- Research Institute for Ancient Books, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
Buddhist meditation, encompassing practices such as Samatha (focused attention), Vipassana (open monitoring), and Metta (loving-kindness), offers unique pathways for transforming consciousness beyond conventional mindfulness. In this article, we review the studies that explore how these distinct meditative techniques systematically cultivate meta-cognitive insight, emotional regulation, and self-inquiry, facilitating profound shifts in awareness and personal growth. Recent neuroscience and psychology studies show that these techniques influence the mind in different ways: they strengthen attentional stability, reshape self-referential thinking, and reorganize emotional patterns. Such modifications are evident in reorganized brain networks (for example, the default-mode network) and in characteristic EEG patterns. While sharing some parallels with Western mindfulness and hypnosis, Buddhist meditation uniquely emphasizes ethical integration and profound introspection. Challenges remain in objectively measuring advanced meditative states, particularly the experience of “no-self” (anattā), due to the reliance on subjective self-report. Future research should incorporate culturally sensitive methodologies, objective behavioral tasks, and interdisciplinary approaches like neurophenomenology to integrate traditional contemplative wisdom with rigorous scientific inquiry.
1 Introduction
Consciousness deepening involves a progression from basic awareness to increasingly refined states of meta-cognitive insight (Travis and Shear, 2010; Poletti et al., 2024). This transformation involves not only noticing thoughts, emotions, and sensory experiences but also adopting a broader perspective on these mental processes. As such, it goes beyond ordinary self-monitoring by altering how one relates to internal phenomena. Recently, neuroscientists have turned their attention to these advanced contemplative states, especially within the context of Buddhist meditation.
Buddhist meditation offers a special way to examine how our minds change. It mainly involves two practices: Samatha (focused attention) and Vipassana (open monitoring). While many Western mindfulness approaches emphasize present-moment awareness, Samatha-Vipassana frameworks integrate both concentrated stabilization and discernment of mental patterns (Sharf, 2015). Consequently, Buddhist meditation presents an opportunity to investigate pathways of consciousness deepening that go beyond symptom reduction or stress management, identifying underlying processes of self-transformation and insight.
Current research is centered on several important areas. First, researchers examine the neurological mechanisms during Buddhist meditation, such as significant shifts in the default mode network (Trousselard et al., 2014; Chowdhury et al., 2023; Laukkonen et al., 2023). Second, work on second-generation mindfulness-based interventions (SG-MBIs) has expanded beyond superficial applications toward deeper clinical integration (Crane et al., 2017; Van Gordon and Shonin, 2020). Finally, debates concerning cultural adaptation and the risks of secular oversimplification raise essential ethical and philosophical questions (Kelly, 2023; Neves-Pereira et al., 2018).
Accordingly, this paper aims to (1) explore how Buddhist meditation can foster more profound levels of consciousness, (2) synthesize major psychological findings, and (3) propose directions for future study, including neuroethical considerations and culturally sensitive frameworks. By doing so, it aspires to bridge traditional Buddhist insights and contemporary scientific understandings of mental health and well-being. Figure 1 presents a neurocognitive pathway model of how core Buddhist meditation practices facilitate consciousness transformation in a systematic manner.
2 Exploring consciousness: psychological mechanisms of Buddhist meditation
Buddhist meditation traditions transform the mind to a greater depth than what ordinary mindfulness exercises can achieve. By training in a structured way, practitioners learn to notice their thoughts and feelings. This awareness paves the way for insights into the fluid and interdependent nature of experiences. This section explores the conceptual underpinnings guiding these transformations and how they integrate with psychological mechanisms for profound personal change over time.
2.1 Buddhist meditation classifications and pathways for deepening consciousness
Buddhist meditation practices focus on organized ways to stabilize and sharpen the mind. This training facilitates the progression of the deepening of consciousness among practitioners. Three core practices—Samatha, Vipassana and Metta—are highlighted both in classical Buddhist scriptures and in modern empirical research. Each of these practices employs distinct mechanisms to transform cognitive processes and emotional states (Lutz et al., 2008; Vago and Silbersweig, 2012; Lippelt et al., 2014; Yordanova et al., 2020).
