Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 January 2023
Sec. Eating Behavior

Sizing up the crowd: Assessing spatial integration difficulties in body size judgements across eating disorder symptomatology

  • 1School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
  • 2Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands

Introduction: Body size judgements are frequently biased, or inaccurate, and these errors are further exaggerated for individuals with eating disorders. Within the eating disorder literature, it has been suggested that exaggerated errors in body size judgements are due to difficulties with integration. Across two experiments, we developed a novel integration task, named the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in Virtual Reality (VR), to assess whether nearby bodies influence the perceived size of a single body. VR was used to simulate the appearance of a small crowd around a central target body.

Method and Results: In Experiment 1 (N = 412), participants were required to judge the size of a central female target within a crowd. Experiment 1 revealed an Ebbinghaus Illusion, in which a central female appeared larger when surrounded by small distractors, but comparatively smaller when surrounded by large distractors. In other words, the findings of Experiment 1 demonstrate that surrounding crowd information is integrated when judging an individual’s body size; a novel measure of spatial integration (i.e., an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR). In Experiment 2 (N = 96), female participants were selected based on high (n = 43) and low (n = 53) eating disorder symptomatology. We examined whether the magnitude of this illusion would differ amongst those with elevated versus low eating disorder symptomatology, in accordance with weak central coherence theory, with the high symptomatology group displaying less spatial integration relative to the low group. The results of Experiment 2 similarly found an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR. However, illusion magnitude did not vary across high and low symptomatology groups.

Discussion: Overall, these findings demonstrate that surrounding crowd information is integrated when judging individual body size; however, those with elevated eating disorder symptomatology did not show any integration deficit on this broader measure of spatial integration.

1. Introduction

Body dissatisfaction is recognized as a serious public health concern. For example, the vast majority of Australian women (up to 80%) report some level of dissatisfaction with their weight and shape (Griffiths et al., 2016, 224). This is particularly concerning given that body dissatisfaction and the overvaluation of body size, weight, and shape are recognized as core contributors to the emergence and maintenance of eating disorders (Stice and Shaw, 2002; Allen et al., 2008; Fursland et al., 2012). Eating disorders, including anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and binge eating disorder, are the third most prevalent chronic illness among adolescent girls (Hoek, 2006; Yeo and Hughes, 2011), and are associated with high levels of functional impairment, high risk for future mental and physical problems, and high mortality rates among individuals experiencing an eating disorder (Berkman et al., 2007; Steinhausen, 2009; Micali et al., 2015; Stice et al., 2017).

When it comes to perceptions of body size, research has shown that judgements are frequently biased or inaccurate. For healthy individuals, the tendency to misjudge body size occur in both the judgement of one’s own body size (Tovée et al., 2003; Wardle et al., 2006; Edwards et al., 2010; Costa et al., 2015; Thaler et al., 2018) and when judging the body size of others (Oldham and Robinson, 2016; Alexi et al., 2018; Gledhill et al., 2019). Importantly, these inaccuracies can lead to difficulties in individuals identifying weight gain or loss in themselves or others. As a result, this may hinder efforts to modify weight-related health behaviors (Solmi et al., 2020).

Moreover, these inaccuracies in body size judgements have significant clinical relevance. Errors in body size judgements have been found to occur more frequently and are more exaggerated among individuals with or at risk of an eating disorder (Whitehouse et al., 1988; Cazzato et al., 2016; Mohr et al., 2016; Moelbert et al., 2017; Gledhill et al., 2019; Alexi et al., 2019b). For example, patients diagnosed with anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa have been shown to overestimate their own body size (Williamson et al., 1993; Gardner and Bokenkamp, 1996; Tovée et al., 2003; Engel and Keizer, 2017; Moelbert et al., 2017) and have been shown to misjudge the body size of others (Tovée et al., 2000). Individuals with binge eating disorder have also been shown to overestimate their body size relative to peers (Nicoli and Junior, 2011).

Severe disturbances in body image can present as extreme for individuals with eating disorders. Not only do body image disturbances further motivate weight-loss behaviors, but can play a key role in the initiation, maintenance (Casper et al., 1979; Gadsby, 2017), and relapse of eating disorders (Castro et al., 2004; Keel et al., 2005; Carter et al., 2012; Gadsby, 2017), as well as being a predictor of decreased treatment success (Roy and Meilleur, 2010). Given the key role that body image disturbances play for those with eating disorders, the current study sought to further understanding of the factors that contribute to body size judgement errors and how these might vary according to eating disorder symptomatology.

Body image disturbances are theorized to be multidimensional. The general consensus is that these dimensions include cognitive/affective, behavioral, and perceptual processes (Cash and Deagle, 1997; Cash et al., 2002; Gaudio et al., 2014; Lewer et al., 2016). Regarding perceptual biases, which are the focus of this study, it has been found that past experience of body shapes can influence body size judgements in a number of ways. One well-established cause of perceptual bias is due to adaptation after-effects, where prolonged exposure to a visual stimulus (typically minutes or longer) can distort the appearance of subsequently viewed stimuli, allowing them to appear more perceptually distinct than they are (Gibson and Radner, 1937). In regards to body size judgements, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that viewing thin body shapes for a prolonged period of time causes subsequently viewed average-sized bodies to appear larger, and vice versa (Winkler and Rhodes, 2005; Hummel et al., 2012, 2013; Robinson and Kirkham, 2014; Brooks et al., 2016; Challinor et al., 2017). A second perceptual bias that tends to occur under more brief exposures is serial dependence, in which judgements are biased by and towards prior experience (Fischer and Whitney, 2014). Serial dependence has been observed for body size judgements, with bodies being perceived to be smaller when preceded by a smaller body, and bodies perceived as larger when preceded by a larger body (Alexi et al., 2018, 2019b; Turnbull et al., 2022). Thirdly, it has long been observed that estimates of large and small magnitudes are biased towards a mean magnitude (Hollingworth, 1910). This perceptual bias for magnitude estimation has been demonstrated in body size judgements, in which healthy participants have been shown to be relatively accurate when judging average body sizes (which tends to be between 60—70 kg). However, participants would increasingly overestimate the size of smaller/lighter bodies below this average, and increasing underestimate the size of larger bodies above this average (Cornelissen et al., 2015, 2016).

These perceptual biases constitute known sources of body size judgement errors for healthy individuals. Critically, individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology exhibit greater distortions in at least some of these perceptual biases (Cornelissen et al., 2013; Mohr et al., 2016; Gledhill et al., 2019; Alexi et al., 2019b). A theory that has gained currency for explaining why individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology may exhibit greater perceptual biases attributes those biases to deficits in integration. Integration is the process of which multiple sources of input are merged with fluency and combined to form a unified representation of an object, action, or context (Iarocci and McDonald, 2006). This concept arose from research showing that individuals with eating disorders, or high in eating disorder symptomatology have difficulties with the integration of certain information, including impaired integration of multisensory information (Eshkevari et al., 2014; Riva and Dakanalis, 2018; Riva and Gaudio, 2018) and impaired spatial integration (e.g., weak central coherence). Weak central coherence has been defined by two components: a poorer ability at global processing (forming a holistic representation) and enhanced local (feature-specific) processing abilities (Happé and Booth, 2008). It has been referred to as a “piecemeal” perceptual processing style characterized by a local bias towards specific information and failing to integrate this information into a global percept (Frith, 1989; Rouse et al., 2004; Bölte et al., 2007; Happé and Booth, 2008). According to Frith (1989), weak central coherence reflects a relative difficulty in integrating surrounding spatial information necessary to obtain higher-order meaning within the context (Frith, 1989). Past research has used different types of measures to investigate weak central coherence. These include the use of global versus local processing paradigm to illustrate deficits in global processing and strengths in local/detail processing. These types of tests include visual tasks such as Group/Embedded Figures Tests (Lang et al., 2014a,b), the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure test (Danner et al., 2012; Keegan et al., 2021), the Fragmented Pictures Task (Harrison et al., 2011), and Object Assembly (Van Autreve et al., 2013). Additionally, research has tested weak central coherence by looking at how strongly elements/features are being integrated into one’s overall percept. This has been measured using visual tasks, including the Ebbinghaus Illusion (e.g., Happé, 1996; Ropar and Mitchell, 1999; Bölte et al., 2007; de Fockert et al., 2007; Gori et al., 2016), Ponzo Illusion (Shen et al., 2015), and the Muller-Lyer illusion (Ropar and Mitchell, 2001). Within a clinical context, difficulties with integration have been shown to occur in different populations, with weak central coherence postulated to underlie core symptomatology of autism spectrum disorders (ASD; Frith, 1989). Notably, it has been found that there is considerable comorbidity between autism and eating disorders (Berkman et al., 2007; Oldershaw et al., 2011; Huke et al., 2013). Indeed, it has also been hypothesized that autistic traits places women at a higher risk of developing anorexia nervosa (Oldershaw et al., 2011). A review by Huke et al. (2013) found that the prevalence of autism in eating disorder populations was significantly higher than that within a healthy control sample, ranging from 8 to 37%. Additionally, there is considerable overlap in traits such as weak central coherence, inflexibility, adherence to routine, restricted interests, social and flexibility difficulties, and repetitive behaviors for both individuals with autism and individuals with eating disorders (Hambrook et al., 2008; Lopez et al., 2008a,b,c; Pooni et al., 2012; Treasure, 2013; Lang et al., 2014a, 2021; Mandy and Tchanturia, 2015; Dell'Osso et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2018; Kerr-Gaffney et al., 2020; Inoue et al., 2021; Keegan et al., 2021). Thus, evidence suggests that both individuals with autism and eating disorder symptomatology share a common perceptual bias related to weak central coherence.

