Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 28 September 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Variability in Language Predictions: Assessing the Influence of Speaker, Text and Experimental Method View all 11 articles

Prediction Impairment May Explain Communication Difficulties in Autism

\nIris Scholten
Iris Scholten1*Catharina A. HartmanCatharina A. Hartman2Petra HendriksPetra Hendriks1
  • 1Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Introduction

A central research question on autism is how the communication difficulties of autistic individuals can be explained. In this opinion paper we put forward the hypothesis that autistic individuals have problems with language because of an underlying impairment in the ability to generate and update predictions about language. Our hypothesis combines well-established findings from the past decade indicating that linguistic predictions facilitate faster language processing with recent evidence suggesting that autistic individuals show abnormalities in predictions outside the field of language. Investigating linguistic predictions in autism can help clarify the mechanisms underlying the communication difficulties of individuals with ASD. Our hypothesis subsumes earlier mechanistic explanations involving theory of mind and executive functions.

Communication in Autism

Communication difficulties are a core component of autism. Research on communication typically distinguishes between structural language (i.e., the form and meaning of words and sentences) and pragmatic language (i.e., the use of language in social situations). In the past, many researchers assumed that autistic individuals mainly had problems with pragmatic language, such as irony (e.g., Happé, 1994; Leekam and Prior, 1994) and metaphors (e.g., Happé, 1994; Martin and McDonald, 2004). Some studies showed, however, that they have difficulties with structural language too (e.g., Brynskov et al., 2017; Wittke et al., 2017). For example, autistic individuals were found to have difficulties with which-questions (Prévost et al., 2017) and object relative clauses (Durrleman et al., 2015). Communication difficulties have been attributed to, among others, problems with theory of mind (ToM) or reduced executive functioning (EF), but these explanations do not speak to language problems beyond pragmatics. Additionally, our progress in gaining knowledge about ToM and EF in autism seems to have stagnated. We see this partly as a consequence of most studies using offline tasks, which only measure the ultimate response in a task but do not provide measures of the processes leading to this particular response (e.g., different cognitive processes can lead to the same outcome regarding ToM judgements or EF responses). Moreover, heterogeneity in outcomes (i.e., not every autistic individual shows inaccurate ToM judgements or EF responses; e.g., Baez et al., 2020; Deschrijver and Palmer, 2020) cannot be well-understood if the processes leading to these responses are not studied too. Thus, rethinking the theoretical foundations beyond ToM and EF is needed to accommodate structural language problems in autism. In addition, online measures (e.g., eye movements or brain activity) linked to the corresponding offline responses will foster insight into why some autistic individuals tend to have pragmatic and structural language problems, whereas others do not.

Recent Developments

Predictions in Language

Communication is fast and full of ambiguity. Thus, comprehenders must keep up with the speed of language and at the same time determine the intended meaning of a sentence (Crocker, 2005). Generating predictions are fundamental herein. Predictions about upcoming language speed up processing and thus help comprehenders to keep up with the speed of spoken language (e.g., Corps et al., 2018, 2019; Fitz and Chang, 2019; Kochari and Flecken, 2019; Shain et al., 2020).

It has been well-established that linguistic predictions are generated when particular linguistic information is activated in language users, even before the input that carries this information becomes available (Pickering and Gambi, 2018). For example, when hearing “John wants salt and pepper on his steak” a comprehender would be highly likely to predict lexical information, i.e., the word “pepper,” after hearing “salt and” before actually hearing “pepper,” because “salt and pepper” is a pair of words frequently used together in a fixed order. Preactivation of linguistic information is also empirically demonstrated by, for example, eye-tracking studies in which participants heard a sentence like “The boy eats the cake” while looking at pictures. Adults and children already look at the correct picture of a cake instead of competing pictures of other objects before they actually hear the word “cake” (e.g., Altmann and Kamide, 1999; Nation et al., 2003; Borovsky et al., 2012; Mani and Huettig, 2012). Prediction of the word “cake” comes from the verb “eats” which requires an object that is edible. This shows that comprehenders use lexical information to predict upcoming language.

