Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 September 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1669915

This article is part of the Research TopicPsychological Factors in Physical Education and Sport - Volume VIView all 20 articles

The relationship between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children: a stratified analysis by age and sex

  • 1School of Physical Education, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 2College of Sports Industry and Leisure, Nanjing Sport Institute, Nanjing, China
  • 3Department of Sports Science, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

Objective: This study aimed to systematically examine the relationship between fundamental motor skills (FMS) and executive function (EF) in Chinese children aged 3 to 10 years. The study specifically focused on age and sex differences, as well as the associations between subcomponents of FMS and EF, to provide empirical evidence for research and intervention strategies targeting motor-cognitive integration in child development.

Methods: A cross-sectional study design was employed, utilizing multi-stage stratified cluster sampling to recruit 2,179 children aged 3 to 10 years from eight cities across four eastern provinces in China (Jiangsu, Zhejiang, Anhui, and Shandong). FMS were assessed using the Test of Gross Motor Development-Third Edition (TGMD-3), which evaluates two major domains: locomotor skills and object control skills. EF was measured using the Childhood Executive Functioning Inventory (CHEXI), a parent-report scale that includes four subcomponents: working memory, inhibition, regulating ability, and planning ability. Statistical analyses included correlation analysis, and hierarchical regression modeling to examine both overall and stratified associations between FMS and EF by age and sex.

Results: A significant negative correlation was found between fundamental motor skills and executive function, indicating that higher motor skill levels were associated with better EF performance. Age-stratified analyses revealed that this association was significantly stronger in the 3–5 years group compared to the 6–10 years group, demonstrating clear age specificity. Sex-stratified results showed that for boys, the association between object control skills and EF, as well as between inhibition and FMS, was stronger. In contrast, girls exhibited significant associations between locomotor skills and EF, inhibition and FMS, and regulating ability and FMS.

Conclusion: This study confirms significant associations between fundamental motor skills and executive function in Chinese children, revealing distinct age-specific and sex-dependent patterns. The findings emphasize the critical importance of implementing targeted motor interventions during key developmental periods, providing both theoretical support and practical guidance for promoting the coordinated development of motor competence and higher-order cognitive functions in children.

1 Introduction

Childhood constitutes a critical period for the development of motor competence (MC) (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2022; Albuquerque et al., 2022; Zhou and Tolmie, 2024; Van Fels et al., 2019). During this phase, children’s MC is primarily reflected in their proficiency in fundamental motor skills (FMS) (Cattuzzo et al., 2016; Utesch et al., 2019), which play a pivotal role in facilitating language, cognitive, and social development (Malambo et al., 2022; Rosenbaum et al., 2001; Bar-Haim and Bart, 2006; Iverson, 2010; Gandotra et al., 2023; Macdonald et al., 2020). FMS are culturally acquired foundational motor patterns that form the neurobehavioral basis for engaging in complex physical activities and sport-specific skills (Barnett et al., 2016). Most research globally categorizes FMS into three main domains: locomotor skills, object control skills, and stability skills (Xin et al., 2019; Gabbard, 2021; Diao, 2018; Rudd et al., 2015). A large body of evidence confirms that FMS not only provide the foundation for more complex motor tasks but also directly influence lifelong participation in physical activities and the adoption of a healthy lifestyle (Stodden et al., 2008; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2022; Zhou and Tolmie, 2024; Gu et al., 2021). The development of FMS is influenced by biological factors such as age and sex (Gandotra et al., 2023; Navarro-Patón et al., 2021b; Chichinina et al., 2025), and may also involve key elements of executive function (EF), which is considered a core foundation of overall child development (Garon et al., 2008). Since Piaget’s theory of the motor-cognition linkage, the Northern Finland Birth Cohort study (1966) has demonstrated that early walking ability significantly predicts executive function 35 years later (Murray et al., 2006; Ridler et al., 2006). As research in this area has advanced into the 21st century, scholars have increasingly focused on investigating the reciprocal interactions between FMS and EF during early childhood (Diamond, 2000). Understanding these interactions holds significant theoretical and practical value for developing evidence-based interventions and optimizing child development outcomes.

Executive function (EF) serves as a core cognitive mechanism in child development (Van Fels et al., 2019), playing a determinant role in key developmental indicators such as psychological well-being, academic achievement, and social adaptation (Albuquerque et al., 2022). Research has shown that during childhood, EF significantly predicts the quality of transition to formal education by supporting the development of behavioral competencies, preschool skill acquisition, and social relationship formation (Willoughby and Hudson, 2023). EF refers to cognitive processes involved in purposeful, goal-directed behavior (Stuss, 1992), achieved through the coordination of fundamental cognitive domains, including language, attention, perception, and motor functions (Neisser, 2014), These processes enable problem-solving, reasoning, planning, and behavioral regulation (Isquith et al., 2005). There is broad consensus that EF encompasses core components such as inhibitory control, working memory, cognitive flexibility, attentional monitoring, planning initiation, and self-regulation (Miyake and Friedman, 2012; Goldstein, 2014). A among these, Miyake et al. (2000) tripartite model, which includes working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility, is widely accepted. Recently, researchers have increasingly highlighted planning capacity as a vital component of EF (Diamond, 2013; Anderson and Reidy, 2012). The Childhood Executive Functioning Inventory (CHEXI), developed based on Barkley’s (1997) hybrid model (Thorell and Nyberg, 2008; Camerota et al., 2018; Moura et al., 2025; Wei et al., 2018), assesses EF through four factors: working memory, planning ability, regulation, and inhibition.

Existing research has established significant age-dependent associations between FMS and EF (Albuquerque et al., 2022; Libertus and Hauf, 2017; Luz et al., 2015; Cook et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2022), with particularly pronounced linkages in younger children that progressively attenuate with advancing age (Albuquerque et al., 2022). This attenuation trajectory may arise from two key neurodevelopmental mechanisms. First, core EF-related brain regions—especially the prefrontal cortex—undergo considerably prolonged developmental cycles compared to motor cortices (Davis et al., 2011), leading to a relative decline in the direct contribution of EF to motor performance after the preschool years (Stuhr et al., 2020). Second, the synergistic relationships among EF subcomponents, such as working memory and inhibitory control, follow a nonlinear developmental trajectory (Brocki and Bohlin, 2004), with significantly stronger coupling of working memory and inhibition observed in 6- to 8-year-olds compared to 9- to 11-year-olds (Spedden et al., 2017). Separately, critical motor milestones in infancy (such as independent walking) significantly predict EF performance in adulthood (Ridler et al., 2006), suggesting a potential neurodevelopmental covariation between early motor experiences and later cognitive abilities. Overall, current research on the relationship between motor and cognitive development predominantly focuses on school-aged children, limiting the generalizability of findings to preschool populations (Gandotra et al., 2023). To fully elucidate the age-specific nature of this association, it is essential to examine a broader age range. Accordingly, this study investigates children aged 3 to 10 years, encompassing both preschool and school-age developmental phases, with the goal of analyzing the age-related characteristics of the FMS-EF association.