While a substantial body of literature acknowledges that difficulties with integration are relevant to understanding body image disturbances, the vast majority of previous research has focused on body size judgements using a single-test body stimuli (Gardner and Bokenkamp, 1996; Urgesi et al., 2012, 2014; Brooks et al., 2016; Cornelissen et al., 2016; Alexi et al., 2018, 2019b). Therefore, questions have largely been restricted to investigating the role that integration has within a single-test body stimulus, such as holistic versus local processing of bodies (Brooks et al., 2018) and multibody sensory integration (Keizer et al., 2012, 2013; Metral et al., 2014; Beckmann et al., 2021). However, we are aware of one study which has provided preliminary evidence of the influence of surrounding bodies on one’s body size judgement (Bateson et al., 2014). In this study, participants were simultaneously presented with two stimulus sets. Each stimulus set displayed a central target body, standing in between two bodies of either a lower or higher body mass index (BMI) class. Specifically, Bateson et al. (2014) found that two surrounding bodies biased the probability of a central target body being perceived as larger or smaller. A target female body was more likely to be judged thinner when standing in between two women with a higher body mass index and more likely to be judged larger when standing in between two women with a lower BMI. However, these influences were not the primary focus for Bateson et al. (2014), with the aforementioned study not measuring the size of this influence. In addition, Bateson et al. (2014) only used two surrounding bodies, rather than looking at integration across a crowd of body sizes. The current study directly tackles the question of whether the body size characteristics of a surrounding crowd influence the perceived size of a central target body, how that varies with body size and importantly, whether that influence differs as a function of eating disorder symptomatology. To simulate a crowd in an ecologically valid manner, body size judgements were measured within an immersive Virtual Reality (VR) setting. This 3D VR environment minimized occlusions between bodies and allowed the central target to appear distanced and distinct from the bystanders as they would in the real world.

There is a long established paradigm for studying these types of size illusions. For instance, the Ebbinghaus Illusion (Ebbinghaus, 1902) is a well-established measure that has been used to demonstrate and study spatial integration (Happé, 1996; Gori et al., 2016). The Ebbinghaus Illusion is a visual size-contrast illusion, where the perceived size of the central target stimulus (e.g., the central circle) is altered by the surrounding stimuli (e.g., the large or small surrounding circles). As shown in Figure 1, the central circle appears larger when surrounded by smaller circles and the central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles. The Ebbinghaus Illusion demonstrates how the integration of surrounding spatial information across visual scenes can bias one’s judgements of size. As evidence of its utility to studies of integration and central coherence, individuals with autism have been shown to exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion, relative to individuals without autism (Happé, 1996; Bölte et al., 2007). Given the evidence that those with eating disorders similarly display weak central coherence, it seems plausible that those high in eating disorder symptomatology might similarly exhibit reduced spatial integration on these types of perceptual tasks/illusions, compared with those low in symptomatology.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The Ebbinghaus illusion.

The first step in such an investigation might begin by asking whether this type of size contrasting illusion occurs for body size judgements. That is, whether we integrate surrounding information (e.g., other bodies) into our judgements of body size. As discussed above, previous literature suggests that that it can (Bateson et al., 2014). We add to that preliminary finding by systematically varying the size of the target and bystanders, in order to help characterize the circumstances in which surrounding bodies do and do not influence size judgements. To simulate a crowd in an ecologically valid manner, body size judgements were measured within an immersive Virtual Reality (VR) setting. This 3D VR environment minimized occlusions and allowed the central target to appear distanced and distinct from the bystanders as they would in the real world.

Accordingly, the aim of Experiment 1 was to develop and demonstrate a novel test of spatial integration — that is, an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR. Specifically, we aimed to investigate if and to what extent the perceived size of a single central female body is influenced by the presence of other female bodies within the scene. We hypothesized that there would be an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, that is, participants would judge the central body to be larger when surrounded by a crowd of very thin bodies (the Small Distractors) and judge the central body to be smaller when surrounded by a crowd of very overweight bodies (the Large Distractors) within a community sample. Based on findings for the traditional Ebbinghaus Illusion, we expect this bias in size judgement to increase as the difference in size of target and surrounding bodies increases (Roberts et al., 2005). In this light, we would not expect an Ebbinghaus Illusion to arise when the size of the central target body and surrounding bodies are of the same size.

Having demonstrated a significant Ebbinghaus Illusion for Body Size in a community sample, Experiment 2 then examined whether the magnitude of this illusion differed amongst those with elevated versus low eating disorder symptomatology. In accordance with weak central coherence theory, and after controlling for the contribution of traits associated with autism, it was hypothesized that individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology would display a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology, who would likely mimic the pattern of the general population. That is, individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology are expected to display less spatial integration of surrounding bodies when judging the size of a central target body.

2. Experiment 1

2.1. Method

2.1.1. Participants

The participants in this study were first year psychology students from The University of Western Australia. All students were given the option to partake in this experiment as part of their laboratory classes and informed consent was obtained. There was no compensation for participation and students were not penalized if they decided not to partake in this experiment. At the end of the experiment, students were asked if they consented to their anonymous data being used for broader research purposes. Only those students who agreed to that use of data were used in this study. Therefore, this study reports data from 419 participants. Seven participants were excluded from the study as they did not discriminate between the body sizes (i.e., they clicked the same area on the response scale throughout the task). This left us with a sample of 412 participants. Previous studies on the Ebbinghaus Illusion have tended to use relatively small samples (e.g., sample sizes of four to 36 participants in classic papers such as Ropar and Mitchell, 1999, 2001; Roberts et al., 2005), and the effect size of the Illusion is not often reported. As such, we accommodated for a small effect size by using a sample from a large undergraduate unit. Calculated with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007), a sensitivity analysis indicated that our sample size of 412 participants would allow us to observe effect sizes for our within-between subject interaction of f = 0.067, α = 0.05 and power (1 − β) = 0.81. The ages of the sample ranged from 18 to 52 years old (M = 20.20; SD = 5.53). The gender breakdown of the sample included 251 female participants (60.92%), 159 male participants (38.59%), and two individuals who identified as other (0.49%). The experimental procedure was approved by the University of Western Australia’s Human Research Ethics Committee and the experiment was performed in accordance with their guidelines and regulations.

2.1.2. Materials

2.1.2.1. Apparatus

Participants wore one of 12 head-mounted HTC Vive Pro™ devices (HTC, Valve Corporation, 2018) where each was connected to a laptop computer (15.6″ Gigabyte Sabre 15-W8, FHD, 120 Hz, Intel® UHD Graphics 630).

2.1.2.2. Stimuli

Forty-nine synthetic computer-generated images (CGI) of female bodies were created for our earlier work (Turnbull et al., 2022), ranging from very underweight (Size 1) to very overweight (Size 7). Using the program Blender 2.79© we created a linear continuum of seven discrete body size categories. Seven unique identities were created for each body size category. The Size 1, Size 4, and Size 7 body size categories were created first in Character Creator©. They were matched by the research team to be perceptually equivalent to the sizes of the smallest or largest body stimuli used in earlier work using the same task and real body images (Alexi et al., 2018, 2019b). Utilizing Shape Keys (e.g., an animation industry technique which smoothly interpolates between the vertices of the two models), we were able to generate the intermediate body types by linearly interpolating between the Size 1 and Size 4 bodies, and between the Size 4 and Size 7 bodies, in equal steps. This generated the Size 2, Size 3, Size 5, and Size 6 bodies. The bodies appeared with their arms somewhat outstretched so that arm position/horizontal extent did not need to vary with body size. In the VR environment, the bodies were 1.8 m tall and were presented approximately 2.2 m away from the participant. All bodies faced the participant. There were seven randomly generated “identities” of each body size, which varied in appearance (i.e., skin color, hair color) and in clothing (i.e., color, pattern). This was to avoid participants associating a specific identity with a specific response. Examples of the body size categories of the body stimuli are shown in Figure 2A and examples of the differing identities are shown in Figure 2B.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Examples of the seven body stimuli from body Size 1 (very underweight) to 7 (very overweight). On the top row, from left to right, are bodies Size 1, Size 2, Size 3, and Size 4. On the bottom row, from left to right, are bodies Size 5, Size 6, and Size 7. (B) Examples of the seven body identities. The original source of these images can be found at Turnbull et al. (2022). The stimuli was created using ©Character Creator and using Shape Keys (e.g., an animation industry technique which smoothly interpolates between the vertices of the two models) to create the intermediate body types by linearly interpolating between the Size 1 and Size 4 bodies. Both figures are reproduced from Turnbull et al. (2022), with permission from SAGE Publications.

