- 1College of Music and Dance, China West Normal University, Nanchong, China
- 2The Graduate School Arts and Culture, Sangmyung University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Introduction: This study investigates the effects of preferences for Chinese and Western classical music on three key psychological constructs in adults: cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. Guided by the World Health Organization’s principles of mental health, the research aims to explore how preferences for these distinct musical traditions relate to individual psychological well-being.
Methods: The study analyzed data from 604 valid responses collected through questionnaires, supplemented by behavioral observations, to examine the impact of classical music preferences on the targeted psychological outcomes.
Results: Findings indicate that a preference for classical music (both Chinese and Western) significantly enhances cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience, with the strongest effect observed on psychological resilience. Among moderating factors, age and educational attainment significantly influenced the relationship between music preference and the three psychological aspects, whereas gender had no significant moderating effect. Additionally, preference for Western classical music was found to be more effective than preference for Chinese classical music across all three dimensions, potentially attributed to its complex harmonic structure and rich emotional expression.
Discussion: These results provide empirical evidence for the link between classical music preference and psychological well-being, offering new insights for the development of music therapy strategies and mental health interventions.
1 Introduction
The World Health Organization (WHO) emphasizes that “there is no health without mental health,” a principle that has achieved global consensus (Kuzman et al., 2020). In its in-depth interpretation of this grand principle, the academic community has widely acknowledged that positive mental health is not a monolithic state but is composed of several key capabilities. Among these, the ability to efficiently manage emotional fluctuations, known as cognitive emotion regulation (Garnefski et al., 2001; Saarikallio, 2011); a stable and positive perception of self-worth, or self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965); and the capacity to adapt and recover from adversity, termed psychological resilience (Connor and Davidson, 2003), are recognized as its three indispensable pillars. These three components are intertwined, collectively forming a dynamic and complex system (Harvey and Boynton, 2021; Surzykiewicz et al., 2022).
Music, particularly classical music, is widely regarded as a tool for regulating psychological states, alleviating stress, and enhancing emotional expression (Rickard, 2004). Research indicates that music can evoke inner emotions, influence cognition, and shape behavioral responses (Koelsch, 2010). However, the psychological benefits of music are not uniform across all individuals and situations. The extent to which a piece of music can promote well-being often depends on an individual’s emotional connection to it. This naturally brings forth the crucial role of music preference, which acts as a key moderating factor between the musical stimulus and the psychological response. Music preference is not merely a transient opinion but a stable trait associated with personality and cognitive style (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003). While some scholars define it as the degree of liking for a certain music genre (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003), this study adopts a more experience-oriented definition: the degree of liking for a specific musical pattern after receiving the complete auditory stimulus, with its influence potentially persisting even after the listening experience has concluded. The congruence between a listener’s preference and the music is crucial for fostering positive emotional and cognitive responses (Saarikallio, 2011).
This study focuses on three core pillars of mental health relevant to the musical experience: cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. The selection of these variables is based on evidence from prior research revealing their specific connections with music. For instance, classical music, with its complex structures and harmonious melodies, provides a unique auditory environment for enhancing cognitive emotion regulation. It can evoke positive emotions while reducing negative ones (Chen, 2023), and individuals who prefer classical music often report using it as a strategy to manage their moods (Olsen et al., 2023). Secondly, music engagement and self-esteem are closely linked. Mastering musical skills, or even just deeply appreciating complex music, can cultivate a sense of accomplishment and self-worth (Culp, 2016). Finally, music is considered an important factor in building psychological resilience. For both children and adults, recreational music activities have been shown to enhance resilience by providing an outlet for stress and promoting adaptive coping mechanisms (Koehler et al., 2023; Tabibnia and Radecki, 2018).
Empirical research is a common method for exploring the multidimensional complexity of music and its effects (Li et al., 2024; Lin et al., 2025; Oncu, 2025; Oza and Behnaz, 2024; Togay and Firat, 2024). However, existing scholarship has disproportionately focused on the Western musical system (Fung, 2018; Hu et al., 2021). This scholarly gap becomes particularly salient when considering the stark differences in structure and cultural connotation between Chinese and Western classical music. Western classical music is known for its complex harmony and polyphonic textures, often guiding listeners through intense and cathartic emotional arcs. In contrast, Chinese classical music places greater emphasis on melody, timbre, and a connection with nature and philosophy, aiming to cultivate a meditative and introspective atmosphere (Thuc, 2018; Wang et al., 2021). These fundamental differences suggest they may yield distinct psychological benefits.
