- 1Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
- 2Institute of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
Introduction: Positive psychological interventions (PPIs) can help mitigate psychological challenges and facilitate the transition to higher education.
Methods: This study presents the results of a quasi-experimental trial of a 6-week universal, multicomponent PPI designed specifically for university students. It compares survey responses of intervention group participants with those of passive control group participants before, immediately after, and 6 months after the intervention.
Results: The results show that the PPI led to significant improvements in mindfulness, positive reframing, and self-compassion at the trait level, and in self-compassion, positive affect, relaxation, subjective physical health, and subjective sleep quality at the state level.
Discussion: The study demonstrates that PPIs can effectively build psychological resources during educational transitions, particularly those related to emotion regulation. However, other resources and outcomes (e.g., character strengths) were less affected. Thus, more tailored approaches and selective interventions are recommended to address these areas comprehensively and ensure a more holistic improvement in student wellbeing.
1 Introduction
Transitioning from secondary to post-secondary education is an exciting yet challenging time for early adults, characterized by various changes (Ketchen Lipson et al., 2015). While such upheavals can unlock potential for positive psychosocial development, such as forming close relationships, they simultaneously increase stress levels and, as a result, vulnerability to developing mental illness (Erikson, 1959; Kroshus et al., 2021). Additional external stressors like the coronavirus pandemic have affected university students. Studies suggest that stress, depression, loneliness, and anxiety have increased since the start of the coronavirus pandemic (Aristovnik et al., 2020; Kohls et al., 2023; Tsiouris et al., 2023). The rise in mental impairment coincides with increased workload, isolation, and financial burdens (Becker and Lörz, 2020; Kreidl and Dittler, 2021; Lörz et al., 2021). Even post-pandemic, students’ mental health remains impaired compared to pre-pandemic levels, with a reduction in social support, resilience, and self-efficacy (Kohls et al., 2023). Hence, there is an urgent need for interventions addressing students’ mental health.
To this end, positive psychology offers a promising approach by emphasizing the development of positive resources and skills, focusing on enhancing wellbeing rather than just treating pathology (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Research indicates that PPIs implemented in university settings promote mental health of first-year students and facilitate the transition to university (Dvořáková et al., 2017; Hirshberg et al., 2022). Participating in a PPI in a university context can lead to a reduction in perceived stress, depression, negative affect, and anxiety and an increase in wellbeing, life satisfaction, mindfulness, self-compassion, optimism and self-efficacy (Dvořáková et al., 2017; Hirshberg et al., 2018; Hirshberg et al., 2022; Hobbs et al., 2022; Lorenz et al., 2022; Smeets et al., 2014; Totzeck et al., 2020). Various types of PPIs exist, some focusing only on single components, such as gratitude (Emmons and McCullough, 2003), and others incorporating multiple components of positive psychology (Chilver and Gatt, 2022). A meta-analysis by Hendriks et al. (2020) reported a significant impact of multicomponent PPIs on increasing subjective wellbeing (Hedges’ g = 0.34), psychological wellbeing (Hedges’ g = 0.39), and reducing depression (Hedges’ g = 0.29), as well as anxiety and stress (Hedges’ g = 0.35).
Psychological constructs can be conceptualized at both the trait level, representing relatively stable, enduring characteristics, and the state level, reflecting transient psychological states that fluctuate across situations (Schmitt and Blum, 2020). Traditionally, research on positive psychological interventions (PPIs) has focused primarily on trait-level changes, assuming that wellbeing-related constructs develop gradually over time (Hendriks et al., 2020). However, constructs such as mindfulness, self-compassion, and affect can also be examined at the state level, providing insights into short-term, dynamic changes that occur in response to interventions (Moskowitz et al., 1997; Blanke and Brose, 2017). According to Whole Trait Theory, traits emerge from accumulated state experiences, meaning that repeated state-level improvements may contribute to long-term trait development (Schmitt and Blum, 2020). Traits, thus, represent a density distribution of states which may very within persons and across situations. Whole Trait Theory provides a framework to examine interpersonal (e.g., gender) and intrapersonal differences in states as well as social-cognitive predictors of variety (e.g., attitudes, knowledge). For intervention research, this is particularly important as it allows us to study intervention effects on inter- and intra-personal levels. Yet, not all constructs are equally responsive to interventions at both levels—some (e.g., mindfulness and self-compassion) may show immediate state improvements, whereas others (e.g., character strengths) may require extended practice for trait-level change (Kiken et al., 2015; Neff and Germer, 2013). By differentiating between state and trait outcomes, this study aims to clarify how PPIs influence psychological resources over different timescales and deepen our understanding of their mechanisms of action.
Despite ample evidence of PPI effectiveness in university students, several questions remain unanswered. First, multicomponent PPIs are underutilized compared to single-component interventions (Schotanus-Dijkstra et al., 2015), despite evidence suggesting their superior effectiveness (Schueller and Parks, 2012). For instance, a meta-analysis by Carr et al. (2021) showed that multicomponent PPIs were more effective than single-component interventions in reducing depression and stress as well as in increasing wellbeing. Second, most PPIs do not differentiate between trait and state levels in their intervention or analysis but examine trait levels alone. Consequently, they view the trait level as general, stable, situation-independent characteristics (Schmitt and Blum, 2020). However, constructs can also be viewed at the state level as patterns of thinking, feeling and behavior of a person in a particular situation that can fluctuate over time (Schmitt and Blum, 2020). It is more plausible that all constructs comprise both state and trait components, making a strict separation between the two inherently difficult (Moskowitz et al., 1997). Consequently, immediately after the intervention, there may be a more pronounced increase at state level compared to trait level of the constructs. In that sense, long-term follow-ups can provide further insight into the development of state- and trait-level variables. Lastly, multicomponent PPIs are usually delivered by mental health professionals or apps. Yet the potential of peer-to-peer approaches, which may bring benefits to effectiveness and cost-efficiency remains untapped (Neuhaus et al., 2022).
