightness is an important indicator of beauty. For example, in Japan, skin brightness is associated with beauty, and brightening cosmetics are favored (Ashikari, 2005; Li et al., 2008). Meanwhile, tanning is prevalent among Caucasians in the West, and darker skin is preferred (Melia and Bulman, 1995). It has been suggested that Chinese people prefer increased brightness and decreased yellowness compared to Caucasians, who prefer increased redness more than the Chinese (Han et al., 2018). In contrast, Malaysian Chinese people reportedly chose increased yellowing over increased redness to indicate a healthier appearance (Tan and Stephen, 2019). In a study using Japanese, Chinese, Thai, and Caucasian facial images, He et al. (2024) reported that East Asian cultures rated reddish skin tones positively, while Caucasians rated yellowish skin tones positively. Thus, skin color preferences vary according to geographic conditions and cultural differences.

The relationship between facial features and impressions has been studied experimentally using facial images (Jaeger et al., 2018; Samson et al., 2010). These studies were conducted in laboratories using print media or computer displays, and evaluated under appropriately controlled lighting conditions. Uniform visual conditions of stimuli presentation reduce noise during the evaluation. However, recently, online surveys have been used to evaluate facial images (Marini et al., 2022; Takehara et al., 2023). In online surveys, the experimenter has no control over the participant’s monitor or lighting conditions, which may lead to larger errors in stimulus evaluation. However, on comparing the results of laboratory experiments and online surveys, it was found that a sufficient sample size could effectively cancel out evaluation noise (Hirao et al., 2021). Online surveys do not require experimenters to prepare special laboratory equipment or travel and can be used to recruit a large number of participants from a wide range of geographic areas. In the present study, online surveys were used to recruit participants from different regions.

The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of intra-individual skin color changes on attractiveness using averaged male and female facial images, as well as to identify differences in geographic and cultural backgrounds. We qualitatively compared the relationship between attractiveness and skin color changes in countries with both near and distant geographic and cultural backgrounds. The study was conducted in Japan and China, which are relatively close geographically and culturally, as well as Italy, which is relatively different. Japan and China, located in East Asia, reportedly share a preference for white skin color (Li et al., 2008). However, while China is influenced by Western culture, the preference for whiteness in Japan may be based on their racial identity, which suggests that the cultural backgrounds may differ (Ashikari, 2005; Li et al., 2008). Italy, located in Europe, is geographically distant from the other two countries, and Caucasians tend to prefer darker skin (Caisey et al., 2006; Melia and Bulman, 1995). As a reference value for the short-term intra-individual changes in skin tone, we used the skin tone change value 1 h after applying a mild psychological stress load, in a previous experiment conducted by the authors (Koizumi et al., 2023, data not shown). The skin color change reflected blood retention and deoxygenation caused by capillary constriction due to psychological stress, and the skin color changed through decreasing L* and b* and increasing a* values. The CIELab color space allows for color representation based on human vision (Martinkauppi, 2002). The Lab* color space is a colorimetric system where L* represents lightness and a* and b* indicate the position on the red-green and yellow-blue axes, respectively, which provides a comprehensive measure of skin color attributes. A decrease in L* corresponds to a darker skin tone, an increase in a* indicates greater redness, and a decrease in b* is associated with stronger blue components. Although Koizumi et al. (2023) conducted their study on Japanese males only, the same skin color change rates were used for the male and female Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian facial images in this study. By applying these skin color change rates, facial images that reproduced skin color changes within individuals were created and used in online surveys in each country. In addition, to understand the effect of each L* a* b* value on attractiveness, we created images in which only brightness was changed, only redness was changed, and both brightness and redness were changed, assuming that cosmetics are used to control skin color. Clarifying the effects of skin color changes on attractiveness will advance the development of skincare and makeup products that improve personal attractiveness.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Surveys were conducted in Japan, China, and Italy to investigate the impact of skin color changes on attractiveness. In the Japanese study, 1,200 healthy individuals living in Japan (600 men and 600 women; mean age: 40.0 years; SD: 11.2) participated in the survey. In the Chinese study, 1,518 healthy individuals who lived in China (717 men and 801 women; mean age: 38.9 years; SD: 10.4) participated in the survey. In the Italian study, 1,200 healthy individuals who lived in Italy (600 men and 600 women; mean age: 39.5 years; SD: 11.5) participated in the survey. This number was considered sufficient to detect a 10% difference in selection rate in a chi-squared test with α = 0.05 and 1 − β = 0.8. None of the participants indicated optical disorders via self-report before the experiment, and all of them owned a personal computer or tablet, excluding mobile phones with small displays. All the participants provided informed consent. The Research Ethics Committee of the Shiseido Global Innovation Center approved this study and all methods followed the approved guidelines.