Therefore, this study aims to fill this gap by systematically analyzing the effects of preferences for Chinese and Western classical music on cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience among Chinese adults. Employing a methodology of questionnaire surveys and behavioral observation, the core variables will be assessed through preference questions and standardized self-report scales. Simultaneously, potential confounding variables such as gender, age, and educational background will be carefully controlled to ensure the reliability and validity of the findings. This research is poised to provide new perspectives for music education and mental health interventions, with findings that could contribute to both theoretical foundations and practical applications, thereby offering valuable insights for policymakers, educators, and mental health professionals alike.
2 Theoretical background and assumptions
According to the SOR theoretical model established by Mehrabian and Russell (1974), individual behavioral responses essentially constitute a chained process whereby environmental stimuli induce psychological state alterations through neurocognitive mediation. This theoretical framework positions external environmental parameters as core independent variables that activate information processing within the central nervous system, with its operational pathway manifesting as sensory stimuli being transformed into psychological evaluation signals via thalamocortical pathways, subsequently triggering adaptive behavioral feedback (Drobesch-Binter, 2000). In interdisciplinary applications, this model demonstrates unique explanatory power in the field of music psychology: Juslin and Västfjäll (2008) revealed through psychoacoustic experiments how musical elements modulate emotional encoding in the limbic system, while Min et al. (2020) systematically demonstrated through ecological momentary assessment (EMA) methodology the reconstructive effects of musical environments on decision-making neural circuits. It is particularly noteworthy that as a cross-modal affective stimulus carrier, the congruence between musical structural features and individual cognitive schemata not only determines the formation of aesthetic preferences (Droe, 2006) but also profoundly engages in the functional construction of emotion regulation systems through neuroplasticity mechanisms. Therefore, we employ this theoretical framework to investigate the relationships between musical preferences and cognitive emotion regulation strategies, self-esteem levels, and psychological resilience.
Classical music has been widely recognized for its influence on cognitive emotion regulation, operating through a combination of physiological, cognitive, and social mechanisms. Research suggests that classical music activates the reward system, triggering endorphin release and reducing stress hormones like cortisol, thereby promoting relaxation and mood regulation (Peistaraite and Clark, 2020). From a cognitive perspective, its complex structures engage attention, fostering emotional reappraisal and memory associations that enhance positive affect. Socially, cultural exposure to classical traditions may reinforce emotional self-regulation through shared aesthetic experiences. In adulthood, musicians leverage these mechanisms to improve self-regulatory learning abilities, particularly through emotion regulation processes (López-íñiguez and Mcpherson, 2021). Even in distressing contexts, music facilitates brain re-engagement via neural pathways linked to reward processing and emotional control (Broughton et al., 2021). Studies on early 20th-century classical performances highlight how motor imagery and emotional observation during musical execution enhance cognitive reappraisal capacities (Pauwels et al., 2014). Therefore, we propose Hypothesis 1a (H1a): A preference for classical music (Chinese or Western) is closely associated with cognitive emotion regulation.
Self-esteem, defined as an individual’s overall assessment of self-worth, benefits from classical music through multiple pathways. Physiologically, compositions like Mozart’s reduce cortisol levels while stimulating endorphin production, improving mood and self-perception (Pauwels et al., 2014). Cognitively, music education fosters skill mastery and self-efficacy, with teachers using musical challenges to build student confidence. Socially, participation in musical communities creates identity reinforcement, where group engagement and cultural heritage enhance self-worth through shared achievements (Culp, 2016). Therefore, we propose Hypothesis 1b (H1b): A preference for classical music (Chinese or Western) is closely related to self-esteem.