To address these questions, this study examines the effects of a 6-week PPI on German university students focusing on both state- and trait-level changes in positive psychological outcomes. The intervention is based on the Positive Psychotherapy approach by Rashid and Seligman (2021) and was co-created with university students via focus group work (n = 8) conducted over 6 months, followed by a pilot trial across two semesters (n = 40). The Positive Psychotherapy approach was adapted to the most common mental health problems among students (i.e., symptoms of depression and anxiety) and the number of sessions was reduced to six, since this was found to be feasible in the pilot study. The PPI addresses six primary positive psychological outcomes, namely character strengths, mindfulness, positive reframing, self-compassion, humor, and gratitude. These were identified as efficacious in previous research and meaningful by the focus group and in the pilot study (Carr et al., 2021). Moreover, in designing the course, all constructs were mapped on the PERMA model (Seligman, 2011) and connected to didactic approaches to ensure that positive emotions (e.g., gratitude), engagement (e.g., use of character and signature strengths), relationships (e.g., humor as a tool for social connectedness), meaning (e.g., using mindfulness and self-compassion to identify values), and accomplishment (e.g., using positive reframing to increase self-efficacy) were addressed. As secondary outcomes, we measure various constructs such as stress, wellbeing, depression, and others, based on previous research (Carr et al., 2021; Hendriks et al., 2020). We predict that participants in the intervention group show significant improvements in primary and secondary outcomes from pretest to posttest as well as follow-up (6 months after the intervention). Moreover, we explore the impact of the intervention at the state-level by examining daily diaries of outcomes before and after the intervention.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Procedure
We recruited participants through flyers, social media (e.g., Instagram), and university channels (university’s sports department and the student health management website).
We recruited the passive control group using the university’s internal pool of test subjects. There was no randomized allocation of participants, because the program was part of the universities’ health initiative. Participants primarily enrolled with the motivation to receive an intervention and might have dropped out if they had been assigned to the control group. Additionally, we advertised among private friends and acquaintances and published university board notices. Study-registration was implemented via the university’s sports department and the student health management website. We contacted the participants via e-mail, which informed them about the procedure and conditions of participation. Following this, they provided their informed consent. The subsequent study procedure is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Study procedure. Intervention group and control group complete trait- and state-assessments before, after and 6 months after the intervention. The number of participants decreases from 215 to 135 after the intervention and down to 94 at follow-up.
One week prior to the beginning of the intervention, the participants completed a trait assessment via SoSci Survey1 and engaged in a 4-day state assessment (i.e., daily diaries). The trait assessment consisted of a 30-min questionnaire that covered different trait constructs. The state assessment consisted of a daily 5-min evening survey conducted over 4 consecutive days via SoSci Survey. After the pretest phase, the intervention group participants took part in the 6-week positive psychological intervention. The intervention consisted of six modules, with one 90-min module per week. The passive control group did not receive any intervention. After the intervention phase, participants completed the trait and state assessments again. Six months after the intervention, we conducted the follow-up assessment, during which the participants once again completed the state and trait surveys.
Data collection started in October 2022 (t0), followed by the 6-week intervention and a posttest in December 2022 (t1). Follow-up data was collected in summer 2023 (t2). To achieve the estimated sample size, we implemented a similar assessment schedule with another wave of participants for the summer semester 2023 and used data of both waves for the analysis.
2.2 Participants
A total of 215 students from two German universities enrolled in the study (intervention group n = 131, passive control group n = 84). Inclusion criteria were speaking and understanding German, and being enrolled in university. People were excluded if they were currently receiving psychotherapy and their therapist advised against participating. Demographic data was available for 212 participants. The participants were 18–42 years old, with an average age of 22.93 years (SD = 4.48). The participants were 79.1% female (n = 170), 16.3% male (n = 35), 2.3% diverse (n = 5) and 0.9% did not provide any information about their sex/gender (n = 2; three missing values for age). Most students were in their first or third semester (n = 132, 61.4%), 8.8% (n = 19) were in their second semester, 28.37% (n = 61) were in their fourth semester and higher. Most students were studying for a Bachelor’s degree (Bachelor of Science: n = 152, 70.7%; Bachelor of Arts: n = 16, 7.4%), 7.4% were studying for a state examination (n = 16), 6% were studying for a Master’s degree (Master of Science: n = 11, 5.1%; Master of Arts: n = 2, 0.9%), 0.9% were studying for a diploma (n = 2) and 6% were studying for another degree (n = 13).
2.3 Intervention
The intervention consisted of 6 weeks of training in various positive psychological skills. The program was delivered as peer education by students from two German universities supported by two faculty members. The intervention followed a peer-to-peer approach, with students not only co-developing but also delivering the program to participants (Byl et al., 2016; Schneider et al., 2024). The student facilitators were paired with another person from the partner university to co-develop and standardize the delivery method of the workshop. All facilitators participated in a 2-day training period where each workshop was practiced and a standard was agreed upon for each. Since the program was co-designed with participants, it considered the university context, organizational aspects, and culture of the target group from the beginning (Topping, 2022). The training aimed to support students in coping with the demands and challenges of studying in a healthy way in the initial phase of their studies. A total of six modules of 90 min each were offered, with each module forming a separate thematic block. The following topics were covered: character and signature strengths, gratitude, mindfulness, positive reframing, self-compassion and humor. Each module included a theoretical introduction, practical exercises, and tips for everyday transfer (a detailed description of each module is presented in the OpenScienceFramework project). After the sessions, the participants received worksheets and brief tasks for home practice.
2.4 Measures
The survey consisted of several validated scales and some original items to measure sociodemographic covariates, positive psychological skills as states and/or traits, and health-related outcomes. Primary outcomes were the constructs addressed in the intervention, while secondary outcomes includes other (mental) health related and positive psychological constructs. State measures were used for constructs where daily fluctuation may occur. An overview of measures is presented in Table 1.