Samatha, or “focused attention” meditation, involves sustained concentration on a chosen object—often the breath or a visual focus. It aims to achieve a state of absorption. During advanced stages, one may notice a marked decline in distractibility accompanied by greater inner calm (Neri et al., 2024). Concentrative meditation (e.g., Samatha) is associated with increased frontal midline theta activity, correlating with attentional stability and reduced discursive thought (Cahn and Polich, 2006).
Vipassana, or “open monitoring” meditation, differs from Samatha. Vipassana meditators observe the uninterrupted flow of stimuli—sensations, thoughts and emotions—without labeling or reacting. Empirical findings indicate that people who have practiced Vipassana meditation for a long time show increased gamma brain activity in a specific brain area (parieto-occipital). This may relate to the adoption of a mindful and receptive awareness and enhanced perceptual clarity of moment-to-moment experience (Cahn et al., 2010). Additionally, mindfulness induces a sense of selflessness. Simultaneously, it reduces gamma activity in the prefrontal cortex, which serves as evidence suggesting that Vipassana meditation undermines the narrative ego (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013).
Metta, or “loving-kindness” meditation, centers on cultivating unconditional goodwill and compassion toward oneself and others. Metta helps reshape the way we organize and experience our emotions. It can thereby reduce ingrained reactions such as hostility, anxiety, or fear (Hofmann et al., 2015; Stefan and Hofmann, 2019; Frick et al., 2020; Stangier et al., 2021). Metta meditation reduces self-other P300 amplitude differences in EEG/ERP studies, indicating balanced emotional processing. Moreover, long-term adherence to this practice engenders self-transcendence and concomitantly enhances interpersonal connectedness and altruistic tendencies (Trautwein et al., 2016).
Table 1 shows the different psychological mechanisms, neurophysiological correlates, and clinical outcomes for these three main meditation techniques. A key marker of this consciousness deepening is the shift from the “narrative self,” characterized by autobiographical identity and persistent internal dialogue, to the “minimal self,” which centers on momentary experience (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2013, 2024). As practitioners move along this continuum, many report reduced cognitive fusion with personal stories and heightened awareness of immediate phenomena (Yang et al., 2024). Closely related to this transition is the cultivation of equanimity. Equanimity is a calm, balanced mind that stays steady no matter what thoughts or situations arise. Researchers measure equanimity as a key sign of advanced meditation, and they find that cultivating this quality can really change the way people deal with difficult situations (Desbordes et al., 2015; Hadash et al., 2016; Yang et al., 2024).
2.2 Psychological mechanism framework
Buddhist meditation’s effects can be illuminated by several psychological theories that detail its underlying mechanisms. These theories show that structured mindfulness practice can improve attention, influence emotions, and change how people see themselves. Thus, meditation builds a strong base for deeper self-discovery and personal growth.
At a foundational level, Meditation operates through dual cognitive systems: a reactive “hot” system driving impulsive responses during stress, and an adaptive “cool” system enabling reflective insights and flexible coping (Lehman et al., 2025). Meditation, specifically Samatha and Metta, appears to modulate this system through distinct pathways: the former enhances attentional stability and meta-awareness to reduce experiential fusion, while the latter cultivates prosocial affect and cognitive reappraisal to reinforce adaptive emotional patterns (Yordanova et al., 2020).
A growing body of research further suggests that what we commonly regard as a fixed “self” is better understood as a dynamic construct that can be broken down and reassembled over time (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2024). Through consistent contemplative practice, the self-model can reorganize in non-linear ways, occasionally giving rise to experiences of “no-self” (anattā), where the usual boundaries of identity are perceived as porous or illusory (Laukkonen and Slagter, 2021; Cooper et al., 2022). Through its open-monitoring style, Vipassana meditation proves especially effective in cultivating the crucial insight. It brings to light the transient and constructed nature of thoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations (Dahl et al., 2015).
Finally, emerging frameworks in cognitive science, such as predictive coding, shed light on how meditation might recalibrate the brain’s Bayesian inferences (Lutz et al., 2019; Dutta, 2025). From this viewpoint, the mind perpetually generates predictions about incoming sensory information and updates them based on the precision of actual sensory data. Meditation practices, by reducing habitual patterns of reactive thought and enhancing mindful observation, can optimize this Bayesian inference process. As a result, practitioners become better at telling the difference between top-down expectations (our biases and beliefs) and bottom-up sensory signals (the raw data of experience). This refined capacity for sensory discrimination and error minimization contributes to a more vivid, less assumption-laden experience of reality (Manjaly and Iglesias, 2020).