Furthermore, existing research suggests that sex-based differences exist in the association patterns between FMS and EF (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2022). Typically, boys exhibit stronger overall FMS-EF linkages than girls (Yang et al., 2022). At the subcomponent level, boys show significant associations between object control skills and EF, whereas girls demonstrate stronger locomotor-EF linkages (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2022; Hirata et al., 2018; Ke et al., 2020; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2023; Navarro-Patón et al., 2021a; Mecías-Calvo et al., 2021). Notably, self-regulatory ability within EF is exclusively associated with girls’ FMS proficiency (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2015). Overall, current evidence on sex-differentiated FMS-EF associations is limited, particularly regarding subcomponent-specific relationships. Therefore, sex-stratified analysis of children aged 3–10 years is a secondary research priority of this study, aimed at clarifying sex-dependent characteristics of FMS-EF linkages.

This study systematically examines the age- and sex-dependent associations between FMS and EF in Chinese children aged 3–10 years, using a cross-sectional design. This is the first such investigation conducted in this population. Using standardized assessments, we evaluated locomotor and object control skills (FMS subdomains) (Xin et al., 2019; Gabbard, 2021; Diao, 2018; Rudd et al., 2015) alongside core EF components: working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and planning capacity (Miyake and Friedman, 2012; Goldstein, 2014). Guided by sensitive period theory in motor development and cognitive developmental stage theory, participants were stratified into two theoretically-informed groups: 3-5-year-olds (sensorimotor exploration and self-identity construction phase) and 6-10-year-olds (cognitive system transformation and motor specialization phase). Based on this theoretical framework, we propose the following hypotheses:

1. Significant bidirectional associations exist between FMS and EF in 3-10-year-olds, where FMS potentially influences EF development, and reciprocally, EF potentially influences FMS development.

2. FMS-EF association patterns demonstrate age-specific characteristics across developmental phases.

3. FMS-EF association patterns exhibit sex-dependent characteristics.

2 Methods

2.1 Study design and participants

This study enrolled 2,560 Chinese children aged 3–10 years using a multistage stratified cluster sampling method.

Stage 1: Geographical and Socioeconomic Stratification. Four eastern provinces (Jiangsu, Zhejiang, Anhui, Shandong) were selected based on regional economic distribution. Within each province, one high-income and one low-income city were identified using per capita disposable income data, resulting in eight cities: Changzhou, Yancheng, Hangzhou, Taizhou, Wuhu, Anqing, Jinan, and Liaocheng.

Stage 2: Educational Institution Stratification. In each city, one primary school and one kindergarten were selected from both urban and suburban districts, yielding a total of 32 institutions.

Stage 3: Classroom-level Cluster Sampling. From each selected classroom, 20–25 children were randomly recruited, resulting in 2,560 participants.

The minimum required sample size was determined using G*Power software (version 3.1.9.7) (Faul et al., 2009). Based on parameters for a two-tailed correlation test (effect size f2 = 0.02, α = 0.05, statistical power [1-β] = 0.95, and a maximum of 5 predictor variables for hierarchical regression), the calculated minimum sample size was 776 participants. Therefore, the final sample size was adjusted to account for the design effect associated with cluster sampling and potential missing data.

Ethical approval was granted by the Shanghai University of Sport Institutional Review Board (Approval No: 102772021RT072). Written informed consent was obtained from all legal guardians, and strict adherence to voluntary participation principles was maintained throughout the study.

2.2 Procedures

All participants underwent assessments for both fundamental motor skills (FMS) and executive function (EF). FMS were evaluated using the Test of Gross Motor Development, Third Edition (TGMD-3). During the assessment, a strategic arrangement of six to eight high-definition cameras was set up within the testing area to capture the movements of all 2,560 children. The testing was conducted by a team of four to six graduate students specializing in physical education. These individuals were responsible for participant registration, test administration, operation of the multiple camera units, and providing standardized demonstrations of the required motor skills. Subsequently, three graduate students, who had received specific training in TGMD-3 administration, independently scored the recorded performances based on standardized scoring criteria for locomotor skills (e.g., running and hopping) and object control skills (e.g., overhand throwing and two-hand catching).

Executive function was measured using paper-based questionnaires. These questionnaires were distributed and completed immediately after the motor skills assessment. A total of 2,230 completed questionnaires were collected, yielding a response rate of 87.1%. After performing quality control checks for completeness and logical consistency, 51 invalid questionnaires were excluded. This resulted in 2,179 valid questionnaires, representing an effective response rate of 97.7%. Ultimately, complete data sets, encompassing both FMS testing and EF assessment, were obtained for 2,179 children (1,079 boys and 1,100 girls), ensuring the integrity of the data.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Anthropometry

Demographic information was collected for each participating child, including sex, age, ethnicity, height, weight, and body mass index (BMI).

2.3.2 Fundamental motor skills

Fundamental motor skills were assessed using the Test of Gross Motor Development, Third Edition (TGMD-3). This instrument is designed for evaluating children aged 3 to 10 years and consists of two subtests: locomotor skills and object control skills, which together encompass a total of thirteen test items. Each test item includes three to five performance criteria. One point is awarded for each criterion successfully met during a trial, and zero points are awarded if a criterion is not met. Each item is administered twice, with a maximum possible score of 100 points. The TGMD-3 enables comparisons both between individuals and across groups and is suitable for use in research and educational settings. The instrument has demonstrated satisfactory applicability for Chinese children aged 3 to 12 years (Li et al., 2022). Internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) for the fundamental motor skill scores within each age group ranged from 0.808 to 0.902, with coefficients reaching 0.95 for both male and female subgroups, indicating good internal reliability.

Furthermore, to evaluate inter-rater reliability specifically for this study (as presented in Table 1), three independent raters scored video recordings of a random sample of fifty children. The resulting ICCs, calculated from this study’s data, showed high inter-rater agreement, with all intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) exhibiting statistical significance (p < 0.001). Specifically, ICCs for the locomotor skills subtest total scores ranged from 0.822 to 0.914, ICCs for the object control skills subtest total scores ranged from 0.886 to 0.940, and ICCs for the overall TGMD-3 total scores ranged from 0.931 to 0.953. These findings support the TGMD-3 as a valid and reliable assessment tool for evaluating fundamental motor skill development in Chinese children (Li et al., 2022).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Inter-rater reliability coefficients for TGMD-3 scoring.