Psychological resilience, an individual’s ability to adapt to stress, is also bolstered by classical music through interacting mechanisms. Physiologically, music regulates autonomic nervous system reactivity, improving heart rate variability and stress recovery (Kegelaers et al., 2021). Cognitively, musicians develop adaptive coping strategies by associating musical patterns with emotional control, enhancing stress tolerance. Socially, immersion in musical traditions provides cultural resources and peer support networks that buffer career-related pressures (Loveday et al., 2023). These combined effects have led to the use of classical music training to manage performance anxiety and enhance mental skills among students, particularly through metacognitive strategies that strengthen resilience (Osborne et al., 2014). Therefore, we propose Hypothesis 1c (H1c): A preference for classical music (Chinese or Western) is closely related to psychological resilience.
However, the psychological effects of music are not universal but are deeply embedded in cultural contexts. To explain how differences in musical structure lead to variations in psychological outcomes, this study draws upon the foundational framework of cultural self-construal theory (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). This theory posits that Western, individualistic cultures tend to foster an “independent self,” whose psychological needs focus on highlighting personal uniqueness, pursuing individual achievement, and directly expressing inner emotions. Conversely, East Asian, collectivistic cultures tend to cultivate an “interdependent self,” whose psychological needs emphasize maintaining interpersonal harmony, adapting to the environment, and achieving inner emotional peace.
This theoretical framework provides us with a key explanatory pathway. Western classical music, characterized by its complex harmony, polyphonic textures, and strong, dramatic “tension-resolution” structures, offers an effective avenue for the “independent self” to experience, process, and cathartically release strong emotions, thereby fulfilling its psychological needs for emotional intensity and self-affirmation (Weiß et al., 2019). In contrast, Chinese classical music is primarily based on monophonic melodies, emphasizing subtle changes in timbre, the linear flow of melody, and the artistic conception created by white space. This structure, which pursues introspection, subtlety, and balance, is highly aligned with the psychological tendency of the “interdependent self” to maintain inner tranquility and integrate into collective harmony (Thuc, 2018; Wang et al., 2021).
Based on the theoretical analysis above, we propose the following directional comparative hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2a (H2a): Compared to Chinese classical music, Western classical music is more effective in enhancing cognitive emotion regulation.
Hypothesis 2b (H2b): Compared to Chinese classical music, Western classical music is more effective in enhancing self-esteem.
Hypothesis 2c (H2c): Compared to Chinese classical music, Western classical music is more effective in enhancing psychological resilience.
3 Method
3.1 Data sources
In April 2024, in order to explore the psychological benefits of Chinese and Western classical music preferences in cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience, we carried out a questionnaire survey among the Chinese public and conducted research combined with statistical analysis. This survey adopted a mixed-methods sampling strategy, with a total of 713 questionnaires distributed, including 108 paper questionnaires and 605 online questionnaires.
To enhance the diversity of the sample, our sampling was conducted through two main channels. Online, we utilized a professional sampling platform (“Wenjuanxing”) which pushed the questionnaire link to users in its national database based on its own stratified algorithms. Offline, our investigators employed a convenience sampling method in public places such as shopping malls, parks, and community centers across different regions of China, as well as at public transportation hubs like railway stations, to intercept and invite the public to participate. It is important to acknowledge that while this multi-pronged approach increases coverage, the non-probability nature of the convenience sampling component means the sample’s generalizability should be considered with care.
To encourage participants to actively complete the survey, we prepared random e-cash red envelopes ranging from 1 to 100 yuan for all respondents. We clearly informed the participants that their feedback would be used for the research on the impact of Chinese and Western classical music on psychological well-being, and that all data would be kept strictly confidential and used for academic research only. Eventually, a total of 713 questionnaires were recovered, with 604 valid questionnaires, and the effective recovery rate reached 84.71%. This high response rate ensures the reliability of the collected data for this specific sample, providing a solid foundation for subsequent analysis.
To address the critical importance of a shared cultural background, all participants in this study were citizens of the People’s Republic of China. This ensures a consistent national context for interpreting their psychological responses to native and foreign musical traditions.
Furthermore, to address potential confounding effects and enhance the validity of our conclusions, we collected detailed demographic data from all participants. These variables, particularly those related to socioeconomic status (e.g., education, income) and cultural background (e.g., urban/rural residence, ethnicity), were systematically recorded. The express purpose of collecting this information was to allow for their inclusion as control variables in the subsequent statistical analyses. This methodological step enables a more precise examination of the relationship between music preference and psychological well-being by statistically accounting for the influence of these key demographic factors.