Taken together, these theoretical models converge on a central theme: Buddhist meditation practices systematically reshape how individuals process and interpret their inner and outer worlds. From moderating emotional reactivity (dual-system theory) to reconfiguring the sense of self (PTS), or refining top-down and bottom-up perceptual loops (predictive coding), each framework underscores the transformative potential of contemplative training.
3 Empirical evidence: from neural correlates to clinical outcomes
Researchers are paying more attention to Buddhist meditation’s impact on the brain. Neuroimaging studies show advanced practitioners display distinctive brain activation and connectivity, especially in self-referential, attentional, and emotion-regulation networks (Guendelman et al., 2017; Guidotti et al., 2023). This leads to potential clinical benefits. Preliminary research suggests that practitioners who engage in long-term meditation experience less anxiety, depression, and stress. This section examines the evidence base, linking Buddhist contemplative methods to measurable outcomes in neuroplasticity, cognition, and therapeutic efficacy.
3.1 Neuroplasticity evidence
Extensive research indicates that Buddhist meditation promotes a flexible restructuring of the brain’s neural pathways. This process can lead to more stable attention, better emotional control, and enhanced meta-awareness. One of the most widely discussed findings involves modifications in the default mode network (DMN), a set of brain regions associated with self-referential thought and mind-wandering (Bremer et al., 2022). Through consistent mindfulness and concentrative practices, meditators enhance the ability to regulate interactions between the brain’s default mode network (DMN) and the salience network. This improves flexible shifts between internal reflection and external focus by optimizing attention allocation to relevant events (Raffone et al., 2019).
Additionally, long-term practitioners of Buddhist meditation exhibit strengthened connections in the brain circuits linking the prefrontal and parietal lobes (Kemmer et al., 2015; Yordanova et al., 2021). Meditation training consistently enhances theta synchronization in left parietal regions across practices (FAM/OMM/LKM), while beta-band connectivity in medial frontal networks distinguishes focused attention (FAM) from open monitoring (OMM). This reflects functional integration of fronto-parietal and cognitive monitoring networks, demonstrating meditation’s role in higher-order cognitive engagement beyond relaxation.
Electroencephalogram (EEG) and event-related potential (ERP) studies provide additional support for these findings by identifying neural signatures associated with specific meditation practices. Concentrative meditation, emphasizing sustained attention, demonstrates robust frontal midline theta enhancement, particularly in advanced practitioners. This theta activity, generated by anterior cingulate and prefrontal regions, indexes focused internal attention and reduced distractibility. While alpha increases may occur during relaxation, theta is the primary electrophysiological marker of concentrative proficiency, distinct from generalized alpha patterns associated with calm alertness (Cahn and Polich, 2006). Moreover, practitioners with extensive mindfulness retreat training demonstrate enhanced exogenous alerting in behavioral tasks, reflected by faster detection of uncued targets and reduced reliance on temporal warnings (Jha et al., 2007). These findings align with the idea that meditation training not only enhances focus but also modulates attention allocation by changing the neural dynamics of the brain (Lutz et al., 2004).
In the domain of higher meditative states, researchers observe a reduction in gamma-band power, an oscillation usually tied to demanding cognitive work (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013). Its suppression in advanced meditation might reflect a refined meta-cognitive insight, wherein the practitioner observes mental content without becoming entangled or over-identified with it. This attenuation of gamma band activity could thus serve as an indicator of deeper introspective access and altered self-referential processing, linking to Buddhist concepts of selflessness or non-attachment.
3.2 Cognitive and behavioral effects
Beyond the neural level, a range of empirical studies has explored how such meditative training translates into cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Key among these is the enhancement of metacognition, or the capacity to monitor and regulate one’s own cognitive processes. Experienced meditators, for instance, detect the very first stirrings of intention with greater clarity (Jo et al., 2015; Lush et al., 2016). This suggests that Buddhist meditation might systematically cultivate introspective acuity, enabling practitioners to notice subtle mental cues before they escalate into broader courses of action.
