Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 08 August 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1618617

This article is part of the Research TopicMental Health in Recreational and Elite SportsView all 29 articles

Optimal dosage and effectiveness of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health: a Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis

  • Faculty of Health Sciences and Sports, Macao Polytechnic University, Macau, Macao SAR, China

Introduction: While imagery practice is effective for performance enhancement, its impact on mental health is inconclusive due to mixed findings and heterogeneous athlete populations. This study aims to evaluate the effectiveness and optimal dosage of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health outcomes.

Methods: A total of 24 randomized controlled trials (RCTs), encompassing 1,294 athletes, were synthesized using a Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis in accordance with the PRISMA 2020 guidelines. Among them, 623 were male and 375 were female; the remaining 296 participants were from studies that did not report sex-specific data. The included trials spanned diverse continents (e.g., America, Asia, Europe), covered a wide age range (from adolescents to adults), and involved both individual and team sports (e.g., gymnastics, soccer, swimming). A Bayesian multilevel approach was selected to account for potential clustering within studies and to provide full posterior distributions for effect estimates, allowing for more robust inferences under uncertainty.

Results: Our research indicate that imagery practice may improve athletes’ mental health [μ(SMD): 0.5, 95% CI: 0.34 to 0.56; HDI: 0.22 to 0.89; BF: 17.16], including reducing anxiety levels [μ(SMD): 0.52, 95% CI: 0.11 to 0.96; HDI: 0.11 to 0.96; BF: 2.33], strengthening self-confidence [μ(SMD): 0.62, 95% CI: 0.1 to 1.13; HDI: 0.12 to 1.15; BF: 2.2], and improving self-efficacy [μ(SMD): 1.36, 95% CI: 0.26 to 2.47; HDI: 0.24 to 2.45; BF: 5.38]. In the athlete model, statistically significant effects of imagery practice were found only among tennis players [μ(SMD): 1.16, 95% CI: 0.19 to 2.48; HDI: 0.13 to 2.39; BF: 4.06]. In moderation analysis, a dosage of 45-min sessions once a week for 100 days may be associated with more favorable mental health outcomes.

Discussion: To our knowledge, limited prior meta-analytic research has employed Bayesian multilevel modeling to examine the effects and moderators of imagery practice on mental health outcomes. While the findings suggest that imagery practice may offer psychological benefits for athletes, these effects appear to vary across contexts and athlete populations. Therefore, imagery should be applied with caution, considering individual differences and potential limitations. This study contributes to sport psychology by offering preliminary empirical guidance for tailoring imagery practice to support athletes’ mental health.

Systematic Review Registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/view/CRD420251050005.

1 Introduction

The mental health of athletes is intrinsically linked to their performance and serves as a crucial metric that must not be overlooked in everyday training. Research indicates that student-athletes exhibit a higher susceptibility to severe mental health problems, including anxiety and depression, in comparison to their peers (Grubic et al., 2021). Current data indicate that the prevalence of anxiety or depression among active elite athletes is 34%, and among retired elite athletes, it is 26% (Gouttebarge et al., 2019). The NCAA poll indicates that the majority of student-athletes desire access to educational resources about mental health (Grubic et al., 2021; National Collegiate Athletic Association, 2022). Imagery practice has been increasingly explored as a promising psychological skills training technique for improving mental health and alleviating negative emotional states among athletes in latest reviews (Li et al., 2024; Mesagno and Beckmann, 2017). While some primary studies have explored its psychological effects, to date, no systematic review or meta-analysis has comprehensively evaluated the mental health outcomes of imagery practice in athletic populations. Moreover, there is a lack of evidence regarding its optimal dosage (e.g., frequency, duration) and whether its effectiveness varies according to athlete characteristics such as age and gender. Therefore, this study aims to apply a Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis to synthesize the psychological effects of imagery practice and to examine potential moderators related to dosage and demographic factors.

Imagery denotes a process akin to perception that transpires in the absence of external sensory stimuli (Munzert et al., 2009; Toth et al., 2020). It has convincing theoretical support that explains its mechanisms and guides its practical applications. Mental simulation theory offers strong evidence for the mechanisms behind mental training, explaining both neural activation during motor imagery and its behavioral effects. According to mental simulation theory, motor imagery activates neural substrates involved in actual movement execution, including the premotor cortex, supplementary motor area (SMA), and even the primary motor cortex (M1) (Jeannerod, 2001; Munzert et al., 2009). This internal activation, though covert, engages motor representations that are functionally equivalent to real actions. Crucially, such activation extends beyond motor control to influence autonomic responses, such as heart rate and respiration, which are also implicated in emotional regulation. For instance, imagery-induced activation can produce physiological arousal states that mirror actual performance, enhancing self-perceived readiness and control (Jeannerod, 1994). These embodied responses serve as a pathway through which motor imagery can reduce anxiety, increase confidence, and support broader psychological outcomes, thus linking neural simulation directly to mental health benefits. Additionally, recent studies suggest that mental imagery is not only beneficial for athletic and emotional regulation but also plays a crucial role in basic cognitive functions. For instance, mental imagery has been linked to improved academic performance through its relationship with visuospatial processing (Commodari, 2024), early literacy and numeracy skills (Guarnera et al., 2019), and geometry problem-solving in children (Fastame, 2021). These findings support the idea that imagery practice may serve as a transferable cognitive skill with broad educational relevance beyond elite sport. Nonetheless, despite robust theoretical backing, the practical efficacy of imagery practice remains ambiguous and necessitates additional exploration.

Empirical evidence suggests that imagery practice can produce a range of psychological benefits for athletes. Several studies report that imagery practice is associated with reduced anxiety (Hut et al., 2023; McAlister and Cutler, 2024), enhanced confidence (Fekih et al., 2021; Hidayat et al., 2023), and improved self-efficacy (Cumming et al., 2006; Feltz and Riessinger, 1990). These outcomes are believed to be influenced by athletes’imagery abilities, which may modulate levels of trait confidence and anxiety regulation (Williams and Cumming, 2016). Furthermore, recent research suggests that motor imagery may help mitigate choking under pressure (Harris et al., 2023; Li et al., 2024; Mesagno and Beckmann, 2017), and appears particularly helpful for injured athletes—potentially enhancing confidence and reducing anxiety during recovery (Monsma et al., 2009; Raboin, 2024). There is also preliminary evidence indicating that athletes report high satisfaction with imagery use (Hammond et al., 2012; Raboin, 2024), and that it may be beneficial across both Western and non-Western populations (Chungath et al., 2022).

However, the overall effectiveness of imagery practice on mental health remains subject to debate. Some studies have found little to no benefit, particularly in certain skill-based sports such as gymnastics (Marshall and Gibson, 2017), shooters (Zervas and Kakkos, 1991), football players (Munroe-Chandler et al., 2005), basketball players (Kanthack et al., 2013) and golfers (Martin and Hall, 1995). In some cases, the content of imagery scripts was criticized for lacking relevance or emotional impact. For injured athletes, longer recovery durations have been linked to reduced engagement with imagery and elevated somatic anxiety (Monsma et al., 2009). Additionally, the optimal dose of imagery practice, such as frequency and session length remains poorly understood (McAlister and Cutler, 2024), and may vary depending on individual or contextual factors. Taken together, while the literature indicates promising psychological outcomes for some athletes, findings are inconsistent across athlete populations and sport types. These discrepancies may stem from differences in intervention design, imagery ability, the constructs targeted (e.g., anxiety vs. confidence), or other moderating factors. A comprehensive meta-analysis is therefore warranted to evaluate the overall effectiveness of imagery practice on mental health outcomes and to clarify the roles of key moderators.

Moreover, traditional meta-analyses often ignore multilevel data structures and parameter uncertainty, which a Bayesian framework can address effectively. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the effects of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health, encompassing factors such as anxiety, mood, self-efficacy, and mental toughness, as well as to determine the appropriate dosage of imagery for athletes’ application. Drawing from current yet constrained studies, we suggest two hypotheses: (1) Imagery practice will significantly enhance athletes’ mental health; (2) The effectiveness of imagery practice may vary depending on factors such as gender, age, dosage, and other individual or contextual variables.

2 Method

This study was registered with PROSPERO (CRD420251000369) and adhered to the PRISMA reporting guidelines (Haddaway et al., 2022). The completed PRISMA checklist is provided in Supplementary File S0 for reference. A Bayesian meta-analysis was conducted following a systematic review, utilizing Covidence, Python version 3, GRADEprofiler, R version 4.4.3, and GetData Graph Digitizer.