2.2 Stimuli

The Japanese survey used 20 averaged computer generated (CG) Japanese facial photographs as visual stimuli. We also used two original average faces (a man and woman in their 20s) created in a previous study (Hirao et al., 2021). Facial photographs to create the average face were taken via a digital camera in an environment where the lighting conditions were kept constant. The hair was secured to avoid covering the face, and the digital camera was positioned directly in front of the face with the eyes open. Images were saved as 1,272 × 1,598 pixels and the average face was created via Photoshop CS4 (version 11; Adobe Inc., 2008). The shape of the facial image was averaged based on the marker point coordinates of the acquired facial image. Skin color was adjusted based on skin color data for the same age and sex, as in previous studies. The same procedure was used to create the original average faces of women in their 20s. To investigate the influence of skin color changes on facial impressions, images were created in which only L* decreased from the original image (Dark), images in which a* increased and b* decreased (Red), and images in which L* and b* decreased and a* increased (Dark & Red). The values (data not shown) obtained in a previous study were used (Koizumi et al., 2023). The skin color change value was the average increase/decrease rate and standard deviation (SD) of cheek L* a* b* values of 13 Japanese men 1 h after mild psychological stress loading, and was used as the reference value for skin color changes that occurred within the same person within a few hours. In addition, to examine the effect of the degree of skin color change, we created images with an average change (+), 1SD change (++) and 2SD change (+++). Skin color was manipulated using the average increase/decrease rate and SD so that L* and b* decreased and a* increased. Table 1 lists the skin color change rates. Ten images were created: one original image, three dark images (+, ++, +++), three red images (+, ++, +++), and three Dark & Red images (+, ++, ++). These were created for each age group and sex, resulting in 20 total images. These images are shown in Figure 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The skin color change rates.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average Japanese facial images. The original average face was created by averaging eight facial images of Japanese men and women in their 20s, obtained in a previous study (unpublished). The shape of the facial image was averaged based on the marker point coordinates of the acquired facial image. Skin color was adjusted based on skin color data for the same age and sex, as in previous studies. “Dark” is a facial image in which only L* is reduced from the original facial image. “Red” is a facial image in which a* is increased and b* is decreased from the original facial image. Dark&Red is a facial image in which L* and b* are decreased and a* is increased from the original facial image. In addition, to examine the effect of the degree of skin color change, we created images with an average change (+), 1SD change (++), and 2SD change (+++). The values (data not shown) obtained in a previous study were used (Koizumi et al., 2023). Created with FantaMorph.

The same procedure was used to create 20 Chinese and Caucasian face images for the Chinese and Italian surveys. The original average face was created by averaging eight images of Chinese or Caucasian men in their 20s obtained in a previous study (unpublished). The average Caucasian male and female faces were created from facial images of eight Caucasian men recruited in France and eight Caucasian women recruited in Italy, respectively. Skin color was adjusted based on skin color data for the same age and sex, as obtained in a previous study (unpublished data). The same procedure was used to create the original average face of women in their 20s. Ten images were created for both sexes by adjusting L*, a*, and b* in the same skin color change rates as in the Japanese study. The resulting Chinese and Caucasian facial images are shown in Figures 2, 3, respectively.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Average Chinese facial images. The original average face was created by averaging eight facial images of Chinese men and women in their 20s, obtained in a previous study (unpublished). The shape of the facial image was averaged based on the marker point coordinates of the acquired facial image. Skin color was adjusted based on skin color data for the same age and sex, as in previous studies. “Dark” is a facial image in which only L* is reduced from the original facial image. “Red” is a facial image in which a* is increased and b* is decreased from the original facial image. Dark&Red is a facial image in which L* and b* are decreased and a* is increased from the original facial image. In addition, to examine the effect of the degree of skin color change, we created images with an average change (+), 1SD change (++), and 2SD change (+++). The values (data not shown) obtained in a previous study were used (Koizumi et al., 2023). Created with FantaMorph.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Average Caucasian facial images. The original average face was created by averaging eight facial images of Caucasian men and women in their 20s, obtained in a previous study (unpublished). The shape of the facial image was averaged based on the marker point coordinates of the acquired facial image. Skin color was adjusted based on skin color data for the same age and sex, as in previous studies. “Dark” is a facial image in which only L* is reduced from the original facial image. “Red” is a facial image in which a* is increased and b* is decreased from the original facial image. Dark&Red is a facial image in which L* and b* are decreased and a* is increased from the original facial image. In addition, to examine the effect of the degree of skin color change, we created images with an average change (+), 1SD change (++), and 2SD change (+++). The values (data not shown) obtained in a previous study were used (Koizumi et al., 2023). Created with FantaMorph.