In a similar way, meditation has been linked to a noticeable decrease in mind-wandering, which is the tendency for attention to stray from its main focus (Feruglio et al., 2021). By training attention in Samatha practice, individuals learn to sustain focus on a singular object or sensation (e.g., the breath) and to gently redirect stray thoughts back to the task at hand. Over time, this disciplined practice of returning to a focal point appears to restructure cognitive habits, resulting in less spontaneous wandering and a more deliberate mode of mental engagement.
The clinical implications of these cognitive changes are now well documented. In the realm of depression treatment, there is growing interest in combining Metta with mindfulness-based approaches (Frick et al., 2020). Metta, which cultivates loving-kindness and benevolence toward oneself and others, directly targets chronic depression by enhancing prosocial motivation and reducing self-criticism (Stangier et al., 2021). Pain management research also underscores the efficacy of Buddhist meditation. Functional MRI reveals that mindfulness deactivates thalamic pathways and default mode networks, reducing pain unpleasantness more effectively than hypnotic suggestions in some contexts (Zeidan et al., 2019; Wipplinger et al., 2023). Further studies are needed to compare specific meditation types (e.g., Vipassana vs. Metta) for targeted pain conditions and clarify their neural substrates.
4 Bridging paradigms: contrasting Buddhist and western psychological practices
Buddhist meditation and Western psychological approaches share goals like reducing distress and enhancing well-being. Yet, they differ in views of the mind, self-transformation, and ethical or philosophical matters. Comparing them reveals that each Buddhist pathways highlights ethics and deep introspection. This section contrasts hypnosis-induced attentional states and mainstream mindfulness interventions with Buddhist methods, highlighting differences in mental training. These contrasting perspectives ultimately complement and challenge each other in achieving holistic well-being. Table 2 lays out the major distinctions between Buddhist meditation, Western mindfulness, and hypnosis.
4.1 Cognitive mechanisms compared to hypnosis
Buddhist meditation and hypnosis share surface-level similarities in eliciting absorption states and promoting autonomic regulation, often manifesting as relaxation that soothes body and mind. These parallels stem from both practices’ capacity to heighten focused attention or leverage suggestibility to guide experiential states (Holroyd, 2003; Penazzi and De Pisapia, 2022). In hypnosis, externally driven suggestions engage cognitive pathways aligned with “cold control” theory (Dienes, 2012; Lush and Dienes, 2019). Under this model, these suggestions temporarily disconnect executive control from conscious awareness, such that people feel their actions occur automatically.
Conversely, Buddhist meditation employs internally guided techniques (e.g., Samatha or Vipassana) to cultivate meta-awareness, allowing practitioners to observe thoughts, emotions, and sensations non-judgmentally (Lehman et al., 2025). Although both practices can lead to altered states, the underlying metacognition is opposite. Hypnosis involves inaccurate metacognition of intentions (experiencing voluntary actions as involuntary), while meditation cultivates enhanced metacognitive access to intentions. Thus, hypnosis relies on disrupted higher-order awareness of volition, whereas meditation refines conscious insight into intentional processes (Lush and Dienes, 2019).
4.2 Distinctions from mainstream mindfulness interventions
Modern mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) are usually presented as secular tools for stress relief and symptom control. This contrasts with traditional Buddhist mindfulness, which is deeply intertwined with moral and ethical precepts in texts such as the Vinaya and the Abhidhamma (Shulman, 2024; Rohde et al., 2024). In Buddhist contexts, ethical conduct, compassion, and community life are foundational elements that shape how mindfulness is cultivated and applied. When these aspects are removed for secular therapeutic settings, the practice may lose some of its transformative potential and philosophical depth.
Furthermore, second-generation mindfulness-based interventions (SG-MBIs) seek to integrate contemplative elements of Buddhist psychology—such as loving-kindness and non-self teachings—into clinical protocols (Van Gordon and Shonin, 2020; Gamaiunova et al., 2022). These SG-MBIs strive to go beyond mere symptom alleviation by exploring deeper existential or spiritual dimensions. In doing so, they underscore the importance of a holistic and ethically grounded framework, thus representing a closer parallel to the original intentions and scope of Buddhist meditation while still addressing contemporary clinical needs.
