Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 July 2025

Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1587405

This article is part of the Research TopicThe Power of Relationships in Human Development: From Prenatal Bonding to Attachment Across the LifespanView all 6 articles

Mapping love: a personality-centered network analysis of relationship satisfaction

  • Department of Personality Psychology, Differential Psychology, and Assessment, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland

Previous research has linked various personality features to relationship satisfaction, primarily investigating bivariate effects. Given the interrelatedness of these personality features, their unique associations with relationship satisfaction remain unclear. The present study addresses this gap by exploring the holistic interplay of relationship satisfaction with related personality features and considering gender as a moderator. With an online self-report survey, relationship satisfaction, attachment, jealousy and trust, self-esteem, relationship self-efficacy, sexual satisfaction, and sociosexuality in 510 women and 300 men (Mage = 26.5 years) were assessed. Network analysis was used to estimate a combined network, while a network comparison test was used to examine gender differences. Insecure attachment, trust, mutuality, and sexual satisfaction uniquely correlated with relationship satisfaction within the combined network. Networks of men and women were largely similar. These results expand the understanding of relationship satisfaction and inform the ongoing debate on gender differences in psychological research.

Introduction

Satisfaction with romantic relationships is associated with higher levels of physical and psychological wellbeing (Dush and Amato, 2005; Proulx et al., 2007) and overall life satisfaction (Be et al., 2013; Yam, 2023). Contrarily, less satisfying relationships are relatively likely to dissolve (Gottman and Levenson, 1992). In the context of marriage, this often results in divorce, a phenomenon that has reached historical highs in recent decades (Ortiz-Ospina and Roser, 2020). Divorces not only impose financial burdens and strain on legal systems, but children of divorced parents also carry comparatively high risks of developing psychological disorders (Sands et al., 2017; Schaan et al., 2019). Addressing these far-reaching issues necessitates understanding what constitutes relationship satisfaction (RSA) and why some individuals are less satisfied with their relationships compared to others. Such knowledge can further help to maximize the benefits of maintaining satisfying relationships.

The degree to which someone is satisfied with a romantic relationship depends on a variety of factors, ranging from societal norms to the presence of children, the frequency, intensity, and severity of conflicts, and experiences of infidelity (Bradbury et al., 2000). Moreover, the extent to which worldviews, personalities, and preferences of individuals are compatible can also affect the quality of relationships (Huston and Houts, 1998). Personality and its relevance to romantic relationships has already drawn considerable scientific interest. Previous investigations have identified various personality features that are associated with RSA, including but not limited to, attachment (Stackert and Bursik, 2003), jealousy (Andersen et al., 1995) and trust (Fitzpatrick and Lafontaine, 2017), self-esteem (Fincham and Bradbury, 1993), relationship self-efficacy (Weiser and Weigel, 2016), sexual satisfaction (Byers, 2005), and sociosexuality (Penke and Asendorpf, 2008). Most studies have focused on bivariate effects, examining how RSA is linked to one feature at a time. However, given the substantial interrelatedness of the mentioned personality features, their unique associations with RSA remain unclear. The present study addresses this gap by exploring a more holistic interplay of RSA with relevant personality features, highlighting their unique associations, and considering gender as a moderator. Its focus lies on heterosexual men and women in monogamous relationships. In the following sections, we will first elaborate on how RSA is defined and measured. Then, we will focus on a selection of personality features linked to RSA and discuss potential gender differences in this context.

Relationship satisfaction

RSA refers to a subjective evaluation of an interpersonal (here: romantic) relationship and answers the question of how someone feels about their relationship at a particular moment in time (Hendrick et al., 1988). Various approaches have been developed to assess RSA. For example, Spanier (1976) proposed that it can be determined by the frequency of affectionate behaviors (e.g., laughing together, kissing) and by the number of domains over which disagreements occur (e.g., household tasks, religious matters). Siffert and Bodenmann (2010) similarly argued that RSA encompasses multiple domains, such as mutual admiration, trust, and sexual satisfaction. In contrast, Fincham and Bradbury (1987) suggested that RSA should be captured as a single global entity, based on how well a relationship fulfills needs or meets expectations (Hendrick, 1988).

Some studies suggest that women experience lower levels of RSA than men (Lesch and Engelbrecht, 2011; Jackson et al., 2014). However, this discrepancy may be primarily attributed to unique sample characteristics (e.g., clinical samples or very specific cultural contexts). Overall, most studies report that levels of RSA are equal among men and women (Erol and Orth, 2014; Fallis et al., 2016).

Attachment

Attachment is arguably the most basic psychological concept that underlies human bonding. Originating from Bowlby's (1969) theory about the development of attachment during infancy, several approaches to attachment in adult relationships exist today (Ravitz et al., 2010). For instance, Brennan et al. (1991) proposed that adult attachment can largely be reflected by two dimensions: anxiety and avoidance. Anxiety refers to the degree of concern individuals experience regarding rejection or abandonment. Avoidance refers to the extent to which individuals are uncomfortable with intimacy and to their preference to maintain emotional distance. Individuals can endorse any combination of anxiety and avoidance levels. Securely attached individuals usually endorse neither anxious nor avoidant attachment.

Insecurely (i.e., anxiously, avoidantly) attached individuals often experience lower levels of RSA, compared to securely attached individuals (Stackert and Bursik, 2003; Hirschberger et al., 2009). Li and Chan (2012) argue that attachment avoidance is particularly detrimental to RSA. While high levels of attachment anxiety are often associated with frequent conflicts, their negative effect on RSA may be buffered by the enjoyment that anxiously attached individuals experience in times when their relationships feel safe (Li and Chan, 2012). Contrarily, avoidantly attached individuals usually seek independence in their lives, thereby avoiding actions that could improve their RSA, such as openly discussing personal boundaries with their partner. As a result, they tend to maintain higher levels of dissatisfaction (Li and Chan, 2012).

Some studies suggest that genders are largely similar regarding adult attachment (Stackert and Bursik, 2003; Bakermans-Kranenburg and Ijzendoorn, 2009). However, other studies revealed that women more frequently endorse anxious attachment, while men are more likely to endorse avoidant attachment (Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991; Butzer and Campbell, 2008). Moreover, gender has been shown to moderate the relationship between attachment and RSA. Barry et al. (2015) found that the negative effect of avoidant attachment on RSA is stronger in women than in men, while the negative effect of anxious attachment is stronger in men than in women.

Romantic jealousy and trust

Romantic jealousy refers to the experience that arises following a perceived or actual threat to a romantic relationship (White, 1981). Experiencing romantic jealousy entails an aversive emotional state, while certain cognitions (e.g., worries about potential or actual extradyadic interest in a partner) and behaviors (e.g., surveillance activities or verbal aggression toward potential rivals) are also induced (Wegner et al., 2018). Although romantic jealousy can originate from a desire to protect a relationship and, thus, potentially enhance RSA (Rydell et al., 2004), it is more frequently negatively associated with RSA, ranging from depression to physical violence (Pines and Aronson, 1983; Andersen et al., 1995).

Meta-analyses on gender differences in romantic jealousy yielded inconclusive results (Harris, 2003; Carpenter, 2012; Sagarin et al., 2012). On the one hand, men tend to display larger discrepancies in sexual and emotional jealousy than women do (Edlund and Sagarin, 2017). On the other hand, gender differences can disappear when controlling for the type of response format in measurement tools (i.e., forced-choice or continuous scales; Carpenter, 2012). Thus, detecting gender differences in romantic jealousy might depend on aspects such as measurement technique, statistical methodology, and the inclusion of moderator variables like relationship status, attachment, and sexual orientation (Edlund and Sagarin, 2017).

Romantic jealousy is closely linked to trust, such that higher levels of jealousy coincide with lower levels of trust (Marshall et al., 2013; Rodriguez et al., 2015). Rempel et al. (1985) proposed that trust involves multiple elements, such as regarding a partner as reliable and helpful, believing that they care, and feeling confident in the relationship's strength. Existing evidence suggests that trust is positively correlated with RSA (Wieselquist, 2009; Fitzpatrick and Lafontaine, 2017). Furthermore, studies on gender differences have repeatedly shown that men and women share similar levels of trust within romantic relationships (Marshall et al., 2013; Fitzpatrick and Lafontaine, 2017; Yilmaz et al., 2023).