2.5 Data analysis
2.5.1 Power analysis
We conducted an a priori power analysis using G*Power to determine the required sample size for the analysis of variance (Faul et al., 2007). With an α = 0.05, a power of β = 0.80, to detect a mean effect of g = 0.34 (based on a meta-analysis on the effectiveness of multicomponent PPI, here for wellbeing as an outcome; Hendriks et al., 2020) and an assumed correlation of the measurement times of r = 0.5, a minimum of 58 subjects is required to effectively determine the interaction of group and time in a repeated measures ANOVA.
2.5.2 Missing data
The analysis revealed a high proportion of missing values at the post-training time points, mainly due to participant dropout. To counteract potential systematic bias in the study sample, we analyzed according to the intention to treat principle (Twisk et al., 2020). We addressed the missing values using multiple imputations (cf. Twisk et al., 2020). The predictors used were previous measurements of the variable to be imputed (e.g., mindfulness at t1 was predicted by mindfulness at t0). We ran 20 imputations. For trait level constructs, estimating missing values was possible for most participants. However, for trait level constructs, it was often only possible to estimate values for parts of the sample, resulting in fewer analyzed cases.
2.5.3 Statistical analysis
Mixed ANOVA were used to analyze trait measures. The factors were experimental group (intervention group vs. control group) and time (t0, t1, t2). Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were used to assess within- and between-group differences.
To analyze state measures, multilevel models were used. Fixed effects were calculated for group, time, and the group by time interaction. Additionally, a random slope was calculated for time. The group variable was coded as 0 = intervention group and 1 = control group. For each time point, the four daily state assessments were combined, resulting in three levels for the time variable (pre, post, and follow-up, respectively). While combining the assessments in this way obscures day to day variance, it is vital to assure that potential pre- to post-intervention changes become noticeable and are not suppressed. Because non-linear effects over time were expected, three models were calculated for each outcome variable, one per comparison of measurement points (i.e., t0–t1, t0–t2, t1–t2). A mediation analysis of trait level changes of primary on secondary outcomes was run, and results are reported in the OpenScienceFramework. We used the software IBM SPSS Statistics 25 (IBM SPSS Statistics (RRID: SCR_016479)).
3 Results
At baseline, the intervention group showed higher scores in problem-focused coping, avoidance-based coping, and wellness behavior. The intervention group also had more participants in higher semesters (see Table 2).
3.1 Trait measures
Tables 3, 4 show the results for the analysis of trait measures (primary and secondary outcomes, respectively). The time × group interaction is significant for the constructs mindfulness, positive reframing, and self-compassion (ps<0.05). Therefore, the groups’ scores on these constructs changed differently over time. Pairwise comparisons revealed significant improvements within the intervention group for mindfulness, positive reframing, and self-compassion (all ps<0.05). Further, other than self-compassion, there are no variables showing a significant difference favoring the intervention group over the control group post-intervention.
3.2 State measures
From t0 to t1, significant improvements within the intervention group were observed for self-compassion, positive affect, relaxation, subjective physical health, and subjective sleep quality compared to passive control participants (see Table 5). However, significant increases in negative affect were also observed in the intervention group. At t2, the improvements in self-compassion and positive affect remain in the intervention group (see OpenScienceFramework for comprehensive results).
3.3 Mediation
No mediating effects of the module constructs (character strengths, mindfulness, positive reframing, self-compassion, and humor) were found for any of the health-related outcomes of interest (wellbeing, depression, stress, positive affect, negative affect, and satisfaction with life). The comprehensive results can be found in the OpenScienceFramework repository.
3.4 Intervention compliance
Descriptive statistics show that on average 42% of participants used the learned exercise(s) at home. Only an average 14% of those participants reported using the exercises every day or 5–6 days of the week following the workshop, whereas an average of 82% used the exercises one to four times during that week. On average, 48% of those participants rated the exercises as rather or very useful, 45% rated them a little or somewhat useful, and 0.6% rated them as not useful at all. Per workshop, an average of 83% of intervention group participants who completed the compliance questionnaires attended the workshop.
Grouping the intervention group into a group that used any of the exercises at home vs. participants who used none of the exercises at home (74 vs. 57 participants, respectively), shows a significant difference after the intervention for positive reframing (t(215) = 2.85, p = 0.005) and negative affect (t(213) = −3.005, p = 0.003). However, no significant differences were observed for any other analyzed outcomes (all ps > 0.05).
Following up on the difference between those groups pertaining to positive reframing, we have re-run the repeated-measures analysis across all three measurement points, grouping the intervention group by exercise use, we find that there is a significant between-group effect (F(1) = 5.839, p = 0.021) (original data, all imputation sets also show p < 0.05). There is also a significant main effect of time in all imputed data sets, but not in the original data (F(2) = 1.003, p = 0.372, all other ps < 0.05). There is no significant time by exercise use interaction term (all ps > 0.05). These results suggest that there is a time-independent difference in positive reframing between the people who did and the people who did not use any exercises at home.
Find the full results for the compliance analysis in the OpenScienceFramework repository.
4 Discussion
This quasi-experimental study aimed to evaluate a 6-week multicomponent PPI within a university student sample via trait and state level assessments before and after the intervention, as well as at 6-month follow-up. The intervention group showed significant post-intervention improvements in trait self-compassion, mindfulness, and positive reframing, whereas the control group did not. While self-compassion and mindfulness improvements persisted at the 6-month follow-up, the improvements in positive reframing were not maintained. These findings align with previous research demonstrating that interventions targeting specific domains such as mindfulness, self-compassion, or positive reframing effectively enhance those particular constructs among university students (Dryden et al., 2023; Haukaas et al., 2018; de Sousa et al., 2021). At the state level, we found improvements in self-compassion, positive and negative affect, relaxation, subjective physical health, and sleep quality, immediately post-training.