The demographic profile of the 604 valid respondents, used for both descriptive and control purposes, is as follows: The age distribution was 5.13% under 18, 27.82% from 18–25, 30.30% from 26–35, 19.54% from 36–45, 11.09% from 46–55, and 6.13% over 56. In terms of gender, 63.08% were male and 36.92% were female. For education level, 9.27% had a junior high school education or below, 21.19% had a high school education, 19.54% had a college education, 39.74% held a bachelor’s degree, and 10.27% held a master’s degree or higher.
Regarding marital status, 69.37% were unmarried, and 30.63% were married. For residence, 62.42% lived in urban areas, and 37.58% in rural areas. In terms of annual income, 20.20% earned less than 50,000 yuan, 38.41% earned 50,000–100,000 yuan, 28.97% earned 100,000–200,000 yuan, 8.44% earned 200,000–300,000 yuan, and 3.97% earned more than 300,000 yuan. The ethnic distribution showed that 76.16% were Han Chinese, while 23.84% belonged to ethnic minorities. For clarity, the term “ethnic minorities” in the context of China refers to the 55 officially recognized non-Han ethnic groups. Including this demographic reflects the country’s multi-ethnic composition. Finally, in terms of religious affiliation, 27.82% identified with a religious belief, while 72.18% did not.
3.2 Ethics approval
The preliminary survey of this study, titled “Comparing the Psychological Benefits that Chinese and Western Classical Music Preferences Bring to Cognitive Emotion Regulation, Self-Esteem, and Psychological Resilience” involved the distribution of 713 questionnaires. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Sangmyung University. In line with national regulations, the study was deemed exempt from full ethics review as it was non-interventional and observational. The research adhered to the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki, ensuring the protection of participants’ rights and confidentiality.
3.3 Definition of variables
The questionnaire was structured into six dimensions: basic information, Western classical music preference, Chinese classical music preference, emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience.
The first dimension collected participants’ demographic information, including age, sex, education level, marital status, area of residence, annual income, ethnicity, and religion. The second dimension assessed participants’ preferences for Western classical music, including chamber music, symphonic music, and foreign piano works, referencing Larson’s (1995) study (Larson, 1995; Liu et al., 2021). The third dimension examined preferences for Chinese classical music, including Chinese folk songs, Chinese folk music, and Chinese opera, also referencing Liu and Larson’s study. This assessment tool comprises 6 measurement dimensions employing a standardized 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree; Table 1).
The fourth dimension focused on emotion regulation, measuring participants’ strategies for managing emotions, such as self-attribution, acceptance of past events, and situational acceptance, based on Garnefski et al. (2001). The fifth dimension evaluated self-esteem, assessing aspects such as satisfaction with oneself and perception of worthlessness, following Rosenberg’s (1965) self-esteem scale. The sixth dimension investigated psychological resilience, including adaptability to change and intimacy, using the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (Connor and Davidson, 2003). For a detailed description of the variables, refer to Table 2.
4 Empirical results and analysis
4.1 Validity testing of questionnaires
4.1.1 Normal distribution test
A fundamental assumption for statistical analysis of questionnaire data is that the data follow a normal distribution. To assess this, we examined the mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis of the questionnaire items. It is generally accepted that when the absolute value of the skewness coefficient is less than 3 and the absolute value of the kurtosis coefficient is less than 8, the data can be considered normally distributed (Park, 2021). The specific test results are shown in Table 3.
Table 3 shows that the absolute values of the skewness coefficients are all below 1, and the absolute values of the kurtosis coefficients are all less than 2. These results indicate that the data conform to a normal distribution.
4.1.2 Reliability analysis
To assess data reliability, Cronbach’s alpha test was conducted using SPSS 20.0. The results, presented in Table 4, indicate that the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for each scale are greater than 0.8, demonstrating a high level of reliability. This suggests that the collected data are internally consistent and reliable.
4.1.3 Validity analysis
Structural validity was assessed using the KMO test, Bartlett’s spherical test, cumulative contribution rate, and factor loading analysis. The KMO test and Bartlett’s spherical test were conducted to determine whether the questionnaire data were suitable for factor analysis. The cumulative contribution rate represents the extent to which common factors explain variance, while factor loading measures the correlation between the original variables and the extracted common factor.