2.3.3 Executive function

Executive function was assessed using the Childhood Executive Functioning Inventory (CHEXI) (Wei et al., 2018). This instrument is designed for use with populations ranging from preschool to adolescence and is completed by parents or teachers. The CHEXI systematically evaluates four core components of executive function through a total of 24 items, organized into subscales measuring working memory, planning ability, regulatory ability, and inhibitory control. Responses are recorded using a five-point Likert scale (1 = completely untrue, 5 = completely true). Subscale scores and a total score are calculated separately. The instrument employs reverse scoring, with higher scores indicating poorer executive function, and therefore, lower scores represent better executive function.

Among Chinese children, the CHEXI has demonstrated satisfactory reliability, with Cronbach’s α coefficients for the working memory, regulatory ability, and inhibitory control subscales ranging from 0.71 to 0.89. These psychometric properties confirm its suitability for early childhood development research and clinical assessment of executive function (Tsai et al., 2020; Wei et al., 2018).

2.3.4 Statistical analysis

Data management and statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 26.0 (IBM, Chicago, IL, United States), with statistical significance set at p < 0.05. Descriptive statistics were used to analyze the total scores of FMS, EF, as well as their subcomponents, along with variables such as sex, age, body mass index (BMI), geographical region, and ethnicity. Pearson correlation analysis was employed to assess the relationship between FMS and EF. Hierarchical linear regression modeling was used to explore the bidirectional predictive relationships between FMS and EF. Specifically, this analysis assessed both the potential predictive relationship of FMS on EF and the reciprocal predictive relationship of EF on FMS. Further analyses examined variations in these relationships across different sex and age groups. In the hierarchical regression analysis, Model 1 included only control variables (BMI, region, and sex) to evaluate the influence of these covariates on the dependent variables. Model 2 added the core independent variable (either FMS or EF) to examine its independent contribution to the dependent variables after controlling for covariates.

3 Results

3.1 Sample characteristics

Sample characteristics are presented in Table 2. Children demonstrated a mean FMS total score of 68.74 ± 13.91, with locomotor skills at 35.11 ± 7.07 and object control skills at 33.64 ± 8.50. The mean EF total score was 58.79 ± 15.04, comprising working memory (22.08 ± 6.10), planning ability (11.39 ± 2.97), regulating ability (12.03 ± 3.71), and inhibition (13.29 ± 4.19).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of demographic information and independent and dependent variables.

3.2 Correlational analysis

Correlation analysis results (Figure 1) revealed significant negative correlations between fundamental motor skills (total score, locomotor skills, and object control skills) and executive function (total score and all subcomponents: working memory, planning ability, regulatory ability, inhibitory ability) in 3–10-year-old children (r = −0.06 to −0.12, p < 0.05), indicating small effect sizes per Cohen’s criteria (r = 0.10 for small effect) (Cohen, 2013). Notably, the strongest negative correlation was observed between inhibitory ability and locomotor skills (r = −0.12, p < 0.01), approaching a small-to-medium effect size (r = 0.30 for medium effect). Additionally, high positive intercorrelations were found among all executive function subcomponents (r = 0.61 to 0.92, p < 0.01), representing large to very large effect sizes (r = 0.50 for large effect).

Figure 1
Correlation matrix heatmap showing relationships between variables such as EF_Total, Inhibition, and Object_control. Correlation values range from -0.12 to 0.92, with stronger correlations in red and weaker in purple.

Figure 1. Correlation analysis between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children.

3.3 The relationship between fundamental movement skills and executive function in children: age stratification

Hierarchical regression analyses (Table 3) demonstrated that when executive function served as the dependent variable in the 3–5-year-old group, Model 1 (control variables) explained 1.9% of the variance (F = 5.602, p < 0.01). Following the addition of fundamental motor skills in Model 2, the explained variance significantly increased to 4.4% (F = 9.625, p < 0.001), with fundamental motor skills exhibiting a significant negative association (β = −0.157, t = −4.614, p < 0.001). In the 6–10-year-old group, Model 1 accounted for 1.3% of the variance (F = 5.593, p < 0.001). After incorporating fundamental motor skills in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 1.7% (F = 5.702, p < 0.001), and fundamental motor skills maintained a significant negative association (β = −0.069, t = −2.442, p < 0.05). When fundamental motor skills were designated as the dependent variable, Model 1 explained 2.0% of the variance in the 3–5-year-old group (F = 6.340, p < 0.001). After adding executive function in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 4.9% (F = 9.814, p < 0.001), with executive function showing a significant negative association (β = −0.157, t = −4.614, p < 0.001). For the 6–10-year-old group, Model 1 accounted for 7.7% of the variance (F = 17.258, p < 0.001). Following the inclusion of executive function in Model 2, the explained variance reached 8.1% (F = 29.164, p < 0.001), and executive function retained a significant negative association (β = −0.065, t = −2.442, p < 0.05).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cross-sectional association between fundamental motor skills and executive function: hierarchical regression analysis by age group.

Further analysis examining subcomponents (Table 4) revealed that when executive function was the dependent variable in the 3–5-year-old group, Model 1 explained 1.9% of the variance (F = 5.602, p < 0.001). After adding locomotor skills in Model 2, the explained variance significantly increased to 4.4% (F = 7.710, p < 0.001), with locomotor skills demonstrating a significant negative association (β = −0.106, t = −2.620, p < 0.01). In the 6–10-year-old group, Model 1 accounted for 1.3% of the variance (F = 5.593, p < 0.001). Following the inclusion of object control skills in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 1.8% (F = 4.830, p < 0.001), and object control skills showed a significant negative association (β = −0.076, t = −2.508, p < 0.01). When fundamental motor skills served as the dependent variable in the 3–5-year-old group, Model 1 explained 2.0% of the variance (F = 5.848, p < 0.001). After adding inhibitory ability in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 5.0% (F = 6.340, p < 0.001), with inhibitory ability exhibiting a significant negative association (β = −0.158, t = −2.747, p < 0.001). In the 6–10-year-old group, Model 1 accounted for 7.7% of the variance (F = 36.760, p < 0.001). Following the incorporation of working memory in Model 2, the explained variance reached 8.4% (F = 17.258, p < 0.001), and working memory demonstrated a significant negative association (β = −0.088, t = −1.961, p < 0.001).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Cross-sectional association between subcomponents of fundamental motor skills and executive function: hierarchical regression analysis by age group.