5 Critical gaps and future directions
Despite a large number of studies on Buddhist meditation emerging in recent years, which have greatly enhanced our knowledge of this ancient practice, progress is still impeded by a stubborn gap between subjective reports (e.g., emergent phenomenology) and objective measurements (e.g., neurobiological states). A central challenge is bridging contemplative frameworks—such as the Buddhist doctrine of not-self (anattā)—with empirical paradigms to develop testable models of mental phenomena (Wright et al., 2023). Reliance on self-report data risks measurement bias and incomplete insights, highlighting the necessity of complementary behavioral methodologies (Nyklíček, 2020). Such approaches reveal divergent aspects of self-awareness—undetectable via questionnaires—and demonstrate added value in capturing nuanced phenomenological experiences like interoception during spontaneous states.
Addressing these gaps requires practical strategies that emphasize cultural sensitivity. For example, the concept of “mindfulness” varies significantly between traditions. Many Buddhist schools associate sati with ethics and philosophy, whereas Western psychology treats it mainly as an attentional or cognitive-behavioral skill (Krägeloh, 2018; Cassaniti, 2022). When there is disagreement among people regarding the intended objectives of meditation, it leads to ambiguity about its purpose. Consequently, this ambiguity permeates the planning of studies and the application of clinical treatments. Additionally, Western-developed scales may fail to capture the ethical or relational dimensions essential to practices like Metta or Chinese Chan (Zen) meditation. Hirayama et al. (2014), for instance, found that direct translations of the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory were insufficient to encompass essential aspects of Eastern contemplative experiences. Equally crucial is guarding against the commodification of mindfulness, which Kelly (2023) argues risks reducing meditation to a superficial, market-driven commodity (“McMindfulness”). This commercialization not only dilutes mindfulness’s transformative potential but also fosters “spiritual bypassing.”
Interdisciplinary collaborations promise new insights into how meditation shifts consciousness at both subjective and neural levels. Methodological innovations from neurophenomenology offer a way to synchronize first-person reports of meditative experience with objective neurobiological measures (Lutz et al., 2024; Yang et al., 2025). In parallel, Bayesian predictive coding models demonstrate that Insight meditation enhances the brain’s capacity for self-assessment. This improvement is likely to facilitate the reversal of detrimental learning patterns in individuals with functional neurological disorders (Dutta, 2025). Bringing predictive coding together with meditation research will allow future studies to map in detail how Buddhist contemplative methods transform cognition, emotion, and self-perception.
Overall, investigations into Buddhist meditation have consistently reported robust effects on neural networks, emotional regulation, and meta-cognitive awareness. However, cultural and research-related challenges still persist, which means we require more advanced measurement tools and analytical methods. Strengthening interdisciplinary ties, embracing culturally informed approaches, and upholding ethical rigor are essential to unlocking the full potential of Buddhist contemplative methods in both clinical and broader societal contexts.
6 Conclusion
In conclusion, this review underscores how Buddhist meditation offers a transformative pathway that extends beyond conventional mindfulness. By systematically cultivating concentrative focus (Samatha), insight-oriented awareness (Vipassana), and compassion-based practices (Metta), practitioners can progressively deepen consciousness and develop refined meta-cognitive insights. The integration of attention training, emotional regulation, and self-inquiry emerges as a distinct framework that can help dissolve habitual thought patterns and foster greater authenticity in daily life. Notably, these practices point toward insights like no-self (anattā), challenging the limitations of self-report measures that may fail to capture the breadth and depth of such advanced experiences.
To strengthen validation of subjective accounts of transformative states, researchers and practitioners ought to collaborate on the design and application of objective or semi-objective methodologies. Such approaches may encompass carefully controlled behavioral tasks as well as precise introspective timing paradigms. In doing so, they can support practitioners’ observations and minimize possible biases in collecting data. Future research will benefit from culturally sensitive and ethically informed approaches, ensuring diverse populations are included and respected. Ultimately, bridging traditional Buddhist contemplative insights with modern scientific investigations may yield a more robust understanding of consciousness, informing therapeutic interventions and enhancing mental health across various contexts.
Author contributions
CW: Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (China).