The analysis results show that the KMO value is 0.993, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity yields an approximate chi-square value of 54243.08 (p < 0.001), confirming that the data are appropriate for factor analysis. As shown in Table 5, five principal components were extracted using an eigenvalue threshold of greater than 1, with a cumulative explained variance of 75.39%. All 73 items aligned with the original factor structure, and no cross-factor loadings were observed. These findings indicate that the scale exhibits relatively strong structural validity.
4.2 Correlation analysis
This study examines the effects of Chinese and Western classical music preferences on individuals’ cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. Table 6 presents the correlation coefficients for these psychological variables.

Table 6. Correlation analysis of Chinese and Western classical music preferences on individual cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience.
The results indicate that Western classical music preferences exhibit greater correlation coefficients for cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience compared to Chinese classical music preferences. Specifically, the correlation coefficients for Western classical music preferences are 0.725 for cognitive emotion regulation (p < 0.001), 0.687 for self-esteem (p < 0.001), and 0.722 for psychological resilience (p < 0.001). In comparison, the correlation coefficients for Chinese classical music preferences are 0.634 (p < 0.001), 0.598 (p < 0.001), and 0.617 (p < 0.001) for the same three variables.
These results suggest that both Western and Chinese classical music preferences have significant positive effects on cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. However, Western classical music preferences demonstrate a stronger effect. This finding is important for understanding the role of music preferences in promoting mental health and provides theoretical support for further exploration of how different music preferences influence mental well-being in different cultural contexts.
4.3 Regression analysis
Correlation analyses identified relationships between Chinese and Western classical music preferences and individuals’ cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience.
To test these hypotheses, separate regression analyses were performed for each psychological variable. Cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience were set as dependent variables, while age, sex, education level, marital status, area of residence, annual income (RMB), ethnicity, religion, and Chinese and Western music preferences were taken as independent variables. Six regression models were constructed, and the results are presented in Table 7.

Table 7. Effects of Chinese and Western classical music preferences on cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience.
The regression analysis of cognitive emotion regulation, centered around Models 1 and 2, disclosed adjusted R2 values of 0.537 and 0.375, along with F-statistics of 78.63 and 41.141, respectively. Both of these attained statistical significance at the p < 0.001 threshold. This reveals that the predictor variables elucidated 53.7 and 37.5% of the variance in cognitive emotion regulation. In Model 1, the regression coefficient for Western classical music preferences stood at 0.609 (t = 25.846, p < 0.001). In Model 2, the regression coefficient for Chinese classical music preferences was 0.559 (t = 20.577, p < 0.001). The Within-group coefficient difference p-value of 0.0872* insinuated, to a certain degree, a marginally significant difference among the within-group coefficients, thus verifying Hypothesis H1a.
The regression analysis of self-esteem, focusing on Models 3 and 4, presented adjusted R2 values of 0.487 and 0.375, with F-statistics of 64.484 and 41.141, respectively. Both were significant at the p < 0.001 level. This signified that the predictor variables accounted for 48.7 and 37.5% of the variance in self-esteem. In Model 3, the regression coefficient for Western classical music preferences was 0.504 (t = 23.318, p < 0.001). In Model 4, the regression coefficient for Chinese classical music preferences was 0.455 (t = 18.445, p < 0.001). Moreover, the Within-group coefficient difference p-value of 0.0408** clearly demonstrated that there was a significant difference among the within-group coefficients, thereby confirming Hypothesis H2b.
The regression analysis of psychological resilience, centered on Models 5 and 6, revealed adjusted R2 values of 0.533 and 0.401, accompanied by F-statistics of 77.464 and 45.891, respectively. Both achieved statistical significance at the p < 0.001 mark. This fully manifested that the predictor variables could account for 53.3 and 40.1% of the variance in psychological resilience, respectively. In Model 5, the regression coefficient for Western classical music preferences was 0.66 (t = 25.734, p < 0.001). In Model 6, the regression coefficient for Chinese classical music preferences was 0.591 (t = 19.641, p < 0.001). Notably, the Within-group coefficient difference p-value of 0.0243** evidently indicated that there was a significant difference among the within-group coefficients, thereby providing robust support for Hypothesis H2c.