3.4 Associations between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children: stratified by sex

Hierarchical regression analyses (Table 5) demonstrated that when executive function served as the dependent variable in the male group, Model 1 (control variables) explained 0.9% of the variance (F = 3.301, p < 0.05). Following the addition of fundamental motor skills in Model 2, the explained variance significantly increased to 1.6% (F = 4.408, p < 0.01), with fundamental motor skills exhibiting a significant negative predictive association (β = −0.123, t = −2.769, p < 0.01). In the female group, Model 1 accounted for 1.7% of the variance (F = 6.229, p < 0.001). After incorporating fundamental motor skills in Model 2, the explained variance reached 2.0% (F = 5.703, p < 0.001), and fundamental motor skills maintained a significant negative association (β = −0.090, t = −2.018, p < 0.01). When fundamental motor skills were designated as the dependent variable in the male group, Model 1 explained 53.4% of the variance (F = 411.167, p < 0.001). After adding executive function in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 53.8% (F = 312.205, p < 0.001), with executive function showing a significant negative association (β = −0.058, t = −2.769, p < 0.01). In the female group, Model 1 accounted for 55.3% of the variance (F = 452.584, p < 0.001). Following the inclusion of executive function in Model 2, the explained variance reached 55.5% (F = 341.408, p < 0.001), and executive function retained a significant negative association (β = −0.041, t = −2.018, p < 0.01).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Cross-sectional association between fundamental motor skills and executive function: hierarchical regression analysis by sex.

Further subcomponent analyses (Table 6) revealed that when executive function was the dependent variable in the male group, Model 1 explained 0.9% of the variance (F = 3.301, p < 0.01). After adding object control skills in Model 2, the explained variance significantly increased to 1.7% (F = 4.650, p < 0.001), with object control skills demonstrating a significant negative association (β = −0.132, t = −2.937, p < 0.01). In the female group, Model 1 accounted for 1.7% of the variance (F = 6.229, p < 0.001). Following the inclusion of locomotor skills in Model 2, the explained variance reached 2.2% (F = 6.020, p < 0.001), and locomotor skills showed a significant negative association (β = −0.082, t = −2.306, p < 0.05). When fundamental motor skills served as the dependent variable in the male group, Model 1 explained 53.4% of the variance (F = 411.167, p < 0.001). After adding inhibitory ability in Model 2, the explained variance increased to 53.9% (F = 313.929, p < 0.001), with inhibitory ability exhibiting a significant negative association (β = −0.069, t = −3.298, p < 0.01). In the female group, Model 1 accounted for 55.3% of the variance (F = 452.584, p < 0.001). Following the incorporation of inhibitory ability in Model 2, the explained variance reached 55.8% (F = 344.978, p < 0.001), and inhibitory ability maintained a significant negative association (β = −0.066, t = −3.233, p < 0.01). When regulatory ability was further added in Model 3, the explained variance increased to 56.0% (F = 277.677, p < 0.001). The magnitude of association for inhibitory ability strengthened (β = −0.114, t = −3.689, p < 0.001), while regulatory ability showed a positive association (β = 0.064, t = 2.075, p < 0.05).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Cross-sectional associations between subcomponents of fundamental motor skills and executive function: hierarchical regression analyses stratified by sex.

4 Discussion

This study revealed a significant negative association between fundamental motor skills and executive function, with higher scores in fundamental motor skills corresponding to lower scores in executive function, and conversely, lower fundamental motor skills scores being associated with higher executive function scores. This relationship demonstrated age-specific patterns, with stronger associations observed in the 3–5-year-old group compared to the 6–10-year-old group. Subcomponent analyses further revealed that among preschool-aged children, locomotor skills exhibited significant associations with executive function, while inhibitory ability was significantly associated with fundamental motor skills. Conversely, in school-aged children, object control skills demonstrated significant associations with executive function, and working memory showed significant associations with fundamental motor skills. Sex-stratified analyses indicated that the reciprocal relationship between overall fundamental motor skills and executive function was stronger in boys than in girls. Subcomponent associations also exhibited sex-specific patterns: Object control skills were significantly associated with executive function, and inhibitory ability was significantly associated with fundamental motor skills in boys; whereas locomotor skills showed significant associations with executive function, and both inhibitory ability and regulatory ability demonstrated significant associations with fundamental motor skills in girls.

4.1 The relationship between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children

This study identified a significant association between fundamental motor skills and executive function among Chinese children aged 3 to 10 years, a finding consistent with association patterns reported in existing literature (Diamond, 2000; Stöckel and Hughes, 2016). The direction of this motor-cognitive relationship aligns with prior evidence from preschool-aged populations (Cook et al., 2019; Yang et al., 2022; Vanhala et al., 2023; Jylänki et al., 2022; Gashaj et al., 2019; Han et al., 2022; Veldman et al., 2019; Albuquerque et al., 2022; Niederer et al., 2011; Piek et al., 2008; Niederer et al., 2011; Willoughby et al., 2021). Several studies have similarly documented association patterns between executive function and fundamental motor skills (Stuhr et al., 2020; Vanhala, 2024; Zelazo et al., 2016; Zelazo and Carlson, 2020; Mcclelland et al., 2013; Adolph and Hoch, 2019), a relationship implicating cognitive processes engaged during complex motor tasks, such as cognitive activities involved in creating and adapting motor plans according to task demands (Best, 2010). Current theoretical frameworks provide multiple explanatory perspectives for understanding this relationship: The reciprocal theory posits that motor and cognitive skills develop synergistically through environmental interactions, whereby movement experiences enhance motor competence, thereby improving environmental interaction efficacy, ultimately facilitating higher-order cognitive development (Kim et al., 2018; Maurer and Roebers, 2019). Furthermore, the automaticity theory proposes that complex skill acquisition depends on foundational skill automation. When tasks simultaneously demand motor and cognitive resources, these compete for limited attentional capacity. Automation of motor skills releases attentional resources to support cognitive task execution (Kim et al., 2018; Maurer and Roebers, 2019). Furthermore, neuroscientific evidence reveals shared neural substrates in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, and connecting structures underlying both fundamental motor skills and executive function (Diamond, 2000; Kim et al., 2018). Neuroimaging studies further delineate neural signatures of this motor-cognitive relationship, demonstrating cerebellar-prefrontal co-activation during complex tasks requiring rapid responses and focused attention, reflecting integrated motor coordination and executive processing (Diamond, 2000; Tomporowski et al., 2015). Consequently, the observation that children with higher fundamental motor skills exhibited lower executive function scores (indicating better executive performance, given the reverse scoring methodology of the executive function measure) demonstrates a statistically significant negative association. This reciprocal association pattern not only supports neurobiological evidence of motor-executive covariation during child development but also aligns with the aforementioned theoretical frameworks (reciprocal theory, automaticity theory, and neural foundations) (Diamond, 2000; Kim et al., 2018; Maurer and Roebers, 2019). This study further reveals distinctive bidirectional association patterns between fundamental motor skills and executive function in Chinese children aged 3–10 years, thereby expanding the evidentiary base in this field. Whereas traditional research often emphasizes unidirectional relationship hypotheses (e.g., motor-to-cognition or cognition-to-motor exclusively), our cross-sectional data demonstrate a significant, reciprocal statistical association. This reflects the statistical covariation between motor and cognitive capacities during early developmental stages. Given methodological variations in measurement tools across studies, we maintain cautious interpretation when comparing findings with related research.