2.1 Eligibility criteria

Studies were deemed eligible if they conformed to the PICOS framework criteria:

Population (P): Participants had to be athletes of any age and competitive level, regardless of their physical or mental health status.

Intervention (I): Eligible studies were required to implement imagery practice either as a standalone intervention or as a component of a broader psychological skills training package (PSTP), as long as imagery was clearly included as one of the components.

Comparator (C): The control condition could consist of either no psychological training (no practice) or an alternative PST intervention that did not include imagery practice.

Outcomes (O): Studies needed to report outcomes related to mental health, such as anxiety, depression, stress, or general mental health.

Study Design (S): Only randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were included.

Additional exclusion criteria were also applied:

Unpublished manuscripts, including master’s or doctoral theses, were excluded. Non-original works such as letters, reviews, and editorials were not considered. Studies without extractable data or those in which both the experimental and control groups received different types of imagery practice were also excluded.

2.2 Information sources

A comprehensive search method was developed utilizing Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and free-text search terms to systematically investigate seven English databases, including SportDiscus, PubMed, PsycINFO, Web of Science, MEDLINE, MEDLINE Complete, and CINAHL, on February 24, 2025. The keywords and subject heading were definitively established by discussion among the three authors (SZ, XZ, and ZN). Detailed search strings for each database are provided in Supplementary File S1. A total of 11,621 studies were obtained using the Covidence online tool and ASReview in Python (van de Schoot et al., 2021) for systematic screening.

2.3 Study selection and data collection

All abstracts were assessed using the machine learning tool ASReview (van de Schoot et al., 2021). ASReview uses active learning, a type of supervised machine learning, where the model is iteratively trained on reviewer-labeled abstracts as relevant or irrelevant. Based on these labels, the system continually re-ranks the remaining records according to their predicted likelihood of inclusion. This process enables ASReview to prioritize more relevant records for manual review, while reducing the overall screening burden (Holzinger, 2016; Wang et al., 2020). To minimize the risk of omitting eligible studies, we followed a conservative application of the SAFE rule, continuing the ASReview-assisted screening until 200 consecutive records were classified as irrelevant, which suggests a low probability of remaining relevant studies (Boetje and Van De Schoot, 2024). While this approach improves efficiency, we acknowledge the potential for algorithmic bias, particularly in early training stages where inclusion decisions are based on a small number of labeled studies. To mitigate this, the reviewers regularly monitored ASReview’s predictions and ensured a diversity of topics in early training labels.

In the subsequent phase, two reviewers (SZ and XZ) employed the Covidence online tool, as advised by PRISMA, to screen entire texts (Haddaway et al., 2022). Eligible full-texts were assessed using Extraction 1.0 forms, with discrepancies addressed by the third reviewer (Veritas Health Innovation, 2023).

2.4 Data items

For each study, the characteristics extracted included authors, publication years, country, intervention, study design, session of imagery practice, sample size, gender, types of athletes, age, training years and outcomes. The outcomes of mental health encompassed worry, self-efficacy, self-criticism, self-concept, perceived difficulty, peaking under pressure, NASA index, mood, mindfulness, mental toughness, mental effort, Hooper index, coping with adversity, confidence and anxiety.

Data were separately extracted by two writers (SZ and XZ) with Covidence, with discrepancies addressed by consultation with the third author (ZN). Data were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (M ± SD). We utilized an online tool named Meta-analysis Accelerator to transform data that was not originally in M ± SD format (Abbas et al., 2024). When data were not presented as exact numbers, Get Data Graph Digitizer (Digitizer, 2020) was used to extract data from graphs.

2.5 Risk of bias assessment

The risk of bias for all included studies was assessed independently in accordance with the criteria outlined in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins et al., 2011). Two authors (SZ and XZ) assessed the included studies through the Cochrane risk of bias (ROB2) criteria in RCTs within Covidence. Seven areas of bias were evaluated: (1) Random sequence generation; (2) Allocation concealment; (3) Blinding of participants and personnel; (4) Blinding of outcome assessment; (5) Incomplete outcome data; (6) Selective reporting; and (7) Other bias. The risk of bias was classified as low, unclear, or high. After individual assessments, the authors reached an agreement through discussion. The conclusive data were recorded in an Excel template and subsequently input into R software to produce risk of bias summary graphs using the robvis package (McGuinness and Higgins, 2021). Studies with more than two but fewer than four categories classified as unclear risk were deemed to have moderate overall risk.

2.6 Publication bias

To evaluate potential publication bias, we employed the PublicationBias R package (Braginsky et al., 2019; Mathur and VanderWeele, 2020), which adopts a sensitivity analysis approach that does not rely on the assumption of funnel plot symmetry. This method estimates the magnitude of publication bias necessary to reduce the observed effect size or its confidence interval to a predefined threshold.

Assuming that statistically significant positive findings were preferentially published (i.e., favor_positive = TRUE), we generated a significance funnel plot to visually differentiate between affirmative results (significant and positive) and non-affirmative ones (either nonsignificant or negative). We also derived the worst-case estimate, calculated solely from non-affirmative studies, as a reference point to assess the robustness of the main findings. In addition, we computed the s-value, which represents the smallest selection ratio (η) required to reduce the effect size to zero. An s-value of “not possible” suggests that no plausible publication bias could fully explain the observed effect.

To complement this modern approach, we also conducted traditional bias assessments using the metafor package. These included a conventional funnel plot, Egger’s test, and, when Egger’s test indicated significant (p < 0.05), we applied the trim-and-fill method. These Frequentist tools provided a secondary line of evidence regarding potential publication bias.

2.7 Certainty in evidence

We assessed the quality of evidence using the GRADE approach, following the guidance provided by Ryan (2016). According to the GRADE framework, the quality of evidence may be downgraded based on five domains:

1. Risk of bias: no downgrading was applied when all or the majority (i.e., at least two-thirds) of the information came from studies assessed as having low risk of bias. When the majority of the evidence came from studies with unclear or moderate risk of bias, the quality was downgraded by one level. If the majority of the information originated from studies at high risk of bias, the evidence was downgraded by two levels.

2. Inconsistency: inconsistency was downgraded by one level when I2 was between 50 and 75%, and by two levels when I2 exceeded 75%.

3. Imprecision: The imprecision criterion is met if the 95% confidence interval (CI) crosses the equivalence line: 0 for standardized mean differences. Additionally, imprecision is considered when the total number of events or cases across all included studies does not meet the Optimal Information Size (OIS), with thresholds of less than 400 for continuous variables. Meeting one of these criteria warrants downgrading the evidence quality by one level, while meeting both criteria warrants downgrading by two levels.

4. Indirectness: when the evidence does not directly address the research question in terms of population, intervention, comparator, or outcome.

5. Publication bias: we downgraded the quality of evidence by one level when both of the following conditions were met: (1) all included randomized controlled trials were of small sample size (approximately 30 participants per group), and (2) the studies were sponsored by industry or manufacturers.

The GRADE methodology categorizes the quality of evidence as high, moderate, low, or very low.

2.8 Statistical analysis

Bayesian mixed-effects models, executed via the brms package (Bürkner, 2018) in R 4.4.3, were employed to analyze variation in effect sizes. Given the inclusion of multiple outcomes per study, a two-level model was implemented: level 1 (effect sizes within studies), level 2 (study-level variation). The Bayesian framework is particularly appropriate for meta-analyses involving a small number of studies, as it allows for the evaluation of both null and alternative hypotheses and yields comprehensive insights into the credibility and probability distribution of parameter estimates (Harrer et al., 2021; Kruschke and Liddell, 2018; Röver, 2020). Random intercepts were specified at both levels to account for clustering and study-specific variability. We constructed models assuming a normal distribution and included random effects for within-study ID and between-study ID to account for heterogeneity in each outcome, utilizing two formulas:

Iwithin2=τwithID2total_Variance×100
Ibetween2=τbetweenID2total_Variance×100

Weakly informative priors (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) were utilized for the random effects (Harrer et al., 2021). Mental health outcomes were examined and categorized according to the imaging tactics employed (imagery alone or imagery practice combined with additional psychological skills training), athlete categories, and mental health results. Four models were conducted as follow:

1. Null model to estimate overall effect sizes. We fitted models on the full dataset for mental health.

2. Imagery model: We classify imagery practice based on its unique characteristics. The first category encompasses imagery practice that is not combined with other psychological skills training, whereas the second category comprises psychological skills training packages (PSTP) that include imagery practice. Secondly, a comparison will be conducted between these two modalities of imagery practice and other psychological skills training, as well as the lack of psychological training (no practice).