Self-esteem

Self-esteem is an overall evaluation of oneself and can be regarded as a global judgment of the extent to which individuals perceive themselves as competent, worthy, and deserving of respect (Rosenberg, 1965). The sociometer theory posits that self-esteem is largely affected by social feedback (Leary and Baumeister, 2000). Receiving attention and appreciation, especially by a romantic partner, might increase levels of self-esteem or protect them from deterioration (Murray et al., 2003). Hence, self-esteem is positively linked to RSA (Fincham and Bradbury, 1993; Shackelford, 2001).

Gender differences in self-esteem appear to be relatively pronounced. Bleidorn et al. (2016) found that men consistently report higher levels of self-esteem compared to women, with a small to medium-sized effect, controlling for age and cultural diversity. Other studies provided further support for this finding (Feingold, 1994; Robins et al., 2002).

Relationship self-efficacy

Bandura (1977) defined self-efficacy as an individual's belief in their ability to successfully perform specific tasks or achieve desired goals. In the context of romantic relationships, self-efficacy describes confidence in the ability to shape and influence outcomes of romantic relationships (Cabeldue and Boswell, 2012). Strong self-efficacy beliefs facilitate the successful attainment of goals, which can evoke feelings of accomplishment and satisfaction (Bandura, 1977). Consistent with this, positive links between relationship self-efficacy and RSA have been reported (Fincham et al., 2000; Weiser and Weigel, 2016).

Riggio et al. (2011) found that women tend to endorse stronger relationship self-efficacy beliefs than men. Lopez et al. (2007) examined the construct in more detail and found women to score higher on the perceived ability to both provide and receive care and support from a partner as well as to constructively discuss important matters within a romantic relationship (mutuality), whereas men scored higher on the perceived ability to regulate negative feelings toward a partner, like frustration, disappointment, and anger (emotional control). They observed no gender differences in differentiation referring to the perceived ability to express needs for separateness and to assertively maintain interpersonal boundaries (Lopez et al., 2007).

Sexual satisfaction

Sexual satisfaction can be defined as “an affective response arising from one's subjective evaluation of the positive and negative dimensions associated with one's sexual relationship” (Lawrance and Byers, 1995, p. 268). Sexual satisfaction is positively linked to RSA and compared to other features relevant to RSA, it exhibits some of the highest correlations (Sprecher, 2002; Byers, 2005; Fallis et al., 2016). It is not clear whether sexual satisfaction should be considered as an independent feature or as a component of RSA (Hassebrauck and Fehr, 2002; Siffert and Bodenmann, 2010). Nevertheless, the consideration of sexual satisfaction seems to be mandatory when investigating individual differences in RSA.

Most studies suggest that men and women are equally satisfied with sexuality in their romantic relationships (Butzer and Campbell, 2008; Peixoto, 2023). Interestingly, sexual satisfaction seems to be a stronger predictor of subsequent RSA for men, compared to women (Hassebrauck and Fehr, 2002; Sprecher, 2002), although the opposite has also been observed (Vohs et al., 2004).

Sociosexuality

Kinsey et al. (1948) originally described sociosexuality as a predisposition or willingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relationships. Individuals with an unrestricted sociosexual orientation are usually comfortable with casual sex, without feeling the need for emotional closeness or intimacy, with the opposite being true for individuals with a more restricted sociosexual orientation (Simpson and Gangestad, 1991). Penke and Asendorpf (2008) identified three components of sociosexuality. Sociosexual behavior represents the number of short-term sexual encounters an individual has had in the past as well as the tendency with which they will do so in the future. Sociosexual attitude entails personal opinions and broad evaluations regarding uncommitted sex. Finally, sociosexual desire refers to the degree to which an individual currently wishes to engage in uncommitted sexual activities. High levels of sociosexual desire can negatively affect RSA (Penke and Asendorpf, 2008). Moreover, high levels of sociosexual desire in both partners have been shown to predict relationship dissolution (Penke and Asendorpf, 2008). Finally, individuals with an unrestricted sociosexual orientation are more likely to engage in infidelity behaviors than individuals with a restricted sociosexual orientation (Mattingly et al., 2011).

While men and women tend to exhibit similar levels of sociosexual behavior, moderate differences in sociosexual attitude and large differences in sociosexual desire have been observed, with men scoring higher on both dimensions (Penke and Asendorpf, 2008). Moreover, men consistently report a more unrestricted global sociosexual orientation than women do (Schmitt, 2005; Sprecher et al., 2013). Furthermore, Webster et al. (2015) found that a negative correlation between sociosexuality and RSA exists for men, but not for women.

The present study

The previous sections have explored the importance of various personality features in romantic relationships and their individual links to RSA. These features stem from different perspectives on personality, such as the social-cognitive (e.g., relationship self-efficacy), the attachment (e.g., avoidance), the trait-based (e.g., jealousy), or the evolutionary approach to human personality (e.g., sociosexual behavior). Therefore, it is not surprising that they are interconnected (e.g., Foster et al., 2007; Gubler et al., 2023; Richter et al., 2022; Riggio et al., 2013), and it is to be expected that they are jointly associated with RSA. Consequently, a holistic approach is necessary to better understand their complex interplay. A network approach is well-suited for this task. Unlike latent variable models or factor analyses, which often assume that observed variables are caused by underlying, unobserved traits, the network perspective proposes that personality features and other psychological phenomena are best understood as complex systems of interacting variables (Borsboom et al., 2021). Consequently, network analysis does not differentiate between independent and dependent variables but emphasizes the reciprocity of (correlational) relationships. It sees personality as an ecosystem of characteristics with stimulating and inhibitory relationships and shifts the focus from shared variance to the direct, unique relationships between observable variables (Costantini et al., 2015). Furthermore, by using partial correlations, the redundancy of different personality features (from the same or different approaches described above) in their association with RSA is explicitly addressed and revealed. These insights are typically lost when variables are aggregated in latent models and factor analyses. Network analysis also offers its own measures (e.g., centrality indices) to characterize the structure and organization of a network. These structural properties can yield important insights into the functioning of the whole system and the roles of individual variables within it. For example, strength centrality indicates the number of connections of a variable to all other variables, closeness centrality reflects the distance of a variable to all others, and betweenness identifies variables that frequently act as bridges between other variables (Costantini et al., 2015). Network analysis has repeatedly been used to investigate personality-related phenomena and is gaining increasing popularity (Costantini et al., 2019; Herzberg and Wildfang, 2018; Nickull et al., 2022). Accordingly, the first aim of this study is to illuminate if and how RSA is connected to attachment, jealousy and trust, self-esteem, relationship self-efficacy, sexual satisfaction, and sociosexuality within a network.

As outlined before, gender differences have been reported for most of the mentioned personality features, although some of this evidence is not consistent. The moderating role of gender in the associations between these personality features and RSA has also been investigated, albeit much less extensively. For example, Nickull et al. (2022) investigated the role of gender in the interplay between sexual satisfaction and RSA. They found that networks of men and women are largely similar, except that sexual satisfaction plays the most central role for men, while sexual desire holds that position for women (Nickull et al., 2022). Personality features were not considered in their study. Thus, to help understand the role of gender in the interplay between personality and RSA, the second aim of this study will be to investigate if and how men and women vary in their respective network structures.

Materials and methods

We report all measures and exclusions in this study. Data and R code are available under https://osf.io/38qrt/?view_only=2e2e5b4c7b864eeface66b8436dce80b.

Participants

Social media platforms, private group chats, and the university's participant pool were used to recruit participants. As a reward, five shopping vouchers worth CHF 20 were raffled off among participants. Psychology students enrolled at the University of Bern received 0.5 course credits in exchange for participation but were not included in the raffle. All participants provided their written informed consent by pressing a confirmation button in the online survey before participating. Participant recruitment started on October 1, 2022, and ended on January 31, 2024. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Human Sciences of the University of Bern prior to its launch (submission number 2022-03-00003). No preregistration was submitted for this exploratory research.