4.2 Age-stratified association patterns between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children

A second key finding was the significant association patterns between executive function and fundamental motor skills across both age groups. Previous studies examining different developmental stages (Davis et al., 2011; Stuhr et al., 2020; Stöckel et al., 2017) have documented covariation between executive function and fundamental motor skills. Our findings extend this evidence by demonstrating age-dependent variation: stronger associations emerged in the 3-5-year-old group compared to the 6-10-year-old group, highlighting the moderating role of age in these relationship patterns.

Henri Wallon’s theoretical framework (Wallon, 1972; Shen and Wang, 2012) posits that children aged 3–5 years traverse a subjective period characterized by psychological transitions: diminishing role-play, strengthening self-assertion, active pursuit of competence validation, and eventual internalization of others’ strengths. Conversely, children aged 6–11 years enter an objective period where psychological focus shifts toward constructing systematic cognitive networks about the external world through synergistic social expansion and cognitive development. This perspective converges with Gallahue’s hourglass model of motor development (Gallahue and Donnelly, 2007; Salehi et al., 2017), which identifies ages 2–7 years as the peak environmental sensitivity period - a phase where fundamental motor skills (locomotion, manipulation, stability) achieve maximal neural plasticity through environmental input. Gabbard’s developmental continuum model further supports this view (Gabbard, 2021; Goodway et al., 2019), designating ages 2–7 years as the fundamental movement phase emphasizing critical pattern establishment, followed by the context-specific skill phase (7 years through adolescence) where foundational movements transform into specialized abilities. Subcomponent analyses revealed: Locomotor skills demonstrated associations with executive function in 3-5-year-olds; Object control skills showed associations with executive function in 6-10-year-olds Noting that boys aged 5–6 years typically outperform girls in object control assessments (Ke et al., 2020; Zheng et al., 2022; Honrubia-Montesinos and Losada-Puente, 2021). Executive function subcomponent analyses indicated: Inhibitory ability associated with fundamental motor skills in 3-5-year-olds; Working memory associated with fundamental motor skills in 6-10-year-olds. This finding aligns with Libertus’ proposition of age-dependent executive subcomponent specialization (Libertus and Hauf, 2017) and supports documented covariation patterns between motor competence and both inhibitory control and working memory in 5-6-year-olds within cross-sectional studies (Stöckel and Hughes, 2016). Piaget’s cognitive development theory (Pakpahan and Saragih, 2022) further elucidates this differentiation: the preoperational stage (3–7 years), characterized by egocentrism, features pronounced inhibitory control development, while the concrete operational stage (7–11 years), marked by logical thinking emergence, exhibits progressive working memory maturation.

Results demonstrated inhibitory ability associations with fundamental motor skills in 3-5-year-olds but not in 6-10-year-olds, whereas working memory associations emerged in the older group. This dissociation reflects heterogeneous developmental trajectories of executive subcomponents (Diamond, 2013; Best and Miller, 2010), wherein working memory shows progressive maturation during 6–10 years while inhibitory control develops most substantially during 3–5 years (Best and Miller, 2010). Supporting evidence comes from Koutsandréou et al. (2016) observation of working memory changes following 10-week motor interventions in 9-10-year-olds. These age-specific patterns likely represent distinct cognitive-motor integration signatures across developmental periods (Stuhr et al., 2020). Consistent with cognitive development frameworks (Best et al., 2009), inhibitory-motor associations predominate during 3–5 years while working memory-motor associations become prominent during 6–10 years. The persistence of working memory associations through preadolescence (Van Fels et al., 2019; Rigoli et al., 2012; Ludyga et al., 2018), aligns with its developmental trajectory and synergistic relationships with other executive components (Best and Miller, 2010; Hartung et al., 2020). When working memory reaches advanced developmental stages, other executive subcomponents may exhibit differential association patterns (Best et al., 2009). Our results exemplify this pattern through significant inhibitory-motor associations in early childhood (3–5 years) and working memory-motor associations in middle childhood (6–10 years).

4.3 Sex-stratified association patterns between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children

The third principal finding established statistically significant associations between executive function and fundamental motor skills in children of both genders (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2022), albeit with distinct gender-specific patterns. These differential patterns potentially arise from complex interactions involving sociocultural factors, environmental characteristics, and physical activity levels (LeGear et al., 2012). Boys demonstrated stronger overall association magnitudes than girls, with differential subdomain linkage characteristics: object control skills exhibited primary associations with executive function and inhibitory control in boys, whereas locomotor skills manifested principal associations with executive function, inhibitory control, and regulatory capacity in girls. This observation aligns with prior research documenting stronger correlations between fundamental motor skills and inhibitory control in boys (Yang et al., 2022), alongside evidence indicating superior performance among preschool girls in balance and manual dexterity assessments, contrasted with boys aged 5–6 years excelling in object control skill evaluations (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2022; Hirata et al., 2018; Ke et al., 2020; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2023; Navarro-Patón et al., 2021a; Mecías-Calvo et al., 2021; Zheng et al., 2022; Honrubia-Montesinos and Losada-Puente, 2021).

Such gender-differentiated patterns likely originate from multilevel interactions. At the neurobiological level, accelerated myelination processes within male sensorimotor cortices (particularly during ages 6–10 years) may underpin enhanced efficiency in fundamental movement execution. However, comparatively weaker prefrontal-limbic connectivity may increase reliance on foundational inhibitory control mechanisms (Sun et al., 2025; Johnson and De Haan, 2015). Conversely, girls may employ neural compensatory mechanisms integrating multimodal cognitive strategies, such as utilizing verbal encoding to facilitate motor learning (Gallahue and Donnelly, 2007). Socioculturally, gendered motor play preferences (e.g., boys’ frequent selection of ball-based activities) (Wang, 2022) and potential disparities in pedagogical practices (e.g., teachers prioritizing throwing skill instruction for boys) (Salvatori and Cherubini, 2024) persistently reinforce bio-social interactive effects. These interactions may selectively shape neural linkages between object manipulation skills and working memory in boys, while fostering associations between locomotor skills and multifaceted executive functions (e.g., inhibition and regulation) in girls.