3. Mental health model: We categorize the derived mental health results according to their characteristics.

4. Athlete model: Athletes are classified based on the sport in which they engage.

Each model was estimated utilizing four Markov chains, with each chain executing 10,000 iterations. We exclusively utilized the Rhat statistic to assess the convergence of the Markov chains. Rhat evaluates the ratio of between-chain to within-chain variation, with values approaching 1 indicating adequate convergence. This measure offers a dependable evaluation of convergence, confirming that chains with Rhat values close to 1 have comprehensively examined the posterior distribution, thus improving the dependability of model inference. Bayes Factors (BFs) (Makowski et al., 2019) were calculated using the reciprocal of the Savage-Dickey density ratio, implemented through the bayesfactor_parameters function in the bayestestR package. Furthermore, Bayes Factors (BF) are commonly interpreted on a graded scale, where BF > 3 represents moderate evidence and BF > 10 indicates strong evidence in favor of the effect (Kass and Raftery, 1995). In complicated hierarchical models, the 95% high-density interval (HDI) (Kruschke and Liddell, 2018) provides a direct and assumption-free depiction of the most likely posterior values. This contrasts with p-values and confidence intervals, which depend on further assumptions.

2.9 Moderation analysis

In this study, moderation analysis was conducted using the brms package in R, including five moderator variables: age, gender, training experience (in years), competitive level, sport type (team or individual), and three moderators included imagery practice duration (in days), weekly practice frequency (sessions per week), and intensity (minutes per session) were examined to determine the optimal dosage of imagery practice for improving mental health. The study was performed using a Bayesian framework, facilitating the calculation of effect sizes while considering measurement error and the hierarchical structure of the data.

To investigate the effects of different moderators on model performance, we first constructed a null model without any moderator variables. Subsequently, we developed separate models, each incorporating a single moderator. The Bayesian R-squared statistic, computed using the bayes_R2 function from the brms package, was employed to estimate the proportion of variance explained by each model. In order to compare the explanatory power of models with and without moderators, we visualized the distribution of R-squared values using density plots. This approach enables a more intuitive understanding of how the inclusion of moderators contributes to model improvement and facilitates direct comparisons with the null model.

3 Results

The results include six parts, namely study selection, the characteristic of included studies, quality assessment, meta-analysis with four models, moderation analysis, quality grade and publication bias.

3.1 Study selection

Figure 1 illustrates the flow chart. A total of 11,621 articles were extracted from seven databases and put into Endnote software for the purpose of eliminating duplicates. Endnote automatically identified and eliminated 2,032 studies as duplicates. A total of 9,589 papers were loaded into the ASReview tool for preliminary screening, encompassing the evaluation of titles and abstracts. Utilizing machine learning models, 1,008 articles were evaluated, resulting in 134 articles being included for full-text screening. Following full-text screening, 116 studies were removed for various reasons, whereas 18 articles were incorporated into this meta-analysis. Six additional studies were incorporated through the citation searching strategy. A total of 24 studies were incorporated into the meta-analysis.

Figure 1
A flowchart depicting a research selection process. Starting from February 2025, 11,621 records are initially found. After removing 2,032 duplicates, 9,589 records are uploaded into ASReview. 1,008 are screened, and 134 proceed to full-text screening. Reasons for excluding 116 records include wrong population, outcomes, study design, unretrievable files, wrong intervention, and language. Ultimately, 18 are included. Six additional records are identified through citation searching, with none excluded or not retrieved. Total records included are 24.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart for the identification of the included studies.

3.2 Characteristic of included studies

The specific characteristics of each study included are available in Supplementary File S2. Twenty-four studies including 1,294 athletes, consisting of 375 females and 623 males, were considered, with 296 participants lacking gender data (Alwan et al., 2013; Kanthack et al., 2013; Mguidich et al., 2024). The included studies involved a diverse range of athlete populations, including dancer (n = 1), gymnast (n = 1), golfer (n = 1), tennis players (n = 3), volleyball player (n = 1), basketball players (n = 4), ice hockey athlete (n = 1), swimmers (n = 2), CrossFit athlete (n = 1), karateka (n = 1), soccer players (n = 3), badminton athlete (n = 1), runner (n = 1), weight lifter (n = 1), and rugby player (n = 1).

With the exception of three research conducted as randomized crossover trials (Page et al., 1999; Rumeau et al., 2023; Taylor and Shaw, 2002), all remaining investigations were randomized parallel trials. Six studies used a combination of imagery practice and other psychological training as intervention methods (25%). Five studies were from Asia (20.8%). Nine studies were from Europe (37.5%). Seven studies were from America (29%). Two studies were from Oceania (8%) and one study were from Africa (4%). Three studies did not provide age information (Alwan et al., 2013; Kanthack et al., 2013; Mguidich et al., 2024).

3.3 Quality assessment

The risk-of-bias summary is illustrated in Figure 2, while the detailed risk-of-bias graph for each study is available in Supplementary File S3. Some studies were classified as high risk (8%) due to the erroneous application of randomization methods, such as block randomization (Marshall and Gibson, 2017; Noh et al., 2007). Over 80% of studies failed to report the implementation of blinding, resulting in an assessment of unclear risk. Due to incomplete data, two studies were classified as having an unclear risk (Alwan et al., 2013; McAlister and Cutler, 2024) and one study was marked as high risk (Hut et al., 2023). No articles have been marked as unclear or high risk in the areas of selective reporting and other biases. More than 50 % of the research were classified as having an unclear risk, hence diminishing the robustness of the data outcomes.

Figure 2
Bar graph illustrating the risk of bias across various factors in a study. Each factor, such as sequence generation and allocation concealment, is represented by a horizontal bar showing three sections: low risk (green), some concerns (yellow), and high risk (red). Sequence generation and selective reporting show the lowest risk, while allocation concealment and blinding have notable concerns. Overall, there are minimal high-risk indicators, but some concerns remain prevalent. A legend clarifies the colors’ meanings.

Figure 2. Risk of bias summary.

3.4 The results of meta-analysis

The meta-analysis is divided into four sections. The first section included the Null model, followed by the imagery model, mental health model and athlete model. The detailed results in each model are illustrated in Supplementary File S4. The convergence of the Markov chain in all four models was accurately evaluated using the Rhat parameter value in the results. The Rhat value across all outcomes was roughly 1.0. Therefore, we did not include the findings on Markov chain convergence in the article.

3.4.1 Null model

In the null model, the overall effect size was calculated for mental health. Twenty-four studies with 1,294 athletes were included in the null model. The forest plot is illustrated in Figure 3. The Bayesian meta-analysis showed a statistically significant effect [μ(SMD): 0.5, 95% CI: 0.34 to 0.56; HDI: 0.22 to 0.89; BF: 17.16], with low between-study heterogeneity and moderate within-study heterogeneity [within I2: 71.37%, between I2: 28.63%].

Figure 3
Forest plot depicting standardized mean differences for various studies, each represented by a blue shape and dot along a horizontal axis. Names are listed on the left with effect sizes and confidence intervals on the right. The pooled effect size is 0.6, marked at the bottom.

Figure 3. The forest plot in null model.

Imagery practice can effectively improve athletes’ mental health. Figure 4 shows the cumulative probability and posterior density distribution of SMD, τwithin and τbetween in athletic performance. As illustrated in Figure 4, the posterior density distributions of τ_between and τ_within indicate substantial heterogeneity between studies (τ ≈ 0.8). Additionally, the posterior distribution of the standardized mean difference (SMD) suggests a moderately significant effect (SMD ≈ 0.6).

Figure 4
Three density plots are shown at the top, labeled as SMD, between_tau, and within_tau. The rightmost plot includes an HDI [0.22, 0.89]. Below, there are three ECDF plots: SMD, between_Tau (Heterogeneity), and within_Tau (Heterogeneity). Each plot has a red dashed line at significant values: 0.8 for SMD and between_Tau, and 1 for within_Tau.