Evidence shows that even brief interventions, such as 90 min of mindfulness training, can effectively increase positive psychological constructs like mindfulness among students, suggesting a singular effect of individual modules (de Sousa et al., 2021). Since we evaluated the impact of the 6-week intervention overall without weekly assessments of effectiveness, it is not clear which modules were driving the observed changes. However, existing evidence suggests that mindfulness, self-compassion, and positive reframing are interrelated, indicating potential synergistic effects within our intervention (Neff and Germer, 2013; Pogrebtsova et al., 2018; Shapiro et al., 2007). Furthermore, our PPI enhanced different states, including self-compassion, positive/negative affect, relaxation, physical health, and sleep quality, immediately post-training. Notably, changes in self-compassion and positive affect were sustained over time. These findings align with previous research showing state enhancement after PPIs (Breines and Chen, 2013; Congard et al., 2020). However, most studies examining state changes in PPIs have primarily focused on single-component interventions, particularly mindfulness interventions (Breines and Chen, 2013; Kiken et al., 2015; Mahmood et al., 2016). Since negative affect also increased initially, together with a sustained increase in self-compassion, this could be a backdraft effect of PPIs (e.g., in self-compassion), where a confrontation with negative emotions or experiences induces negative affect, but self-compassion can help in coping with it (Ewert et al., 2021). While this effect was not hypothesized or conceptually embedded in our study, it is a plausible explanation based on recent literature (Hoffmann et al., 2024). Thus, our study demonstrates promising results, highlighting the necessity for further research to explore state changes following multicomponent PPIs.
Contrary to our expectations, no significant changes occurred in other psychological outcomes on trait- or state-levels (such as character strengths and coping humor). Similarly, most health-related outcomes, including depression and wellbeing, did not improve following the intervention, in contrast to previous research findings (Chilver and Gatt, 2022; Hendriks et al., 2020; Hobbs et al., 2022). Interestingly, our results suggest that state–trait relationships may vary across psychological constructs. For example, both state- and trait changes occurred in self-compassion, whereas for mindfulness, only the trait level changed. According to Whole Trait Theory (Schmitt and Blum, 2020), the development of traits is thought to result from accumulated state-level experiences. However, our results imply that trait shifts can occur even in the absence of observable state-level changes—or that our state-level measurement was not sensitive enough to detect micro-level fluctuations. These results underscore the importance of assessing both levels distinctly, as well as the need for finer-grained, temporally sensitive measures to better understand how daily experiences translate into enduring personal change. Several factors could explain the absence of state-level effects. The intervention’s duration may have been insufficient, as interventions lasting 9–12 weeks have shown particular effectiveness in increasing psychological wellbeing (Hendriks et al., 2020). Furthermore, changes in module outcomes could be temporary and not fully captured by our pre- and post-training assessments. The daily diary schedule was a low-threshold implementation of daily data, but a more frequent assessment (e.g., multiple times per day) might illustrate more fluctuation in daily experiences and, thus, micro-level effects of the intervention (Wrzus and Neubauer, 2023). Therefore, it remains unclear whether the modules (e.g., targeting humor and character strengths) were ineffective or whether the measurement was inadequate to detect changes. We recommend more fine-grained assessment (e.g., multiple ambulatory assessments, weekly evaluation following each module). Lastly, not being able to estimate data for all missing values may have led to reduced statistical power, making our tests not sensitive enough to detect changes.
Moreover, participants may not have fully integrated the exercises into their daily lives, which may have weakened the outcomes, especially in the long term. For example, consistent effort and sustained practice of PPI exercises are crucial for reducing depressive symptoms (Seligman et al., 2005). Although each module contained recommendations, exercises, and motivational incentives for daily practice, participants used them to varying degrees. Future PPIs should encourage participants to engage more actively in the exercises at home and evaluate the implementation more thoroughly.
Finally, we must acknowledge the context of the implementation. Data collection started during the COVID-19 pandemic, with depressive symptoms, anxiety, suicidal ideation, feelings of loneliness, and stress being particularly high among university students (Aristovnik et al., 2020; Kohls et al., 2023; Tsiouris et al., 2023). On the one hand, this might increase their willingness to participate in the intervention to address these issues. On the other hand, since the PPI was not a therapeutic intervention, students might have had different expectations or sought more individualized, professional support that go beyond universal, group-based PPIs.
4.1 Strengths, limitations, and outlook
The study examined the impact of a 6-week, multicomponent PPI on state and trait levels of positive psychological skills and relevant health-related outcomes in German university students. It provides evidence for the feasibility of the intervention and its impact on self-compassion, positive reframing, and mindfulness over time. The peer education approach also allows students of relevant fields (e.g., psychology) to gather work experience and broaden their skill set. Nevertheless, the study has some limitations. Most notably, the study lacks randomization due to it being implemented as a university mental health initiative. Therefore, intervention participants were expecting to enroll in a program that supports their mental health. This expectation may already create a beneficial effect for them, and lead to higher motivation (Constantino et al., 2011). Furthermore, the intervention group is larger than the control group and differs on several baseline measures (e.g., problem-focused coping, or wellness-behavior), potentially introducing bias into their relationships on other variables. Future studies should employ randomized allocation, ideally with individualized module assessments (e.g., dismantling studies) to disentangle specific effects and with different types of controls (e.g., active controls, online vs. face-to-face groups). This way, other factors like group support, positive expectations, or trainer commitment can also be examined (Goleman and Davidson, 2017). It further allows for an analysis of the active components that make up the intervention effect, informing future interventions which modules are more effective than others (although one must beware of survivorship bias and not falsely reinforce modules that already work while neglecting the modules that need reinforcement). Secondly, the sample was homogeneous, predominantly young, highly educated, White, and female, thus limiting the findings’ generalizability (Oyserman et al., 2002). Future studies with more diverse samples, including participants from different cultural backgrounds and older individuals, are necessary for broader applicability. Furthermore, comparing clinical (e.g., students with depressive symptoms), subclinical (e.g., students with high levels of neuroticism), and non-clinical student samples would offer valuable insights into potential differential effects, revealing practical implications for each group. Thirdly, to reduce participants’ burden in our ambulatory assessment of daily state-like variables, we reduced the number of items per construct. For example, our sleep quality measurement consisted of only one item. While this approach is face valid, longer measures would be preferable in future research to picture a broader measure of several constructs. Additionally, the internal consistency of some measures was below adequate levels (e.g., Locus of Control, Brief-COPE). Fourthly, the absence of randomization may introduce self-selection bias, indicating that participants in the intervention group may have been more motivated and optimistic. Another limitation concerns missing values and participant dropout. While multiple imputations are the current preferred method for handling missing data (Allison, 2010), they are estimations that may significantly differ from actual values (Baltes-Götz, 2013). Furthermore, estimating missing state values has its limitations; for most variables only a few missing values could be imputed and for others none (e.g., sleep quality, subjective physical health). Especially for state-level measures it was often not possible to estimate values for all participants, reducing the statistical power for state-level analyses Therefore, results for, e.g., relaxation, subjective physical health, and subjective sleep quality may be biased by dropout. To address the challenge posed by the high dropout rate, future research should consider employing other strategies aimed at reducing participant-dropout (Scharfstein et al., 2012), most importantly by offering higher incentives for fully completing all questionnaires.