Comprehenders also predict upcoming language on the basis of syntactic information. For example, Lukyanenko and Fisher (2016) showed that 3-year-old children are already faster and more likely to shift their gaze from an incorrect picture displaying only one thing to a correct picture displaying multiple things upon hearing a verb that requires a plural noun (e.g., “Where are the cookies?”) compared to a verb that is uninformative about the number of the noun (“Do you see the cookies?”). Thus, toddlers use agreement between the number marking (singular or plural) of a verb and a noun to predict features of an upcoming noun, resulting in a faster identification of the correct picture. The studies described above show that preactivation of lexical as well as syntactical information helps to do some of the processing ahead of time so that comprehenders can process language fast, despite the speed with which sentences are produced and despite the pervasive ambiguity of language (Pickering and Gambi, 2018).

Given the ambiguity in language and the need for fast processing, predictions can sometimes also steer comprehenders in the wrong direction. This is the case if a comprehender's initial prediction turns out to be false. For example, in a sentence like “The horse raced past the barn fell” (Bever, 1970), comprehenders will predict that “the horse” is the subject of the sentence, because there is a tendency to interpret the first noun phrase in a sentence as the subject, and subjects as agents. Subsequently, they will predict that “raced” is the sentence's main verb (referring to what the horse did) and that “past the barn” is the direction in which the horse raced. Thus, after hearing “the horse raced past the barn,” comprehenders will predict that the sentence is complete. However, upon hearing the final word of the sentence (the verb “fell”) comprehenders will discover that the sentence was not yet complete, and that “raced past the barn” was a specification of “the horse” (i.e., the horse that was raced past the barn). That is, comprehenders will discover that they were led up the garden path. These so-called garden path sentences thus require comprehenders to update their initial incorrect prediction by replacing it with a new prediction, to arrive at the intended interpretation of the sentence.

Predictions Are Related to ToM and EF

Autistic individuals often have difficulties with language and communication. It is conceivable that these difficulties are a consequence of problems with predictions, and that the difficulties with ToM and EF often observed in autistic persons are linked to these prediction problems. ToM enables comprehenders to deduce why a person acted in a certain way or to anticipate how a person is likely to act. In this sense, ToM tasks are inherently prediction tasks, as comprehenders need to predict the actions of a person following a certain observation. Therefore, difficulties with ToM may be caused by a reduced ability to predict another person's behavior. EF is related to predictions as well, especially in situations where an initial prediction turns out to be incorrect. In such situations, comprehenders are required to inhibit their initial but false prediction and switch to an alternative interpretation while holding all information active in their working memory. Therefore, updating an incorrect predictions requires EF, in particular, cognitive inhibition (MacLeod, 2008), cognitive flexibility (Miyake and Friedman, 2012) and working memory (Miyake and Shah, 1999). This thus shows how ToM and EF are related to generating and updating predictions. However, as said earlier, the language difficulties seen in autism are broader than the hypotheses of ToM and EF can explain individually or together. In contrast, prediction impairments during language processing may explain the pragmatic and non-pragmatic language difficulties seen in autism and subsume the hypotheses of ToM and EF. Hence, ToM and EF are related to predictive abilities, but the process of generating and updating predictions is broader and has more explanatory power.