Figure 4. The cumulative probability, HDI distribution and posterior density distribution (SMD, τwithin and τbetween) in athletic performance.

3.4.2 Mental health model

The mental health model presents 15 outcomes on various mental health markers, as illustrated in Figure 5 of the forest plot. However, only three indicators of mental health showed statistical significance, including anxiety [μ(SMD): 0.52, 95% CI: 0.11 to 0.96; HDI: 0.11 to 0.96; BF: 2.33], confidence [μ(SMD): 0.62, 95% CI: 0.1 to 1.13; HDI: 0.12 to 1.15; BF: 2.2] and self-efficacy [μ(SMD): 1.36, 95% CI: 0.26 to 2.47; HDI: 0.24 to 2.45; BF: 5.38], with low within-study and between-study heterogeneity [within I2: 17.11%, between I2: 18.67%].

Figure 5
A violin plot displays the effect size (SMD) for various psychological factors. Each factor, including worry, self-efficacy, and anxiety, is represented by a horizontal violin shape with a dot indicating the median, spread across a range from -2.5 to 5.0. The plot helps visualize the distribution and central tendency of each factor’s effect size, with a vertical dashed line at zero for reference.

Figure 5. The forest plot in mental health model.

3.4.3 Imagery model

In imagery model, 12 pairs of comparisons were included. The forest plot is illustrated in Figure 6. Only two pair of comparisons showed statistical significance, including Imagery practice VS No practice [μ(SMD): 0.84, 95% CI: 0.26 to 1.43; HDI: 0.25 to 1.42; BF: 9.13] and Imagery practice VS Video observation [μ(SMD): –3.1, 95% CI: −4.26 to −1.95; HDI: −4.24 to −1.93; BF: 4680], with low within-study heterogeneity and high between-study heterogeneity [within I2: 5.62%, between I2: 71.33%].

Figure 6
Forest plot comparing the effect sizes of different psychological stress training programs (PSTP) and imagery practices against various strategies. The plot displays comparisons against relaxation, no practice, mindfulness, hypnosis, focus strategy, CBT, autogenic training, video observation, and PSTP with a range of effect sizes from -3 to 3 standardized mean difference (SMD). Each method’s effect is shown with a distribution curve and a central point estimate. Negative values indicate less effective than the comparator, while positive values indicate more effective.

Figure 6. The forest plot in imagery model.

3.4.4 Athlete model

In athlete model, 16 types of athlete results were included. The forest plot is presented in Figure 7. Only one type of athlete showed statistical significance, including tennis players [μ(SMD): 1.16, 95% CI: 0.19 to 2.48; HDI: 0.13 to 2.39; BF: 4.06], with low within-study and between-study heterogeneity [within I2: 14.42%, between I2: 33.44%].

Figure 7
Violin plot illustrating the effect size (SMD) of different types of athletes, from weight lifters to badminton players, along a central dotted line representing zero effect size. Each type has a unique color and distribution shape.

Figure 7. The forest plot in athlete model.

3.5 Moderation analysis

Five moderators regarding athletes characteristics including age, gender, training experience (in years), competitive level, sport type (team or individual), and three moderators concerning imagery dose encompassing imagery practice duration (in days), frequency (sessions per week) and intensity (minutes per session) were included in the moderation analysis. Regarding the moderation analysis of population characteristics, the charts are illustrated in the Supplementary File S5. The detailed results of moderation analysis by population characteristics is presented in Table 1. The results in the gender model show that imagery practice was less effective among female athletes than males (estimates: −0.01, −0.01 to −0.001). In the model of competitive level, imagery practice is more effective for elite athletes (estimates: 0.95, 0.24 to 1.67). In the model of sport type (team or individual), imagery practice was more effective on individual sports (estimates: 0.69, 0.4 to 0.99).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The detailed results of moderation analysis by population characteristics.

The optimal modality appeared to be moderated by gender and sport type (team or individual). Compared with base model (R2 = 0.84), R2 was higher for gender model and sport type model (R2 = 0.85). R2 in training experience model (in years) (R2 = 0.79) and competitive level model (R2 = 0.75) is lower than base model. The regression plots and R2 density plot is presented in Supplementary File S6.

Three moderators concerning imagery dosage were incorporated into the moderation analysis. Regarding the duration of imagery practice, 100 days of imagery practice [coefficient estimates: 5.52, 95%CI: 2.77 to 8.11] is more effective than 80 days [coefficient estimates: 2.59, 95%CI: 1.52 to 3.64], 50 days of practice [coefficient estimates: 0.38, 95%CI: −0.08 to 0.85] and 20 days [coefficient estimates: 0.75, 95%CI: −0.08 to 1.59]. The regression plot is presented in Figure 8.

Figure 8
Graph showing predicted effect size by imagery practice over 100 days. Effect size decreases initially, reaching a minimum around day 50, then rises sharply. Shaded areas indicate credible intervals at 0.95, 0.8, and 0.5 levels, with darker shades representing higher confidence. Dashed red line at zero on the y-axis.

Figure 8. The moderation analysis by duration (days).

Regarding the weekly dose of imagery practice, the effect of practicing one time a week [coefficient estimates: 1.33, 95%CI: 0.69 to 1.99] is better than practicing three times a week [coefficient estimates: 0.45, 95%CI: 0.15 to 0.74] or seven times a week [coefficient estimates: 0.05, 95%CI: −0.35 to 0.44]. The regression plot is presented in Figure 9.

Figure 9
Line chart showing the predicted effect size by weekly imagery practice dose. The effect size starts high around two and decreases as doses increase from zero to seven. Shaded areas represent credible intervals at 0.95, 0.8, and 0.5 levels, becoming narrower with more doses.

Figure 9. The moderation analysis by weekly dose (times).

Regarding the intensity of imagery practice, practicing for 45 min at a time [coefficient estimates: 1.12, 95%CI: 0.2 to 2.04] is more effective than practicing for 10 [coefficient estimates: 0.53, 95%CI: 0.18 to 0.88], 20 [coefficient estimates: 0.73, 95%CI: 0.33 to 0.12], 30 [coefficient estimates: 0.93, 95%CI: 0.33 to 1.57], and 60 min [coefficient estimates: 1.22, 95%CI: −0.18 to 2.6]. The regression plot is presented in Figure 10.

Figure 10
Line graph titled “Predicted Effect Size by Imagery Practice” showing the predicted effect size (SMD) against intensity in minutes. The effect size increases with intensity. Shaded regions represent credible intervals at 0.95, 0.8, and 0.5, with the darkest shade indicating the highest confidence interval. The x-axis ranges from 0 to 60 minutes, and the y-axis ranges from 0 to 5 SMD. A red dashed line is present at the bottom.

Figure 10. The moderation analysis by intensity (minutes).

Thus, the optimal modality appeared to be moderated by duration. Compared with base model (R2 = 0.84), R2 was higher for duration model (R2 = 0.91) and intensity model (R2 = 0.89). R2 in weekly model (R2 = 0.77) is lower than base model. The R2 density plot is presented in Supplementary File S6. The detailed results of moderation analysis by imagery dose is presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The detailed results of moderation analysis by imagery dose.

3.6 Quality grade in each outcome

The quality grade in mental health was based on the sample size, results of meta-analysis, and quality assessment to assess the risk of bias, inconsistency of results, indirectness, and imprecision of effect estimates. The results showed that the quality grade of the null model was low due to potential risk of bias and considerable heterogeneity. However, because the level of heterogeneity was substantially reduced in the mental health model and the athlete model, their quality grades were rated as moderate. In contrast, the quality grade of the imagery model remained low. The plot is illustrated in Figure 11.

Figure 11
Table summarizing a study on imagery practice for mental health. It compares four models: null, mental health, imagery, and athlete, with assumed and corresponding risks, relative effects, number of participants, and evidence quality. The null model shows a higher effect, while the other models have zero standard deviations higher. Evidence quality varies from low to moderate across 1,294 participants in 24 studies. Definitions for confidence intervals and evidence grades are included, indicating research reliability.

Figure 11. Quality grade of mental health.

3.7 Publication bias

In the funnel plot generated using the PublicationBias package, studies were categorized as either affirmative (i.e., statistically significant and in the expected direction) or non-affirmative. A disproportionate concentration of affirmative studies was observed, particularly among those with higher standard errors (i.e., smaller sample sizes), suggesting the possibility of selective reporting driven by statistical significance rather than random variation. The funnel plot is illustrated in Figure 12.