Overall, 2,180 individuals accessed our online survey programmed on Qualtrics, which contained questionnaires to measure the study variables and to collect information pertaining to romantic relationships (e.g., sexual orientation, experiences of infidelity) and sociodemographic characteristics. For this study, only data from individuals who self-reported as heterosexual men or women and who were in a committed, monogamous relationship at the time of participation were considered for analysis. Due to these criteria, data from 788 individuals were excluded. Data from a further 582 individuals were excluded due to an excess of 20% missing values. The final sample consisted of 810 individuals (510 female). The average age was 26.5 years (SD = 8.7 years), ranging from 18 to 80 years. The average relationship duration was 4.3 years (SD = 5.9 years). Thirty percent of participants reported that they had previously experienced some form of infidelity (i.e., emotional, sexual, other) within a romantic relationship. Participants were well-educated, with 52% possessing a university entrance certificate, and 34% a university degree.

To tackle the second research objective, two subgroups consisting of 300 men and 300 matched women were formed (see Statistical analyses). On average, men were 28.9 years old (SD = 9.6 years), while women had an average age of 27.6 years (SD = 8.6 years). The age difference between genders was not significant, t(598) = 1.697, p = 0.090, Cohen's d = 0.139. The difference between the two groups in relationship duration with 5.1 years (SD = 6.8 years) for men and 4.5 years for women (SD = 6.4 years) was not statistically significant, t(598) = 1.611, p = 0.306, d = 0.084. The female group (M = 3.93, SD = 1.09) differed significantly in educational level from the male group (M = 3.73, SD = 1.19) with χ(6)2 = 14.178, p = 0.028, Cramér's V = 0.15. Finally, 30% of men and 36% of women reported having experienced infidelity within a romantic relationship. This difference was not significant, χ(1)2 = 2.168, p = 0.141, Cramér's V = 0.05.

Measures

Relationship satisfaction

The German version (Sander and Böcker, 1993) of the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS; Hendrick, 1988) was used to assess RSA, which is suitable for both marital and non-marital partnerships (Renshaw et al., 2011). The scale consists of seven items (e.g., “How good is your relationship compared to others?”) to be rated on a five-point response scale ranging from very poor (1) to very good (5), with higher values indicating higher levels of RSA. The RAS shows high internal consistency (α = 0.89), and its factorial validity has been confirmed (Dinkel and Balck, 2005).

Attachment

The revised, eight-item version of the Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire in German (ECR-RD8; Ehrenthal et al., 2021) was utilized to measure adult attachment style in romantic relationships. It comprises eight items, reflecting anxious or avoidant attachment tendencies, measured by four items each. Items (e.g., “I often worry that my partner will not want to stay with me.”) are rated on a seven-point response scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7), with higher scores indicating greater levels of attachment insecurity. The ECR-RD8 exhibits good reliability (McDonald's ω > 0.80 for both subscales) and convergent validity with other attachment scales (Ehrenthal et al., 2021).

Participants also completed the German version of the Adult Attachment Scale (Schmidt et al., 2004) as an additional measure of attachment style. However, due to its weaker psychometric properties compared to the ECR-RD8, data collected from this scale were excluded from our analysis.

Romantic jealousy and trust

Romantic jealousy and trust were both assessed using the German self-report questionnaire developed by Bauer (1988). Therein, jealousy is measured with 10 items (e.g., “I always want to know what my partner is doing when he/she is not with me.”) while trust is measured with five items (e.g., “I believe that my partner is absolutely open with me.”). Items are rated on a six-point response scale ranging from does not apply at all (1) to applies exactly (6), with higher scores indicating higher levels of jealousy and trust. The scale shows good reliability (α = 0.87) and convergent validity with general jealousy tendencies (Schmitt et al., 1995).

Self-esteem

Self-esteem was assessed with the German translation (Ferring and Filipp, 1996) of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). The RSES consists of 10 items (e.g., “Overall, I am satisfied with myself”). Items are rated on a four-point response scale ranging from does not apply at all (1) to applies exactly (4), with higher values indicating higher levels of self-esteem. The German scale exhibits good internal consistency (α = 0.84; Collani and Herzberg, 2003).

Relationship self-efficacy

For this study, the English language Relationship Self-Efficacy Scale (Lopez et al., 2007) was translated into German following the guidelines of the International Test Commission (http://www.intestcom.org) and with the consent of the test author, Frederick G. Lopez. The questionnaire consists of 25 items measuring confidence in the ability to contribute to the success of a relationship. Relationship self-efficacy is underpinned by three latent factors: mutuality (16 items, e.g., “How confident are you in your ability to openly and directly address significant disagreements?”), emotional control (four items, e.g., “How confident are you in your ability to stay calm when you and your partner are having a serious argument?”), and differentiation (five items, e.g., “How confident are you in your ability to tell your partner when you need to be alone?”). Items are rated on a nine-point response scale, ranging from not at all confident (1) to very confident (9), with higher scores indicating higher levels of relationship self-efficacy. The English version of the questionnaire demonstrates very good (α = 0.94) internal consistency and construct validity was supported by significant correlations with attachment components and RSA (Lopez et al., 2007).

Sexual satisfaction

Sexual satisfaction was assessed with the subscale Sexuality in the Relationship from the Partnership Quality Questionnaire (FPQ; Siffert and Bodenmann, 2010). The subscale consists of five items (e.g., “Our partnership is sexually satisfying to me.”), each rated on a five-point response scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Higher scores indicate higher levels of sexual satisfaction. The subscale shows very high internal consistency (α = 0.94) and convergent validity with all subscales correlating with the RAS (Siffert and Bodenmann, 2010). In our study, all subscales of the FPQ were presented to participants. However, only the sexual satisfaction subscale was included in our analysis, as most constructs represented by the other subscales were already considered in the network (e.g., trust).

Sociosexuality

To measure sociosexuality, the revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI-R; Penke and Asendorpf, 2008) was employed. The questionnaire assesses sociosexual behavior (e.g., “With how many partners have you had sex within the past 12 months?”), sociosexual attitude (e.g., “Sex without love is OK”), and sociosexual desire (e.g., “In everyday life, how often do you have spontaneous fantasies about having sex with someone you have just met?”). Each subscale is measured by three items, captured on a nine-point response scale with varying labels. Higher scores indicate a more unrestricted sociosexual orientation. Internal consistency of the subscales ranges from α = 0.76 to α = 0.88 (Penke, 2011).

Statistical analyses

Network analysis was conducted to tackle both research objectives. It is suitable to investigate statistical relationships among multiple variables by visualizing the underlying partial correlation matrix in an easily interpretable manner (Costantini et al., 2015). Typically, networks consist of nodes representing the network variables and edges reflecting the statistical association among them. Centrality indices can be computed, which provide information about the importance of individual network variables (Costantini et al., 2015). Additionally, indirect bivariate effects can be inferred from networks, which can serve as a basis for investigating potential mediation effects (Epskamp and Fried, 2018).

All statistical analyses were conducted using R (version 4.3.1; Team, 2023). The analyzed data set did not include any missing values. For the upcoming sections, we followed the reporting guidelines laid out by Burger et al. (2023). As a first step, a mean score was computed for each subscale where applicable, resulting in 13 network nodes: RSA, attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, jealousy, trust, self-esteem, mutuality, emotional control, differentiation, sexual satisfaction, sociosexual attitude, sociosexual desire, and sociosexual behavior. Despite their conceptual overlap, treating each subscale as a distinct node in the network aligns with the theoretical shift toward viewing psychological phenomena as complex systems of interacting variables (Borsboom et al., 2021). This approach allows for a more detailed examination of the direct relationships, complex structure, and specific components of the examined features.