5 Discussion
From a cross-cultural comparative perspective, this study systematically investigated the differential impacts of preferences for Chinese and Western classical music on cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. Through the analysis of 604 valid samples, the study reveals the correlation between music preferences and mental health indicators, providing a theoretical basis for the practice of music therapy.
The results show that preferences for both Chinese and Western classical music are significantly and positively correlated with cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience, thus verifying the core hypotheses H1a-H1c. With its unique artistic charm, classical music can exert a positive regulatory effect on an individual’s psychological state through various physiological, cognitive, and social mechanisms. These findings are consistent with previous research, further consolidating the understanding of music’s broad impact on mental health (Osborne et al., 2014; Pauwels et al., 2014; Peistaraite and Clark, 2020).
In further comparisons, we found that the preference for Western classical music had stronger effects than the preference for Chinese classical music in enhancing cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience, which also confirmed hypotheses H2a, H2b, and H2c. This advantage of Western classical music may stem from its complex harmonic structure and rich emotional expression. Western music emphasizes harmonic complexity and polyphonic textures, utilizing a “tension-resolution” model in sonata form to evoke emotional catharsis through vertical chord progressions and to activate reward-related brain regions such as the ventral striatum. This may allow it to more effectively regulate emotions and enhance self-esteem and psychological resilience. In contrast, Chinese classical music is primarily based on monophonic melodies built on the pentatonic scale, emphasizing horizontal melodic contours and subtle changes in timbre. For instance, the breathy overtones and strategic pauses of the guqin are used to cultivate a meditative and introspective state, which aligns with the Daoist and Confucian concepts of natural harmony and emotional restraint. These cultural and musical structural differences render the psychological regulation of Chinese classical music more subtle and implicit.
However, the interpretation of this result must be approached with caution and framed within the “cultural positionality” of the participants. It must be emphasized that this finding arises from a specific cultural context—Chinese participants comparing their own cultural music with a foreign one—and should by no means be misinterpreted as a judgment that Chinese music is of lesser universal value than Western music. A more persuasive explanation is that cultural familiarity plays a key confounding role. In the context of globalization, the internet, and modern education systems, Chinese listeners’ exposure to and familiarity with Western classical theory and repertoire may have surpassed their in-depth knowledge of their own traditional music. This familiarity bias, rather than the inherent qualities of the music itself, is highly likely to be the primary reason for the observed difference in effects. Therefore, the findings of this study cannot be generalized to describe the functional role of Chinese music for all cultural groups.
6 Conclusion
This study explored the comprehensive effects of Chinese and Western classical music on adults’ cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience, and examined the moderating role of individual factors. The findings indicate that classical music significantly enhances all measured psychological indicators, with the most pronounced impact on resilience. Specifically, compared to Chinese classical music, Western classical music demonstrated stronger effects on cognitive emotion regulation and self-esteem. Among the moderating factors, age and education level had a significant influence, whereas gender did not.
Based on these findings, this study holds important practical implications and policy value. On an applied level, it is recommended to integrate the structural properties of music with specific clinical needs. For example, future research could explore integrating the “tension-resolution” model of Western classical music into trauma-focused therapies to enhance emotional catharsis and resilience. At the public health policy level, efforts should move beyond general funding increases toward more targeted interventions, such as providing differentiated musical resources and guidance based on the cultural backgrounds and needs of different socioeconomic groups.
Although this study provides valuable insights, its limitations must be acknowledged. First, the study primarily relied on questionnaires and failed to directly measure and control for key variables such as cultural familiarity, personal music preferences, and listening frequency. Second, the cross-sectional design cannot establish causal relationships. Furthermore, the study’s interdisciplinary contextualization requires strengthening. This research only conducts comparisons within the domain of music and fails to make horizontal comparisons between the psychological effects of music and other types of cognitive or emotional stimuli (e.g., visual arts, natural landscapes), which limits our assessment of music’s uniqueness and relative effectiveness as an intervention tool. Therefore, future research should adopt longitudinal designs and expand the cultural diversity of samples to more deeply analyze the underlying mechanisms of music’s psychological benefits.