2.1.2.3. Ebbinghaus illusion for bodies’ configuration

As shown in Figures 3A,B, the scene presented to participants consisted of one central body (the to-be-judged body) surrounded by six bodies (inducers) to simulate a “crowd.” The central body had a red fixation cross in the center of the body, indicating that this was the body to be judged. This configuration contains broad range of locations for where the inducers located. The six inducers were positioned equidistance from each other on a 1 m radius around the central body and placed to minimize occlusion. This configuration meant that the two furthest inducers were standing 24° left and right from the central body, respectively. The four remaining inducers were standing between 10 and 14° left and right from the central body. These four closest inducers were placed in a range that is typical for the Ebbinghaus Illusion (Roberts et al., 2005). In the Small Distractor condition, the six inducers were all Size 1 bodies, as illustrated in Figure 3A. In the Large Distractor condition, the six inducers were all Size 7 bodies, as illustrated in Figure 3B.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

(A) Stimuli shown for participants in the “small distractor” condition, with the central body (Size 4) surrounded by very underweight inducers (Size 1 bodies). (B) Stimuli shown for participants in the “large distractor” condition, with the central body (Size 4) surrounded by six very overweight inducers (Size 7 Bodies). The stimuli was created using ©Character Creator and using Shape Keys (e.g., an animation industry technique which smoothly interpolates between the vertices of the two models) to create the intermediate body types by linearly interpolating between the Size 1 and Size 4 bodies.

2.1.2.4. Bodyline task

Our VR bodyline task was adapted from the original bodyline task developed by Alexi et al. (2018) to measure body size judgements. The task was run through “Unity” software (Unity Technologies, 2019). Participants were instructed to make a body size judgement of the central female target only. To be clear, this was a judgement about other bodies, made from a third-person point of view, not a judgement of the body relative to the crowd, or of one’s own body size. This body size judgement was made using the bodyline, a visual analogue scale (VAS) consisting of a white, unmarked horizontal line scored from 1.0 to 7.0 (Alexi et al., 2018, 2019b). The VAS represented a continuum, in which participants were given explicit verbal instructions to make a body size judgement of the central target body. Participants were informed that the left of the VAS corresponded with the central body being judged as very underweight, and the right side corresponded with the central body being judged as very overweight. Unlike the traditional method developed and used by Alexi et al. (2018), here the VAS did not have any anchor images at either end of the scale. Each trial began with a blank black screen and a red fixation cross in the center. The crowd of bodies were then presented for 500 ms, which is double the presentation time of stimuli in previous research using the bodyline task (i.e., 250 ms; Alexi et al., 2018, 2019a,b). The longer presentation was intended to allow participants sufficient time to orient themselves to the target stimuli within the VR environment. After the presentation time, the target bodies disappeared and participants were required to respond on the VAS before proceeding to the next trial. There was a 1 s interval between trials. The VAS remained on the screen between stimuli presentations. For make their responses, participants saw a “laser beam” emitted from the controller. Responses were made by pushing down the trigger button on the HTC Vive Pro hand controller, at a location which corresponded to the participants’ judgement on the VAS.

2.1.3. Procedure

This study was conducted in a series of time-limited undergraduate laboratory classes, where up to 12 participants were able to be tested at the same time. All participants gave informed consent and were fitted with the HTC Vive Pro headset and given a VR hand controller. In the VR environment, participants entered their age and gender. Participants then read a series of instructions for the bodyline task, instructing them to judge the size of the central body stimulus and to ignore the surrounding inducers.

Participants within each class were randomly assigned to one of two conditions; the Small Distractors condition, or the Large Distractors condition. In each condition participants completed 14 practice trials, in which they were presented with the full spectrum of central target body stimuli sizes (Size 1–7) twice. Following the practice trials, participants completed 150 trials. Consistent with prior research, the presentation order of the body images was fixed across all participants to ensure that each body size category both preceded and followed each other Body Size category, including its own, an equivalent number of times (Alexi et al., 2018, 2019b). This order is important for assessing serial dependencies (trial to trial perceptual biases), which are not assessed in the current study, but we replicated the original task for consistency with previous research. The experiment took approximately 15 min to complete for each participant. Following the completion of their task, participants were debriefed on the goals of the experiment.

2.2. Results

2.2.1. Data screening

Prior to data analyses, all data for each participant were screened. Each individual’s body size judgement data were fit with a linear regression. If the fitted slope was not statistically greater than zero, it indicated that the individual did not perceive any differences between the seven body size categories and resulted in their subsequent removal from analyses. As previously mentioned, this resulted in the removal of seven participant’s data, as they did not discriminate between the seven body size categories.

Data were also assessed for outliers. Individual scores were examined according to the outlier criterion of three standard deviations above or below the mean for that variable (Howell et al., 1998; Alexi et al., 2019a,b). All outliers that were identified were subsequently Winsorized (Reifman and Keyton, 2010). Next, normality was assessed using the criterion of skew <|2.00| and kurtosis <|7.00| (Curran et al., 1996; Gignac, 2019). All variables achieved sufficient normality, with skew and kurtosis values falling within accepted limits.

2.2.2. Assessment of participant differences between conditions

To verify that the Small Distractor and Large Distractor groups did not significantly differ on any baseline variables, an independent samples t-test was conducted to compare age. No significant difference was found between the Small Distractor and Large Distractor conditions for age, t(410) = 0.832, p = 0.406. In addition, a chi-square analysis was conducted to compare gender between the Small and Large Distractor conditions. There were no significant differences in breakdown of gender, χ2(2) = 0.960, p = 0.619. Descriptive characteristics for these variables are displayed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means (SD) for age and gender breakdown for the small and large distractor conditions.

2.2.3. An Ebbinghaus illusion for bodies

Each participant’s mean size judgement in each condition is displayed in Figure 4A. To address our primary research aim, determining whether there is an Ebbinghaus Illusion when making body size judgements, we conducted a 2 (Distractor condition: Small Distractors, Large Distractors) × 7 (body size category: 1–7) mixed-model ANOVA, with body size judgements as the dependent variable.1

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Mean individual ratings for each body size category for all participants. (B) A summary of mean body size judgements for the small distractor and large distractor conditions. There was a significant difference between the conditions. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error of the mean. In (A), the data for the small distractor condition has been nudged horizontally, for clarity. Created using GraphPad Prism.

All statistical assumptions were examined. The assumption of sphericity was tested using Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity. This assumption was violated for body size category. As such, the conservative Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment was used (Gignac, 2019). All other statistical assumptions were satisfied.

As expected, there was a large significant main effect of body size category, F(2.59, 1064.15) = 12539.72, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.968, demonstrating that participants were able to discriminate between the seven body size categories, judging the central body to be larger when it was physically larger.

Next, there was a small significant main effect of distractor condition on body size judgements, F(1, 410) = 20.954, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.049. On average, bodies were judged to be larger when surrounded by the Small Distractors and judged to be smaller when surrounded by Large Distractors (Figure 4A). This is consistent with the traditional Ebbinghaus Illusion and therefore, our results demonstrate an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR.

The above described main effect is somewhat qualified by the presence of a small significant interaction between distractor condition and body size category, F(2.59, 1063.15) = 8.37, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.020. However, inspection of the data for each body size category in Figure 4A indicates that this interaction simply reflects variation in the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies across body size category. That is, six out of seven body size categories appear to show the typical Ebbinghaus Illusion, such that the central target body was judged to be larger when surrounded by Small Distractors and judged to be smaller when surrounded by Large Distractors. The exception was the largest body category (Size 7), where judgements for the Small Distractor and Large Distractor conditions were equivalent. To formalize that interpretation, we used Bonferroni’s post-hoc analyses, corrected for multiple comparisons. This analysis revealed that the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR was significant for 3 out of the 7 body size categories (body size category 2–4). The non-significant Ebbinghaus Illusion for larger body sizes (5–7) is not surprising given participant reports of poorer precision in judging larger bodies and given the additional occlusions created in the Large Distractor condition. These points will be discussed in further detail in the General Discussion. In short, our experiment was designed to assess the nature of the surrounding bodies’ influence for a range of body sizes. However, if we simply collapse across body size to restrict the question to whether or not perceived size varies by distractor condition (i.e., an Ebbinghaus Illusion), then the answer is yes, there is a small significant effect t(2882) = 1.73, p = 0.042, d = 0.064, as shown in Figure 4B.