2.3 Questionnaire

The questionnaire included two perceptual items that used bipolar scales (bright–dark and red–yellow) and “attractiveness” impression of the face measured using a monopole scale. Perceptual questions were used to confirm how the participants perceived the skin color of the facial images. All questionnaire items used a five-point Likert scale, with scores ranging from −2 to +2. For example, the bright–dark dipole scale appeared as follows: +2, “match to bright”; +1, “a slight match to bright”; 0, neutral; −1, “match to dark”; and −2, “a slight match to dark.” The monopole scale categories appeared as: +2, “I think so very much”; +1, “I think so”; 0, neutral; −1, “I do not think so”; and −2, “I do not think so very much.”

2.4 Procedure

The participants were divided into two groups: one evaluated facial images of men in their 20s and the other evaluated facial images of women in their 20s. The participants viewed the stimuli on their personal computers or tablets and completed the questionnaire at home. At the beginning of the questionnaire, the participants were requested to select the terminal that they were using to respond to the questionnaire. Participants who chose any option other than desktops, laptops, or tablets were excluded. The facial images were evaluated individually. One facial image was displayed at the center of the monitor, and two perceptual questions were presented below the image. When the participant answered the questions and clicked, the facial image changed. This process was repeated for 10 images. Afterwards, a facial image was displayed at the center of the monitor, and questions about attractiveness were displayed below. The facial images were changed each time the question was answered, and the process was repeated for 10 images. Images and questions were switched at the participants’ pace, and there was no time limit for responses. Order of appearance of the images was randomized. The survey in Japan was conducted by Nippon Information Inc., while the surveys in China and Italy were conducted by Glocal Insight Co., Ltd. Each survey’s interface was adjusted to ensure a consistent format.

2.5 Statistical analysis

The proportion of participants who selected “match to bright (or red)” and “a slight match to bright (or red)” for the perception question was calculated (impression score). The impression score for the attractiveness question was calculated as the proportion of participants who selected “I think so very much” and “I think so.” A chi-squared test was used to determine whether there was a significant difference from the original average face. The null hypothesis was that there would be no difference in the ratio of the number of respondents to the questions for the original facial images. p-values were corrected using the Bonferroni method. The significance level α was set at α = 0.05.

3 Results

3.1 The Japanese study

Table 2 shows the impression scores from the perception questionnaire for Japanese facial images. For the item “Looks bright,” impression scores of the Dark (+, ++, and +++) and Dark&Red (+, ++, and +++) images of male and female faces significantly decreased compared to the original facial image. For men in their 20s, in the Red face, impression scores of “Looks bright” decreased even though L* was not changed [Red+: χ2 (1) = 12.07, p = 0.009; Red++: χ2 (1) = 12.71, p = 0.007; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 23.48, p < 0.001]. In contrast, for women in their 20s, in the Red face, impression scores of “Looks bright” increased [Red++: χ2 (1) = 59.63, p < 0.001, Red+++: χ2 (1) = 63.50, p < 0.001].

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The impression score on the perception questionnaire for Japanese facial images.

For the item “Looks red,” impression scores of male Red and Dark&Red faces increased [20s male Red+: χ2 (1) = 41.52, p < 0.001; 20s male Red++: χ2 (1) = 729.51, p < 0.001; 20s male Red+++: χ2 (1) = 2173.00, p < 0.001; 20s male Dark&Red+: χ2 (1) = 129.50, p < 0.001; 20s male Dark&Red++: χ2 (1) = 342.59, p < 0.001; 20s male Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 774.63, p < 0.001]. For women in their 20s, impression scores of Red++, Red+++, Dark&Red++, and Dark&Red+++ significantly increased [Red++: χ2 (1) = 235.22, p < 0.001; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 1538.21, p < 0.001; Dark&Red++: χ2 (1) = 54.40, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 1136.87, p < 0.001]. No increase or decrease in redness was observed in the facial images in which a* did not change.

Table 3 shows the impression scores for the attractiveness of Japanese facial images. For male faces, the impression score of attractiveness of the Dark+++ face significantly decreased (χ2 (1) = 9.69, p = 0.017). In comparison, impression scores of attractiveness of male Red+ and Red++ faces significantly increased [Red+: χ2 (1) = 10.25, p = 0.012; Red++: χ2 (1) = 12.71, p = 0.007; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 10.25, p = 0.012]. For female faces, impression scores of Dark and Dark&Red faces significantly decreased [Dark+: χ2 (1) = 58.89, p < 0.001; Dark++: χ2 (1) = 100.70, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 143.39, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+: χ2 (1) = 9.73, p = 0.016; Dark&Red++: χ2 (1) = 13.77, p = 0.002; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 20.74, p < 0.001]. In contrast, impression scores of female Red++ and Red+++ faces significantly increased [Red++: χ2 (1) = 95.69, p < 0.001; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 45.51, p < 0.001].

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The impression score for attractiveness of Japanese facial images.

