Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 February 2016
Sec. Cognition

Commentary: Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: on the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses

\r\nAndreas B. Eder*Andreas B. Eder1*Klaus RothermundKlaus Rothermund2Bernhard HommelBernhard Hommel3
  • 1Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Jena, Jena, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, Netherlands

A commentary on
Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: on the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses

by Kozlik, J., Neumann, R., and Lozo, L. (2015). Front. Psychol. 6:563. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00563

In a recent review paper on theoretical explanations of affective stimulus-response compatibility (aSRC) effects between positive/negative stimuli and approach/avoidance-related movements, Kozlik et al. (2015; KNL) argue that an evaluative-coding approach cannot fully account for aSRC effects with facial actions and that motivational orientations provide a better explanation. Their arguments are based on three observations that they consider incompatible with an evaluative-coding approach (Eder and Rothermund, 2008, ER) and the Theory of Event Coding (TEC; Hommel et al., 2001) from which the approach is derived: (1) attempts to dissociate evaluative coding and motivational orientation showed separable contributions from these two factors (Krieglmeyer et al., 2010); (2) aSRC can be easily changed in manual actions (by instructions of action labels) but hardly in facial actions (Neumann et al., 2014); and (3) the hemispheric asymmetry in the control of positive/negative facial expressions matches the hemispheric asymmetry assumed for motivational orientations (Davidson et al., 1990). In the following, we will explain why none of these three observations is inconsistent with an evaluative-coding approach and (4) why the motivational-systems approach represents little more than a rephrasing of observations. Rather than contrasting both accounts, we propose a theoretical integration of cognitive and motivational aspects as a more promising approach for future research.

1. Krieglmeyer et al. (2010) report a “motivational” compatibility effect (on top of an evaluative-coding effect) “that was independent of evaluative compatibility between stimulus valence and response label valence.” However, we note that this effect (a) actually interacted with evaluative compatibility (Exp. 1); (b) was markedly reduced (Exp. 2A) or eliminated (Exp. 2B) in non-evaluative tasks; and (c) is open to an alternative interpretation with internal recoding mechanisms. As noted by ER (Eder and Rothermund, 2008, Footnote 7), “internal recodings of action representations are likely …if perceptual action frames (e.g., moving the stimulus toward or away from the viewer) are more salient for action control.” Manikin movements toward and away from words may hence have triggered action re-coding in “toward” and “away” in some conditions, which explains a small instruction-independent aSRC effect.

2. We agree that smiling and frowning are linked to positive and negative affects, respectively, more rigidly than non-facial actions commonly are. We also agree that aSRC effects for facial responses are less affected by instructions of arbitrary action goals (e.g., performing “rain” and “sun” actions; Neumann et al., 2014). However, we fail to see why that might imply different mechanisms and explanations. In fact, the affectively extended TEC can easily account for all of these observations. According to TEC, perceptions and actions are represented by networks of distributed feature codes that represent their perceivable features. An action plan would thus code, among other things, the anticipated kinesthetic feedback from a smile or visual feedback after a lever movement. Considering that affect is derived from bodily sensations (James, 1884), feature networks should also incorporate codes representing the positivity/negativity of action consequences (Beckers et al., 2002; Eder et al., 2015). Perceptual and action events of the same valence then share feature codes—which explains aSCR (Eder and Klauer, 2007, 2009; Eder et al., 2012).

Associations between movements and codes of their sensory consequences emerge through experience and as a function of contingency and contiguity, which also holds for facial expressions (Ekman et al., 1980; Kunde et al., 2011). The current action goal moderates feature networks by weighting features on goal-related dimensions more strongly (Memelink and Hommel, 2013), and verbal instructions can help to disambiguate goal-relevant action consequences (Eder et al., 2013). This means that the size of aSCR effects should depend on the strength of the acquired association and on the current goal/feature weighting (for evidence see Phaf et al., 2014). It makes sense to assume that affective consequences of smiling are more systematically/strongly associated with the responsible motor pattern than affective consequences of pulling something toward one's body. This implies that the relative contribution of previously acquired associations with affective codes is stronger for facial than for manual actions, which explains why the former are more immune to goal manipulations.

3. KNL cite several findings suggesting that facial expressions of happiness and of disgust are associated with particularly strong activation of left and right frontal cortex, respectively. While these observations actually do not speak to the involvement of motivational factors, KNL follow Davidson et al.'s (1990) terminology in simply labeling happiness an “approach emotion” and disgust a “withdrawal emotion.” Furthermore, previous research showed that only smiles that include activation of the orbicularis pars lateralis (Duchenne's marker) result in greater left frontal activation, while other smiles do not (Ekman and Davidson, 1993). Thus, even if we would agree to a close link between voluntary smiles and motivational brain systems, it is unclear whether Neumann et al.'s (2014) facial task without Duchenne's marker was sufficient to engage these systems.