2.3. Discussion

The results of Experiment 1 demonstrate an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, indicating that spatial integration of crowd information can lead to biased perceptual judgements of individual body size. It was found that the central body was generally perceived to be larger when surrounded by Small Distractors and perceived to be smaller when surrounded by Large Distractors. There was variation in the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies, reinforcing the importance of measuring the illusion across a range of body sizes. Essentially, the illusion was greatest when the target body is average sized (2–4), and therefore maximally different from both the Small (Size 1) and Large (Size 7) distractors. Based on findings for the traditional Ebbinghaus Illusion (Roberts et al., 2005), we expected this bias in size judgement to increase as the difference in size of the central target and the surrounding bodies increased. These findings then add to the existing literature on the factors that influence body size judgements and associated. Specifically, we build on Bateson et al. (2014), by demonstrating that the influence of the crowd on judgements of perceived body size depends on the size relationship between the target and the bystander (inducer) and demonstrate the utility of VR technology that mimics real world situations for studying body size judgements.

3. Experiment 2

Experiment 1 confirmed our novel Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR to be a valid measure of spatial integration of bodies. The aim of Experiment 2 was to investigate whether the magnitude of Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies varies amongst individuals differing in eating disorder symptomatology. Based on prior reports of weak spatial integration by those high in eating disorder symptomatology (e.g., Lopez et al., 2008a,b,c; Lang et al., 2014a) we hypothesized that those high in eating disorder symptoms would exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies than those low in eating disorder symptoms.

3.1. Method

3.1.1. Participants

A total of 96 first year undergraduate psychology students from the University of Western Australia were invited to participate in return for course credit. Only female students aged 17—25 were eligible to participate, as research indicates this demographic to be most at risk in the development of eating disorders (Hudson et al., 2007; Stice et al., 2013; Rohde et al., 2017) and is consistent with previous related studies (Dondzilo et al., 2017; Alexi et al., 2019b). Calculated with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007), this sample size afforded us the ability to observe an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies for our within-subject interaction with effects of f = 0.15, α = 0.05 and power (1 − β) = 0.81, and to observe effect sizes as small as f = 0.291, α = 0.05 and power (1 − β) = 0.81 for the comparison of the Ebbinghaus Illusion between eating disorder groups. The age of the sample ranged from 17 to 24 years old (M = 18.58; SD = 1.38), with a mean BMI of 21.56 (SD = 4.06), falling within the healthy BMI weight range (Flegal et al., 2014). All participants reported normal or corrected to normal (e.g., contact lenses or glasses) visual acuity. Participants were able to wear visual corrective aids, if required, within the Virtual Reality headsets. The experimental procedure was approved by the institutional Human Research Ethics Committee and the experiment was performed in accordance with their guidelines and regulations.

3.1.2. Materials

3.1.2.1. Titmus test

The Titmus stereoacuity test (Stereo Optical, 2017) was administered as a pre-screening measure to ensure all participants could resolve three-dimensional (3D) visual cues within the VR environment. Wearing polarized glasses, participants judged four black contoured stimuli characterized by disparate elements across nine trials, identifying the circle appearing forward in the plane of reference (Garnham and Sloper, 2006). Participants were discontinued after two consecutive incorrect answers. All participants (N = 96) exhibited stereoacuity within the normal range (i.e., acuity better than 30 s of disparity; Bell et al., 2013) and could proceed with the experimental task. Previous research has validated the use of the Titmus test in the assessment of coarse stereoacuity (Fawcett and Birch, 2003).

3.1.2.2. Apparatus

The apparatus was identical to Experiment 1. However, in Experiment 2, participants were testing individually in a quiet room, not a classroom setting. Additionally, in Experiment 2, questionnaire measures were administered online via Qualtrics. For the additional measure of BMI in Experiment 2, weighing scales and a tape measure were used to calculate participants’ height and weight to calculate BMI. BMI is a factor known to impact on body size judgements (Cornelissen et al., 2015; Thaler et al., 2018) and was used as a control, consistent with previous research (Dondzilo et al., 2016, 2017; Alexi et al., 2019b).

3.1.2.3. Stimuli

The synthetic female body stimuli, Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies configuration, and bodyline task were identical to Experiment 1. Experiment 2 additionally included several questionnaire measures as detailed below.

3.1.3. Measures

3.1.3.1. Eating disorder examination-questionnaire 6.0

The EDE-Q (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994) is a well-validated self-report measure used in the assessment and diagnosis of eating disorder symptomatology (Peterson et al., 2007; Berg et al., 2012). The EDE-Q consists of 28 items with reference to the preceding 28 days. Twenty-two of the EDE-Q items examine the attitudinal components of eating disorder symptomatology. Participants responded to each item (e.g., “Have you had a definite fear that you might gain weight?”) using a 7-point Likert scale (0 = complete absence of feature, to 6 = acute presentation of feature). Subscale scores were averaged to produce a global EDE-Q score ranging from 0 to 6 (Mond et al., 2006). Higher scores indicated greater ED symptomatology. The remaining six items measured the frequency of one’s engagement in binge eating behaviors and were omitted from the computation of global EDE-Q scores in this study. Participant’s EDE-Q scores were used as a screening exercise to recruit eligible participants, which will be discussed in the Procedure. The Cronbach’s alpha for the 22-item global EDE-Q score in our sample was α = 0.99.

3.1.3.2. Autism-spectrum quotient

The AQ (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) is a 50-item self-report measure designed to quantify traits and behaviors associated with autism among adults within the general population. Given the considerable association between autism and eating disorder symptomatology previously discussed, we measured AQ to control for its contribution within our findings. The original item-level scoring format of the AQ coded responses dichotomously (i.e., into “agreement” or “disagreement”). Responses that endorsed the autistic trait each score one point, regardless of the strength of endorsement, while responses that do not endorse the trait score zero, also regardless of endorsement strength. Therefore, total AQ scores traditionally could range from 0 to 50. Recently, researchers have begun adopting a 1–4 Likert scoring strategy to increase scale discriminability. Such scoring strategy results in the total scale AQ scores ranging from 50 to 200. For this study, we adopted the 1–4 Likert scoring format for the AQ. Participants were required to respond to each item (e.g., “I find social situations easy”) using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “definitely agree” (1) to “definitely disagree” (4). Higher scores indicated higher levels of autistic-like traits (Ruzich et al., 2015). The Cronbach’s alpha for the total AQ score within this sample was α = 0.83.

3.1.4. Procedure

The procedure of Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1, except as described below. Firstly, participants voluntarily completed the EDE-Q as a screening measure prior to recruitment. Only global EDE-Q scores from female participants aged 17—24 years old were considered. In accordance with previous research comparing high and low symptomatology groups (Jansen et al., 2005; Dickter et al., 2018), global EDE-Q scores were rank ordered. Two groups were established based on the highest and lowest third of global EDE-Q scores. Only participants who were assigned to either of these groups were recruited to participate in this experiment. In total, 43 participants were recruited from the group of potential participants with high EDE-Q scores (M = 4.40; SD = 0.69) and 53 participants were recruited form the group of potential participants with low EDE-Q scores (M = 0.37; SD = 0.25). All eligible participants were contacted to participate in this study. Participants gave informed consent and completed the Titmus stereoacuity test prior to commencing the bodyline experiment.

To calculate an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies’ magnitude (i.e., integration strength) for each participant, in Experiment 2 participants completed both small and large distractor conditions (i.e., now a within subject factor). Participants were randomly assigned to begin in one of two conditions. This was counterbalanced across all participants, with the alternative condition completed next. Following this task, participants then completed questionnaires via Qualtrics. Finally, each participant’s height and weight were recorded to calculate body mass index (BMI).

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Data screening

To ensure all participants discriminated between the seven body size categories within the bodyline task, a linear regression analysis was conducted on the mean body size judgements for both Small Distractor and Large Distractor conditions separately, across body sizes 1–7. Denoting a minimum level of performance, slope values significantly greater than zero indicated that participants perceived size differences between the seven body size categories. All participants (N = 96) obtained a significant slope value and were all retained for further analysis.

Data were assessed for outliers. Variables were examined according to the outlier criterion of three standard deviations above or below the mean (Howell et al., 1998; Alexi et al., 2019b). All outliers that were identified in the questionnaire data, performance data, and for BMI were subsequently Winsorized (Reifman and Keyton, 2010). Next, normality was assessed using the criterion of skew <|2.00|and kurtosis <|7.00|(Curran et al., 1996; Gignac, 2019). All variables achieved sufficient normality, with skew and kurtosis values falling within accepted limits.

3.2.2. Assessment of trait differences between groups

Descriptive statistics corresponding to the obtained measures are displayed in Table 2. To assess the differences between the high and low EDE-Q groups across a number of baseline dimensions, a series of independent samples t-tests were conducted. There was no significant difference between the EDE-Q groups in age. As expected, there was a large significant difference between the two groups on EDE-Q global score, with the high EDE-Q obtaining a significantly higher score than the low EDE-Q group. These differences are similar to those reported in other literature [as shown in (Berrisford-Thompson et al., 2021)]. Additionally, as expected, there were large significant differences in BMI, with a higher BMI for the high EDE-Q group (Berrisford-Thompson et al., 2021). Additionally, there was a significant difference in AQ total scores, with the high EDE-Q group scoring significantly higher on the AQ total, relative to the low EDE-Q group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean (SD) values and obtained t-test results for both high and low EDE-Q groups (N = 96).