In summary, this study confirms the benefits of exposure to classical music for mental health and reveals the differential effects of different musical systems within a specific cultural context. The research underscores that the future direction should be a deeper integration of music education, clinical psychotherapy, and cultural studies. This interdisciplinary collaboration is essential for leveraging music more precisely and effectively to enhance individual and societal resilience and well-being.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The requirement of ethical approval was waived by Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Sangmyung University for the studies involving humans because In line with national regulations, the study was deemed exempt from full ethics review as it was non-interventional and observational. The research adhered to the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki, ensuring the protection of participants’ rights and confidentiality. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The ethics committee/institutional review board also waived the requirement of written informed consent for participation from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin because Written informed consent was waived, as this online survey entailed no more than minimal risk to participants and was unlikely to adversely impact their rights and welfare.
Author contributions
XS: Writing – original draft. YW: Writing – original draft. LL: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Broughton, M. C., Dimmick, J., and Dean, R. T. (2021). Affective and cognitive responses to musical performances of early 20th century classical solo piano compositions. Music. Percept. 38, 245–266. doi: 10.1525/mp.2021.38.3.245
Chen, L. (2023). Influence of music on the hearing and mental health of adolescents and countermeasures. Front. Neurosci. 17. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1236638
Connor, KM, and Davidson, JR (2003). Connor–Davidson resilience scale. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Connor-Davidson resilience scale.
Culp, M. E. (2016). Improving self-esteem in general music. Gen. Music Today 29, 19–24. doi: 10.1177/1048371315619962
Drobesch-Binter, B. (2000). “Gesundheitsförderung und Suchtvorbeugung — eine Positionierung” in Suchtvorbeugung in Österreich. K. Fellöcker, S. Franke (eds.). (Vienna: Springer Vienna).
Droe, K. (2006). Music preference and music education: a review of literature. Update Appl. Res. Music Educ. 24, 23–32. doi: 10.1177/87551233060240020103
Fung, A. (2018). Motivation and resilience: a mother-daughter dyad’s pursuit of tertiary music education, a longitudinal study. Int. J. Adult Commun. Prof. Learn. 24, 27–44. doi: 10.18848/2328-6318/CGP/v24i03/27-44
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., and Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personal. Individ. Differ. 30, 1311–1327. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6
Harvey, J., and Boynton, K. (2021). Self-disclosure and psychological resilience: the mediating roles of self-esteem and self-compassion. Interpersona 15, 90–104. doi: 10.5964/ijpr.4533
Hu, X., Chen, J., and Wang, Y. (2021). University students’ use of music for learning and well-being: a qualitative study and design implications. Inf. Process. Manag. 58:102409. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2020.102409
Juslin, P. N., and Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 559–575. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293
Kegelaers, J., Schuijer, M., and Oudejans, R. R. (2021). Resilience and mental health issues in classical musicians: a preliminary study. Psychol. Music 49, 1273–1284. doi: 10.1177/0305735620927789
Koehler, F., Schäfer, S. K., Lieb, K., and Wessa, M. (2023). Differential associations of leisure music engagement with resilience: a network analysis. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 23:100377. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2023.100377
Koelsch, S. (2010). Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.002
Kuzman, M. R., Curkovic, M., and Wasserman, D. (2020). Principles of mental health care during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. Psychiatry 63:e45. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.54
Larson, R. (1995). Secrets in the bedroom: adolescents' private use of media. J. Youth Adolesc. 24, 535–550. doi: 10.1007/BF01537055
Li, Y., Yang, Z., Zheng, H., and Zhang, X. (2024). A hybrid LSTM-SNN approach for robust multimodal zero-shot learning. International Journal of Complexity in Applied Science and Technology 1:143881. doi: 10.1504/IJCAST.2024.143881
Lin, J., Sun, Y., and Gao, Y. (2025). A solution to portfolio optimisation based on random forest and long short-term memory networks. International Journal of Complexity in Applied Science and Technology 1, 211–232. doi: 10.1504/IJCAST.2025.145845
Liu, X., Liu, Y., Shi, H., and Zheng, M. (2021). Effects of mindfulness meditation on musical aesthetic emotion processing. Front. Psychol. 12:648062. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648062
López-íñiguez, G., and Mcpherson, G. E. (2021). Regulation of emotions to optimize classical music performance: a quasi-experimental study of a cellist-researcher. Front. Psychol. 12:627601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627601
Loveday, C., Musgrave, G., and Gross, S. (2023). Predicting anxiety, depression, and wellbeing in professional and nonprofessional musicians. Psychol. Music 51, 508–522. doi: 10.1177/03057356221096506
Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
Min, Z., Jie, Z., Xiao, X., Mengyuan, Q., Youhai, L., Hui, Z., et al. (2020). How destination music affects tourists’ behaviors: travel with music in Lijiang, China. Asia Pac. J. Tour. Res. 25, 131–144. doi: 10.1080/10941665.2019.1683046
Olsen, K. N., Terry, J., and Thompson, W. F. (2023). Psychosocial risks and benefits of exposure to heavy metal music with aggressive themes: current theory and evidence. Curr. Psychol. 42, 21133–21150. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03108-9
Oncu, E. (2025). Integrating CNNs and ANNs: a comprehensive AI framework for enhanced breast cancer detection and diagnosis. International Journal of Complexity in Applied Science and Technology 1, 281–299.