Second, the estimateNetwork function from the bootnet package (version 1.5.6; Epskamp et al., 2018) was employed for network estimation. The Extended Bayesian Information Criterion was used to select the network that best fits the data (Chen and Chen, 2008). Using the graphical LASSO (glasso; Friedman et al., 2008), trivially small edges were removed. While default settings of the bootnet package were largely retained, the EBIC tuning parameter was set to 0.9. This value exceeds the commonly recommended range in psychological network research (Epskamp and Fried, 2018), reflecting a more conservative model selection approach. A higher tuning parameter imposes a stronger penalty on edge inclusion, resulting in a sparser network. This helps reduce the likelihood of false positives (i.e., spurious associations), particularly valuable in exploratory settings, and may enhance both the interpretability and replicability of the network structure. Notably, results using a lower tuning parameter (i.e., 0.5) produced a similar network structure, suggesting that the findings are robust to this analytic choice. The sum of all absolute edge weights was used to represent the global strength of the network. Visualization of the network was performed using the qgraph package (version 1.9.8; Epskamp et al., 2012) and the Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm (Fruchterman and Reingold, 1991). To assess the accuracy and stability of edge weights, 1,500 bootstrap samples were simulated.

Third, to investigate the relative importance of each node, strength and closeness centrality were examined (Costantini et al., 2015). Strength centrality reflects the total sum of all edge weights of a node and indicates how strongly the node is connected to all other nodes in the network. Closeness centrality quantifies how well-connected a node is, that is, to what extent a node reciprocally stimulates or inhibits surrounding nodes (Costantini et al., 2015). To assess the accuracy of centrality indices, we used case-drop bootstrapping based on 1500 samples (Epskamp et al., 2018). The resulting Correlation Stability coefficient (CS coefficient) quantifies the rank order stability of centrality measures using stepwise reduction of the sample. The CS coefficient measures the proportion of the sample that can be omitted while ensuring that the correlation between the rank order of centrality measures in the reduced sample and the original sample remains at least 0.7, with a 95% probability. To ensure that centrality measures remain interpretable, CS coefficients should be at least 0.25, ideally 0.50 (Epskamp et al., 2018).

Finally, to compare networks between women and men, two groups were formed. RSA is susceptible to temporal changes and common covariates include age and relationship duration (Erol and Orth, 2014). Hence, participants were assigned so that both groups showed similar levels in these variables. The MatchIt package (version 4.5.5; Ho et al., 2007) was used for this purpose. To achieve identical sample sizes for each group, the data from all men (n = 300) were used, along with data from the 300 women who best matched these men in the specified criteria. To compare networks for men and for women, the NCT function from the NetworkComparisonTest package (version 2.2.2) was employed (van Borkulo et al., 2023), using 2000 iterations.

Results

Descriptive statistics for each network variable are presented in Table 1. Values of skewness and kurtosis show that most network variables follow an approximate normal distribution. Departure from normality (absolute skewness >2 or absolute kurtosis >4; Kim, 2013) in trust was deemed unproblematic due to a sufficiently large sample size (Sainani, 2012). The Supplementary material contains a table presenting the zero-order correlations of all study variables.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of relationship satisfaction and related personality features (N = 810).

General network structure

Figure 1 depicts the estimated network structure of 810 individuals. Out of 78 possible edges, 52 edges remained substantial and were retained in the network, meaning that 26 edges were set to zero by the glasso algorithm (Friedman et al., 2008). Global network strength was 5.51. Edge weights ranged from r = −0.50 (jealousy–trust) to r = 0.42 (differentiation–mutuality).

Figure 1
Network diagram illustrating relationships between psychological and relational features. Nodes represent features such as relationship satisfaction, jealousy, and trust, with connected lines indicating partial correlations. Lines vary in thickness and color, with blue and red representing positive and negative correlations respectively. Labels and abbreviations explain each feature such as RSA for relationship satisfaction and AAV for attachment avoidance. The diagram emphasizes how these features interconnect.

Figure 1. Estimated network structure of relationship satisfaction and related personality features (N = 810). Blue bars represent positive edges between nodes, whereas red bars represent negative edges. Line thickness reflects effect size, with thicker edges reflecting stronger partial correlations.

To explore the position and edges of RSA within the network, zero-order Pearson correlations and partial correlations (i.e., edge weights) of RSA with all network variables were computed (see Table 1). Most partial correlations were much smaller compared to their corresponding zero-order correlations and, in many cases, approached zero. Nodes that formed substantial partial correlations with RSA included attachment anxiety (r = −0.17), attachment avoidance (r = −0.22), trust (r = 0.10), mutuality (r = 0.24), and sexual satisfaction (r = 0.22). These variables uniquely correlated with RSA, persisting beyond the confounding effect of other nodes. Contrarily, the unique associations of RSA with jealousy (r = 0.01), self-esteem (r = 0.03), emotional control (r = 0.04), differentiation (r = 0.07), sociosexual attitude (r = 0.00), sociosexual desire (r = −0.01), and sociosexual behavior (r = −0.05) became negligible after controlling for the confounding effect of other nodes.

Figure 2 presents the bootstrapped confidence intervals and estimated values of all 78 edge weights. Confidence intervals were relatively small, indicating high robustness of the estimated edge weights. Note that confidence intervals in this context do not provide any information regarding null hypothesis testing, due to problems associated with family-wise error rates (Epskamp and Fried, 2018). In other words, if a confidence interval did not contain zero, this does not necessarily mean that the edge weight was significantly different from zero. Instead, confidence intervals should merely be regarded as indicators of the accuracy and stability of edge weights (Epskamp and Fried, 2018).

Figure 2
Line graph showing the relationship between various psychological features. The x-axis represents edge values ranging from -0.6 to 0.6. The y-axis lists variable pairs like MUT-DIF and AAV-TRU. Black dots indicate bootstrap means, while red dots represent sample data. A key explains abbreviations, such as RSA for relationship satisfaction and AAV for attachment avoidance. The line trends upward with a shaded area depicting variability.

Figure 2. Bootstrapped accuracy test on 78 edge weights with 95% confidence intervals (N = 810). Each of the 78 edge weights is represented by both a black dot and a red dot. Black dots indicate average edge weights based on 1,500 bootstrap samples. Red dots indicate estimated edge weights in the study sample. Gray areas represent 95% confidence intervals for each value.

Figure 3 presents the results of our centrality analyses. The robustness of both strength centrality (CS coefficient = 0.75) and closeness centrality (CS coefficient = 0.75) was high. RSA showed the highest closeness centrality (closeness = 2.07), and the second highest strength centrality (strength = 1.26). A similar finding surfaced for mutuality, exhibiting the highest strength centrality (strength = 2.28), and the second highest closeness centrality (closeness = 0.87).

Figure 3
Line graph displaying variables such as trust, self-esteem, and jealousy across two columns labeled Strength and Closeness. Lines depict variations in negative to positive range for each column.

Figure 3. Estimated values for strength and closeness centrality of relationship satisfaction and related personality features (N = 810). For easier comparison of centrality indices, z-scores are displayed on the x-axis, rather than raw values.

Network comparison

The second aim of this study was to compare network structures between heterosexual men and women. For valid comparisons of correlations (or edges in networks), scales should demonstrate measurement invariance across groups, at least on a metric level (Thompson, 2016). Therefore, invariance analyses were conducted for all subscales, using structural equation modeling (Thompson, 2016). The precondition of at least partial metric invariance of the subscales for women and men was given, as demonstrated in the Supplementary material. There, we also report where model modifications were necessary to obtain metric invariance.

Table 2 contains the results of group mean comparison tests for all network variables. Women and men did not significantly differ in RSA, attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, trust, self-esteem, mutuality, differentiation, and sociosexual behavior. Women scored significantly higher than men on jealousy and sexual satisfaction, with small effect sizes each (Cohen, 1988). Men scored significantly higher than women on emotional control (medium effect), sociosexual attitude (small effect), and sociosexual desire (large effect).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and group comparisons by gender for relationship satisfaction and related personality features (n = 300 per group).