3.2.3. Spatial integration of the crowd by eating disorder symptomatology

To examine differences in spatial integration and body size judgements among individuals varying in ED symptomatology, a 2 (between-subjects variable: high vs. low ED symptomatology) × 7 (within-subjects variable: body size categories) × 2 (within-subjects variable: Small Distractor vs. Large Distractor size condition) mixed-design ANOVA was conducted. The dependent variable was the body size judgement of the central test body given on each trial which was measured using the bodyline VAS. The variables of BMI and autistic traits (AQ total) were entered as covariates in the analysis to statistically control for the influence of BMI and autistic traits upon body size judgements. All statistical assumptions were examined. The assumption of sphericity was tested using Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity. This assumption was violated for body size category and the interaction between body size category and distractor size. As such, the conservative Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment was used (Gignac, 2019). All other statistical assumptions were satisfied.

As expected, participants were able to discriminate between the seven body size categories, judging the central body to be larger as the body size category increased. This was supported by a large significant within-subjects main effect of body size category, F(2.26, 208.17) = 29.41, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.242.

To determine whether there is an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, we looked at the difference in body size judgements when the central body is surrounded by the Small Distractors or Large Distractors. We found a small significant main effect of distractor size, F(1, 92) = 4.36, p = 0.040, partial η2 = 0.045. That is, participants on average are judging the central body to be larger when surrounded by Small Distractors and judged to be smaller when surrounded by Large Distractors. Therefore, an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies was observed, similar to Experiment 1, even while controlling for additional variables of AQ and BMI.

Having established that our results demonstrate an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, we looked at whether the high and low eating disorder symptomatology groups differed in their body size judgements. We found a small significant main effect of EDE-Q group, F(1, 92) = 5.66, p = 0.019, partial η2 = 0.058. This showed that individuals in the high EDE-Q group were providing larger mean body size judgements relative to the low EDE-Q group, as shown in Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mean individual ratings for each body size category for all participants. Created using GraphPad Prism.

3.2.4. Does the Ebbinghaus illusion for bodies vary across body size category?

Having established an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, we next tested whether the illusion varies across body size category, akin to Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, we were able to control for BMI and AQ, and no longer find a significant interaction between distractor size and body size category, F(2.06, 243.16) = 2.11, p = 0.109, partial η2 = 0.022.

3.2.5. Are individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology providing larger body size judgements, relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology?

The main effect of the EDE-Q group must be interpreted with caution due to a small significant interaction effect between body size category and EDE-Q group, F(2.26, 208.17) = 3.57, p = 0.025, partial η2 = 0.037. This shows that there was variation in participant’s body size judgements across the seven body size categories as a function of the participant’s EDE-Q group.

To formalize that interpretation, we used Bonferroni’s post-hoc analyses, corrected for multiple comparisons. To do this, we collapsed each body size category across distractor condition to obtain a mean rating for each body size category. Then we compared each body size category rating across high and low EDE-Q groups. It was found that individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology were providing significantly larger judgements to body sizes category 5 and 6 relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology, with t(94) = 3.15, p = 0.014, d = 0.64 and t(94) = 2.72, p = 0.049, d = 0.56, respectively. No other body size category comparisons were significant.

3.2.6. Do individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus illusion, relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology?

We hypothesized that individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology would exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies, relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology. However, there was no significant interaction between distractor size and eating disorder symptomatology group, F(1, 92) = 0.52, p = 0.473, partial η2 = 0.006. Thus, we did not find any evidence of the Ebbinghaus Illusion varying based on eating disorder symptomatology. Both groups exhibit integration and therefore our hypothesis regarding differences in spatial integration between groups was not supported.

3.3. Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 again showed that participants demonstrated an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, even when controlling for AQ and BMI. In addition, individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology provided significantly larger body size judgements to larger bodies (Sizes 5 and 6), relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology. Importantly, we did not find any evidence of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies varying based on eating disorder symptomatology group.

4. General discussion

The current study aimed to develop a novel measure of spatial integration of body size – an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR. Specifically, we aimed to investigate if and how the perceived size of a female body is influenced by the presence of other bodies within the scene. We achieved this aim. For Experiment 1, it was hypothesized that there would be an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies, that is, participants would judge the central body to be larger when surrounded by a crowd of very thin bodies (the Small Distractors), and vice versa. As predicted, we observed an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, indicating that spatial integration of crowd information can lead to biased body size perception. It was found that the central body was perceived to be larger when surrounded by Small Distractors and perceived to be smaller when surrounded by Large Distractors, thus supporting our hypothesis. Additionally, the effect of a crowd on body size judgements was not consistent, with variation in the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies across body size category, as expected from work on the traditional Ebbinghaus Illusion (Roberts et al., 2005).

In Experiment 2, we investigated whether the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies varied amongst individuals differing in eating disorder symptomatology. It was hypothesized that individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology would exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies, relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology. We replicated the finding that participants demonstrated an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies. However, the illusion did not vary based on eating disorder symptomatology. Therefore, our hypothesis that higher eating disorder symptomatology would exhibit a smaller Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies was not supported.

Our findings in both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 demonstrated an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR, which indicated that spatial integration of crowd information can lead to biased body size perception. Furthermore, even when controlling for the appropriate variables including BMI and traits associated with autism (measured by AQ) in Experiment 2, the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies remained significant. Our results provide evidence of a further mechanism leading to body size misperceptions: surrounding spatial information (external to the target body) is integrated and can influence one’s judgement of body size.

Both experiments builds upon the work of Bateson et al. (2014), revealing the importance of looking at a range of body sizes. As revealed in Experiment 1, there was variation in the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies, across body size category. That is, the integration of spatial information influences one’s judgements of body sizes, however this does not occur equally for all target bodies. In Experiment 1, this was reflected in a non-significant Ebbinghaus Illusion for the Size 1 body and for largest body sizes (5–7). For the larger bodies, this result was somewhat unsurprising given that previous literature has shown that body size judgement accuracy is related to body size. As shown by Cornelissen et al. (2016), as bodies become overweight and obese, it becomes increasingly difficult for individuals to judge their body weight or to detect an increase in size (known as Weber’s law, where the “just noticeable difference” between two stimuli becomes larger as the magnitude of the judged object increases). By systematically manipulating target and distractors body sizes, our results characterize how the size relationship between target and distractors influences the magnitude of this body size illusion. It seems that the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies is the most noticeable when the target is a thin to average size female body, distinct in size from the surrounding crowd.

Despite the substantial amount of research regarding integration difficulties and weak central coherence in eating disorder populations (Lopez et al., 2008a,b,c; Blumberg et al., 2014; Lang et al., 2014a, 2021; Keegan et al., 2021), the illusion did not significantly differ between individuals high and low in eating disorder symptomatology as hypothesized. There could be several plausible reasons for this result. First, eating disorder symptomatology is a continuum ranging from normal eating behaviors, through to elevated eating disorder symptomatology, and clinical eating disorders (Shisslak et al., 1995). Therefore, while Experiment 2 used a representative sample of the extreme (e.g., high, and low) eating disorder symptomatology groups, this still may limit the degree to which these current findings can be generalized to a clinical eating disorder population. In saying this, the high eating disorder symptomatology group demonstrated higher levels of eating disorder symptomatology relative to previous research investigating these higher-risk groups (Jansen et al., 2005; Berrisford-Thompson et al., 2021) and even samples containing current and recovered eating disorder patients (Engel et al., 2022). Regardless, given that our high eating disorder symptomatology group was not a clinical sample, it may be possible that this group did not display a high enough severity of eating disorder symptoms. Furthermore, the EDE-Q used in this study provides an overall measure of eating disorder symptomatology (e.g., eating concern, restraint, weight concern, and shape concern) but is unable to discriminate between the eating disorder diagnoses. Given the higher BMI found in this high eating disorder symptomatology group, it may be possible that many of our participants in the high group had symptoms in line with bulimia nervosa, binge eating disorder, and other specified feeding and eating disorders rather than anorexia nervosa. Future research could extend these questions and methodology to those diagnosed with a clinical eating disorder to compare to the general population. In addition, this design may still be relevant to other clinical populations. As previous discussed, research has found that spatial integration difficulties, such as weak central coherence, are proposed to underlie core symptomatology of ASD (Frith, 1989). Therefore, this test of integration may be of interest to explore with other clinical populations displaying spatial integration difficulties, such as individuals differing in levels of traits associated with autism, and whether these difficulties extend to influencing upon their body size judgements.