Conflict of interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Brown, K. W., Gallagher, S., Barendregt, H., Bauer, P., Giommi, F., et al. (2024). Pattern theory of selflessness: How meditation may transform the self-pattern. Mindfulness 15, 2114–2140. doi: 10.1007/s12671-024-02418-2
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: A neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front. Psychol. 4:912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912
Bremer, B., Wu, Q., Mora, Álvarez, M. G., Hölzel, B. K., Wilhelm, M., et al. (2022). Mindfulness meditation increases default mode, salience, and central executive network connectivity. Sci. Rep. 12:13219. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-17325-6
Cahn, B. R., and Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG. ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 132, 180–211. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180
Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., and Polich, J. (2010). Occipital gamma activation during Vipassana meditation. Cogn. Process 11, 39–56. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0352-1
Cassaniti, J. (2022). Memory, ghosts and the good life: Sati in Theravada cultural contexts. Anthropol. Today 38, 4–8. doi: 10.1111/1467-8322.12706
Chowdhury, A., Van Lutterveld, R., Laukkonen, R. E., Slagter, H. A., Ingram, D. M., and Sacchet, M. D. (2023). Investigation of advanced mindfulness meditation “cessation” experiences using EEG spectral analysis in an intensively sampled case study. Neuropsychologia 190:108694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108694
Cooper, A. C., Ventura, B., and Northoff, G. (2022). Beyond the veil of duality—topographic reorganization model of meditation. Neurosci. Conscious. 2022:niac013. doi: 10.1093/nc/niac013
Crane, R. S., Brewer, J., Feldman, C., Kabat-Zinn, J., Santorelli, S., Williams, J. M. G., et al. (2017). What defines mindfulness-based programs? The warp and the weft. Psychol. Med. 47, 990–999. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716003317
Dahl, C. J., Lutz, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2015). Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: Cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 515–523. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001
Desbordes, G., Gard, T., Hoge, E. A., Hölzel, B. K., Kerr, C., Lazar, S. W., et al. (2015). Moving beyond mindfulness: Defining equanimity as an outcome measure in meditation and contemplative research. Mindfulness 6, 356–372. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0269-8
Dienes, Z. (2012). “Is hypnotic responding the strategic relinquishment of metacognition?,” in Foundations of metacognition, eds M. J. Beran, J. Brandl, J. Perner, and J. Proust (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 267–278. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199646739.003.0017
Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: A MEG neurophenomenological study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:582. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582
Dutta, A. (2025). Bayesian predictive coding hypothesis: Brain as observer’s key role in insight. Med. Hypotheses 195:111546. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2024.111546
Feruglio, S., Matiz, A., Pagnoni, G., Fabbro, F., and Crescentini, C. (2021). The impact of mindfulness meditation on the wandering mind: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 131, 313–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.032
Frick, A., Thinnes, I., and Stangier, U. (2020). Metta-based group meditation and individual cognitive behavioral therapy (MeCBT) for chronic depression: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 21:20. doi: 10.1186/s13063-019-3815-4
Gamaiunova, L., Kreibig, S. D., Dan-Glauser, E., Pellerin, N., Brandt, P.-Y., and Kliegel, M. (2022). Effects of two mindfulness based interventions on the distinct phases of the stress response across different physiological systems. Biol. Psychol. 172:108384. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108384
Guendelman, S., Medeiros, S., and Rampes, H. (2017). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: Insights from neurobiological, psychological, and clinical studies. Front. Psychol. 8:220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00220
Guidotti, R., D’Andrea, A., Basti, A., Raffone, A., Pizzella, V., and Marzetti, L. (2023). Long-Term and meditation-specific modulations of brain connectivity revealed through multivariate pattern analysis. Brain Topogr. 36, 409–418. doi: 10.1007/s10548-023-00950-3
Hadash, Y., Segev, N., Tanay, G., Goldstein, P., and Bernstein, A. (2016). The decoupling model of equanimity: Theory, measurement, and test in a mindfulness intervention. Mindfulness 7, 1214–1226. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0564-2