Figure 12
Scatter plot showing the relationship between estimated standard error and point estimate. Orange points represent affirmative data, and gray points represent non-affirmative data. A diagonal line crosses the plot, with a concentration of points near the line, particularly on the left. The x-axis is labeled “Point estimate,” and the y-axis is labeled “Estimated standard error.”

Figure 12. The funnel plot by Publicationbias package.

To quantify the degree of bias required to attenuate the observed effect size to zero, we calculated the s-value. The analysis yielded an s-value of 8.86, with a 95% confidence interval lower bound of 1.96. This indicates that affirmative studies would need to be approximately 8.86 times more likely to be published than non-affirmative studies for the overall effect to be explained away. As the lower bound exceeds 1, the observed effect appears relatively robust and unlikely to be fully attributable to publication bias.

Additionally, a traditional funnel plot generated using the metafor package revealed asymmetry, with an overrepresentation of studies on the right-hand side of the plot. Egger’s test further confirmed this asymmetry (p = 0.0005), indicating potential small-study effects. However, the Trim-and-Fill procedure did not identify any missing studies on the left side of the plot (estimated number = 0, SE = 4.92), suggesting that the observed asymmetry may not reflect a simple pattern of missing negative or non-significant studies. Taken together, while the Trim-and-Fill method did not impute missing studies, the results from the significance funnel plot and Egger’s test indicate that some degree of publication bias is likely present, albeit not necessarily in a form detectable by symmetry-based approaches. The Trim-and-Fill funnel plot is illustrated in Figure 13.

Figure 13
Funnel plot showing the relationship between observed outcomes and standard error for mental health studies, utilizing the trim-and-fill method. Data points are spread around a central funnel, indicating potential publication bias.

Figure 13. The trim-and-fill funnel plot by Metafor package.

4 Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis investigating the impact of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health. This systematic review and meta-analysis consolidate information about the impact of (1) imagery practice alone or imagery in conjunction with other psychological skills training (PST) on mental health, and (2) the dosage of imagery on athletes’ mental health.

4.1 Summary of finding

This meta-analysis demonstrates that imagery practice may improve athletes’ mental health, particularly in reducing anxiety and enhancing self-efficacy and confidence. The effect is more pronounced among male and individual sport athletes, with tennis players showing the strongest improvement. Notably, the combination of imagery and other psychological skills training did not yield additional benefits. A dosage of 45 min per week over 100 days may be associated with more favorable outcomes. However, given the limited number of included studies (only 24 RCTs), this study should be considered preliminary. The findings must be interpreted with caution, and more high-quality empirical research is needed to provide stronger evidence.

4.2 The effect of imagery practice on mental health

The findings indicate that imagery practice may positively impact athletes’ mental health, leading to decreased anxiety, heightened self-confidence, and improved self-efficacy, particularly among tennis players. Certain investigations furnish corroborative evidence for this finding (Dhiman and Bedi, 2010; Fekih et al., 2021; Lee Howard and Reardon, 1986; Page et al., 1999; Zandi and Masomi, 2010; Afzali and MolaNorouzi, 2023). Some scholars have revealed potential mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of imagery practice, indicating that improvements in cognitive ability may lead to reduced state anxiety (Li et al., 2024). Positive imagery is considered an important predictor of cognitive anxiety, as a confident mindset plays a crucial role in effectively alleviating competition-related stress among young individuals (Strachan and Munroe-Chandler, 2006). However, during the implementation of imagery practice, researchers should pay attention to whether athletes have prior experience with imagery practice. Athletes lacking imagery practice experience may exhibit reduced imagery vividness and controllability (Page et al., 1999). Therefore, while imagery practice may be a promising tool for supporting athletes’mental health, its application should be individualized, and future studies are encouraged to compare it systematically with other psychological strategies to determine context-specific effectiveness.

Moreover, the findings indicate that imagery practice can augment athletes’ confidence and self-efficacy. Certain studies have proposed that Motivational General-Mastery (MG-M) imagery is an advisable type of imagery practice for enhancing athletes’ self-efficacy, with positive emotions playing a substantial role in the association between MG-M imagery utilization and self-efficacy (Hammond et al., 2012; Wirbiezcas, 2019). Athletes with higher levels of self-efficacy tend to use imagery more frequently and have better imagery abilities (Volgemute et al., 2021), and there is a significant positive correlation between athletes’ imagery ability, self-efficacy, and athletic performance (Volgemute et al., 2024). Additionally, Di corrado et al. (2025) have suggested that there is also a strong positive correlation between dynamic images and self-efficacy, whereas static images show the opposite pattern (Di Corrado et al., 2025). However, these conclusions are primarily based on limited empirical evidence. Due to the scarcity of data, the present meta-analysis was unable to evaluate whether different types of imagery practice differ significantly in their effects. Therefore, practicing imagery correctly might help enhance athletes’self-efficacy, while athletes with higher self-efficacy levels may also be more likely to engage in imagery more frequently during training.

Similarly, Callow et al. have summarized that dynamic images can enhance the vividness of imagery practice and are the best imagery strategy for improving athletes’ confidence (Callow et al., 2001, 2006). Furthermore, athletes who experience vivid dynamic images often feel more confident because this process allows them to rehearse and improve their performance (Di Corrado et al., 2020). Additionally, some evidence suggests that mastering imagery ability is the best predictor of athletes’ confidence improvement and can also alleviate their negative emotional state (Budnik-Przybylska et al., 2022; Williams and Cumming, 2012; Williams and Cumming, 2016). Hall et al. concluded further that cognitive specific and MG-M imagery were significant predictors of sport confidence in practice and competition (Hall et al., 2009). There is a positive correlation between the use of MG-M imagery and the improvement of athlete confidence (Strachan and Munroe-Chandler, 2006). Thus, appropriate types of imagery practice are necessary to improve athletes’ mental health in a targeted manner. However, due to limited data, we were unable to explore the differential effects of various types of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health in this study. Future research is warranted to specifically investigate whether different types of imagery practice produce distinct effects on athletes’ mental health.

Finally, a small number of studies have indicated that the integration of imagery practice with other psychological skills training may contribute to improvements in athletes’ confidence. Hidayat et al. concluded that the combination of imagery practice and self-talk can effectively improve the confidence of novice badminton players (Hidayat et al., 2023). Two interventions can complement each other. Imagery practice can connect the motor system and verbal system through internal representation, while self-talk generates language that activates the image motion of internal representation (Longstaff, 2011). Additionally, research indicates that the combination of imagery and relaxation yields a more substantial positive impact on athletes’ mental toughness than imagery practice alone (Bhambri et al., 2005). However, some participants during experiments expressed that combining multiple psychological skills training would increase the difficulty and complexity of the training (Weinberg et al., 1983). Therefore, despite existing evidence supporting the effectiveness of multi-component psychological skills training in promoting athletes’ mental health, our meta-analysis was unable to confirm the efficacy of combining imagery practice with other psychological strategies, largely due to inadequate sample sizes and insufficient data to support robust conclusions.

Additionally, imagery practice does not seem to be the most effective among all psychological skills training. Some evidence showed that imagery practice is less successful than relaxation techniques to improve athletes’ anxiety level (Karimian et al., 2010), and our findings also find that the effect of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health appears to be notably weaker than that of video observation. This discrepancy may be due to the inclusion of only a single study involving video-based interventions, leading to limited data support. Therefore, this was an exploratory analysis, and the findings should be interpreted with caution. Additional high-quality research is needed to further evaluate and validate the comparative effectiveness of these approaches. At present, no definitive conclusions can be drawn regarding the relative superiority of these interventions.