Figure 4 shows the estimated network structures for women and men, respectively. To compare networks, respective measures of global strength, network structure, and edge weight differences were computed. Concerning global strength, the NCT revealed no significant gender difference (difference in global strength = 0.21, p = 0.705). Thus, the total sum of edge weights was approximately equal in both networks. Next, a difference test between the two precision matrices yielded a statistically significant result (maximum edge difference = 0.27, p = 0.006), indicating that at least one edge weight significantly differed between the two networks.

Figure 4
Network diagrams illustrate relationships between psychological features for women and men. Nodes represent variables like relationship satisfaction, attachment anxiety, and self-esteem. Lines indicate connections, with varying thickness and color representing strength and type of relationship. Annotations define each abbreviation.

Figure 4. Estimated network structure of relationship satisfaction and related personality features for women (n = 300) and men (n = 300). Blue bars represent positive edges between nodes, whereas red bars represent negative edges. Line thickness reflects effect size, with thicker edges reflecting stronger partial correlations.

To identify which edge(s) varied between networks, an edge weight difference test was performed. Six edge weights were significantly different between women and men: sociosexual behavior–sociosexual attitude (women: r = 0.40; men: r = 0.18; p = 0.001), jealousy–trust (women: r = −0.34; men: r = −0.59; p = 0.005), attachment anxiety–sexual satisfaction (women: r = 0.00; men: r = −0.08; p = 0.016), trust–differentiation (women: r = 0.07; men: r = 0.00; p = 0.025), RSA–trust (women: r = 0.17; men: r = 0.03; p = 0.023), and RSA–attachment avoidance (women: r = −0.15; men: r = −0.31; p = 0.027). However, after applying a Bonferroni correction for multiple tests, none of the observed differences remained significant, indicating that there were no substantial differences between the networks of men and women.

Discussion

Previous research has identified various personality features that are linked to RSA but has primarily focused on bivariate effects. These features stem from different personality approaches and provide different perspectives on similar constructs. Due to this interrelatedness, it remains uncertain how each personality feature uniquely correlates with RSA. The present study sheds light on this uncertainty by exploring the interplay of RSA with its related personality features, using network analysis. The first aim was to determine the unique connections and the position of RSA within a network structure, consisting of attachment, jealousy and trust, self-esteem, relationship self-efficacy, sexual satisfaction, and sociosexuality. RSA and mutuality were the most central nodes in the network. Attachment, trust, mutuality, and sexual satisfaction were directly linked to RSA, whereas its links to jealousy, self-esteem, emotional control, differentiation, and sociosexuality became negligible. The second aim of this study was to explore the moderating role of gender. Networks of men and women were largely similar, implying that no substantial differences between genders exist in the interplay between RSA and personality.

RSA and mutuality were the most integral nodes in the network, as measured by strength and closeness centrality, thereby showcasing high potential to reciprocally activate or inhibit surrounding nodes (Costantini et al., 2015). This result supports the idea that RSA is intricately tied to a complex interplay of personality features that pertain to romantic relationships, with these features strongly depending on one another. Mutuality plays a similarly crucial role in this interplay, exhibiting a particularly strong connection to RSA.

Attachment anxiety and avoidance were negatively, trust, mutuality, and sexual satisfaction positively associated with RSA in the network, that is, after controlling for the effect of other relevant variables. This is in line with previous reports of positive correlations between RSA and trust (Fitzpatrick and Lafontaine, 2017), relationship self-efficacy (Weiser and Weigel, 2016), and sexual satisfaction (Byers, 2005), and with reports of negative correlations between RSA and insecure attachment (Stackert and Bursik, 2003). The present results not only confirm these previously reported associations but also reinforce their stance, as they appear to persist beyond the confounding effect of related variables.

The positive association between RSA and trust indicates that individuals who have faith in the strength of their relationship and who believe that their partner will provide help when needed, are generally more satisfied within the relationship. Similarly, individuals who are satisfied with sexual activities within the relationship are likely to experience higher levels of RSA. Moreover, mutuality emerged as a particularly important correlate of RSA, further supporting the finding that individuals with a conviction of being able to mutually provide and receive emotional support within a romantic relationship are concurrently more satisfied with it. Furthermore, anxious and avoidant attachment both correlate negatively with RSA. Individuals who either fear being abandoned or who are uncomfortable with emotional intimacy, tend to be less satisfied with their relationship, regardless of other features, such as trust or jealousy.

While some features formed substantial edges with RSA in the network, other features that have previously been shown to correlate with RSA took comparatively peripheral positions in the network, namely jealousy (Andersen et al., 1995), self-esteem (Fincham and Bradbury, 1993), and sociosexuality (Webster et al., 2015).

In our network, the link between self-esteem and RSA was primarily expressed through attachment anxiety, which was directly connected to both constructs and formed the shortest indirect path between them. This aligns with previous findings showing negative correlations between attachment anxiety and both self-esteem (Foster et al., 2007) and RSA (Stackert and Bursik, 2003). Although a direct edge between self-esteem and RSA was present, it was notably weak. This stands in contrast to much of the literature reporting stronger associations between the two (Fincham and Bradbury, 1993). Since network edges reflect associations that remain after controlling for other variables, this weak edge suggests that the relationship between self-esteem and RSA may be largely accounted for by shared connections with other variables, particularly attachment anxiety. Hence, individuals with low self-esteem are not necessarily dissatisfied with their relationships per se, but may experience heightened attachment anxiety, which in turn shapes the perception of their relationships. Furthermore, given the view that self-esteem comprises various domains (e.g., academic, physical, social; Shackelford, 2001), only its relational aspect may be meaningfully tied to RSA, which could explain the relatively modest direct association observed in the network.

Regarding jealousy, previous studies have found a negative correlation with RSA (Andersen et al., 1995), as confirmed by our correlational analyses. Much like the link between RSA and self-esteem, this direct association was substantially reduced in the network, implying that jealousy and RSA mostly covary due to their respective associations with trust and attachment anxiety.

Finally, no substantial edges were present among facets of sociosexuality and RSA in the network. What these features share in variance was mainly captured by sexual satisfaction. More specifically, sociosexual desire and sexual satisfaction formed a substantial edge, as well as sexual satisfaction and RSA. This pattern aligns with previous findings, showcasing a positive association between sexual satisfaction and RSA (Butzer and Campbell, 2008).

Taken together, when looking at the interplay of RSA and personality features holistically, some features play a more prominent role (trust, mutuality, sexual satisfaction, anxious, and avoidant attachment), whereas others have a less profound impact (jealousy, self-esteem, sociosexuality, emotional control, and differentiation). These insights only emerge once the interplay between personality and RSA is viewed from a holistic, comprehensive perspective, and by taking many relevant variables into account.

The second aim of this study was to investigate whether and how gender affects the network structure of RSA and its corresponding personality features. A large body of research has investigated gender differences in personality features, yielding both robust and inconclusive findings. Furthermore, evidence regarding the moderating effect of gender on the associations between personality and RSA is scarce, and synthesizing existing literature into a conclusive picture remains challenging. To address this issue, additional network analyses were conducted separately for men and women.

Regarding mean differences, men scored higher on emotional control as well as sociosexual attitude and desire than women. Women scored higher than men on jealousy and sexual satisfaction. Finally, no differences were found in RSA, attachment anxiety and avoidance, trust, self-esteem, mutuality, differentiation, and sociosexual behavior. However, more relevant to the present purpose was the fact that the associations between those features did not differ between men and women, despite some differences in means.

Networks of men and women were highly similar in this study. The findings align with the gender similarity hypothesis (Hyde, 2005) and the notion that within-gender variation often exceeds between-gender variation (Feingold, 1994; Schmitt, 2005). Some gender differences emerged, particularly in the edges between RSA and trust, as well as avoidant attachment. Although these differences did not remain significant after Bonferroni correction, small but true effects may have been obscured due to this rather conservative method. Such strict corrections are used to avoid type I errors, but this increases the risk of failing to detect true differences and increases type II errors. Thus, while the overall pattern supports the idea that men and women are more similar than different in how personality and RSA are connected, this conclusion should be drawn with caution. Accordingly, the network interpretation for the total sample appears broadly applicable to both men and women, though subtle differences may exist.