Predictions Beyond Language

Recent studies have illustrated abnormalities in predictive abilities in autistic individuals outside the domains of language (see Cannon et al., 2021, for a recent review of empirical evidence). Neuroimaging and eye-tracking studies using tasks in which participants are presented with predictable repetitive stimuli that are infrequently interrupted by an unpredictable deviant stimulus found that autistic individuals showed an altered response compared to neurotypical controls (Jeste et al., 2015; Balsters et al., 2017; Lawson et al., 2017; Goris et al., 2018). The results of these studies suggest that autistic individuals may be less surprised when their predictions are being violated, as indicated by reduced brain responses (e.g., Lawson et al., 2017). This finding has been taken to mean that their predictions are less strong compared to those of neurotypical individuals (Pellicano and Burr, 2012), potentially because they struggle to generate precise internal prior beliefs about the world (both social and non-social; Pellicano and Burr, 2012; Friston et al., 2013; van de Cruys et al., 2014; Lawson et al., 2017), especially in temporally demanding environments where the environment changes fast in time (e.g., Pellicano and Burr, 2012; Hohwy et al., 2016; Vogel et al., 2019). Being less surprised after encountering a violation to a prediction could, in turn, mean that it is also harder to update a prediction, because the main function of surprise is to interrupt an ongoing action and reorient attention to the new, possibly significant stimulus (Kalat, 2015). This fits well with the finding in the language domain that predictions allow for faster processing. Indeed, the abnormalities in predictive abilities within autistic individuals found outside the language domain triggered our question if these abnormalities also occur within the language domain.

Our Proposal

To explain the language and communication problems of autistic individuals, we put forward the Linguistic Prediction Impairment Hypothesis. This hypothesis states that autistic individuals show abnormalities in generating and updating predictions about language and can be seen as the linguistic version of a more general hypothesis about predictive processing differences between autistic individual and neurotypicals (see Pellicano and Burr, 2012; Sinha et al., 2014; van de Cruys et al., 2014). We hypothesize that this explains why autistic individuals:

• process language slower than their neurotypical peers (e.g., Kamio et al., 2007; Henderson et al., 2011; Bavin et al., 2014; Arunachalam and Luyster, 2018), as predictions facilitate faster language comprehension (Corps et al., 2018);

• have more problems with pragmatic language than structural language (e.g., Ludlow et al., 2017), as pragmatic language is more influenced by contextual information, making it less predictable;

• have difficulties with structural language nonetheless (e.g., Brynskov et al., 2017; Prévost et al., 2017; Wittke et al., 2017), as predictions are generated at every level of language (sounds, words, sentences and their meanings);

• have particular problems interpreting language when the initial prediction turns out to be incorrect (as is evidenced by the findings of Durrleman et al., 2015; Martins et al., 2018; Sukenik and Friedmann, 2018; Was et al., 2018), as updating predictions requires EF which is often found to be impaired in autistic individuals;

• show individual variability in their linguistic performance (e.g., Pearson and Hodgetts, 2020), as predictive abilities may vary strongly in autistic individuals;

• tend to have difficulties with ToM tasks as well as linguistic tasks requiring speaker-hearer coordination, as predictions about other people's actions are needed to succeed in these tasks (Schuwerk et al., 2016).

Concluding Remarks

In this opinion paper, we have put forward the Linguistic Prediction Impairment Hypothesis, which states that autistic individuals have difficulties with language because they have difficulties with generating and updating predictions about language. This hypothesis could provide directions for further research and lead to new insights on language processing in autism, especially when focusing on the identification of subtle effects of predictions using methods that capture language processing online. While focused on the language and communication difficulties of autistic individuals, our proposal has broader implications. Prediction generation and prediction updating are less needed in restricted and repetitive situations. In such situations the future is more predictable and autistic individuals may therefore prefer such situations and behaviors (see also Pellicano and Burr, 2012; van de Cruys et al., 2014; Hohwy et al., 2016; Vogel et al., 2019). This allows for the integration of the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria of (A) impairments in social communication and interaction and (B) restricted, repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Thus, our proposal would result in a more unified view of the core features of autism. Moreover, by emphasizing the importance of using online tasks measuring language processing instead of offline tasks merely registering the ultimate response, our proposal may lead to a better insight in the cognitive processes underlying linguistic behavior and may additionally increase our insights in the role of ToM and EF in generating and updating predictions about language.