On the other side, the results of this study indicated a favorable effect of imagery practice exclusively among tennis players, with no statistically significant effects observed in other athlete groups. This finding is consistent with recent evidence showing that imagery practice has been used to enhance motivation, self-confidence, and emotional regulation, while reducing anxiety among tennis players (Fekih et al., 2021; Nicolas et al., 2025; Robin and Dominique, 2022; Terry et al., 1995). The positive effects of imagery practice in tennis players may be due to the sport’s high cognitive and emotional demands. Theoretical models suggest that tennis involves frequent pauses, complex visuospatial processing, and the need for emotional control, which closely align with the psychological mechanisms of imagery practice (Robin and Dominique, 2022). Moreover, research has shown that the effectiveness of internal versus external imagery may vary by skill type, such as open or closed tennis skills, supporting a task-specific approach to imagery design (Dana and Gozalzadeh, 2017). However, the lack of statistically significant results in other athlete groups may be attributed to insufficient data rather than the ineffectiveness of imagery practice itself. Similarly, the significant effects observed in tennis players should be interpreted with caution, as they are based on data from only three included studies, limiting the robustness of this finding. Therefore, although some positive evidence supports the effectiveness of imagery practice in improving athletes’ mental health, there remains a significant lack of data for specific sports and types of imagery practice. This limitation, marked by insufficient sample sizes, hinders the ability to draw robust conclusions.

4.3 The dose–response relationship of imagery practice on mental health

To our knowledge, this is the first multilevel meta-analysis exploring the optimal dosage of imagery practice for improving athletes’ mental health. The results showed that imagery practice for about 45 min, once a week for one hundred days may be associated with greatest positive effect on athletes’ mental health.

Prior research or reviews have proposed optimal imagery dosage primarily for performance enhancement, while neglecting the exploration of imagery dosage for mental health. For instance, Driskell et al. suggested that the optimal dosage for psychological skills training suitable for athletes is 20.8 min (Driskell et al., 1994). Furthermore, Itoh et al. have indicated that four weekly and 13–15 min each time for imagery practice have the best effect on improving the performance of basketball players (Itoh et al., 2022, 2023). The latest meta-analysis showed the optimal dose of imagery practice for muscle growth in adult populations is 4 weeks, three times a week, each lasting 15 min (Paravlic et al., 2018). It is also suggested that the more imagery practice is conducted, the more obvious the benefits for athletes (Simonsmeier et al., 2018). This supports our conclusion that long-term imagery practice is beneficial for improving athletes’ mental health.

Additionally, regarding the intensity of imagery practice, our findings suggest that a 45-min session may yield more favorable outcomes compared to 60-min sessions. However, this result may be attributed to the limited number of studies examining 60-min imagery interventions, which constrains the robustness of the available evidence. To our knowledge, our study is among the first to explore the potential optimal dosage of imagery practice for enhancing athletes’ mental health. However, given that only 24 randomized controlled trials were included, the limited sample size may affect the stability and generalizability of the conclusions. Further empirical studies with larger and more diverse samples are warranted to strengthen the robustness of these findings.

On the other hand, moderation analysis shows that both age and gender may be negatively correlated with the effect of imagery practice on mental health, which means that the older the participants, the less significant the effect of imagery practice, and the effect of imagery practice on female athletes seems to be less pronounced in female athletes than in males. However, Robin et al. concluded that in current research on imagery practice, the representation of female athletes is relatively weak (Robin and Dominique, 2022). Therefore, this study was an exploratory analysis, and the findings should be interpreted with caution. Dose–response patterns may differ by sport, gender, and age. Due to the lack of data, this conclusion is not robust, so additional research is needed to provide more robust evidence.

4.4 Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this study represents the first Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis to comprehensively synthesize the effects of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health. It is also the first to propose an optimal dosage of imagery practice tailored to enhance mental health outcomes in athletic populations. This provides further sufficient evidence for the potential application of imagery practice in the field of sports, enhances the development prospects of imagery practice in the athlete population, and increases the attention of athletes and coaches to psychological skills training.

However, there are still some limitations that need to be addressed. First, this meta-analysis included a relatively small number of randomized controlled trials, and the sample size of athletes across studies was limited. Future research should incorporate larger sample sizes and high-quality randomized controlled designs to provide more robust and reliable evidence. Secondly, although the high heterogeneity observed in the null models was substantially reduced in both the athlete model and the mental health model, it still undermines the robustness of the study’s conclusions. The heterogeneity may be attributed to variations in athlete types and mental health outcomes, thereby limiting the reliability and generalizability of the results. Thirdly, evidence of publication bias was detected in the data, which further weakens the robustness of the findings and lowers the overall quality of the evidence. To improve the reliability of future research, we emphasize the need for more rigorous experimental designs, including the proper implementation of blinding procedures during both the intervention and outcome assessment phases. Additionally, random assignment should be conducted in a fully randomized and standardized manner to ensure internal validity and minimize selection bias.

Fourthly, this study did not investigate which specific types of imagery practice (e.g., motivational-general mastery [MG-M], cognitive-specific [CS], dynamic vs. static imagery) are most effective for improving particular aspects of athletes’ mental health, such as anxiety, confidence, or self-efficacy. Future research should systematically compare the effects of different imagery techniques across diverse psychological outcomes and athlete populations (e.g., by age, sport type, or performance level) to determine the most beneficial combinations. Fifthly, although our findings did not reveal significant mental health benefits from combining imagery practice with other psychological skills training, this result may stem from the limited number of studies addressing such combinations. We encourage future research to examine whether certain psychological skills (e.g., self-talk, goal-setting, or mindfulness) synergize more effectively with imagery in promoting mental health. Finally, while it is hypothesized that integrating imagery practice with physical exercise may lead to greater mental health benefits than imagery alone, this remains an open question. We emphasize that this suggestion is speculative and requires empirical validation through well-designed experimental studies. Simultaneously, most of the research on imagery practice primarily focuses on athletes, often overlooking other demographic groups. Future studies should expand beyond athletes to investigate the effects of imagery practice on the physical capabilities and mental health of students, children, or the elderly, as more comprehensive data is needed to support these claims.

5 Conclusion

To our knowledge, this study is the first to employ a Bayesian multilevel framework to systematically evaluate the effects and optimal dosage of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health. Results demonstrate that imagery practice significantly may enhance mental health outcomes, particularly anxiety, confidence, and self-efficacy, with possible optimal benefits observed at a dosage of 45 min per week for 100 days. These findings extend existing psychological skills training literature by providing evidence-based dosage guidelines and highlighting differential effects by sport type and gender.

Future research should address several gaps. First, larger and more diverse RCTs are needed to validate the observed dosage-response relationships. Second, studies should explore which types of imagery practice (e.g., MG-M vs. CS imagery) are most effective for specific psychological outcomes. Third, researchers should examine whether combining imagery with other psychological techniques (e.g., self-talk, mindfulness) enhances its effectiveness. Finally, expanding the study population to include non-athlete groups such as adolescents, older adults, or clinical populations could broaden the applicability of imagery-based interventions.

Despite its strengths, this study has several limitations. First, most of the analyses (e.g., Gender, Age, Imagery types) in this study should be considered preliminary due to limited data availability. As a result, the conclusions remain unstable and require further confirmation through large-scale, high-quality experimental studies in the future. Second, moderate heterogeneity across studies may have influenced pooled estimates. Third, evidence of publication bias suggests a potential inflation of the observed effects. Fourth, variations in intervention content and delivery were not examined in detail, limiting conclusions about the most effective imagery types.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://osf.io/k2buz/?view_only=103fffebd2ba4a9bb8e428e04c902c33.

Author contributions

XZ: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. SZ: Data curation, Resources, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Software, Writing – review & editing. SN: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. YL: Software, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. TL: Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing. CI: Validation, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. ZN: Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

We express our profound gratitude to all the students and the professor who participated to this meta-analysis. Additionally, we have created a graphical abstract and compiled a complete reference list for all studies included in this meta-analysis; please refer to Supplementary Files S7, S8, respectively.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1618617/full#supplementary-material

References

Abbas, A., Hefnawy, M. T., and Negida, A. (2024). Meta-analysis accelerator: a comprehensive tool for statistical data conversion in systematic reviews with meta-analysis. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 24:243. doi: 10.1186/s12874-024-02356-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Afzali, S., and MolaNorouzi, K. (2023). The effect of mindfulness and PETTLEP imagery on competitive state anxiety and the performance of equestrian athletes of show jumping discipline. Motor Learn. Growth J. 16, 83–99. doi: 10.22059/jsmdl.2023.364125.1745

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alwan, M. A., Zakaria, H. A. A., Rahim, M. R. A., Hamid, N. A., and Fuad, M. D. F. (2013). Comparison between two relaxation methods on competitive state anxiety among college soccer teams during pre-competition stage. Int. J. Advanced Sport Sci. Res. 1, 90–104.