Osborne, M. S., Greene, D. J., and Immel, D. T. (2014). Managing performance anxiety and improving mental skills in conservatoire students through performance psychology training: a pilot study. Psychol. Well-Being 4:18. doi: 10.1186/s13612-014-0018-3
Oza, B., and Behnaz, A. (2024). Using knowledge graphs for enabling collaborative financial market data analytical processes. Int. J. Complexity Appl. Sci. Technol. 1:143884. doi: 10.1504/IJCAST.2024.143884
Park, E. (2021). Validity and reliability of the caregiving difficulty scale in mothers of children with cerebral palsy. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 18:5689. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18115689
Pauwels, E. K. J., Volterrani, D., Mariani, G., and Kostkiewics, M. (2014). Mozart, music and medicine. Med. Princ. Pract. 23, 403–412. doi: 10.1159/000364873
Peistaraite, U., and Clark, T. (2020). Emotion regulation processes can benefit self-regulated learning in classical musicians. Front. Psychol. 11:568760. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568760
Rentfrow, P. J., and Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi's of everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of music preferences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1236–1256. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236
Rickard, N. S. (2004). Intense emotional responses to music: a test of the physiological arousal hypothesis. Psychol. Music 32, 371–388. doi: 10.1177/0305735604046096
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 326.
Saarikallio, S. (2011). Music as emotional self-regulation throughout adulthood. Psychol. Music 39, 307–327. doi: 10.1177/0305735610374894
Surzykiewicz, J., Skalski, S. B., Sołbut, A., Rutkowski, S., and Konaszewski, K. (2022). Resilience and regulation of emotions in adolescents: serial mediation analysis through self-esteem and the perceived social support. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 19:8007. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19138007
Tabibnia, G., and Radecki, D. (2018). Resilience training that can change the brain. Consult. Psychol. J. Pract. Res. 70, 59–88. doi: 10.1037/cpb0000110
Thuc, N. A. (2018). Origin and cultural meaning of classical Chinese music and dance. VNU J. Foreign Stud. 34:4329. doi: 10.25073/2525-2445/vnufs.4329
Togay, B. O., and Firat, C. (2024). Comprehensive faults analysis on the direct current side of photovoltaic systems using logistic model tree algorithm. Int. J. Complexity Appl. Sci. Technol. 1:143882. doi: 10.1504/IJCAST.2024.143882
Wang, X., Wei, Y., Heng, L., and Mcadams, S. (2021). A cross-cultural analysis of the influence of timbre on affect perception in Western classical music and Chinese music traditions. Front. Psychol. 12:732865. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.732865
Keywords: classical music, cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, psychological resilience, music preference
Citation: Shan X, Wang Y and Luo L (2025) Comparing the psychological benefits that Chinese and Western classical music preferences bring to cognitive emotion regulation, self-esteem, and psychological resilience. Front. Psychol. 16:1623200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1623200
Edited by:
Lei Wang, Tongji University, ChinaReviewed by:
Yuki Morijiri, Tokyo Gakugei University, JapanMaoqing Zhang, Henan University of Technology, China
Copyright © 2025 Shan, Wang and Luo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Long Luo, bHVvbG9uZ0Bjd251LmVkdS5jbg==