3.2 The Chinese study

Table 4 shows the impression scores of the perception questionnaires for Chinese facial images. For the category “Looks bright,” impression scores of the Dark++, Dark+++, Dark&Red+, and Dark&Red+++ facial images of men in their 20s significantly decreased compared to the original facial image [Dark++: χ2 (1) = 63.43, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 90.77, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 21.99, p < 0.001]. For the faces of women in their 20s, impression scores of Dark+, Dark++, Dark+++, and Dark&Red+++ faces decreased compared to the original facial image [Dark+: χ2 (1) = 9.31, p = 0.041; Dark++: χ2 (1) = 31.30, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 76.02, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 41.82, p < 0.001]. Meanwhile, impression scores of female Red++ and Red+++ faces increased [Red++: χ2 (1) = 13.20, p = 0.005; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 27.37, p < 0.001]. For the question “Looks red,” impression scores increased for all facial images with changes in a* compared to the original facial image. Furthermore, impression scores increased for some facial images where a* did not change [20s male Dark+: χ2 (1) = 10.36, p = 0.023; 20s male Dark++: χ2 (1) = 15.65, p = 0.001; 20s female Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 12.58, p = 0.007].

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The impression score on the perception questionnaire for Chinese facial images.

Table 5 lists the impression scores of the Chinese facial images. In Dark++, Dark+++, Red+++, and all Dark&Red facial images of men in their 20s, impression scores of attractiveness significantly decreased [Dark++: χ2 (1) = 30.56, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 49.04, p < 0.001; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 69.38, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+: χ2 (1) = 24.39, p < 0.001; Dark&Red++: χ2 (1) = 19.56, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 104.98, p < 0.001]. For women in their 20s, impression scores significantly decreased for Dark++, Dark+++, Dark&Red++, and Dark&Red+++ facial images (Dark++: χ2 (1) = 9.05, p = 0.024; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 32.84, p < 0.001; Dark&Red++: χ2 (1) = 13.35, p = 0.002; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 59.85, p < 0.001).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The impression score for attractiveness of Chinese facial images.

3.3 The Italian study

Table 6 shows the impression scores on the perception questionnaire for the Caucasian facial images. For the category “Looks bright,” impression scores of the Dark++ and Dark+++ male and female faces significantly decreased compared to the original facial image [20s male Dark++: χ2 (1) = 38.92, p < 0.001; 20s male Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 80.06, p < 0.001; 20s female Dark++: χ2 (1) = 38.76, p < 0.001; 20s female Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 86.43, p < 0.001]. For men in their 20s, impression scores for the Dark&Red face decreased [Dark&Red+: χ2 (1) = 13.46, p = 0.005; Dark&Red++: χ2(1) = 39.95, p < 0.001; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 38.92, p < 0.001]. For the male Red+++ face, impression scores of “Looks bright” decreased even though L* was not changed [χ2 (1) = 11.87, p = 0.010]. For female faces, impression scores significantly decreased for Dark&Red+ and Dark&Red++ faces [Dark&Red+: χ2 (1) = 16.54, p = 0.001; Dark&Red++: χ2(1) = 13.34, p = 0.005]. In contrast, for the Red+++ face of women in their 20s, impression scores of “Looks bright” increased [χ2 (1) = 39.80, p < 0.001].

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. The impression score on the perception questionnaire for Caucasian facial images.

For the category “Looks red,” impression scores of all Red and Dark&Red faces significantly increased compared to the original faces. For the male face, impression scores of “Looks red” in Dark faces significantly decreased [Dark+: χ2 (1) = 74.10, p < 0.001; Dark++: χ2 (1) = 150.93, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 239.05, p < 0.001]. In comparison, for female faces, impression scores of “Looks red” in Dark faces increased [Dark+: χ2 (1) = 25.14, p < 0.001; Dark++: χ2 (1) = 50.99, p < 0.001; Dark+++: χ2 (1) = 14.43, p = 0.003].

Table 7 shows the impression scores for attractiveness of the Caucasian facial images. For men in their 20s, the impression score of attractiveness in the Red+++ and Dark&Red+++ faces significantly decreased compared to the original face [Red+++: χ2 (1) = 9.79, p = 0.016; Dark&Red+++: χ2 (1) = 11.40, p = 0.007]. However, impression scores of attractiveness in the female Red++ and Red+++ faces significantly increased [Red++: χ2 (1) = 11.32, p = 0.007; Red+++: χ2 (1) = 13.08, p = 0.003].

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. The impression score for attractiveness of Caucasian facial images.