These outcomes reflect the engagement profile of executive function components—particularly inhibitory control and regulatory capacity—during complex motor tasks, specifically within movement planning, online adjustment, sustained attentional focus, and environmental adaptation processes (Diamond, 2000). Current evidence substantiates inhibitory control, working memory (in certain studies), and regulatory capacity as salient correlates of motor skill proficiency. From an applied perspective, designing developmentally tailored interventions incorporating structured motor activities (e.g., aerobic exercise, martial arts), computerized cognitive training (e.g., N-back paradigms), or artistic group activities (e.g., drama, music), combined with healthy lifestyle practices (e.g., positive mindset, sufficient sleep) (Diamond and Ling, 2016; Diamond, 2012), may more effectively foster synergistic development of brain function and motor-cognitive abilities across genders. Motor-cognitive interventions require age-specific design, Preschoolers (3–5 years) benefit from locomotor activities with inhibitory challenges (e.g., rule-switching games), leveraging peak plasticity. School-aged children (6–10 years) need object-control tasks integrating working memory (e.g., tactical sports). Boy’s object-control advantage warrants gender-differentiated strategies. These theory-informed approaches transform statistical associations into targeted neuroeducation applications through movement-based pedagogy.

4.4 Limitations and implications

Notwithstanding its foundation in large-scale sample data and systematic analysis of age- and gender-stratified associations between fundamental motor skills (FMS) and executive function (EF) in children aged 3–10 years, this study exhibits limitations requiring consideration. The statistically significant negative FMS-EF correlations, while meaningful, demonstrated modest effect magnitudes (r = 0.06–0.12) within Cohen’s small-effect range (r = 0.10), suggesting real-world practical associations may be limited without synergistic dual-domain interventions. Firstly, the cross-sectional design precludes causal inference regarding developmental sequencing or underlying mechanisms between motor competence and executive function, necessitating longitudinal approaches to clarify dynamic interrelationships. Secondly, exclusive reliance on parent-reported EF measures risks reporting bias; future studies should implement multi-method assessments incorporating behavioral tasks and teacher ratings to enhance ecological validity. Finally, regional sampling from Eastern China may constrain generalizability, warranting validation across diverse geographical and cultural contexts to elucidate potential sociocultural influences on these association patterns.

5 Conclusion

This large-scale cross-sectional study examined age- and gender-stratified associations between fundamental motor skills and executive function in Chinese children aged 3–10 years. Analyses revealed bidirectional negative correlations between these domains, with stronger associations observed in preschoolers (3–5 years) compared to school-aged children (6–10 years). Distinct sex-specific patterns emerged in overall association strength and subcomponent-level relationships.

These findings demonstrate age-related and gender-based variations in motor-cognitive developmental linkages during childhood. The research contributes empirical evidence to understanding synergistic motor-executive development, underscoring the significance of concurrent engagement during sensitive periods.

Stratified patterns suggest developmentally tailored motor-cognitive engagement strategies: Preschool interventions could integrate locomotor activities with inhibitory challenges, while school-aged programs might combine object-control training with working memory tasks, considering sex-specific motor profiles. Such approaches may support integrated development during sensitive windows.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Institutional Review Board of Shanghai University of Sport (Approval No.: 102772021RT072). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

CK: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. KL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. ZW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. XW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. XZ: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by National Key Research and Development Program of China (2024YFC2707903).

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge all participants and investigators in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adolph, K. E., and Hoch, J. E. (2019). Motor development: embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70:141. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102836

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Albuquerque, M. R., Rennó, G. V. C., Bruzi, A. T., Fortes, L. S., and Malloy-Diniz, L. F. (2022). Association between motor competence and executive functions in children. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 11, 495–503. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2021.1897814

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. J., and Reidy, N. (2012). Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 22:345. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., and Bart, O. (2006). Motor function and social participation in kindergarten children. Soc. Dev. 15, 296–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00342.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett, L. M., Stodden, D., Cohen, K. E., Smith, J. J., Lubans, D. R., Lenoir, M., et al. (2016). Fundamental movement skills: an important focus. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 35, 219–225. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2014-0209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 30, 331–351. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 29, 180–200. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brocki, K. C., and Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: a dimensional and developmental study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 26, 571–593. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camerota, M., Willoughby, M. T., Kuhn, L. J., and Blair, C. B. (2018). The childhood executive functioning inventory (Chexi): factor structure, measurement invariance, and correlates in us preschoolers. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 322–337. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2016.1247795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattuzzo, M. T., Dos Santos Henrique, R., Ré, A. H. N., de Oliveira, I. S., Melo, B. M., de Sousa Moura, M., et al. (2016). Motor competence and health related physical fitness in youth: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 19, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chichinina, E., Gavrilova, M., and Drid, P. (2025). Executive function skills predict motor competence in preschool children. BMC Pediatr. 25:235. doi: 10.1186/s12887-025-05582-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cook, C. J., Howard, S. J., Scerif, G., Twine, R., Kahn, K., Norris, S. A., et al. (2019). Associations of physical activity and gross motor skills with executive function in preschool children from low-income south African settings. Dev. Sci. 22:e12820. doi: 10.1111/desc.12820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, E. E., Pitchford, N. J., and Limback, E. (2011). The interrelation between cognitive and motor development in typically developing children aged 4–11 years is underpinned by visual processing and fine manual control. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 569–584. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02018.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 71:44-56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 335–341. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453722

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18:34. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diao, Y. (2018) Research on Fundamental Movement Skills Development and Educational Enhancement in Children Aged 3–10 Years. Shanghai, China: East China Normal University.

Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., Herrero-Nivela, S.-L. C., and Herrero-Nivela, M. L. (2022). Teacher-rated executive functions, gender and relative age: independent and interactive effects on observed fundamental motor skills in kindergarteners. Front. Psychol. 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.848525

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabbard, C. (2021). Lifelong motor development : Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Gallahue, D. L., and Donnelly, F. C. (2007). Developmental physical education for all children. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Gandotra, A., Kotyuk, E., Bizonics, R., Khan, I., Petánszki, M., Kiss, L., et al. (2023). An exploratory study of the relationship between motor skills and indicators of cognitive and socio-emotional development in preschoolers. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 20, 50–65. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2022.2028617

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., and Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 134, 31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gashaj, V., Oberer, N., Mast, F. W., and Roebers, C. M. (2019). Individual differences in basic numerical skills: the role of executive functions and motor skills. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 182, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, S. N. J. (Ed.) (2014). Handbook of executive functioning. New York: Springer.

Google Scholar

Goodway, J. D., Ozmun, J. C., and Gallahue, D. L. (2019). Understanding motor development: Infants, children, adolescents, adults: Infants, children, adolescents, adults. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning.