Limitations and future research

Self-report measures on sensitive topics such as sexual satisfaction and jealousy may be subject to social desirability bias, potentially leading participants to underreport negative emotions or agree more strongly with socially accepted statements. This could affect the accuracy of the observed associations in the network. Prior research has shown that individuals often adjust their responses on intimate topics to align with perceived social norms or to maintain a positive self-image (Tourangeau and Yan, 2007). While anonymity was ensured to mitigate such biases, their influence cannot be entirely ruled out.

Despite the wide age range of participants in this study, the average age was relatively low. This may limit the extent to which the findings apply to older individuals, and caution is warranted when interpreting the results across different age groups.

A post-hoc power analysis, conducted with the R package powerly (version 1.8.6; Constantin et al., 2023), revealed that at least 1,904 participants would be required to achieve a statistical power of 0.8 while maintaining similar network characteristics (nodes = 13; density = 0.67; coefficient range = −0.6 to 0.5). However, this estimation requires an a priori definition of the network structure. Due to the exploratory nature of the present study, no such definition was formulated and according to the conducted stability tests, the sample size appeared sufficiently large to yield robust results for the combined network (Epskamp and Fried, 2018).

Our network contained only undirected effects; hence, causal pathways were not explored. Directed hypotheses could be examined in future investigations, but would require longitudinal data (Borsboom et al., 2021). Applying such an approach to the present study variables would help better understand the meaning of the relationships identified in the present study.

Another avenue for future research involves the investigation of partner effects. The results of the present study imply that similarities rather than differences in personality features between romantic partners might be slightly more purposeful if the goal is to maintain a satisfying relationship. Yet, it has not been specifically investigated if individuals who are satisfied with their relationships exhibit similar levels of relevant personality features as their romantic partners. Focusing on network comparisons on a dyadic level could further contribute to understanding individual differences in RSA.

Finally, this study demonstrated that mutuality plays a key role within the relational network. To improve RSA, one practical avenue may lie in enhancing self-efficacy beliefs, specifically related to mutuality. Evidence suggests that self-efficacy beliefs are susceptible to training interventions (Vîslǎ et al., 2022). Therapeutic interventions, particularly couples therapy, can provide a structured and safe environment in which partners can develop and strengthen mutual behaviors such as emotional attunement, responsiveness, and collaborative problem-solving (Roddy et al., 2020). As couples gain mastery in these areas, their confidence in their ability to co-create a supportive and reciprocal relationship is likely to increase, thereby reinforcing mutuality and, potentially, enhancing overall RSA (cf. Satir, 1976).

Conclusion

In summary, the present study confirms most of the extant evidence regarding the role of personality in RSA, while also providing a more differentiated perspective on this complex interplay. Mutuality, a specific domain of relationship self-efficacy, appears to play a key role in maintaining satisfying relationships and could guide interventions that are designed to enhance satisfaction with romantic relationships. While men and women differ in a few aspects that constitute RSA, their respective networks are largely similar. Results contribute to the ongoing debate regarding gender differences in personality and generally support the notion that men and women share more similarities than differences when it comes to RSA and personality features that pertain to it.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: Data and R code are available under http://osf.io/wxgyd.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Human Sciences of the University of Bern. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

OS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. DG: Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. UR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – review & editing. ST: Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Open access funding by University of Bern.

Acknowledgments

We thank Robin Zbinden for his valuable help in the data collection process.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1587405/full#supplementary-material

References

Andersen, P. A., Eloy, S. V., Guerrero, L. K., and Spitzberg, B. H. (1995). Romantic jealousy and relational satisfaction: a look at the impact of jealousy experience and expression. Commun. Rep. 8, 77–85. doi: 10.1080/08934219509367613

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2009). The first 10,000 adult attachment interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups. Attach. Hum. Dev. 11, 223–263. doi: 10.1080/14616730902814762

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84, 191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, J. A., Seager, M., and Brown, B. (2015). Gender differences in the association between attachment style and adult relationship satisfaction. New Male Stud. 4, 63–74. Available online at: http://newmalestudies.com/OJS/index.php/nms/article/view/195

Google Scholar

Bartholomew, K., and Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a four-category model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 226–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, B. (1988). Eifersucht in Partnerschaften: Zusammenhang der Eifersuchtsdisposition mit ausgewählten Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen und demographischen Variablen [Jealousy in Relationships: Correlation of Jealousy Disposition with Selected Personality Traits and Demographic Variables] (Unpublished dotoral dissertation). University of Trier.

Google Scholar

Be, D., Whisman, M. A., and Uebelacker, L. A. (2013). Prospective associations between marital adjustment and life satisfaction. Pers. Relatsh. 20, 728–739. doi: 10.1111/pere.12011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleidorn, W., Arslan, R. C., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Gebauer, J. E., Potter, J., et al. (2016). Age and gender differences in self-esteem – a cross-cultural window. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 396–410. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borsboom, D., Deserno, M. K., Rhemtulla, M., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., McNally, R. J., et al. (2021). Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science. Nat. Rev. Methods Primer 1:58. doi: 10.1038/s43586-021-00055-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., and Beach, S. R. H. (2000). Research on the nature and determinants of marital satisfaction: a decade in review. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 964–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00964.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., and Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles, gender and parental problem drinking. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 8, 451–466. doi: 10.1177/026540759184001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burger, J., Isvoranu, A.-M., Lunansky, G., Haslbeck, J. M. B., Epskamp, S., Hoekstra, R. H. A., et al. (2023). Reporting standards for psychological network analyses in cross-sectional data. Psychol. Methods 28, 806–824. doi: 10.1037/met0000471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butzer, B., and Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and relationship satisfaction: a study of married couples. Pers. Relatsh. 15, 141–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00189.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byers, E. S. (2005). Relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction: a longitudinal study of individuals in long-term relationships. J. Sex Res. 42, 113–118. doi: 10.1080/00224490509552264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeldue, M., and Boswell, S. S. (2012). Predictors of relationship self-efficacy in undergraduates. Psi Chi J. Psychol. Res. 17, 154–162. doi: 10.24839/2164-8204.JN17.4.154

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, C. J. (2012). Meta-analyses of sex differences in responses to sexual versus emotional infidelity: men and women are more similar than different. Psychol. Women Q. 36, 25–37. doi: 10.1177/0361684311414537

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., and Chen, Z. (2008). Extended Bayesian information criteria for model selection with large model spaces. Biometrika 95, 759–771. doi: 10.1093/biomet/asn034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Collani, G., and Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwetgefühl von Rosenberg [A revised version of the German adaptation of Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale]. Z. Für Differ. Diagn. Psychol. 24, 3–7. doi: 10.1024//0170-1789.24.1.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantin, M. A., Schuurman, N. K., and Vermunt, J. K. (2023). A general Monte Carlo method for sample size analysis in the context of network models. Psychol. Methods. doi: 10.1037/met0000555. [Epub ahead of print].