Author Contributions

IS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CH and PH gave feedback on the first draft. All authors contributed to writing and revising the manuscript and read and approved the final version.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research NWO (PhD in the Humanities Grant no. 322-89-012, IS).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Altmann, G. T. M., and Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73, 247–264. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.26613/jca/3.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arunachalam, S., and Luyster, R. J. (2018). Lexical development in young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): How ASD may affect intake from the input. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 2659–2672. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baez, A. C., Dajani, D. R., Voorhies, W., Parladé, M. V., Alessandri, M., Britton, J. C., et al. (2020). Parsing heterogeneity of executive function in typically and atypically developing children: a conceptual replication and exploration of social function. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 707–718. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04290-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsters, J. H., Apps, M. A. J., Bolis, D., Lehner, R., Gallagher, L., and Wenderoth, N. (2017). Disrupted prediction errors index social deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Brain 140, 235–246. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bavin, E. L., Kidd, E., Prendergast, L., Baker, E., Dissanayake, C., and Prior, M. (2014). Severity of autism is related to children's language processing. Autism Res. 7, 687–694. doi: 10.1002/aur.1410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. Cogn. Dev. Lang. 279, 1–61.

Google Scholar

Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., and Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one's age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 112, 417–436. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brynskov, C., Eigsti, I. M., Jørgensen, M., Lemcke, S., Bohn, O. S., and Krøjgaard, P. (2017). Syntax and morphology in danish-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 373–383. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, J., Brien, A. M. O., Bungert, L., and Sinha, P. (2021). Prediction in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review of empirical evidence. Autism Res. 13, 474–488. doi: 10.1002/aur.2482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corps, R. E., Crossley, A., Gambi, C., and Pickering, M. J. (2018). Early preparation during turn-taking: Listeners use content predictions to determine what to say but not when to say it. Cognition 175, 77–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corps, R. E., Pickering, M. J., and Gambi, C. (2019). Predicting turn-ends in discourse context. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 615–627. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1552008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, M. W. (2005). “Retional models of comprehension: Addressing the performance paradox,” in: Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum). doi: 10.5465/ame.1998.1109048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deschrijver, E., and Palmer, C. (2020). Reframing Social Cognition: Relational Versus Representational Mentalizing. Psychol. Bull. 146, 941–969. doi: 10.1037/bul0000302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durrleman, S., Hippolyte, L., Zufferey, S., Iglesias, K., and Hadjikhani, N. (2015). Complex syntax in autism spectrum disorders: A study of relative clauses. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 50, 260–267. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitz, H., and Chang, F. (2019). Language ERPs reflect learning through prediction error propagation. Cogn. Psychol. 111, 15–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Lawson, R., and Frith, C. D. (2013). On hyperpriors and hypopriors: comment on pellicano and burr. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17:1. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goris, J., Braem, S., Nijhof, A. D., Rigoni, D., Deschrijver, E., Van de Cruys, S., et al. (2018). Sensory prediction errors are less modulated by global context in autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 3, 667–674. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of Theory of Mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, L. M., Clarke, P. J., and Snowling, M. J. (2011). Accessing and selecting word meaning in autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 52, 964–973. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02393.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J., Paton, B., and Palmer, C. (2016). Distrusting the present. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 15, 315–335. doi: 10.1007/s11097-015-9439-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeste, S. S., Kirkham, N., Senturk, D., Hasenstab, K., Sugar, C., Kupelian, C., et al. (2015). Electrophysiological evidence of heterogeneity in visual statistical learning in young children with ASD. Dev. Sci. 18, 90–105. doi: 10.1111/desc.12188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalat, J. W. (2015). Biological Psychology, 12th Edn. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