Given that the study exclusively utilized self-report measures, potential biases, such as social desirability (Fisher and Katz, 2000), may have influenced the results. Employing more objective measures, including behavioral assessments or evaluations by third parties (e.g., fellow students, parents, siblings, or partners), might mitigate such biases. A more fine-grained ambulatory assessment (e.g., multiple times a day across multiple days) could enhance ecological validity, reduce potential memory effects, and allow for an investigation of knowledge translation in everyday life (Shiffman et al., 2008; Wrzus and Neubauer, 2023).
A potential confounding factor is that control group participants could have had contact with intervention group participants, during which they may have exchanged information about the intervention’s contents. Thereby the control group would have been exposed to parts of the intervention, potentially affecting their outcomes.
Pertaining to practical implications, peer-to-peer multi-component positive psychological interventions are feasible and draw much interest from students. Participants generally comply with the exercises and consider them rather useful, although the effect of using the exercises at home remains unclear. Organizationally, such an intervention can be employed through the university’s sports and wellbeing administration. Intervention facilitators can be students with little to some knowledge in psychology with a training period of 2–3 sessions rehearsing the workshop and getting acquainted with the basic information on their presented module. To this end, a program supervisor with extended knowledge in positive psychology is needed. Further, some sort of incentive for the facilitators would be sensible, e.g., course credit, or a monetary reimbursement. Future studies could include staff members (e.g., teachers) into the intervention, drawing on findings that students may benefit from their teachers’ wellbeing as it creates a learning atmosphere that fosters wellbeing (Lo and Punzalan, 2025). This is promising for whole campus initiatives that aim to foster educator and student wellbeing concurrently, thus creating shared experiences and potentially, a positive social identity.
4.2 Conclusion
The current high levels of stress and mental health impairment among students attributed to the aftermath of the pandemic, multiple crises, and uncertainty about the future (Kohls et al., 2023; Zimmer et al., 2021) underscore the necessity for PPIs, which could boost students’ mental health and wellbeing. Since robustly evaluated interventions to enhance students’ mental health in Germany remain rare (Schweighart et al., 2023), this study provides a significant contribution. The findings of this research support the hypothesis that PPIs facilitate the development of positive psychological resources such as self-compassion, mindfulness, and positive reframing among university students.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/mn4yz/?view_only=17c795c9507a4fb9a53bd052a2e1b037.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethikkomission der Universität Potsdam, Universität Potsdam and Ethikkommission an der Universitätsmedizin Greifswald, Universität Greifswald. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
CE: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LP: Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. WV: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ST: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank (in alphabetical order) Alina Bechstädt, Anna Makeeva, Anne-Sophie Baars, Elisa Noack, Fabia Saina Wucherer, Franka Richter, Franziska Schubert, Jennifer Fischer, Julian Fladerer, Leandra Heußmann, Ludwig Krause, Markus Knapp, Nicolas Kurt Edelmann, Rita Kessler-Huth, Simon Barth, Thomas Kostack, Vincent Hulman for their contributions to conceptualization, methodology, investigation, resources, and supervision of this work. Furthermore, we would like to thank the FeelGoodCampus of the University of Potsdam for their support in advertising the program.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
References
Abler, B., and Kessler, H. (2009). Emotion regulation questionnaire–Eine deutschsprachige Fassung des ERQ von gross und John [emotion regulation questionnaire - a German version of the ERQ by gross and John]. Diagnostica 55, 144–152. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.55.3.144
Allison, P. D. (2010). “20: Missing Data” in Handbook of survey research. eds. P. V. Marsden and J. D. Wright. 2nd ed (Leeds, England: Emerald Group Publishing), 631–657.