4 Discussion

This study aimed to elucidate the impact of individual skin color changes on attractiveness via average facial images of both men and women and examine the influence of geographical and cultural backgrounds. To qualitatively compare the relationship between attractiveness and changes in skin color in countries with similar and different geographical and cultural backgrounds, experiments were conducted in Japan, China, and Italy. To examine the effect of skin color changes on facial impressions, images were derived from the original by decreasing only L* (Dark), increasing a* and decreasing b* (Red), and decreasing L* and b* while increasing a* (Dark & Red). Additionally, to investigate the impact of the degree of skin color change, images with average (+), 1SD (++), and 2SD changes (+++) were created. Questions regarding perception and attractiveness were designed to examine how changes in skin color were perceived. Investigating the impact of individual skin color changes on attractiveness and differences owing to geographical and cultural backgrounds is crucial for the development of global skincare and makeup products.

4.1 Skin color perception

Perception questions identified how changes in skin color were perceived. In all the surveys, a decrease in apparent brightness due to a decrease in L* and an increase in apparent redness due to an increase in a* and decrease in b* were observed. In Red faces, an increase in apparent brightness was observed in female faces in all the studies. An increase in a* increased apparent brightness in Japanese female faces (Yoshikawa et al., 2012). Our results supported those of previous studies and revealed that this illusion effect occurred in Japanese and also Chinese and Caucasian faces.

In contrast, changes in apparent redness due to a decrease in L* and apparent brightness due to increases in a* and decreases in b* were also observed. In the Japanese study, no change in apparent redness with decreasing L* was observed. Meanwhile, in the Chinese study, appearance of redness increased with decreasing L* only in the male face. In the Italian study, a decrease in L* decreased and increased the appearance of redness on male and females faces, respectively. Perception of skin color in human faces was influenced by the skin colors encountered in daily life (Shimakura and Sakata, 2022). Tanning influences skin color changes in daily life; however, the way skin color changes due to tanning also differs (Fitzpatrick, 1988). Asians, such as Japanese and Chinese individuals, tend to become relatively darker after they tan (Chan et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2006). Conversely, Caucasians are more likely to develop a reddish hue when tanned (Gupta and Sharma, 2019). These differences in common skin color changes may have influenced the perception of skin color. Additionally, differences between East Asian and Western Caucasian cultures influenced facial recognition processes (Blais et al., 2008). Hence, perception of skin color may be influenced by physiological characteristics and cultural backgrounds. However, to understand whether skin color perception varies based on geographical and cultural backgrounds, further controlled studies are warranted.

4.2 Influence of skin color on attractiveness

The impression questions examined the impact of skin color change on attractiveness. In the Japanese and Chinese survey, attractiveness decreased for Dark faces of both sexes. Conversely, no change in attractiveness was observed for Dark faces in the Italian survey. Skin brightness is an important indicator of beauty in Asian countries, including China and Japan (Ashikari, 2005; Li et al., 2008). Reduced attractiveness due to reduced brightness in Chinese and Japanese faces in this study supported the results of previous studies.

In contrast, Western Caucasians tend to favor tanning (Melia and Bulman, 1995). Indeed, darker skin is more attractive than lighter skin on Caucasian female faces (Fink et al., 2001). However, research revealed that an increase in L* increased apparent health (Stephen et al., 2011). In this study, no change in attractiveness was observed for faces with a decrease in L* alone. This finding was not consistent with those of previous studies. Skin color changes reproduced in this study referred to the changes in skin color that could occur within the same day: L* was reduced by a maximum of 3 units, a smaller change than that in Stephen et al.’s (2011) study (−5.2 units). Thus, our findings may differ from those of previous reports owing to the relatively small color changes. Furthermore, the effect of L* on facial attractiveness was considered to be larger in Japan and China than in Italy, as the decrease in L* caused a decrease in attractiveness in Japan and China. These results indicate that the effects of L* reduction on facial attractiveness are similar in Japan and China, which are in Asia, as reported. Conversely, in Italy, which is geographically and culturally distant from Japan and China, the effect of L* reduction on facial attractiveness may be less significant.

The influence of the Red faces on attractiveness varied across different countries. Facial redness increases attractiveness (Stephen et al., 2009; Thorstenson et al., 2017). In fact, for Japanese faces, attractiveness increased for Red faces of both sexes. In contrast, attractiveness tended to decrease for Chinese faces, except for female faces. Furthermore, Caucasian faces exhibited an increase in attractiveness for Red faces of females, whereas attractiveness decreased for male faces. In male faces, redness increased attractiveness and also aggression and dominance (Stephen et al., 2012), and a gender bias existed regarding dominance (Oh et al., 2020). Opposite results of changes in attractiveness due to redness in Caucasian male and female faces may have been influenced by gender biases regarding dominance and aggression. Furthermore, gender bias may have also influenced the decrease in attractiveness observed in the Chinese male Red faces. For Japanese faces, attractiveness increased for Red faces of both sexes. Different effects of facial redness on attractiveness compared with Chinese faces suggested that the effects of facial color on attractiveness may differ even between regions with close geographic and cultural backgrounds. In fact, the Japanese preferred reddish faces (He et al., 2024). Furthermore, this trend was more pronounced compared with that of Chinese individuals. Thus, the results support regional differences in attractiveness associated with changes in skin color. However, detailed studies should determine the effect of redness on attractiveness, as the original facial shape and skin color also differ.