Google Scholar

Bhambri, E., Dhillon, P. K., and Sahni, S. P. (2005). Effect of psychological interventions in enhancing mental toughness dimensions of sports persons. J. Indian Acad. Appl. Psychol. 31, 65–70.

Google Scholar

Boetje, J., and Van De Schoot, R. (2024). The SAFE procedure: a practical stopping heuristic for active learning-based screening in systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Syst. Rev. 13:81. doi: 10.1186/s13643-024-02502-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braginsky, M., Mathur, M., and VanderWeele, T. J. (2019). PublicationBias: sensitivity analysis for publication bias in meta-analyses

Google Scholar

Budnik-Przybylska, D., Huzarska, I., and Karasiewicz, K. (2022). Does imagery ability matter for the relationship between temperament and self-confidence in team and individual sport disciplines? Front. Psychol. 13:893457. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.893457

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bürkner, P.-C. (2018). Advanced Bayesian multilevel Modeling with the R package brms. The R Journal 10:395. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2018-017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Callow, N., Hardy, L., and Hall, C. (2001). The effects of a motivational general-mastery imagery intervention on the sport confidence of high-level badminton players. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 72, 389–400. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2001.10608975

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Callow, N., Roberts, R., and Fawkes, J. Z. (2006). Effects of dynamic and static imagery on vividness of imagery, skiing performance, and confidence. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity 1:Article 2. doi: 10.2202/1932-0191.1001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chungath, A. F., Sudhesh, N. T., Gupta, S., and Divekar, S. (2022). Efficacy of a video Modeling and imagery-controlled trial intervention in a non-Western adolescent population: a case study. Case studies in sport and exercise. Psychology 6:S1-24-S1-37. doi: 10.1123/cssep.2022-0009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Commodari, E. (2024). Mental imagery in education: What impact on the relationships with visuospatial processing and school performance in junior high school students?

Google Scholar

Cumming, J., Nordin, S. M., Horton, R., and Reynolds, S. (2006). Examining the direction of imagery and self-talk on dart-throwing performance and self efficacy. Sport Psychol. 20, 257–274. doi: 10.1123/tsp.20.3.257

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dana, A., and Gozalzadeh, E. (2017). Internal and external imagery effects on tennis skills among novices. Percept. Mot. Skills 124, 1022–1043. doi: 10.1177/0031512517719611

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhiman, C., and Bedi, H. S. (2010). Effect of autogenic training and mental imagery on the trait anxiety of the hockey players. Br. J. Sports Med. 44:i60.1-i60. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2010.078725.201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Guarnera, M., Guerrera, C. S., Maldonato, N. M., Di Nuovo, S., Castellano, S., et al. (2020). Mental imagery skills in competitive young athletes and non-athletes. Front. Psychol. 11:633. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00633

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Tortella, P., Coco, M., Guarnera, M., Tusak, M., and Parisi, M. C. (2025). Mental imagery and stress: the mediating role of self-efficacy in competitive martial arts athletes. Front. Psychol. 16:1517718. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1517718

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Digitizer, G. G. (2020). Getdata-Graph-Digitizer. Available online. (2020). Available online at: http://getdata-graph-digitizer.com/ (Accessed March 2025).

Google Scholar

Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., and Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? J. Appl. Psychol. 79, 481–492. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.79.4.481

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fastame, M. C. (2021). Visuo-spatial mental imagery and geometry skills in school-aged children. Sch. Psychol. Int. 42, 324–337. doi: 10.1177/0143034321992458

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fekih, S., Zguira, M. S., Koubaa, A., Bettaieb, A., Hajji, J., Bragazzi, N. L., et al. (2021). Effects of mental training through imagery on the competitive anxiety of adolescent tennis players fasting during Ramadan: a randomized, controlled experimental study. Front. Nutr. 8:713296. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.713296

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feltz, D. L., and Riessinger, C. A. (1990). Effects of in vivo emotive imagery and performance feedback on self-efficacy and muscular endurance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 12, 132–143. doi: 10.1123/jsep.12.2.132

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouttebarge, V., Castaldelli-Maia, J. M., Gorczynski, P., Hainline, B., Hitchcock, M. E., Kerkhoffs, G. M., et al. (2019). Occurrence of mental health symptoms and disorders in current and former elite athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 700–706. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2019-100671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grubic, N., Jain, S., Mihajlovic, V., Thornton, J. S., and Johri, A. M. (2021). Competing against COVID-19: have we forgotten about student-athletes’ mental health? Br. J. Sports Med. 55, 950–951. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2021-104218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarnera, M., Pellerone, M., Commodari, E., Valenti, G. D., and Buccheri, S. L. (2019). Mental images and school learning: a longitudinal study on children. Front. Psychol. 10:2034. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haddaway, N. R., Page, M. J., Pritchard, C. C., and McGuinness, L. A. (2022). PRISMA 2020: an R package and shiny app for producing PRISMA 2020-compliant flow diagrams, with interactivity for optimised digital transparency and open synthesis. Campbell Syst. Rev. 18:e1230. doi: 10.1002/cl2.1230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. R., Munroe-Chandler, K. J., Cumming, J., Law, B., Ramsey, R., and Murphy, L. (2009). Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence. J. Sports Sci. 27, 327–337. doi: 10.1080/02640410802549769

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, T., Gregg, M., Hrycaiko, D., Mactavish, J., and Leslie-Toogood, A. (2012). The effects of a motivational general-mastery imagery intervention on the imagery ability and self-efficacy of inter-collegiate golfers. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity 7. doi: 10.1515/1932-0191.1066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., and Ebert, D. D. (2021). Doing Meta-analysis with R: A hands-on guide. 1st Edn. Boca Raton, FL, USA: Chapman and Hall/CRC.

Google Scholar

Harris, D., Wilkinson, S., and Ellmers, T. (2023). From fear of falling to choking under pressure: a predictive processing perspective of disrupted motor control under anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 148:105115. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidayat, Y., Yudiana, Y., Hambali, B., Sultoni, K., Ustun, U. D., and Singnoy, C. (2023). The effect of the combined self-talk and mental imagery program on the badminton motor skills and self-confidence of youth beginner student-athletes. BMC Psychol. 11:35. doi: 10.1186/s40359-023-01073-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Altman, D. G., Gotzsche, P. C., Juni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The cochrane collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holzinger, A. (2016). Interactive machine learning for health informatics: when do we need the human-in-the-loop? Brain Informatics 3, 119–131. doi: 10.1007/s40708-016-0042-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hut, M., Minkler, T. O., Glass, C. R., Weppner, C. H., Thomas, H. M., and Flannery, C. B. (2023). A randomized controlled study of mindful sport performance enhancement and psychological skills training with collegiate track and field athletes. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 35, 284–306. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2021.1989521

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Itoh, S., Morris, T., and Spittle, M. (2022). Examining the frequency variable in the imagery dose-response relationship. Asian J. Sport and Exercise Psychol. 2, 122–130. doi: 10.1016/j.ajsep.2022.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Itoh, S., Morris, T., and Spittle, M. (2023). Examining duration in the imagery dose-response relationship. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity 18:20220020. doi: 10.1515/jirspa-2022-0020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 187–202. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14, S103–S109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanthack, T. F. D., Bigliassi, M., Vieira, L. F., and Altimari, L. R. (2013). Efeito agudo da imagética no desempenho de lances livres e percepção de autoeficácia em atletas. Revista Brasileira Cineantropometria Desempenho Humano 16, 47–57. doi: 10.5007/1980-0037.2014v16n1p47

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimian, M., Kashefolhagh, F., Dadashi, M. S., and Chharbaghi, Z. (2010). The effect of relaxation and mental imagery on self-efficacy, competitive anxiety and sportive performance. Br. J. Sports Med. 44:i57.2-i57. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2010.078725.192

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kass, R. E., and Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 90, 773–795. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruschke, J. K., and Liddell, T. M. (2018). The Bayesian new statistics: hypothesis testing, estimation, meta-analysis, and power analysis from a Bayesian perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 178–206. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1221-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee Howard, W., and Reardon, J. P. (1986). Changes in the self concept and athletic performance of weight lifters through a cognitive-hypnotic approach: an empirical study. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 28, 248–257. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1986.10402661

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Moritz, S. E., and Liu, H. (2024). Motor imagery as a potential tool to alleviate choking under pressure in sports performance. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity 19:20240012. doi: 10.1515/jirspa-2024-0012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Longstaff, L. (2011). Factors affecting the optimal use of imagery and self talk in golfers : University of Northumbria at Newcastle.