While we found an Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR in both experiments, it was characterized by a small effect size. It is possible that the size of our Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies was too small to usefully demonstrate reductions in the size of the illusion/spatial integration according to eating disorder symptomatology. It is possible a modification of the task would increase the effect size for the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies in VR. Roberts et al. (2005) identified several factors that impact on the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion. One factor is the spacing between inducers and the central target, which may be compressed or expanded, depending on the size of the elements. Spacing the targets and distractors was challenging in our design, particularly in the Large Distractor condition, where the larger bodies created occlusions between distractors, potentially reducing their effectiveness (see Figure 1B). In addition, it was also possible for the inducers to occlude the central target body itself. By comparison, target occlusions were greatly reduced if not entirely absent for smaller body sizes. These occlusion effects created by the larger body sizes may also have contributed to the small or absent illusion for large to extreme (Sizes 5–7) body sizes in Experiment 1. Future research could adjust the spacing, size, posture and positioning of the surrounding inducer bodies.

It also important to acknowledge that there may be potential ceiling and floor effects reducing the magnitude of the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies. As previously discussed, Experiment 1 showed a non-significant Ebbinghaus Illusion for the smallest (Size 1) and largest (Size 7) bodies. Average judgements of a Size 1 body are less than 1.5, close to the end of the VAS. When participants are required to judge a Size 1 body surrounded by Size 7 distractors, being near the end of the scale already may inhibit the judgement and becoming biased toward an even smaller body size (floor effect). The reverse, or ceiling effects may also impact the size of the illusion for Size 7 judgements. Future studies could also consider running this experiment using only target body sizes 2–6. By doing so, we would be able to ensure that the distractors will always be bigger or smaller than the target stimuli.

Overall, it seems useful to note that our study does demonstrate that individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology are integrating spatial information and do not vary in the magnitude of this integration relative to individuals low in eating disorder symptomatology. The lack of significant difference between our high and low groups suggests that that spatial integration difficulties may not represent in tasks involving integration across a crowd when making body size judgements. That is, when the integration involves multiple discrete objects in a scene. This test of integration may also be of interest to explore what types of information from the surrounding inducers when making body size judgements, including the integration of local body features (e.g., hip-to-waist ratios, stomach circumference, etc.) or more holistic features (e.g., the body as a whole). Although this is not to say that these spatial integration difficulties would not emerge for a clinical eating disorder sample, these findings further help researchers understand when spatial integration difficulties emerge (or not) for individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology.

It seems plausible that the integration difficulties reported for individuals high in eating disorder symptomatology are specific to the integration of information within a single body, rather than across a scene. The importance of the single body is further reinforced by our results in Experiment 2, where we did observe differences between those high and low in eating disorder symptomatology, on single target body size judgements. This shows that how a single target body was judged differentiates between the high and low eating disorder symptomatology groups, rather than differences in spatial integration across the crowd of bodies. This leaves open the possibility that integration difficulties reported in previous literature are restricted to incidences involving a single target body. Therefore, our findings further support the use of single bodies in investigating integration difficulties within the context of eating disorder symptomatology.

The findings of this study provide an important extension to our limited understanding to how features external to a single target body can influence its perceived size. As previously discussed, to our knowledge, only Bateson et al. (2014, p. 195) has reported the impact that other bodies can have on size judgements. Our study further extends upon the findings of Bateson et al. (2014) in multiple ways. First and foremost, our results broaden understanding of bystander influence by systematically investigating and mapping how this size illusion varies as a function of target body size and of the target and distractor size difference. Next, we implemented this effect in a 3D setting, a novel extension to Bateson et al. (2014). Furthermore, we directly measured changes in body size judgements, rather than a related but indirect construct measuring change in the probability of being assigned the label of larger versus smaller in simultaneous stimuli presentations, as in Bateson et al. (2014).

Finally, we studied bystander influence using 3D bodies within a VR environment. This is a potentially important consideration given research showing that 3D cues can enhance and/or alter sensitivity to objects and object recognition (Bennett and Vuong, 2006; Burke et al., 2007; Bell et al., 2013; Caziot and Backus, 2015). In addition, Turnbull et al. (2022) found that 3D cues influence body size judgements by providing viewers with increased stimulus certainty (e.g., addition of volumetric information of lower stomach and bust regions). Furthermore, from an ecological perspective, the bodies that we often see in real life are surrounded by other people. By representing the crowd of bodies in 3D, we were able to create a design where the crowd of bodies could exist without occluding each other.

Our research also has limitations that should be acknowledged. First, using computer-generated (CG) stimuli prompts one limitation that the precise physical weight and thus BMI of our CG stimuli were unknown. Therefore, we cannot provide an exact estimate of veridical performance. Additionally, the use of CG stimuli over the use of real body images may also pose other limitations. Alexi et al. (2019a) have shown that using CG stimuli reduces discriminability between extreme body categories. This has been hypothesized to be due to the impoverished representation of textural elements in extreme CG imagery, such as the lack of cellulite or hollowed skin surfaces, which may be used as markers of body weight (Alexi et al., 2019a). However, it should be noted that research by Cornelissen et al. (2016) and Tovée et al. (2012) found comparable findings between real body images and CG stimuli. Additionally, the use of CG stimuli may have its advantages in allowing researchers to systematically control for body characteristics, such as weight and size, and allow for consistency between body size categories (Moussally et al., 2017). This study ensured that on each trial, each body varied in appearance, clothing and overall lighting. Furthermore, in our study, our CG stimuli have the advantage of including stereoscopic (e.g., three-dimensional) cues, which provide further realism and ecological validity, relative to 2D CG stimuli used in previous studies (Turnbull et al., 2022).

We also acknowledge that our study presented female body stimuli, but not male body stimuli. This is important to consider given that judgements of male and female bodies may differ. For context, research has shown that judgements of male body size are mediated by both body fat and muscularity (Adams et al., 2005; Jones and Crawford, 2005; Tylka, 2011). Furthermore, adiposity and muscularity are shown to be two independent dimensions considered in perceptions of body size and composition (McCreary et al., 2006; Coetzee et al., 2010; Sturman et al., 2017), however this distinction has not been captured in our stimulus continuum. Since bodies also vary in muscle mass, particularly for men, future studies may explore whether the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies occurs using male body stimuli. Furthermore, while the focus of this study has been on a female demographic shown to be most at risk of development of an eating disorder (Hudson et al., 2007; Stice et al., 2013; Rohde et al., 2017), it is important to acknowledge that peak onset for eating disorders in men is at aged 18—20 years old (Allen et al., 2013; Raevuori et al., 2014). Given the increasing rates of body dissatisfaction and eating disorders in young men (Dakanalis et al., 2016), it would be important to re-create Experiment 2 using male body stimuli and male participants varying in eating disorder symptomatology. This would help us understand whether the Ebbinghaus Illusion for Bodies differs based on the gender of the stimuli and eating disorder symptomatology of the sample used. This would further contribute to a robust understanding of spatial integration and weak central coherence within males high in eating disorder symptomatology.

In summary, using a novel design, we wanted to understand the factors that can impact upon body size judgements. While previous research has opted to look at the judgement of a single, isolated test body, this study demonstrates that spatial integration of a crowd of surrounding bodies can influence body size judgements. In the general population, it was found that the integration of the Large Distractors resulted in the target body being perceived as smaller and the integration of the Small Distractors result in the target being perceived as larger. Critically, our results revealed that those high and low in eating disorder symptomatology demonstrated equivalent integration. Using VR technology to simulate a crowd and increase ecological validity, this study provides a novel explanation of why we make errors in body size judgements. Overall, the findings add to the growing body of literature demonstrating the malleability of body size judgements and provide further support on the design choices of researchers looking to examine body size and shape judgement errors.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The University of Western Australia Human Research Ethics Committee. Approval #: 2021/ET000004. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

GT, SL, BK, and JB designed the study. YL provided technical assistance with the virtual reality, stimuli creation, and software. Testing and data collection were performed by GT, SL, BK, JA, GM, and JB. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^As a preliminary analysis, gender was included as an independent variable. There was no significant effect of gender on body size judgements. Given that gender was not the primary focus of this study, no further analyses including gender were conducted.