4. Is KNL's “motivational orientation account” a reasonable theoretical alternative that explicates aSRC effects in terms of a different mechanism? Unfortunately, no mechanism is presented, which renders the referred-to “motivational orientations” theoretically empty boxes without additional explanatory value. “Explaining” approach-avoidance tendencies through activations of corresponding approach-avoidance systems is but a re-description of the to-be-explained phenomenon in allegedly explanatory terms, which we consider about as meaningful as, say, explaining perception through a “perception system.” Truly useful scientific models require the specification of the contents of motivation systems and the processes operating on these contents. How are positive and negative stimuli translated into corresponding behaviors? How does a “motivation system” select and energize movement patterns producing approach or avoidance? Our expanded TEC approach has the potential to provide answers to these critical questions, including those that KNL interpret as too challenging. Instead of contrasting both approaches, we suggest to integrate perceptual, emotional, and motivational aspects of human action into a unitary theoretical model that makes concrete, testable assumptions about the structures and processes of actions effecting approach and avoidance (see Eder and Hommel, 2013; Eder and Rothermund, 2013).

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

Preparation of this comment was supported by grant ED 201/2-2 of the German Research Foundation (DFG) to AE.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Beckers, T., De Houwer, J., and Eelen, P. (2002). Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: the case of the associative affective Simon effect. Psychol. Res. 66, 166–173. doi: 10.1007/s00426-002-0090-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., and Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology. I. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 330–341. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., and Hommel, B. (2013). Anticipatory control of approach and avoidance: an ideomotor approach. Emot. Rev. 5, 275–279. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477505

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., and Klauer, K. C. (2007). Common valence coding in action and evaluation: affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli. Cogn. Emot. 21, 1297–1322. doi: 10.1080/02699930701438277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., and Klauer, K. C. (2009). A common-coding account of the bidirectional evaluation–behavior link. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 138, 218–235. doi: 10.1037/a0015220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., Müsseler, J., and Hommel, B. (2012). The structure of affective action representations: temporal binding of affective response codes. Psychol. Res. 76, 111–118. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0327-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., and Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 137, 262–281. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., and Rothermund, K. (2013). “Emotional action: an ideomotor model,” in Handbook of Psychology of Emotions: Recent Theoretical Perspectives and Novel Empirical Findings, Vol. 1, eds C. Mohiyeddini and M. Eysenck (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 11–38.

Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., and De Houwer, J. (2013). Affective compatibility between stimuli and response goals: a primer for a new implicit measure of attitudes. PLoS ONE 8:e79210. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., De Houwer, J., and Hommel, B. (2015). Directive and incentive functions of affective action consequences: an ideomotor approach. Psychol. Res. 79, 630–649. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0590-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Davidson, R. J. (1993). Voluntary smiling changes regional brain activity. Psychol. Sci. 4, 342–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00576.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Roper, G., and Hager, J. C. (1980). Deliberate facial movement. Child Dev. 51, 886–891. doi: 10.2307/1129478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., and Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 849–937. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind 9, 188–205.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozlik, J., Neumann, R., and Lozo, L. (2015). Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: on the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses. Front. Psychol. 6:563. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J. D., and De Raedt, R. D. (2010). Being moved: valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychol. Sci. 21, 607–613. doi: 10.1177/0956797610365131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunde, W., Lozo, L., and Neumann, R. (2011). Effect-based control of facial expressions: evidence from action–effect compatibility. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 820–826. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0093-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memelink, J., and Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: a basic principle in cognitive control. Psychol. Res. 77, 249–259. doi: 10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, R., Lozo, L., and Kunde, W. (2014). Not all behaviors are controlled in the same way: different mechanisms underlie manual and facial approach and avoidance responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/a0032259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phaf, R. H., Mohr, S. E., Rotteveel, M., and Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and affect: a meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Front. Psychol. 5:378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: evaluative coding account, motivational systems, approach-avoidance, stimulus-response compatibility, Theory of Event Coding (TEC)

Citation: Eder AB, Rothermund K and Hommel B (2016) Commentary: Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: on the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses. Front. Psychol. 7:163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00163

Received: 20 January 2016; Accepted: 28 January 2016;
Published: 17 February 2016.

Edited by:

Tom Beckers, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

John Stins, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2016 Eder, Rothermund and Hommel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andreas B. Eder, andreas.eder@uni-wuerzburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.