Aristovnik, A., Keržič, D., Ravšelj, D., Tomaževič, N., and Umek, L. (2020). Impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on life of higher education students: a global perspective. Sustainability 12:8438. doi: 10.3390/su12208438
Baltes-Götz, B. (2013). Behandlung fehlender Werte in SPSS und Amos [treatment of missing values in SPSS and Amos]. Available online at: https://www.uni-trier.de/fileadmin/urt/doku/bfw/bfw.pdf
Becker, K., and Lörz, M. (2020). Studieren während der Corona-Pandemie: die finanzielle situation von Studierenden und mögliche Auswirkungen auf das studium [Studying during the corona-pandemic: the financial situation of students and potential effects on studying]. Deutsches Zentrum für Hochschul-und Wissenschaftsforschung. 9, 1–12. doi: 10.34878/2020.09.dzhw_brief
Beierlein, C., Kovaleva, B., Kemper, C. J., and Rammstedt, A. (2014). Allgemeine Selbstwirksamkeit Kurzskala (ASKU) [general self-efficay short scale (ASKU)]. Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen (ZIS). doi: 10.6102/zis35
Beierlein, C., Kovaleva, A., László, Z., Kemper, C. J., and Rammstedt, B. (2015). Kurzskala zur Erfassung der Allgemeinen Lebenszufriedenheit (L-1) [Short scale for measuring general life satisfaction (L-1)]. Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen (ZIS). doi: 10.6102/zis229
Blanke, E. S., and Brose, A. (2017). Mindfulness in daily life: a multidimensional approach. Mindfulness 8, 737–750. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0651-4
Breines, J. G., and Chen, S. (2013). Activating the inner caregiver: the role of support-giving schemas in increasing state self-compassion. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.015
Breyer, B., and Bluemke, M. (2016). Deutsche version der positive and negative affect schedule PANAS (GESIS panel) [German version of the positive and negative affect schedule (GESIS panel)]. Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen (ZIS). doi: 10.6102/zis242
Byl, E., Struyven, K., Meurs, P., Bieke, A., Tom, V., Nadine, E., et al. (2016). The value of peer learning for first-year postgraduate university students’ social and academic integration. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 228, 299–304. doi: 10.1016/J.SBSPRO.2016.07.044
Carr, A., Cullen, K., Keeney, C., Canning, C., Mooney, O., Chinseallaigh, E., et al. (2021). Effectiveness of positive psychology interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 16, 749–769. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2020.1818807
Chilver, M. R., and Gatt, J. M. (2022). Six-week online multi-component positive psychology intervention improves subjective wellbeing in young adults. J. Happiness Stud. 23, 1267–1288. doi: 10.1007/s10902-021-00449-3
Cohen, S., and Williamson, G. (1988). “Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States” in The social psychology of health. eds. S. Spacapan and S. Oskamp (Thousand Oaks, California, United States: Sage), 31–68.
Congard, A., Andreotti, E., Dauvier, B., Le Vigouroux, S., and Antoine, P. (2020). Modification of affective trajectory in a positive psychology intervention. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 12, 770–786. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12
Constantino, M. J., Arnkoff, D. B., Glass, C. R., Ametrano, R. M., and Smith, J. Z. (2011). Expectations. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 184–192. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20754
de Sousa, G. M., de Lima-Araújo, G. L., de Araújo, D. B., and de Sousa, M. B. C. (2021). Brief mindfulness-based training and mindfulness trait attenuate psychological stress in university students: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychol. 9:21. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00520-x
Drixler, K., Morfeld, M., Glaesmer, H., Brähler, E., and Wirtz, M. A. (2020). Validation of the short-form-health-survey-12 (SF-12 version 2.0) assessing health-related quality of life in a normative German sample. Z. Psychosom. Med. Psychother. 66, 272–286. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2020.66.3.272
Dryden, R. P., Campbell, A. V., Perry, R. P., Hamm, J. M., Chipperfield, J. G., Parker, P. C., et al. (2023). Assisting students in debt to overcome academic setbacks with a cognitive-reframing motivation intervention. J. Educ. Psychol. 115:55. doi: 10.1037/edu0000770
Dvořáková, K., Kishida, M., Li, J., Elavsky, S., Broderick, P. C., Agrusti, M. R., et al. (2017). Promoting healthy transition to college through mindfulness training with first-year college students: pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 65, 259–267. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2017.1278605
Emmons, R. A., and McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377
Ewert, C., Vater, A., and Schröder-Abé, M. (2021). Self-compassion and coping: a Meta-analysis. J. Mindfulness 12, 1063–1077. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01563-8
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
Fisher, R. J., and Katz, J. E. (2000). Social-desirability bias and the validity of self-reported values. Psychol. Mark. 17, 105–120. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(200002)17:2<105::AID-MAR3>3.0.CO;2-9
Gander, F., Wagner, L., Amann, L., and Ruch, W. (2022). What are character strengths good for? A daily diary study on character strengths enactment. J. Posit. Psychol. 17, 718–728. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2021.1926532
Goleman, D., and Davidson, R. J. (2017). Altered traits. Science reveals how meditation changes your mind, brain, and body. New York City, USA: Avery Publishing.