Hirayama, M. S., Milani, D., Rodrigues, R. C. M., Barros, N. F. D., and Alexandre, N. M. C. (2014). A percepção de comportamentos relacionados à atenção plena e a versão brasileira do Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory. Ciênc. Saúde Colet. 19, 3899–3914. doi: 10.1590/1413-81232014199.12272013
Hofmann, S. G., Petrocchi, N., Steinberg, J., Lin, M., Arimitsu, K., Kind, S., et al. (2015). Loving-Kindness meditation to target affect in mood disorders: A proof-of-concept study. Evidence-Based Compl. Altern. Med. 2015:269126. doi: 10.1155/2015/269126
Holroyd, J. (2003). The science of meditation and the state of hypnosis. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 46, 109–128. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2003.10403582
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., and Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 109–119. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.2.109
Jo, H.-G., Hinterberger, T., Wittmann, M., and Schmidt, S. (2015). Do meditators have higher awareness of their intentions to act? Cortex 65, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.015
Kelly, B. D. (2023). Mindful, mindless, or misunderstood? A critical perspective of the mindfulness concept. Ir. J. Psychol. Med. 40, 491–493. doi: 10.1017/ipm.2022.31
Kemmer, P. B., Guo, Y., Wang, Y., and Pagnoni, G. (2015). Network-based characterization of brain functional connectivity in Zen practitioners. Front. Psychol. 6:603. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00603
Krägeloh, C. U. (2018). “Mindfulness, heedfulness, and ethics,” in Handbook of ethical foundations of mindfulness, eds S. Stanley, R. E. Purser, and N. N. Singh (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 85–100. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-76538-9_5
Laukkonen, R. E., and Slagter, H. A. (2021). From many to (n)one: Meditation and the plasticity of the predictive mind. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 199–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.021
Laukkonen, R. E., Sacchet, M. D., Barendregt, H., Devaney, K. J., Chowdhury, A., and Slagter, H. A. (2023). Cessations of consciousness in meditation: Advancing a scientific understanding of nirodha samāpatti. Prog. Brain Res. 280, 61–87. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2022.12.007
Lehman, B. J., Fogel, C. A., and Wiederaenders, E. (2025). How meditation promotes well-being: Applying a dual-system theory. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Comp. 19:e70037. doi: 10.1111/spc3.70037
Lippelt, D. P., Hommel, B., and Colzato, L. S. (2014). Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: Effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity – A review. Front. Psychol. 5:1083. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01083
Lush, P., and Dienes, Z. (2019). Time perception and the experience of agency in meditation and hypnosis. Psych. J. 8, 36–50. doi: 10.1002/pchj.276
Lush, P., Naish, P., and Dienes, Z. (2016). Metacognition of intentions in mindfulness and hypnosis. Neurosci. Conscious. 2016:niw007. doi: 10.1093/nc/niw007
Lutz, A., Abdoun, O., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Trautwein, F.-M., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2024). An overview of neurophenomenological approaches to meditation and their relevance to clinical research. Biol. Psychiatry: Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimag. 10, 411–424. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.11.008
Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., and Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 16369–16373. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407401101
Lutz, A., Mattout, J., and Pagnoni, G. (2019). The epistemic and pragmatic value of non-action: A predictive coding perspective on meditation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.019
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005
Manjaly, Z.-M., and Iglesias, S. (2020). A computational theory of mindfulness based cognitive therapy from the “Bayesian Brain” perspective. Front. Psychiatry 11:404. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00404
Neri, B., Callara, A. L., Vanello, N., Menicucci, D., Zaccaro, A., Piarulli, A., et al. (2024). Report from a tibetan monastery: EEG neural correlates of concentrative and analytical meditation. Front. Psychol. 15:1348317. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1348317
Neves-Pereira, M. S., Carvalho, M. A. B. D., and Aspesi, C. D. C. (2018). Mindfulness and Buddhism: Contributions of Buddhist philosophical and religious teachings to the fields of psychology and education. Gifted Educ. Int. 34, 144–154. doi: 10.1177/0261429417716347
Nyklíček, I. (2020). Aspects of self-awareness in meditators and meditation-naïve participants: Self-report versus task performance. Mindfulness 11, 1028–1037. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01318-5
Penazzi, G., and De Pisapia, N. (2022). Direct comparisons between hypnosis and meditation: A mini-review. Front. Psychol. 13:958185. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.958185