This study examined the effects of skin color changes on attractiveness of the same person in Japan, China, and Italy. Consequently, it was revealed that changes in skin color within the same individual affected attractiveness in all the surveys. Furthermore, changes in attractiveness varied by country. Influence of skin brightness on attractiveness demonstrated similar trends in Japan and China; however, different trends were observed regarding skin redness, even between geographically close countries, such as Japan and China. Differences in the effects of these skin tones on attractiveness may be influenced by cultural backgrounds. Turner (2012) proposed the concept of “social skin,” stating that skin serves as a social boundary. Yamamoto et al. (2002) reported that preferred skin tones were influenced by cultural factors based on surveys conducted in Japan and South Korea. In Japan, the phenomenon of skin whitening has become a social trend, as evidenced by the prevalence of advertisements for whitening cosmetics (Ashikari, 2005). Furthermore, differences in women’s physical preferences and men’s masculinity across cultures suggest that gender bias may vary between cultures (Swami and Tovée, 2005; DeBruine et al., 2010). Differences in the impact of skin tone on attractiveness observed in this study could be owing to a complex interplay of social and cultural backgrounds. Future studies should examine how the influence of skin color on attractiveness is shaped by cultural backgrounds. These findings indicate the importance of global cosmetics development based on the fact that preferred skin color changes vary according to geographic and cultural backgrounds and sex.

4.3 Limitations

This study has several limitations. From the perspective of personal information protection and ease of control, average faces were used; however, morphed faces lose individual features, such as expressions (Korshunov and Ebrahimi, 2013). To investigate the impact of skin color on attractiveness in the real world, considering elements, such as expressions and morphology, is necessary. The facial images used in this study had original Lab* values that differed across countries. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that differences in the initial skin color values influenced the results. Additionally, regarding the amount of skin color change, we used values from previous studies conducted on Japanese individuals for standardization; however, physiological verification is required to determine whether the same changes occur in faces from different countries. Furthermore, since this survey used different stimuli and participants, quantitative comparisons could not be made. Moreover, the survey was conducted online; hence, visual conditions, such as monitors and lighting, may have varied across the studies. Future controlled research should conduct quantitative comparisons. This study utilized a 5-point Likert scale to evaluate attractiveness. However, biases in response styles, such as acquiescence response and mid-point response styles, may vary across cultures (Van Vaerenbergh and Thomas, 2013). Furthermore, the perception of attractiveness involves multiple factors, such as perceived healthiness, and personality traits, such as aggressiveness, which may not be adequately evaluated via a single-dimensional scale, such as the Likert scale (Stephen et al., 2012; Thorstenson et al., 2017). Future studies should incorporate evaluation methods that consider these biases and the complexity of attractiveness. In this study, the survey that used images of Caucasian faces was conducted in Italy; however, the perception of attractiveness may differ among different European populations. A study examined the roles of skin color, hair color, and body weight in the perception of female attractiveness across eight countries and found that even among the same Western countries, evaluations of attractive skin tones varied (Swami et al., 2008). Future research should explore the impact of skin color on attractiveness among Caucasians with different cultural backgrounds.

5 Conclusion

This study examined how skin color changes that can occur within individuals affect attractiveness in Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian faces, using averaged male and female facial images. In each country, we found that intra-individual changes in skin color affected facial attractiveness. The effects of skin color change on attractiveness differed not only between Japan and Italy, which have different geographic and cultural backgrounds, but also between Japan and China, which are relatively close. Furthermore, the results indicated a gender bias in the perception of facial attractiveness, even within the same country. These results reveal that the impact of intra-individual skin color variation on attractiveness varies by geographic and cultural backgrounds and sex, and will advance the development of global skincare and makeup products that improve individual attractiveness.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because owing to confidentiality agreements with the participants. The data used in this study are available only at the Shiseido Global Innovation Center. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to a295by5rb2l6dW1pQHNoaXNlaWRvLmNvbQ==.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Research Ethics Committee of the Shiseido Global Innovation Center. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KK: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MN: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. NH: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. HO: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Makito Suzuki and Eijiro Hara for their assistance with surveys.

Conflict of interest

KK, MN, and NH were employed by Shiseido Co., Ltd. This study received funding from Shiseido MIRAI Technology Institute, Shiseido Co., Ltd.

The funders had the following involvement in the study: The funders were involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article and the decision to submit it for publication.

None of the authors have been members of the Research Ethics Committee of the Shiseido Global Innovation Center, nor have they been involved in the ethical review or approval of this study at any point in time.