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., Perugini, M., Mõttus, R., Waldorp, L. J., et al. (2015). State of the aRt personality research: a tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R. J. Res. Personal. 54, 13–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costantini, G., Richetin, J., Preti, E., Casini, E., Epskamp, S., and Perugini, M. (2019). Stability and variability of personality networks. A tutorial on recent developments in network psychometrics. Personal. Individ. Differ. 136, 68–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinkel, A., and Balck, F. (2005). An evaluation of the German Relationship Assessment scale. Swiss J. Psychol. 64, 259–263. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.64.4.259

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dush, C. M. K., and Amato, P. R. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 22, 607–627. doi: 10.1177/0265407505056438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edlund, J. E., and Sagarin, B. J. (2017). Sex differences in jealousy: a 25-year retrospective. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 259–302. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrenthal, J. C., Zimmermann, J., Brenk-Franz, K., Dinger, U., Schauenburg, H., Brähler, E., et al. (2021). Evaluation of a short version of the experiences in close relationships-revised questionnaire (ECR-RD8): results from a representative German sample. BMC Psychol. 9:140. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00637-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., and Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 195–212. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O. J., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., and Borsboom, D. (2012). qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i04

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epskamp, S., and Fried, E. I. (2018). A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol. Methods 23, 617–634. doi: 10.1037/met0000167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erol, R. Y., and Orth, U. (2014). Development of self-esteem and relationship satisfaction in couples: two longitudinal studies. Dev. Psychol. 50, 2291–2303. doi: 10.1037/a0037370

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallis, E. E., Rehman, U. S., Woody, E. Z., and Purdon, C. (2016). The longitudinal association of relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction in long-term relationships. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 822–831. doi: 10.1037/fam0000205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 116, 429–456. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferring, D., and Filipp, S.-H. (1996). Messung des Selbstwertgefühls: Befunde zu Reliabilität, Validität und Stabilität der Rosenberg-Skala [Measurement of self-esteem: Findings on reliability, validity, and stability of the Rosenberg Scale]. Diagnostica 42, 284–292.

Google Scholar

Fincham, F. D., and Bradbury, T. N. (1987). The assessment of marital quality: a reevaluation. J. Marriage Fam. 49, 797–809. doi: 10.2307/351973

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincham, F. D., and Bradbury, T. N. (1993). Marital satisfaction, depression, and attributions: a longitudinal analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 442–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.442

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincham, F. D., Harold, G. T., and Gano-Phillips, S. (2000). The longitudinal association between attributions and marital satisfaction: direction of effects and role of efficacy expectations. J. Fam. Psychol. 14, 267–285. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.2.267

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, J., and Lafontaine, M.-F. (2017). Attachment, trust, and satisfaction in relationships: investigating actor, partner, and mediating effects. Pers. Relatsh. 24, 640–662. doi: 10.1111/pere.12203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. D., Kernis, M. H., and Goldman, B. M. (2007). Linking adult attachment to self-esteem stability. Self Identity 6, 64–73. doi: 10.1080/15298860600832139

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, J., Hastie, T., and Tibshirani, R. (2008). Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics 9, 432–441. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxm045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fruchterman, T. M. J., and Reingold, E. M. (1991). Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Softw. Pract. Exp. 21, 1129–1264. doi: 10.1002/spe.4380211102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M., and Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital processes predictive of later dissolution: behavior, physiology, and health. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 221–233. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.221

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gubler, D. A., Schlegel, K., Richter, M., Kapanci, T., and Troche, S. J. (2023). The green-eyed monster in social media – development and validation of a digital jealousy scale. Psychol. Test Adapt. and Develop. 4, 13–27. doi: 10.1027/2698-1866/a000033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. R. (2003). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy, including self-report data, psychophysiological responses, interpersonal violence, and morbid jealousy. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 102–128. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_102-128

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassebrauck, M., and Fehr, B. (2002). Dimensions of relationship quality. Pers. Relatsh. 9, 253–270. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. J. Marriage Fam. 50, 93–98. doi: 10.2307/352430

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendrick, S. S., Hendrick, C., and Adler, N. L. (1988). Romantic relationships: love, satisfaction, and staying together. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 980–988. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.980

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herzberg, P. Y., and Wildfang, S. (2018). Essstörungssymptome und Persönlichkeit: Implikationen für die Diagnostik aus einer Netzwerkperspektive [Eating disorder symptoms and personality: implications for diagnostics from a network perspective]. Z. Für Psychiatr. Psychol. Psychother. 66, 187–194. doi: 10.1024/1661-4747/a000355

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirschberger, G., Srivastava, S., Marsh, P., Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (2009). Attachment, marital satisfaction, and divorce during the first fifteen years of parenthood. Pers. Relatsh. 16, 401–420. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2009.01230.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D. E., Imai, K., King, G., and Stuart, E. A. (2007). Matching as nonparametric preprocessing for reducing model dependence in parametric causal inference. Polit. Anal. 15, 199–236. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpl013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huston, T. L., and Houts, R. M. (1998). “The psychological infrastructure of courtship and marriage: the role of personality and compatibility in romantic relationships,” in The Developmental Course of Marital Dysfunction, ed. T. N. Bradbury (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 114–151. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511527814.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. Am. Psychol. 60, 581–592. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. B., Miller, R. B., Oka, M., and Henry, R. G. (2014). Gender differences in marital satisfaction: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 76, 105–129. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12077

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H.-Y. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor. Dent. Endod. 38, 52–54. doi: 10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., and Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. W. B. Saunders. Indiana University Press. doi: 10.1097/00005053-194903000-00016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrance, K.-A., and Byers, E. S. (1995). Sexual satisfaction in long-term heterosexual relationships: the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction. Pers. Relatsh. 2, 267–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1995.tb00092.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leary, M. R., and Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: sociometer theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 32, 1–62. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesch, E., and Engelbrecht, S.-K. (2011). Relationship satisfaction and gender differences in a South African farm-worker community. South Afr. Rev. Sociol. 42, 58–77. doi: 10.1080/21528586.2011.563542

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, T., and Chan, D. K. -S. (2012). How anxious and avoidant attachment affect romantic relationship quality differently: a meta-analytic review. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 406–419. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, F. G., Morúa, W., and Rice, K. G. (2007). Factor structure, stability, and predictive validity of college students' relationship self-efficacy beliefs. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 40, 80–96. doi: 10.1080/07481756.2007.11909807

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, T. C., Bejanyan, K., Castro, G., and Lee, R. A. (2013). Attachment styles as predictors of Facebook-related jealousy and surveillance in romantic relationships. Pers. Relatsh. 20, 1–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01393.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattingly, B. A., Clark, E. M., Weidler, D. J., Bullock, M., Hackathorn, J., and Blankmeyer, K. (2011). Sociosexual orientation, commitment, and infidelity: a mediation analysis. J. Soc. Psychol. 151, 222–226. doi: 10.1080/00224540903536162

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. L., Griffin, D. W., Rose, P., and Bellavia, G. M. (2003). Calibrating the sociometer: the relational contingencies of self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 63–84. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.63

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nickull, S., Källström, M., and Jern, P. (2022). An exploratory network analysis of sexual and relationship satisfaction comparing partnered cisgendered men and women. J. Sex. Med. 19, 711–718. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2022.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz-Ospina, E., and Roser, M. (2020). Marriages and Divorces. Our World in Data. Available online at: https://ourworldindata.org/marriages-and-divorces (Accessed April 4, 2024).

Google Scholar

Peixoto, M. M. (2023). Differences in solitary and dyadic sexual desire and sexual satisfaction in heterosexual and nonheterosexual cisgender men and women. J. Sex. Med. 20, 597–604. doi: 10.1093/jsxmed/qdad033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Penke, L. (2011). “Revised sociosexual orientation inventory,” in Handbook of Sexuality-Related Measures, eds. T. D. Fisher, C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, and S. L. Davis (Abingdon, VA; Oxfordshire: Taylor and Francis), 622–625.