Haukaas, R. B., Gjerde, I. B., Varting, G., Hallan, H. E., and Solem, S. (2018). A randomized controlled trial comparing the attention training technique and mindful self-compassion for students with symptoms of depression and anxiety. Front. Psychol. 9:827. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00827
Hendriks, T., Schotanus-Dijkstra, M., Hassankhan, A., Jong, J. D., and Bohlmeijer, E. (2020). The efficacy of multi-component positive psychology interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 357–390. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00082-1
Hirshberg, M. J., Colaianne, B. A., Greenberg, M. T., Inkelas, K. K., Davidson, R. J., Germano, D., et al. (2022). Can the academic and experiential study of flourishing improve flourishing in college students? A multi-university study. Mindfulness 13, 2243–2256. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01952-1
Hirshberg, M. J., Goldberg, S. B., Schaefer, S. M., Flook, L., Findley, D., and Davidson, R. J. (2018). Divergent effects of brief contemplative practices in response to an acute stressor: a randomized controlled trial of brief breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude or an attention control practice. PLoS One 13:e0207765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207765
Hobbs, C., Armitage, J., Hood, B., and Jelbert, S. (2022). A systematic review of the effect of university positive psychology courses on student psychological wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 13:1023140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1023140
Hoffmann, C. F. A., Schröder-Abé, M., and Ewert, C. (2024). The role of monitoring and acceptance in daily affective stress reactivity: an ambulatory assessment study. Mindfulness 15, 2609–2621. doi: 10.1007/s12671-024-02443-1
Hupfeld, J., and Ruffieux, N. (2011). Validierung einer deutschen version der self-compassion scale (SCS-D) [Validation of a German version of the self-compassion scale (SCS-D)]. Zeitschrift fur Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie 40, 115–123. doi: 10.1026/1616-3443/a000088
Janke, S., and Glöckner-Rist, A. (2012). Deutsche version der satisfaction with life scale (SWLS) [German version of the satisfaction with life scale (SWLS)]. Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen (ZIS). doi: 10.6102/zis147
Ketchen Lipson, S., Gaddis, S. M., Heinze, J., Beck, K., and Eisenberg, D. (2015). Variations in student mental health and treatment utilization across US colleges and universities. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 63, 388–396. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2015.1040411
Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., and Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 81, 41–46. doi: 10.1016/J.PAID.2014.12.044
Knoll, N., Rieckmann, N., and Schwarzer, R. (2005). Coping as a mediator between personality and stress outcomes: a longitudinal study with cataract surgery patients. Eur. J. Personal. 19, 229–247. doi: 10.1002/per.546
Kohls, E., Guenthner, L., Baldofski, S., Brock, T., Schuhr, J., and Rummel-Kluge, C. (2023). Two years COVID-19 pandemic: development of university students’ mental health 2020-2022. Front. Psych. 14:1122256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1122256
Kovaleva, A., Beierlein, C., Kemper, C. J., and Rammstedt, B. (2012). Eine Kurzskala zur Messung von Kontrollüberzeugung: Die Skala Internale-Externale Kontrollüberzeugung-4 (IE-4) [A short scale for measuring loci of control: the scale internal-external-loci of Control-4 (IE-4)]. GESIS – Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften. Available online at: https://www.gesis.org/fileadmin/kurzskalen/working_papers/IE4_Workingpaper.pdf
Kreidl, C., and Dittler, U. (2021). “2: die Corona-Lehre: Wahrnehmung der Studierenden [The corona-teaching: perception of university students]” in Wie Corona die Hochschullehre verändert [How corona changes university teaching]. eds. U. Dittler and C. Kreidl (Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer), 15–35.
Kroshus, E., Hawrilenko, M., and Browning, A. (2021). Stress, self-compassion, and well-being during the transition to college. Soc. Sci. Med. 269:113514, 113514. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113514
Leicht, M. C. (2015). Lachen macht Gesund? Die Wirkung von humor als Copingstrategie bei Studierenden [Laughter makes you healthy? The effects of humor as a coping strategy in students]. [Unpublished Bachelor’s thesis].
Lo, N., and Punzalan, C. (2025). The impact of positive psychology on language teachers in higher education. J. Univ. Teach. Learn. Pract. 22. doi: 10.53761/5ckx2h71
Lorenz, T., Algner, M., and Binder, B. (2022). A positive psychology resource for students? Evaluation of the effectiveness of the 6 minutes diary in a randomized control trial. Front. Psychol. 13:896741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.896741
Lörz, M., Zimmer, L. M., and Koopmann, J. (2021). Herausforderungen und Konsequenzen der Corona-Pandemie für Studierende in Deutschland [Challenges and consequences of the corona-pandemic for university students in Germany]. Psychol. Erzieh. Unterr. 68, 312–318. doi: 10.2378/peu2021.art28d
Löwe, B. P., and Spitzer, R. L. (2002). PHQ-D: Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten [PHQ-D: Health questionnaire for patients]. Berlin, Germany: Pfizer GmbH.
Mahmood, L., Hopthrow, T., and Moura, G. R. D. (2016). A moment of mindfulness: computer-mediated mindfulness practice increases state mindfulness. PLoS One 11:e0153923. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0153923
Martin, R. A., and Lefcourt, H. M. (1983). Sense of humor as a moderator of the relation between stressors and moods. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 1313–1324. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.6.1313
Moskowitz, D. S., Brown, K. W., and Côté, S. (1997). Reconceptualizing stability: using time as a psychological dimension. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 6, 127–132. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772890
Neff, K. D., and Germer, C. K. (2013). A pilot study and randomized controlled trial of the mindful self-compassion program. J. Clin. Psychol. 69, 28–44. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21923
Neuhaus, M., Young, T., Ferris, L. J., Grimmel, C. L. M., and Reid, N. (2022). A narrative review of peer-led positive psychology interventions: current evidence, potential, and future directions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:8065. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19138065
Oosterhoff, B., Whillock, S., Tintzman, C., and Poppler, A. (2022). Temporal associations between character strengths and civic action: a daily diary study. J. Posit. Psychol. 17, 729–741. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2021.1940247
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses, vol. 128. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association, 3–72.
Patzelt, J., and Opitz, I. (2014). “Deutsche version des academic procrastination state inventory (APSI-d) [German version of the academic procrastination state inventory (APSI-d)]” in ZUMA-Informationssystem. Elektronisches Handbuch sozialwissenschaftlicher Erhebungsinstrumente, Version (Bd. 8). ed. A. Glöckner-Rist. Mannheim, Germany: ZUMA.
Pogrebtsova, E., Craig, J., Chris, A., O’Shea, D., and González-Morales, M. G. (2018). Exploring daily affective changes in university students with a mindful positive reappraisal intervention: a daily diary randomized controlled trial. Stress. Health 34, 46–58. doi: 10.1002/smi.2759
Rashid, T., and Seligman, M. (2021). Positive Psychotherapie: Ein Therapiemanual [Positive psychotherapy: a therapy manual]. 1st Edn. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Scharfstein, D. O., Hogan, J., and Herman, A. (2012). Randomized trials in orthopaedic surgery: advances and future directions. On the prevention and analysis of missing data in randomized clinical trials: the state of the art. J. Bone Joint Surg. Am. 94, 80–84. doi: 10.2106/JBJS.L.00273
Schmitt, M., and Blum, G. S. (2020). “State/trait interactions” in Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. eds. V. Zeigler-Hill and T. K. Shackelford (New York, USA: Springer), 5206–5209.