Google Scholar

Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M., and Lüdecke, D. (2019). bayestestR: describing effects and their uncertainty, existence and significance within the Bayesian framework. J. Open Source Softw. 4:1541. doi: 10.21105/joss.01541

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, E. A., and Gibson, A.-M. (2017). The effect of an imagery training intervention on self-confidence, anxiety and performance in acrobatic gymnastics – a pilot study. J. Imagery Res. Sport and Physical Activity 12:20160009. doi: 10.1515/jirspa-2016-0009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, K. A., and Hall, C. R. (1995). Using mental imagery to enhance intrinsic motivation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 54–69. doi: 10.1123/jsep.17.1.54

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, M. B., and VanderWeele, T. J. (2020). Sensitivity analysis for publication bias in meta-analyses. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C Appl. Stat. 69, 1091–1119. doi: 10.1111/rssc.12440

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAlister, A., and Cutler, D. (2024). A pilot feasibility study comparing mindfulness and imagery interventions on sport anxiety in division 1 volleyball players. J. Sport Behav. 47, 59–65.

Google Scholar

McGuinness, L. A., and Higgins, J. P. T. (2021). Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): an R package and shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments. Res. Synth. Methods 12, 55–61. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesagno, C., and Beckmann, J. (2017). Choking under pressure: theoretical models and interventions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 16, 170–175. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.05.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mguidich, H., Zoudji, B., and Khacharem, A. (2024). Which modality is best for delivering an imagery script? Evidence for desirable difficulty effect. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1–19, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2024.2345704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monsma, E., Mensch, J., and Farroll, J. (2009). Keeping your head in the game: sport-specific imagery and anxiety among injured athletes. J. Athl. Train. 44, 410–417. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-44.4.410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munroe-Chandler, K. J., Hall, C. R., Fishburne, G. J., and Shannon, V. (2005). Using cognitive general imagery to improve soccer strategies. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 5, 41–49. doi: 10.1080/17461390500076592

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munzert, J., Lorey, B., and Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Res. Rev. 60, 306–326. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.12.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Collegiate Athletic Association. (2022). NCAA Student-Athlete COVID-19 Well-being Study: Survey Results—December 2021. Available online at: http://www.ncaa.org/about/resources/research/ncaa-student-athlete-covid-19-well-being-study

Google Scholar

Nicolas, R., Carien, R., Ouarti, Y., and Laurent, D. (2025). Beneficial effects of imagination of successful action after an actual error on baseline performances in non-expert young tennis players. Psychol. Res. 89:23. doi: 10.1007/s00426-024-02051-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noh, Y.-E., Morris, T., and Andersen, M. B. (2007). Psychological intervention programs for reduction of injury in ballet dancers. Res. Sports Med. 15, 13–32. doi: 10.1080/15438620600987064

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, S. J., Sime, W., and Nordell, K. (1999). The effects of imagery on female college swimmers’ perceptions of anxiety. The Sport Psychologist 13, 458–469. doi: 10.1123/tsp.13.4.458

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paravlic, A. H., Slimani, M., Tod, D., Marusic, U., Milanovic, Z., and Pisot, R. (2018). Effects and dose–response relationships of motor imagery practice on strength development in healthy adult populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 48, 1165–1187. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0874-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raboin, S. (2024). Visualizing victory: the role of imagery in empowering athletes battling obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity 19:20240006. doi: 10.1515/jirspa-2024-0006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robin, N., and Dominique, L. (2022). Mental imagery and tennis: a review, applied recommendations and new research directions. Movement Sport Sci. Motricité. 127, 57–75. doi: 10.1051/sm/2022009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Röver, C. (2020). Bayesian random-effects Meta-analysis using the bayesmeta R package. J. Stat. Softw. 93, 1–51. doi: 10.18637/jss.v093.i06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumeau, V., Grospretre, S., and Babault, N. (2023). Post-activation performance enhancement and motor imagery are efficient to emphasize the effects of a standardized warm-up on Sprint-running performances. Sports 11:108. doi: 10.3390/sports11050108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. (2016). How to GRADE the quality of the evidence. Cochrane Consumers and Communication Group. Available online at: http://cccrg.cochrane.org/author-resources (Accessed April 2025).

Google Scholar

Simonsmeier, B. A., Frank, C., Gubelmann, H., and Schneider, M. (2018). The effects of motor imagery training on performance and mental representation of 7- to 15-year-old gymnasts of different levels of expertise. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 7, 155–168. doi: 10.1037/spy0000117

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strachan, L., and Munroe-Chandler, K. (2006). Using imagery to predict self-confidence and anxiety in young elite athletes. J. Imagery Res. Sport Physical Activity. 1, 1–19. doi: 10.2202/1932-0191.1004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. A., and Shaw, D. F. (2002). The effects of outcome imagery on golf-putting performance. J. Sports Sci. 20, 607–613. doi: 10.1080/026404102320183167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Terry, P., Coakley, L., and Karageorghis, C. (1995). Effects of intervention upon Precompetition state anxiety in elite junior tennis players: the relevance of the matching hypothesis. Percept. Mot. Skills 81, 287–296. doi: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.1.287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Toth, A. J., McNeill, E., Hayes, K., Moran, A. P., and Campbell, M. (2020). Does mental practice still enhance performance? A 24 year follow-up and meta-analytic replication and extension. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 48:101672. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101672

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Schoot, R., de Bruin, J., Schram, R., Zahedi, P., de Boer, J., Weijdema, F., et al. (2021). An open source machine learning framework for efficient and transparent systematic reviews. 3, 125–133. doi: 10.1038/s42256-020-00287-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veritas Health Innovation (2023). Covidence systematic review software [Computer software]. Available online at: www.covidence.org (Accessed March 2025).

Google Scholar

Volgemute, K., Vazne, Ž., and Krauksta, D. (2021). The relationship between imagery and physical self-efficacy in athletes. LASE J. Sport Sci. 12, 3–11.

Google Scholar

Volgemute, K., Vazne, Z., and Krauksta, D. (2024). An intervention into imagery and self-efficacy: enhancing athletic achievements of alpine skiers. Educ. Sci 14:513. doi: 10.3390/educsci14050513

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Nayfeh, T., Tetzlaff, J., O’Blenis, P., and Murad, M. H. (2020). Error rates of human reviewers during abstract screening in systematic reviews. PLoS One 15:e0227742. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, R. S., Chan, R., and Jackson, A. (1983). Mental preparation strategies and performance: is a combination of techniques better than a single technique? J. Sports Sci. 1, 211–216. doi: 10.1080/02640418308729682

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, S. E., and Cumming, J. (2012). Sport imagery ability predicts trait confidence, and challenge andthreat appraisal tendencies. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 12, 499–508. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2011.630102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, S. E., and Cumming, J. (2016). Athlete imagery ability: a predictor of confidence and anxiety intensity and direction. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 14, 268–280. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2015.1025809

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirbiezcas, M. (2019). The mediation of affect on imagery use and self-efficacy in collegiate athletes. Georgia Southern University.

Google Scholar

Zandi, H. G., and Masomi, H. (2010). The effects of imagery in soccer players perceptions of anxiety during penalty kick. Br. J. Sports Med. 44:i61.1-i61. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2010.078725.205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zervas, Y., and Kakkos, V. (1991). Visuomotor behavior rehearsal in archery shooting performance. Percept. Mot. Skills 73, 1183–1190. doi: 10.2466/pms.1991.73.3f.1183

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: imagery practice, psychological skills training, athletes, mental health, sports

Citation: Zhang X, Zhao S, Ng S, Liu Y, Liang T, In Lao C and Ning Z (2025) Optimal dosage and effectiveness of imagery practice on athletes’ mental health: a Bayesian multilevel meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1618617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1618617

Received: 26 April 2025; Accepted: 28 July 2025;
Published: 08 August 2025.

Edited by:

Martin James Turner, Manchester Metropolitan University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Elena Commodari, University of Catania, Italy
Mehmet Söyler, Cankiri Karatekin University, Türkiye

Copyright © 2025 Zhang, Zhao, Ng, Liu, Liang, In Lao and Ning. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shiao Zhao, emhhb3NoaWFvNTMwQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.