References

Adams, G., Turner, H., and Bucks, R. (2005). The experience of body dissatisfaction in men. Body image 2, 271–283. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexi, J., Cleary, D., Dommisse, K., Palermo, R., Kloth, N., Burr, D., et al. (2018). Past visual experiences weigh in on body size estimation. Sci. Rep. 8:215. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18418-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexi, J., Dommisse, K., Cleary, D., Palermo, R., Kloth, N., and Bell, J. (2019a). An assessment of computer generated stimuli for use in studies of body size estimation and bias. Front. Psychol. 10:2390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexi, J., Palermo, R., Rieger, E., and Bell, J. (2019b). Evidence for a perceptual mechanism relating body size misperception and eating disorder symptoms. Eat. Weight Disord. 24, 615–621. doi: 10.1007/s40519-019-00653-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, K. L., Byrne, S. M., McLean, N. J., and Davis, E. A. (2008). Overconcern with weight and shape is not the same as body dissatisfaction: evidence from a prospective study of pre-adolescent boys and girls. Body Image 5, 261–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, K. L., Byrne, S. M., Oddy, W. H., and Crosby, R. D. (2013). DSM–IV–TR and DSM-5 eating disorders in adolescents: prevalence, stability, and psychosocial correlates in a population-based sample of male and female adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 720–732. doi: 10.1037/a0034004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Tovée, M. J., George, H. R., Gouws, A., and Cornelissen, P. L. (2014). Humans are not fooled by size illusions in attractiveness judgements. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, N., Baumann, P., Herpertz, S., Trojan, J., and Diers, M. (2021). How the unconscious mind controls body movements: body schema distortion in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 54, 578–586. doi: 10.1002/eat.23451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, J., Kanji, J., and Kingdom, F. A. (2013). Discrimination of rotated-in-depth curves is facilitated by stereoscopic cues, but curvature is not tuned for stereoscopic rotation-in-depth. Vis. Res. 77, 14–20. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, D. J., and Vuong, Q. C. (2006). A stereo advantage in generalizing over changes in viewpoint on object recognition tasks. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 1082–1093. doi: 10.3758/BF03193711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, K. C., Peterson, C. B., Frazier, P., and Crow, S. J. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the eating disorder examination and eating disorder examination-questionnaire: a systematic review of the literature. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 428–438. doi: 10.1002/eat.20931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkman, N. D., Lohr, K. N., and Bulik, C. M. (2007). Outcomes of eating disorders: a systematic review of the literature. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 40, 293–309. doi: 10.1002/eat.20369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berrisford-Thompson, J., Sayers, S., Bell, J., Dondzilo, L., and Kennedy, B. L. (2021). Blinded by bodies: elevated eating disorder symptomatology is associated with increased attentional priority for thin bodies. Body Image 39, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumberg, J., Allen, K., and Byrne, S. (2014). Measuring central coherence and set shifting in anorexia nervosa: the navon figures task. J. Eat. Disord. 2:1. doi: 10.1186/2050-2974-2-S1-O45

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Holtmann, M., Poustka, F., Scheurich, A., and Schmidt, L. (2007). Gestalt perception and local-global processing in high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1493–1504. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0231-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, K. R., Clifford, C. W., Stevenson, R. J., Mond, J., and Stephen, I. D. (2018). The high-level basis of body adaptation. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5:172103. doi: 10.1098/rsos.172103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, K. R., Mond, J. M., Stevenson, R. J., and Stephen, I. D. (2016). Body image distortion and exposure to extreme body types: contingent adaptation and cross adaptation for self and other. Front. Neurosci. 10:334. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D., Taubert, J., and Higman, T. (2007). Are face representations viewpoint dependent? A stereo advantage for generalising across different views of faces. Vis. Res. 47, 2164–2169. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, J. C., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Norwood, S. J., Bewell-Weiss, C. V., Crosby, R. D., Woodside, D. B., et al. (2012). A prospective study of predictors of relapse in anorexia nervosa: implications for relapse prevention. Psychiatry Res. 200, 518–523. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, T. F., and Deagle, E. A. (1997). The nature and extent of body-image disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 22, 107–126. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199709)22:2<107::AID-EAT1>3.0.CO;2-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, T. F., Fleming, E. C., Alindogan, J., Steadman, L., and Whitehead, A. (2002). Beyond body image as a trait: the development and validation of the body image states scale. Eat. Disord. 10, 103–113. doi: 10.1080/10640260290081678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., Halmi, K. A., Goldberg, S. C., Eckert, E. D., and Davis, J. M. (1979). Disturbances in body image estimation as related to other characteristics and outcome in anorexia nervosa. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 60–66. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.1.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro, J., Gila, A., Puig, J., Rodriguez, S., and Toro, J. (2004). Predictors of rehospitalization after total weight recovery in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 36, 22–30. doi: 10.1002/eat.20009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caziot, B., and Backus, B. T. (2015). Stereoscopic offset makes objects easier to recognize. PLoS One 10:e0129101. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cazzato, V., Mian, E., Mele, S., Tognana, G., Todisco, P., and Urgesi, C. (2016). The effects of body exposure on self-body image and esthetic appreciation in anorexia nervosa. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 695–709. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4498-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Challinor, K. L., Mond, J., Stephen, I. D., Mitchison, D., Stevenson, R. J., Hay, P., et al. (2017). Body size and shape misperception and visual adaptation: an overview of an emerging research paradigm. J. Int. Med. Res. 45, 2001–2008. doi: 10.1177/0300060517726440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V., Chen, J., Perrett, D. I., and Stephen, I. D. (2010). Deciphering faces: Quantifiable visual cues to weight. Perception 39, 51–61. doi: 10.1068/p6560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelissen, K. K., Bester, A., Cairns, P., Tovée, M. J., and Cornelissen, P. L. (2015). The influence of personal BMI on body size estimations and sensitivity to body size change in anorexia spectrum disorders. Body Image 13, 75–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelissen, K. K., Gledhill, L. J., Cornelissen, P. L., and Tovée, M. J. (2016). Visual biases in judging body weight. Br. J. Health Psychol. 21, 555–569. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelissen, P. L., Johns, A., and Tovée, M. J. (2013). Body size over-estimation in women with anorexia nervosa is not qualitatively different from female controls. Body Image 10, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, L. D. C. F., Silva, D. A. S., Almeida, S. D. S., and Vasconcelos, F. D. A. G. D. (2015). Association between inaccurate estimation of body size and obesity in schoolchildren. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 37, 220–226. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, P. J., West, S. G., and Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 1, 16–29. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dakanalis, A., Pla-Sanjuanelo, J., Caslini, M., Volpato, C., Riva, G., Clerici, M., et al. (2016). Predicting onset and maintenance of men's eating disorders. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 16, 247–255. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2016.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danner, U. N., Sanders, N., Smeets, P. A., van Meer, F., Adan, R. A., Hoek, H. W., et al. (2012). Neuropsychological weaknesses in anorexia nervosa: set-shifting, central coherence, and decision making in currently ill and recovered women. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 685–694. doi: 10.1002/eat.22007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Fockert, J., Davidoff, J., Fagot, J., Parron, C., and Goldstein, J. (2007). More accurate size contrast judgments in the Ebbinghaus illusion by a remote culture. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 738–742. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell'Osso, L., Carpita, B., Gesi, C., Cremone, I., Corsi, M., Massimetti, E., et al. (2018). Subthreshold autism spectrum disorder in patients with eating disorders. Compr. Psychiatry 81, 66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickter, C. L., Burk, J. A., Fleckenstein, K., and Kozikowski, C. T. (2018). Autistic traits and social anxiety predict differential performance on social cognitive tasks in typically developing young adults. PLoS One 13:e0195239. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dondzilo, L., Rieger, E., Palermo, R., Byrne, S., and Bell, J. (2016). Association between rumination factors and eating disorder behaviours in young women. Adv. Eat. Disord. 4, 84–98. doi: 10.1080/21662630.2015.1118642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dondzilo, L., Rieger, E., Palermo, R., Byrne, S., and Bell, J. (2017). The mediating role of rumination in the relation between attentional bias towards thin female bodies and eating disorder symptomatology. PLoS One 12:e0177870. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebbinghaus, H. (1902). Grundzüge der psychologie, vol. 1 Leipzig: Verlag von Veit & Comp.

Google Scholar

Edwards, N. M., Pettingell, S., and Borowsky, I. W. (2010). Where perception meets reality: self-perception of weight in overweight adolescents. Pediatrics 125, e452–e458. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, M. M., Gadsby, S., Corcoran, A. W., Keizer, A., Dijkerman, H. C., and Hohwy, J. (2022). Waiting longer, feeling fatter: effects of response delay on tactile distance estimation and confidence in females with anorexia nervosa. Brain Behav. 12:e2422. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, M. M., and Keizer, A. (2017). Body representation disturbances in visual perception and affordance perception persist in eating disorder patients after completing treatment. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16362-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M. R., Haggard, P., and Treasure, J. (2014). Persistent body image disturbance following recovery from eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 400–409. doi: 10.1002/eat.22219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairburn, C. G., and Beglin, S. J. (1994). Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self-report questionnaire? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 16, 363–370.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawcett, S. L., and Birch, E. E. (2003). Validity of the Titmus and Randot circles tasks in children with known binocular vision disorders. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 7, 333–338. doi: 10.1016/S1091-8531(03)00170-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, J., and Whitney, D. (2014). Serial dependence in visual perception. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 738–743. doi: 10.1038/nn.3689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegal, K. M., Kit, B. K., and Graubard, B. I. (2014). Body mass index categories in observational studies of weight and risk of death. Am. J. Epidemiol. 180, 288–296. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 3, 465–468.