The remaining author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

All authors declare no other competing interests.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ashikari, M. (2005). Cultivating Japanese whiteness: the ‘whitening’ cosmetics boom and the Japanese identity. J. Mater. Cult. 10, 73–91. doi: 10.1177/1359183505050095

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blais, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers, C., Fiset, D., and Caldara, R. (2008). Culture shapes how we look at faces. PLoS One 3:e3022. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caisey, L., Grangeat, F., Lemasson, A., Talabot, J., and Voirin, A. (2006). Skin color and makeup strategies of women from different ethnic groups. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 28, 427–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-2494.2006.00329.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, I. L., Cohen, S., da Cunha, M. G., and Maluf, L. C. (2019). Characteristics and management of Asian skin. Int. J. Dermatol. 58, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/ijd.14153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clydesdale, G. J., Dandie, G. W., and Muller, H. K. (2001). Ultraviolet light induced injury: immunological and inflammatory effects. Immunol. Cell Biol. 79, 547–568. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1711.2001.01047.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Crawford, J. R., Welling, L. L., and Little, A. C. (2010). The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 2405–2410. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.2184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Bunse, L., Matts, P. J., and D’Emiliano, D. (2012). Visible skin colouration predicts perception of male facial age, health and attractiveness. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 34, 307–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2494.2012.00724.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Grammer, K., and Matts, P. J. (2006). Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Grammer, K., and Thornhill, R. (2001). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness in relation to skin texture and color. J. Comp. Psychol. 115, 92–99. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.92

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T. B. (1988). The validity and practicality of sun-reactive skin types I through VI. Arch. Dermatol. 124, 869–871. doi: 10.1001/archderm.1988.01670060015008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grammer, K., and Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness. J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 233–242. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.3.233

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, V., and Sharma, V. K. (2019). Skin typing: Fitzpatrick grading and others. Clin. Dermatol. 37, 430–436. doi: 10.1016/j.clindermatol.2019.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Kandrik, M., Fasolt, V., et al. (2018). Cultural differences in preferences for facial coloration. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 154–159. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.11.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Sato, H., and Mizokami, Y. (2024). Cross-cultural comparison of the influence of skin-color change on facial impressions. Iperception 15:20416695241288032. doi: 10.1177/20416695241288032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, A. J., Holzleitner, I. J., Talamas, S. N., and Perrett, D. I. (2016). Perception of health from facial cues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150380. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0380

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirao, N., Koizumi, K., Ikeda, H., and Ohira, H. (2021). Reliability of online surveys in investigating perceptions and impressions of faces. Front. Psychol. 12:733405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.733405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hönigsmann, H., Schuler, G., Aberer, W., Romani, N., and Wolff, K. (1986). Immediate pigment darkening phenomenon. A reevaluation of its mechanisms. J. Invest. Dermatol. 87, 648–652. doi: 10.1111/1523-1747.ep12456326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, B., Wagemans, F. M. A., Evans, A. M., and van Beest, I. (2018). Effects of facial skin smoothness and blemishes on trait impressions. Perception 47, 608–625. doi: 10.1177/0301006618767258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2004). When facial attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception 33, 569–576. doi: 10.1068/p3463

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health: support for a “good genes” explanation of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 417–429. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00083-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, A. L., Porcheron, A., Sweda, J. R., Morizot, F., and Russell, R. (2016). Coloration in different areas of facial skin is a cue to health: the role of cheek redness and periorbital luminance in health perception. Body Image 17, 57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, B., Lee, J., Park, S., and Suk, H. J. (2023). Method and analysis of color changes of facial skin after applying skin makeup. Vis. Res. 209:108247. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2023.108247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koizumi, K., Hirao, N., Yamanami, H., and Ohira, H. (2023). Effects of mild psychological stress on facial impressions. Front. Psychol. 14:1186046. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1186046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korshunov, P., and Ebrahimi, T. (2013). Using face morphing to protect privacy. In 2013 10th IEEE international conference on advanced video and signal based surveillance (pp. 208–213). IEEE.

Google Scholar

Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., and Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social blushing. Psychol. Bull. 112, 446–460. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, E. P., Min, H. J., Belk, R. W., Kimura, J., and Bahl, S. (2008). Skin lightening and beauty in four Asian cultures. Adv. Consum. Res. 35, 444–449. Available online at: https://www.academia.edu/download/35821215/www.acrwebsite.org_volumes_v35_naacr_vol35_273.pdf

Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Lai, W., Wang, X. M., Li, L., Tian, Y., Lu, Y., et al. (2006). Skin phototyping in a Chinese female population: analysis of four hundred and four cases from four major cities of China. Photodermatol. Photoimmunol. Photomed. 22, 184–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0781.2006.00225.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marini, M., Paglieri, F., Ansani, A., Caruana, F., and Viola, M. (2022). Facial impression of trustworthiness biases statement credibility unless suppressed by facemask. Curr. Psychol. 43, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03277-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinkauppi, B. (2002). Face colour under varying illumination: analysis and applications. University of Oulu. Available online at: https://urn.fi/URN:ISBN:9514267885