Google Scholar

Gu, Y., Chen, Y., Ma, J., Ren, Z., Li, H., and Kim, H. (2021). The influence of a table tennis physical activity program on the gross motor development of Chinese preschoolers of different sexes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2627. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, X., Zhao, M., Kong, Z., and Xie, J. (2022). Association between fundamental motor skills and executive function in preschool children: a cross-sectional study. Front. Psychol. 13:978994. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.978994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartung, J., Engelhardt, L. E., Thibodeaux, M. L., Harden, K. P., and Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2020). Developmental transformations in the structure of executive functions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 189:104681. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirata, S. K. Y., Yasunaga, M., Suzuki, K., Okumura, Y., Okuzumi, H., et al. (2018). Applicability of the movement assessment battery for children-second edition (Mabc-2) for Japanese children aged 3–6 years: a preliminary investigation emphasizing internal consistency and factorial validity. Front. Psychol. 9:452. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01452

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Honrubia-Montesinos, C. G.-M. P., and Losada-Puente, L. (2021). Motor development among Spanish preschool children. Children 8:41. doi: 10.3390/children8010041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isquith, P. K., Crawford, J. S., Espy, K. A., and Gioia, G. A. (2005). Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 11, 209–215. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. J. Child Lang. 37, 229–261. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., and De Haan, M. D. (2015). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: An introduction. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Jylänki, P., Mbay, T., Hakkarainen, A., Sääkslahti, A., and Aunio, P. (2022). The effects of motor skill and physical activity interventions on preschoolers’ cognitive and academic skills: a systematic review. Prev. Med. 155:106948. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106948

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ke, L. D. W., Wang, Y., Duan, W., Hua, J., and Barnett, A. (2020). The movement Abc-2 test in China: comparison with Uk norms for 3–10 year olds. Res. Dev. Disabil. 105:103742. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Duran, C. A., Cameron, C. E., Duran, C. A. K., and Grissmer, D. (2018). Developmental relations among motor and cognitive processes and mathematics skills. Child Dev. 89, 476–494. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12752

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsandréou, F. W. M., Niemann, C., and Budde, H. (2016). Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on children’s working memory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1144–1152. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000869

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LeGear, M., Greyling, L., Sloan, E., Bell, R. I., Williams, B. L., Naylor, P. J., et al. (2012). A window of opportunity? Motor skills and perceptions of competence of children in kindergarten. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 9:29. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-29

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Wang, X., and Aulrich, D. (2022). Study on the reliability and validity of Tgmd-3 in the basic motor skills test of children aged 3-12 years in China. Journal of Wuhan University of physical. Education 56, 86–92. doi: 10.15930/j.cnki.wtxb.2022.03.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Libertus, K., and Hauf, P. (2017). Editorial: motor skills and their foundational role for perceptual, social, and cognitive development. Front. Psychol. 8:301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00301

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S., Chen, S.-T., and Cai, Y. (2022). Associations between gross motor coordination and executive functions: considering the sex difference in Chinese middle-aged school children. Front. Psychol. 13:875256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.875256

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Herrmann, C., Mücke, M., Andrä, C., Brand, S., Pühse, U., et al. (2018). Contingent negative variation and working memory maintenance in adolescents with low and high motor competencies. Neural Plast. 2018, 1–9. doi: 10.1155/2018/9628787

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luz, C., Rodrigues, L. P., and Cordovil, R. (2015). The relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in 4th grade children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 12, 129–141. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2014.966073

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Macdonald, K., Milne, N., Orr, R., and Pope, R. (2020). Associations between motor proficiency and academic performance in mathematics and reading in year 1 school children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 20:11. doi: 10.1186/s12887-020-1967-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malambo, C., NOVá, A., Clark, C., and Musálek, M. (2022). Associations between fundamental movement skills, physical fitness, motor competency, physical activity, and executive functions in pre-school age children: a systematic review. Children 9:1059. doi: 10.3390/children9071059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, M. N., and Roebers, C. M. (2019). Towards a better understanding of the association between motor skills and executive functions in 5-to 6-year-olds: the impact of motor task difficulty. Hum. Mov. Sci. 66:607-20. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2019.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcclelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., and Piccinin, A. (2013). Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child Res. Q. 28, 314–324. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mecías-Calvo, M. A.-G. V., Cons-Ferreiro, M., and Navarro-Patón, R. (2021). Is it possible to reduce the relative age effect through an intervention on motor competence in preschool children? Children 8:386. doi: 10.3390/children8050386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileva-Seitz, V. R., Ghassabian, A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van den Brink, J. D., Linting, M., Jaddoe, V. W. V., et al. (2015). Are boys more sensitive to sensitivity? Parenting and executive function in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 130:193. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moura, O., Pereira, M., Albuquerque, C. P., and Simões, M. R. (2025). Factor structure and measurement invariance of the childhood executive functioning inventory (Chexi) across parents and teachers in Portuguese children. Child Neuropsychol. 1, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2025.2455469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, G. K., Moilanen, K., Miettunen, J., Glahn, D. C., Cannon, T. D., Jones, P. B., et al. (2006). Infant motor development is associated with adult cognitive categorisation in a longitudinal birth cohort study. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 25–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01450.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro-Patón, R., Arufe-Giráldez, L.-B. J., Sanmiguel-Rodríguez, A., Lago-Fuentes, C., and Mecías-Calvo, M. (2021a). Gender differences on motor competence in 5-year-old preschool children regarding relative age. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3143. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18063143

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro-Patón, R., Mecías-Calvo, M., Rodríguez Fernández, J. E., and Arufe-Giráldez, V. (2021b). Relative age effect on motor competence in children aged 4–5 years. Children 8:115. doi: 10.3390/children8020115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neisser, U. (2014). Cognitive psychology: Classic edition. New York: Psychology press.

Google Scholar

Niederer, I., Kriemler, S., Gut, J., Hartmann, T., Schindler, C., Barral, J., et al. (2011). Relationship of aerobic fitness and motor skills with memory and attention in preschoolers (Ballabeina): a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr. 11:34. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-11-34

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pakpahan, F. H., and Saragih, M. (2022). Theory of cognitive development by Jean Piaget. J. Appl. Linguist. Jakarta, Indonesia: Cetta Publishing House. 2, 55–60. doi: 10.52622/joal.v2i2.79

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piek, J. P., Dawson, L., Smith, L. M., and Gasson, N. (2008). The role of early fine and gross motor development on later motor and cognitive ability. Hum. Mov. Sci. 27, 668–681. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridler, K. V., Tanskanen, P., Miettunen, J., Chitnis, X., Suckling, J., Murray, G. K., et al. (2006). Fronto-cerebellar systems are associated with infant motor and adult executive functions in healthy adults but not in schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 15651–15656. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602639103

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Kane, R., et al. (2012). An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 54, 1025–1031. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04403.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A., Carlson, R. A., and Gilmore, R. O. (2001). Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 453–470. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.453