Poletti, S., Bauer, P., and Lutz, A. (2024). Worldviews from within: A qualitative investigation of metaphysical and ethical beliefs among european long-term buddhist practitioners and novice mindfulness practitioners. Mindfulness 15, 2647–2667. doi: 10.1007/s12671-024-02448-w
Raffone, A., Marzetti, L., Del Gratta, C., Perrucci, M. G., Romani, G. L., and Pizzella, V. (2019). Toward a brain theory of meditation. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 207–232. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.028
Rohde, J. S., Goldy, S., Graziosi, M., Ferguson, M. A., Thrul, J., and Yaden, D. B. (2024). Centering and flourishing: An online intervention study assessing the effects of a Christian contemplative practice on stress-reduction and human flourishing. BMC Psychol. 12:373. doi: 10.1186/s40359-024-01836-0
Sharf, R. H. (2015). Is mindfulness Buddhist? (and why it matters). Transcult. Psychiatry 52, 470–484. doi: 10.1177/1363461514557561
Shulman, E. (2024). Ethics, mindfulness, and consciousness: A study of their relation in early Buddhism. Mindfulness 15, 2415–2427. doi: 10.1007/s12671-024-02434-2
Stangier, U., Frick, A., Thinnes, I., Arens, E. A., and Hofmann, S. G. (2021). Metta-Based therapy for chronic depression: A wait list control trial. Mindfulness 12, 2929–2942. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01753-y
Stefan, S., and Hofmann, S. G. (2019). Integrating metta into cbt: How loving kindness and compassion meditation can enhance CBT for treating anxiety and depression. CPE 1:e32941. doi: 10.32872/cpe.v1i3.32941
Trautwein, F.-M., Naranjo, J. R., and Schmidt, S. (2016). Decentering the self? Reduced bias in self- vs. other-related processing in long-term practitioners of loving-kindness meditation. Front. Psychol. 7:1785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01785
Travis, F., and Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: Categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 1110–1118. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.007
Trousselard, M., Steiler, D., Claverie, D., and Canini, F. (2014). L’histoire de la mindfulness à l’épreuve des données actuelles de la littérature?: Questions en suspens. L’Encéphale 40, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2014.08.006
Vago, D. R., and Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296
Van Gordon, W., and Shonin, E. (2020). Second-Generation mindfulness-based interventions: Toward more authentic mindfulness practice and teaching. Mindfulness 11, 1–4. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01252-1
Wipplinger, F., Holthof, N., Andereggen, L., Urman, R. D., Luedi, M. M., and Bello, C. (2023). Meditation as an adjunct to the management of acute pain. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 27, 209–216. doi: 10.1007/s11916-023-01119-0
Wright, M. J., Sanguinetti, J. L., Young, S., and Sacchet, M. D. (2023). Uniting contemplative theory and scientific investigation: Toward a comprehensive model of the mind. Mindfulness 14, 1088–1101. doi: 10.1007/s12671-023-02101-y
Yang, W. F. Z., Chowdhury, A., Sparby, T., and Sacchet, M. D. (2025). Deconstructing the self and reshaping perceptions: An intensive whole-brain 7T MRI case study of the stages of insight during advanced investigative insight meditation. NeuroImage 305:120968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120968
Yang, W. F. Z., Sparby, T., Wright, M., Kim, E., and Sacchet, M. D. (2024). Volitional mental absorption in meditation: Toward a scientific understanding of advanced concentrative absorption meditation and the case of jhana. Heliyon 10:e31223. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e31223
Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Mauro, F., Nicolardi, V., Simione, L., Calabrese, L., et al. (2020). Common and distinct lateralised patterns of neural coupling during focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation. Sci. Rep. 10:7430. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-64324-6
Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Nicolardi, V., Simione, L., Mauro, F., Garberi, P., et al. (2021). Attentional and cognitive monitoring brain networks in long-term meditators depend on meditation states and expertise. Sci. Rep. 11:4909. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84325-3
Keywords: Buddhist meditation, consciousness transformation, psychological mechanisms, metacognition, therapeutic efficacy, cultural sensitivity
Citation: Wang C (2025) Beyond mindfulness: how Buddhist meditation transforms consciousness through distinct psychological pathways. Front. Psychol. 16:1649564. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1649564
Received: 18 June 2025; Accepted: 17 July 2025;
Published: 04 August 2025.
Edited by:
Selenia Di Fronso, eCampus University, ItalyReviewed by:
A. C. Shivaram, Swami Vivekananda Yoga Anusandhana Samsthana, IndiaCopyright © 2025 Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Cheng Wang, d2FuZ2NoZW5nemp1QGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==