Google Scholar

Melia, J., and Bulman, A. (1995). Sunburn and tanning in a British population. J. Public Health Med. 17, 223–229. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.pubmed.a043097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohiuddin, A. K. (2019). Sunscreen and suntan preparations. ARC J. Pharm. Sci. 5, 8–44. doi: 10.20431/2455-1538.0502002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, D., Dotsch, R., Porter, J., and Todorov, A. (2020). Gender biases in impressions from faces: empirical studies and computational models. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 323–342. doi: 10.1037/xge0000638

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Palermo, R., Simmons, L. W., Peters, M., Lee, K., et al. (2007). Perceived health contributes to the attractiveness of facial symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism. Perception 36, 1244–1252. doi: 10.1068/p5712

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samson, N., Fink, B., and Matts, P. J. (2010). Visible skin condition and perception of human facial appearance. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 32, 167–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2494.2009.00535.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimakura, H., and Sakata, K. (2022). Color criteria of facial skin tone judgment. Vis. Res. 193:108011. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Coetzee, V., Law Smith, M., and Perrett, D. I. (2009). Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect perceived human health. PLoS One 4:e5083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Coetzee, V., and Perrett, D. I. (2011). Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 216–227. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Oldham, F. H., Perrett, D. I., and Barton, R. A. (2012). Redness enhances perceived aggression, dominance and attractiveness in men’s faces. Evol. Psychol. 10, 562–572. doi: 10.1177/147470491201000312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., Rozmus-Wrzesinska, M., Voracek, M., Haubner, T., Danel, D., Pawłowski, B., et al. (2008). The influence of skin tone, body weight, and hair colour on perceptions of women's attractiveness and health: a cross-cultural investigation. J. Evol. Psychol. 6, 321–341. doi: 10.1556/jep.6.2008.4.4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Tovée, M. J. (2005). Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: a cross-cultural study. Body Image 2, 115–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takehara, T., Kaigawa, M., Kobayashi, A., and Yamaguchi, Y. (2023). Impact of face masks and sunglasses on attractiveness, trustworthiness, and familiarity, and limited time effect: a Japanese sample. Discov. Psychol. 3:5. doi: 10.1007/s44202-023-00066-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, K. W., and Stephen, I. D. (2019). Skin color preferences in a Malaysian Chinese population. Front. Psychol. 10:1352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorstenson, C. A., Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., and Perrett, D. I. (2017). Facial redness increases men’s perceived healthiness and attractiveness. Perception 46, 650–664. doi: 10.1177/0301006616680124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, T. S. (2012). The social skin. HAU J. Ethnogr. Theory 2, 486–504. doi: 10.14318/hau2.2.026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vaerenbergh, Y., and Thomas, T. D. (2013). Response styles in survey research: a literature review of antecedents, consequences, and remedies. Int. J. Public Opin. Res. 25, 195–217. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/eds021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, S., and Kageyama, Y. (2017). Relationship between color information and stress by facial image analysis (in Japanese). Proceedings of the School of Engineering of Tokai University. 57, 51–57.

Google Scholar

Yamamoto, M., Lim, Y. H., Aoki, N., and Kobayashi, H. (2002). On the preferred reproduction of flesh color in Japan and South Korea, investigated for all generation. J. Soc. Photogr. Sci. Technol. Jpn. 65, 363–368. doi: 10.11454/photogrst1964.65.363

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshikawa, H., Kikuchi, K., Yaguchi, H., Mizokami, Y., and Takata, S. (2012). Effect of chromatic components on facial skin whiteness. Color. Res. Appl. 37, 281–291. doi: 10.1002/col.20685

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: facial attractiveness, facial perception, survey, Chinese, Japanese, Caucasian

Citation: Koizumi K, Nagashima M, Hirao N and Ohira H (2025) Simulated intra-individual skin color changes and their impact on facial attractiveness across Japanese, Chinese, and Caucasian faces. Front. Psychol. 16:1600306. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1600306

Received: 26 March 2025; Accepted: 21 May 2025;
Published: 05 September 2025.

Edited by:

Javier Villanueva-Valle, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Givago Silva Souza, Federal University of Pará, Brazil
Anna Kasielska-Trojan, Medical University of Lodz, Poland
José Carlos Sánchez-Ferrer, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico
Diana Platas, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Mexico

Copyright © 2025 Koizumi, Nagashima, Hirao and Ohira. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Koyo Koizumi, a295by5rb2l6dW1pQHNoaXNlaWRvLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.