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudd, J. R., Barnett, L. M., Butson, M. L., Farrow, D., Berry, J., and Polman, R. C. J. (2015). Fundamental movement skills are more than run, throw and catch: the role of stability skills. PLoS One 10:e0140224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salehi, S. K., Sheikh, M., and Talebrokni, F. S. (2017). Comparison exam of Gallahue’s hourglass model and Clark and Metcalfe’s the mountain of motor development metaphor. Adv. Phys. Educ. 7:217. doi: 10.4236/ape.2017.73018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvatori, M. L., and Cherubini, D. (2024). Gender stereotypes in physical education: State of the art and future perspectives in primary school. Pedagogy Phys. Cult. Oleg Dukhovnyi Kharkiv National Academy of Physical Culture. 28, 231–238. doi: 10.15561/26649837.2024.0308

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., and Wang, H. (2012). Research on themed spaces in children's parks based on child psychology theories: a case study of Fuzhou children's park. J. Nanjing For. Univ. 12, 92–96. doi: 10.16397/j.cnki.1671-1165.2012.04.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. D., Valtr, L., and Verbecque, E. (2023). Do girls have an advantage compared to boys when their motor skills are tested using the movement assessment battery for children, 2nd edition? Children 10:1159. doi: 10.3390/children10071159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spedden, M. E., Malling, A. S. B., Andersen, K. K., and Jensen, B. R. (2017). Association between gross-motor and executive function depends on age and motor task complexity. Dev. Neuropsychol. 42, 495–506. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2017.1399129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stöckel, T., and Hughes, C. M. (2016). The relation between measures of cognitive and motor functioning in 5-to 6-year-old children. Psychol. Res. 80:543-54. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0662-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stöckel, T., Wunsch, K., and Hughes, C. M. (2017). Age-related decline in anticipatory motor planning and its relation to cognitive and motor skill proficiency. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:283. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00283

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stodden, D. F., Goodway, J. D., Langendorfer, S. J., Roberton, M. A., Rudisill, M. E., Garcia, C., et al. (2008). A developmental perspective on the role of motor skill competence in physical activity: an emergent relationship. Quest 60, 290–306. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2008.10483582

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuhr, C., Hughes, C. M., and Stöckel, T. (2020). The role of executive functions for motor performance in preschool children as compared to young adults. Front. Psychol. 11:1552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01552

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T. (1992). Biological and psychological development of executive functions. Brain Cogn. 20, 8–23. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90059-U

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, L., Zhao, T., Liang, X., Xia, M., Li, Q., Liao, X., et al. (2025). Human lifespan changes in the brain’s functional connectome. Nat. Neurosci. 28, 891–901. doi: 10.1038/s41593-025-01907-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorell, L., and Nyberg, L. (2008). The childhood executive functioning lnventory (Chexl): a new rating instrument for parents and teachers. Dev. Neuropsychol. 4, 536–552. doi: 10.1080/87565640802101516

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., Mccullick, B., Pendleton, D. M., and Psece, C. (2015). Exercise and children's cognition: the role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. J. Sport Health Sci. 4, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C., Thorell, L., Siu, A., Hsu, Y. H., and Lin, H. L. (2020). Taiwanese traditional-Chinese childhood executive functioning inventory: revision, investigation of psychometric properties, and establishing of norms. Psychol Test 67, 119–143.

Google Scholar

Utesch, T., Bardid, F., Büsch, D., and Strauss, B. (2019). The relationship between motor competence and physical fitness from early childhood to early adulthood: a meta-analysis. Sports Med. 49, 541–551. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01068-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Fels, I. M. J., Smith, J., De Bruijn, A. G. M., et al. (2019). Relations between gross motor skills and executive functions, controlling for the role of information processing and lapses of attention in 8-10 year old children. PLoS One 14:e0224219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhala, A. (2024). Developmental associations of fundamental motor skills and executive functions in preschoolers — the role of the physical activity and the effects on early numeracy. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 34:220. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2024.100220

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhala, A., Haapala, E. A., Sääkslahti, A., Hakkarainen, A., Widlund, A., and Aunio, P. (2023). Associations between physical activity, motor skills, executive functions and early numeracy in preschoolers. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 23, 1385–1393. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2022.2092777

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veldman, S. L. C., Santos, R., Jones, R. A., Sousa-Sá, E., and Okely, A. D. (2019). Associations between gross motor skills and cognitive development in toddlers. Early Hum. Dev. 132, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.04.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallon, H. (1972). The psychological development of the child. Int. J. Ment. Health 1, 29–39. doi: 10.1080/00207411.1972.11448595

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, T. (2022). Developmental characteristics and interrelationships of physical activity, physical fitness, and motor skills in preschool children. Chin. Sport Sci. Technol. 58, 49–61. doi: 10.16470/j.csst.2020068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, W., Xie, Q., Zhu, J., He, W., and Li, Y. (2018). Reliability and validity analysis of the children's executive function scale in Chinese preschool children. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 26, 26–29.

Google Scholar

Willoughby, M. T., and Hudson, K. (2023). Contributions of motor skill development and physical activity to the ontogeny of executive function skills in early childhood. Dev. Rev. 70:101102. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2023.101102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willoughby, M., Hudson, K., Hong, Y., and Wylie, A. (2021). Improvements in motor competence skills are associated with improvements in executive function and math problem-solving skills in early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1463–1470. doi: 10.1037/dev0001223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xin, F., Cai, Y., and Bao, R. (2019). Systematic review of fundamental movement skills intervention studies for young children in international contexts. Chin. Sport Sci. 39, 83–97. doi: 10.16469/j.css.201902009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Li, Y., and Fu, R. (2022). Research on the characteristics and relationship between gross motor and executive function development of 3-6-year-old children. Chin. Sports Sci. Technol. 58, 51–58. doi: 10.16470/j.csst.2020057

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Blair, C. B., and Willoughby, M. T. (2016). Executive function: Implications for education. Ncer 2017–2000. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research.

Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychol. Neurosci. 13, 273–298. doi: 10.1037/pne0000208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Y. Y. W., Korivi, M., Liu, Y., and Hong, F. (2022). Gender differences in fundamental motor skills proficiency in children aged 3–6 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:8318. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19148318

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., and Tolmie, A. (2024). Associations between gross and fine motor skills, physical activity, executive function, and academic achievement: longitudinal findings from the Uk millennium cohort study. Brain Sci. 14:121. doi: 10.3390/brainsci14020121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children, fundamental motor skills, executive function, age, gender

Citation: Kong C, Cai Y, Li K, Wang Z, Wen X and Zhang X (2025) The relationship between fundamental motor skills and executive function in children: a stratified analysis by age and sex. Front. Psychol. 16:1669915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1669915

Received: 20 July 2025; Accepted: 21 August 2025;
Published: 05 September 2025.

Edited by:

Qing Yi, Dalian University, China

Reviewed by:

Zhihua Yin, East China Normal University, China
Yucui Diao, Soochow University, China

Copyright © 2025 Kong, Cai, Li, Wang, Wen and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yujun Cai, Y2FpeXVqdW5Ac3VzLmVkdS5jbg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.