Google Scholar

Penke, L., and Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Beyond global sociosexual orientations: a more differentiated look at sociosexuality and its effects on courtship and romantic relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1113–1135. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pines, A., and Aronson, E. (1983). Antecedents, correlates, and consequences of sexual jealousy. J. Pers. 51, 108–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1983.tb00857.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., and Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 576–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00393.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., and Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment measures: a 25-year review. J. Psychosom. Res. 69, 419–432. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.08.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rempel, J. K., Holmes, J. G., and Zanna, M. P. (1985). Trust in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 95–112. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.95

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Renshaw, K. D., McKnight, P., Caska, C. M., and Blais, R. K. (2011). The utility of the relationship assessment scale in multiple types of relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 28, 435–447. doi: 10.1177/0265407510377850

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, M., Schlegel, K., Thomas, P., and Troche, S. J. (2022). Adult attachment and personality as predictors of jealousy in romantic relationships, Front. Psychol. 13:861481. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.861481

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riggio, H. R., Weiser, D. A., Valenzuela, A. M., Lui, P. P., Montes, R., and Heuer, J. (2011). Initial validation of a measure of self-efficacy in romantic relationships. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 601–606. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riggio, H. R., Weiser, D. A., Valenzuela, A. M., Lui, P. P., Montes, R., and Heuer, J. (2013). Self-efficacy in romantic relationships: prediction of relationship attitudes and outcomes. J. Soc. Psychol. 153, 629–650. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2013.801826

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tracy, J. L., Gosling, S. D., and Potter, J. (2002). Global self-esteem across the life span. Psychol. Aging 17, 423–434. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.3.423

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roddy, M. K., Walsh, L. M., Rothman, K., Hatch, S. G., and Doss, B. D. (2020). Meta-analysis of couple therapy: effects across outcomes, designs, timeframes, and other moderators. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 88:583. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, L. M., DiBello, A. M., Øverup, C. S., and Neighbors, C. (2015). The price of distrust: trust, anxious attachment, jealousy, and partner abuse. Partn. Abuse 6, 298–319. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.6.3.298

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400876136

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., and Bringle, R. G. (2004). Jealousy and commitment: perceived threat and the effect of relationship alternatives. Pers. Relatsh. 11, 451–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00092.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagarin, B. J., Martin, A. L., Coutinho, S. A., Edlund, J. E., Patel, L., Skowronski, J. J., et al. (2012). Sex differences in jealousy: a meta-analytic examination. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 595–614. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sainani, K. L. (2012). Dealing with non-normal data. PMandR 4, 1001–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2012.10.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, J., and Böcker, S. (1993). Die Deutsche Form der Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS): Eine kurze Skala zur Messung der Zufriedenheit in einer Partnerschaft [The German version of the Relationship Assessment scale (RAS): a short scale for measuring satisfaction in a dyadic relationship]. Diagnostica 39, 55–62.

Google Scholar

Sands, A., Thompson, E. J., and Gaysina, D. (2017). Long-term influences of parental divorce on offspring affective disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 218, 105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Satir, V. (1976). Making Contact. Millbrae, CA: Celestial Arts.

Google Scholar

Schaan, V. K., Schulz, A., Schächinger, H., and Vögele, C. (2019). Parental divorce is associated with an increased risk to develop mental disorders in women. J. Affect. Disord. 257, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.06.071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, S., Strauss, B., Höger, D., and Brähler, E. (2004). Die Adult Attachment Scale (AAS) – Teststatistische Prüfung und Normierung der deutschen Version [The Adult Attachment Scale (AAS) – Psychometric evaluation and normation of the German version]. PPmP 54, 375–382. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-815000

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: a 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 247–311. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, M. J., Falkenau, K., and Montada, L. (1995). Zur Messung von Eifersucht über stellvertretende Emotionsbegriffe und zur Bereichsspezifität der Eifersuchtsneigung [On the measurement of jealousy via vicarious emotional terms and on the domain specificity of the tendency toward jealousy]. Diagnostica 41, 131–149.

Google Scholar

Shackelford, T. K. (2001). Self-esteem in marriage. Personal. Individ. Differ. 30, 371–390. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00023-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siffert, A., and Bodenmann, G. (2010). Entwicklung eines neuen multidimensionalen Fragebogens zur Erfassung der Partnerschaftsqualität (FPQ) [Development of a new multidimensional questionnaire to assess relationship quality (FPQ)]. Z. Für Fam. 22, 242–255. doi: 10.20377/jfr-277

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, J. A., and Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 870–883. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.870

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38, 15–28. doi: 10.2307/350547

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprecher, S. (2002). Sexual satisfaction in premarital relationships: associations with satisfaction, love, commitment, and stability. J. Sex Res. 39, 190–196. doi: 10.1080/00224490209552141

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprecher, S., Treger, S., and Sakaluk, J. K. (2013). Premarital sexual standards and sociosexuality: gender, ethnicity, and cohort differences. Arch. Sex. Behav. 42, 1395–1405. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0145-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stackert, R. A., and Bursik, K. (2003). Why am I unsatisfied? Adult attachment style, gendered irrational relationship beliefs, and young adult romantic relationship satisfaction. Personal. Individ. Differ. 34, 1419–1429. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00124-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Team, R. C. (2023). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org (Accessed May 22, 2024).

Google Scholar

Thompson, M. S. (2016). “Assessing measurement invariance of scales using multiple-group structural equation modeling,” in Principles and Methods of Test Construction, eds. K. Schweizer and C. DiStefano (Göttingen: Hogrefe), 218–244.

Google Scholar

Tourangeau, R., and Yan, T. (2007). Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychol. Bull. 133:859. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.859

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Borkulo, C. D., van Bork, R., Boschloo, L., Kossakowski, J. J., Tio, P., Schoevers, R. A., et al. (2023). Comparing network structures on three aspects: a permutation test. Psychol. Methods 28, 1273–1285. doi: 10.1037/met0000476

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vîslǎ, A., Allemand, M., and Flückiger, C. (2022). Within- and between-patients associations between self-efficacy, outcome expectation, and symptom change in cognitive behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. J. Clin. Psychol. 79, 86–104. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23407

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohs, K. D., Catanese, K. R., and Baumeister, R. F. (2004). “Sex in ‘his' versus ‘her' relationships,” in Handbook of Sexuality in Close Relationships, eds. J. H. Harvey, A. Wenzel, and S. Sprecher (Mahwah, NJ: Psychology Press), 455–474.

Google Scholar

Webster, G. D., Laurenceau, J. -P., Smith, C. V., Mahaffey, A. L., Bryan, A. D., and Brunell, A. B. (2015). An investment model of sociosexuality, relationship satisfaction, and commitment: evidence from dating, engaged, and newlywed couples. J. Res. Personal. 55, 112–126. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.02.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wegner, R., Roy, A. R. K., Gorman, K. R., and Ferguson, K. (2018). Attachment, relationship communication style and the use of jealousy induction techniques in romantic relationships. Personal. Individ. Differ. 129, 6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiser, D. A., and Weigel, D. J. (2016). Self-efficacy in romantic relationships: direct and indirect effects on relationship maintenance and satisfaction. Personal. Individ. Differ. 89, 152–156. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

White, G. L. (1981). Jealousy and partner's perceived motives for attraction to a rival. Soc. Psychol. Q. 44, 24–30. doi: 10.2307/3033859

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wieselquist, J. (2009). Interpersonal forgiveness, trust, and the investment model of commitment. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 26, 531–548. doi: 10.1177/0265407509347931

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yam, F. C. (2023). The relationship between partner phubbing and life satisfaction: the mediating role of relationship satisfaction and perceived romantic relationship quality. Psychol. Rep. 126, 303–331. doi: 10.1177/00332941221144611

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, C. D., Lajunen, T., and Sullman, M. J. M. (2023). Trust in relationships: a preliminary investigation of the influence of parental divorce, breakup experiences, adult attachment style, and close relationship beliefs on dyadic trust. Front. Psychol. 14:1260480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1260480

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: relationship satisfaction, attachment, jealousy, self-esteem, relationship self-efficacy, sociosexuality, network analysis

Citation: Schulz OT, Gubler DA, Raemy UE and Troche SJ (2025) Mapping love: a personality-centered network analysis of relationship satisfaction. Front. Psychol. 16:1587405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1587405

Received: 04 March 2025; Accepted: 16 June 2025;
Published: 21 July 2025.

Edited by:

Dario Alparone, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, France

Reviewed by:

Philipp Yorck Herzberg, Helmut Schmidt University, Germany
Jacky Ho, University of Saint Joseph, Macao SAR, China

Copyright © 2025 Schulz, Gubler, Raemy and Troche. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Danièle Anne Gubler, ZGFuaWVsZS5ndWJsZXJAdW5pYmUuY2g=; Stefan Johannes Troche, c3RlZmFuLnRyb2NoZUB1bmliZS5jaA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.