Schneider, M. L., Cha, B. S., Borghouts, J., Eikey, E. V., Schueller, S. M., Stadnick, N. A., et al. (2024). Individual and organizational outcomes of engaging peers in the cocreation of digital mental health interventions. Psychol. Serv. doi: 10.1037/ser0000889
Schneider, E. E., Schönfelder, S., Domke-Wolf, M., and Wessa, M. (2020). Measuring stress in clinical and nonclinical subjects using a German adaptation of the perceived stress scale. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 20, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2020.03.004
Schotanus-Dijkstra, M., Drossaert, C. H. C., Pieterse, M. E., Walburg, J. A., and Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2015). Efficacy of a multicomponent positive psychology self-help intervention: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res. Protocols 4:e4162. doi: 10.2196/resprot.4162
Schueller, S. M., and Parks, A. C. (2012). Disseminating self-help: positive psychology exercises in an online trial. J. Med. Internet Res. 14:e63. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1850
Schweighart, R., Thätz, J., Demar, L., Zehl, F., Neuderth, S., and Löbmann, R. (2023). Welche Bedarfe haben Studierende hinsichtlich gesundheitsförderlicher Maßnahmen und welche Sind ihnen bekannt? Eine Befragung an zwei Hochschulstandorten [what needs do students have concerning health-improvement measures and which measures do they know? A survey at two universities]. Prävention und Gesundheitsförderung 1–9, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s11553-023-01031-w
Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: a visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York City, USA: Free Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions. Am. Psychol. 60, 410–421. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.5.410
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., and Biegel, G. M. (2007). Teaching self-care to caregivers: effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Train. Educ. Prof. Psychol. 1, 105–115. doi: 10.1037/1931-3918.1.2.105
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., and Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4, 1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415
Sirois, F. M. (2001). The wellness behaviors inventory. Unpublished manual. UK: Department of Psychology, Durham University.
Smeets, E., Neff, K. D., Alberts, H., and Peters, M. (2014). Meeting suffering with kindness: effects of a brief self-compassion intervention for female college students. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 794–807. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22076
Snyder, E., Cai, B., DeMuro, C., Morrison, M. F., and Ball, W. (2018). A new single-item sleep quality scale: results of psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic primary insomnia and depression. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 14, 1849–1857. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.7478
Steghaus, S., and Poth, C. H. (2022). Assessing momentary relaxation using the relaxation state questionnaire (RSQ). Sci. Rep. 12:16341. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-20524-w
Topping, K. J. (2022). Peer education and peer counselling for health and well-being: a review of reviews. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:6064. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19106064
Totzeck, C., Teismann, T., Hofmann, S. G., Brachel, R. V., Pflug, V., Wannemüller, A., et al. (2020). Loving-kindness meditation promotes mental health in university students. Mindfulness 11, 1623–1631. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01375-w
Tsiouris, A., Werner, A. M., Tibubos, A. N., Mülder, L. M., Reichel, J. L., Heller, S., et al. (2023). Mental health state and its determinants in German university students across the COVID-19 pandemic: findings from three repeated cross-sectional surveys between 2019 and 2021. Front. Public Health 11:1163541. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1163541
Twisk, J. W. R., Rijnhart, J. J. M., Hoekstra, T., Schuster, N. A., Ter Wee, M. M., and Heymans, M. W. (2020). Intention-to-treat analysis when only a baseline value is available. contemporary clinical trials. Communications 20:100684. doi: 10.1016/j.conctc.2020.100684
Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmüller, V., Kleinknecht, N., Grossmann, P., and Schmidt, S. (2004). Empirische Erfassung der Achtsamkeit–die Konstruktion des Freiburger Fragebogens zur Achtsamkeit (FFA) und weitere Validierungsstudien [Empirical measurement of mindfulness - the construction of the Freiburg Questionnare for mindfulness (FFA) and further validation studies]. Achtsamkeit und Akzeptanz in der Psychotherapie, 727–770. Available online at: https://www.nina-rose.de/daten/2004_Walach_etal_Empirische_Erfassung_Buch.pdf
Westermann, R., Heise, E., and Spies, K. (2018). “Kurzfragebogen zur Erfassung der Studienzufriedenheit [Verfahrensdokumentation, Fragebogen und Erläuterungen zum Fragebogen] [short questionnaire for measuring student satisfcation [Process documentation, questionnaire and explanations for the questionnaire]]” in Leibnitz-Institut für Psychologie (Trier, Germany: Open Test Archive).
World Health Organization (1998). Regional Office for Europe Wellbeing measures in primary health care: the Depcare Project. In Report on a WHO Meeting. Available online at: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/349766
Wrzus, C., and Neubauer, A. B. (2023). Ecological momentary assessment: a meta-analysis on designs, samples, and compliance across research fields. Assessment 30, 825–846. doi: 10.1177/10731911211067538
Zimmer, L. M., Lörz, M., and Marczuk, A. (2021). Studieren in Zeiten der Corona-Pandemie: vulnerable Studierendengruppen im Fokus [Studying in times of the corona-pandemic: vulnerable student groups in focus]. Deutsches Zentrum für Hochschul- und Wissenschaftsforschung (DZHW) 2, 1–9. doi: 10.34878/2021.02.dzhw_brief
Keywords: positive psychology, intervention, state-trait, university students, Ecological momentary assessment
Citation: Ewert C, Palme L, Voigt WHA and Tomczyk S (2025) Supporting the transition to higher education: examination of state- and trait-level effects of a multicomponent positive psychological intervention for university students. Front. Psychol. 16:1631713. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1631713
Edited by:
Abdolvahab Samavi, University of Hormozgan, IranReviewed by:
Noble Lo, Lancaster University, United KingdomKonstantinos Karampas, Panteion University, Greece
Copyright © 2025 Ewert, Palme, Voigt and Tomczyk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Wilhelm H. A. Voigt, d2lsaGVsbS52b2lndEB1bmktcG90c2RhbS5kZQ==