Google Scholar

Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., and Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1116–1122. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0254-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochari, A. R., and Flecken, M. (2019). Lexical prediction in language comprehension: a replication study of grammatical gender effects in Dutch. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 239–253. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1524500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawson, R. P., Mathys, C., and Rees, G. (2017). Adults with autism overestimate the volatility of the sensory environment. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1293–1299. doi: 10.1038/nn.4615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leekam, S. R., and Prior, M. (1994). Can autistic children distinguish lies from jokes? A second look at second-order belief attribution. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35, 901–915. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb02301.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludlow, A. K., Chadwick, E., Morey, A., Edwards, R., and Gutierrez, R. (2017). An exploration of sarcasm detection in children with attention hyperactivity deficit disorder. J. Commun. Disord. 70, 25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2017.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukyanenko, C., and Fisher, C. (2016). Where are the cookies? Two- and three-year-olds use number-marked verbs to anticipate upcoming nouns. Cognition 146, 349–370. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (2008). “The concept of inhibition in cognition,” in: Inhibition in Cognition, eds D. S. Gorfein and C. M. MacLeod (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association). doi: 10.1037/11587-001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mani, N., and Huettig, F. (2012). Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake-But only for skilled producers. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 843–847. doi: 10.1037/a0029284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, I., and McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 311–328. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000029553.52889.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martins, A., Santos, A. L., and Duarte, I. (2018). “Comprehension of relative clauses vs. control structures in SLI and ASD,” in: Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press).

Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (1999). Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambirdge: Cambirdge University Press. doi: 10.1017/cbo9781139174909

CrossRef Full Text

Nation, K., Marshall, C. M., and Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Investigating individual differences in children's real-time sentence comprehension using language-mediated eye movements. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 86, 314–329. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, A., and Hodgetts, S. (2020). Can cerebral lateralisation explain heterogeneity in language and increased non-right handedness in autism? A literature review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 105:103738. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., and Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes “too real”: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickering, M. J., and Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychol. Bull. 144, 1002–1044. doi: 10.1037/bul0000158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prévost, P., Tuller, L., Barthez, M. A., Malvy, J., and Bonnet-Brilhault, F. (2017). Production and comprehension of French wh-questions by children with autism spectrum disorder: A comparative study with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 38, 1095–1131. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuwerk, T., Sodian, B., and Paulus, M. (2016). Cognitive mechanisms underlying action prediction in children and adults with autism spectrum condition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 3623–3639. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2899-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shain, C., Blank, I. A., van Schijndel, M., Schuler, W., and Fedorenko, E. (2020). fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia 138:107307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., et al. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 15220–15225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416797111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sukenik, N., and Friedmann, N. (2018). ASD is not DLI: Individuals with autism and individuals with syntactic DLI show similar performance level in syntactic tasks, but different error patterns. Front. Psychol. 9:279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., van der Hallen, R., van Eylen, L., Boets, B., De-Wit, L., et al. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychol. Rev. 121, 649–675. doi: 10.1037/a0037665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, D., Falter-Wagner, C. M., Schoofs, T., Krämer, K., Kupke, C., and Vogeley, K. (2019). Interrupted time experience in autism spectrum disorder: empirical evidence from content analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 22–33. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3771-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Was, C. A., Sansosti, F. J., and Graham, E. (2018). Eye movements while processing syntactically ambiguous sentences in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: preliminary outcomes and benchmarking data. Glob. J. Intellectual Dev. Disabil. 4, 76–81. doi: 10.19080/gjidd.2018.04.555645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittke, K., Mastergeorge, A. M., Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., and Naigles, L. R. (2017). Grammatical language impairment in autism spectrum disorder: exploring language phenotypes beyond standardized testing. Front. Psychol. 8:532. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, language processing, predictive processing, predictions, prediction impairment, language and communication difficulties

Citation: Scholten I, Hartman CA and Hendriks P (2021) Prediction Impairment May Explain Communication Difficulties in Autism. Front. Psychol. 12:734024. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.734024

Received: 30 June 2021; Accepted: 02 September 2021;
Published: 28 September 2021.

Edited by:

Franziska Kretzschmar, Leibniz Institute for the German Language (IDS), Germany

Reviewed by:

David H. V. Vogel, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Germany
Fernanda Dreux M. Fernandes, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2021 Scholten, Hartman and Hendriks. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Iris Scholten, bC5pLnNjaG9sdGVuJiN4MDAwNDA7cnVnLm5s

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.