Google Scholar

Fursland, A., Byrne, S., Watson, H., La Puma, M., Allen, K., and Byrne, S. (2012). Enhanced cognitive behavior therapy: a single treatment for all eating disorders. J. Couns. Dev. 90, 319–329. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2012.00040.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gadsby, S. (2017). Distorted body representations in anorexia nervosa. Conscious. Cogn. 51, 17–33. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, R. M., and Bokenkamp, E. D. (1996). The role of sensory and nonsensory factors in body size estimations of eating disorder subjects. J. Clin. Psychol. 52, 3–15. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199601)52:1<3::AID-JCLP1>3.0.CO;2-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnham, L., and Sloper, J. (2006). Effect of age on adult stereoacuity as measured by different types of stereotest. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 90, 91–95. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2005.077719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudio, S., Brooks, S. J., and Riva, G. (2014). Nonvisual multisensory impairment of body perception in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review of neuropsychological studies. PLoS One 9:e110087. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J., and Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 453–467. doi: 10.1037/h0059826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gignac, G. E. (2019). How2statsbook (online edition 1). Author.

Google Scholar

Gledhill, L., George, H., and Tovee, M. (2019). Perceptual not attitudinal factors predict the accuracy of estimating other women’s bodies in both women with anorexia nervosa and controls. Front. Psychol. 10:997. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gori, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2016). Visual illusions: an interesting tool to investigate developmental dyslexia and autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:175. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, S., Hay, P., Mitchison, D., Mond, J. M., McLean, S. A., Rodgers, B., et al. (2016). Sex differences in the relationships between body dissatisfaction, quality of life and psychological distress. Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health 40, 518–522. doi: 10.1111/1753-6405.12538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hambrook, D., Tchanturia, K., Schmidt, U., Russell, T., and Treasure, J. (2008). Empathy, systemizing, and autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a pilot study. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 47, 335–339. doi: 10.1348/014466507X272475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 873–877. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01483.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G., and Booth, R. D. (2008). The power of the positive: revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 50–63. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, A., Tchanturia, K., and Treasure, J. (2011). Measuring state trait properties of detail processing and global integration ability in eating disorders. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 12, 462–472. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2010.551666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoek, H. W. (2006). Incidence, prevalence and mortality of anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 389–394. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000228759.95237.78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollingworth, H. L. (1910). The central tendency of judgment. The J. Philo. Psychol. Sci. Methods 7, 461–469. doi: 10.2307/2012819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, D., Rogier, M., Yzerbyt, V., and Bestgen, Y. (1998). Statistical methods in human sciences. New York: Wadsworth, 721.

Google Scholar

HTC, Valve Corporation. (2018). HTC Vive Pro Eye series. [Apparatus]. Available at: https://www.vive.com/au/product/#pro%20eye%20series

Google Scholar

Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G. Jr., and Kessler, R. C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 348–358. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huke, V., Turk, J., Saeidi, S., Kent, A., and Morgan, J. F. (2013). Autism spectrum disorders in eating disorder populations: a systematic review. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 21, 345–351. doi: 10.1002/erv.2244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummel, D., Rudolf, A. K., Brandi, M. L., Untch, K. H., Grabhorn, R., Hampel, H., et al. (2013). Neural adaptation to thin and fat bodies in the fusiform body area and middle occipital gyrus: an fMRI adaptation study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 3233–3246. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummel, D., Rudolf, A. K., Untch, K.-H., Grabhorn, R., and Mohr, H. M. (2012). Visual adaptation to thin and fat bodies transfers across identity. PLoS One 7:e43195. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iarocci, G., and McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 77–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0044-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue, T., Otani, R., Iguchi, T., Ishii, R., Uchida, S., Okada, A., et al. (2021). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in children with anorexia nervosa and avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder. Biopsychosoc. Med. 15, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s13030-021-00212-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, A., Nederkoorn, C., and Mulkens, S. (2005). Selective visual attention for ugly and beautiful body parts in eating disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 183–196. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. C., and Crawford, J. K. (2005). Adolescent boys and body image: Weight and muscularity concerns as dual pathways to body dissatisfaction. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 34, 629–636. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-8951-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keegan, E., Tchanturia, K., and Wade, T. D. (2021). Central coherence and set-shifting between nonunderweight eating disorders and anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 54, 229–243. doi: 10.1002/eat.23430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keel, P. K., Dorer, D. J., Franko, D. L., Jackson, S. C., and Herzog, D. B. (2005). Postremission predictors of relapse in women with eating disorders. Am. J. Psychiatr. 162, 2263–2268. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keizer, A., Smeets, M. A., Dijkerman, H. C., Uzunbajakau, S. A., van Elburg, A., and Postma, A. (2013). Too fat to fit through the door: first evidence for disturbed body-scaled action in anorexia nervosa during locomotion. PLoS One 8:e64602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keizer, A., Smeets, M. A. M., Dijkerman, H. C., van Elburg, A., and Postma, A. (2012). Aberrant somatosensory perception in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 200, 530–537. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr-Gaffney, J., Halls, D., Harrison, A., and Tchanturia, K. (2020). Exploring relationships between autism spectrum disorder symptoms and eating disorder symptoms in adults with anorexia nervosa: a network approach. Front. Psychol. 11:401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, K., Kerr-Gaffney, J., Hodsoll, J., Jassi, A., Tchanturia, K., and Krebs, G. (2021). Is poor global processing a transdiagnostic feature of body dysmorphic disorder and anorexia nervosa? A meta-analysis. Body Image 37, 94–105. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, K., Lopez, C., Stahl, D., Tchanturia, K., and Treasure, J. (2014a). Central coherence in eating disorders: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 15, 586–598. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2014.909606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, K., Stahl, D., Espie, J., Treasure, J., and Tchanturia, K. (2014b). Set shifting in children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa: an exploratory systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 394–399. doi: 10.1002/eat.22235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewer, M., Nasrawi, N., Schroeder, D., and Vocks, S. (2016). Body image disturbance in binge eating disorder: a comparison of obese patients with and without binge eating disorder regarding the cognitive, behavioral and perceptual component of body image. Eat. Weight Disord. 21, 115–125. doi: 10.1007/s40519-015-0200-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, C., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., Booth, R., Holliday, J., and Treasure, J. (2008a). An examination of the concept of central coherence in women with anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 143–152. doi: 10.1002/eat.20478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, C., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., and Treasure, J. (2008b). Central coherence in eating disorders: a systematic review. Psychol. Med. 38, 1393–1404. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, C. A., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., and Treasure, J. (2008c). Central coherence in women with bulimia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 340–347. doi: 10.1002/eat.20511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandy, W., and Tchanturia, K. (2015). Do women with eating disorders who have social and flexibility difficulties really have autism? A case series. Mol. Autism 6, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-6-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCreary, D. R., Karvinen, K., and Davis, C. (2006). The relationship between the drive for muscularity and anthropometric measures of muscularity and adiposity. Body image 3, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metral, M., Guardia, D., Bauwens, I., Guerraz, M., Lafargue, G., Cottencin, O., et al. (2014). Painfully thin but locked inside a fatter body: abnormalities in both anticipation and execution of action in anorexia nervosa. BMC. Res. Notes 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-7-707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micali, N., Solmi, F., Horton, N. J., Crosby, R. D., Eddy, K. T., Calzo, J. P., et al. (2015). Adolescent eating disorders predict psychiatric, high-risk behaviors and weight outcomes in young adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 652–659.e1. e651. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moelbert, S. C., Klein, L., Thaler, A., Mohler, B. J., Brozzo, C., Martus, P., et al. (2017). Depictive and metric body size estimation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 57, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, H. M., Rickmeyer, C., Hummel, D., Ernst, M., and Grabhorn, R. (2016). Altered visual adaptation to body shape in eating disorders: implications for body image distortion. Perception 45, 725–738. doi: 10.1177/0301006616633385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mond, J. M., Hay, P. J., Rodgers, B., and Owen, C. (2006). Eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q): norms for young adult women. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moussally, J. M., Rochat, L., Posada, A., and Van der Linden, M. (2017). A database of body-only computer-generated pictures of women for body-image studies: Development and preliminary validation. Behavior research methods 49, 172–183. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0703-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicoli, M. G., and Junior, R. D. L. (2011). Binge eating disorder and body image perception among university students. Eat. Behav. 12, 284–288. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldershaw, A., Treasure, J., Hambrook, D., Tchanturia, K., and Schmidt, U. (2011). Is anorexia nervosa a version of autism spectrum disorders? Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 19, 462–474. doi: 10.1002/erv.1069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldham, M., and Robinson, E. (2016). Visual weight status misperceptions of men: why overweight can look like a healthy weight. J. Health Psychol. 21, 1768–1777. doi: 10.1177/1359105314566257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C. B., Crosby, R. D., Wonderlich, S. A., Joiner, T., Crow, S. J., Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2007). Psychometric properties of the eating disorder examination-questionnaire: factor structure and internal consistency. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 40, 386–389. doi: 10.1002/eat.20373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pooni, J., Ninteman, A., Bryant-Waugh, R., Nicholls, D., and Mandy, W. (2012). Investigating autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in early onset eating disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 583–591. doi: 10.1002/eat.20980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raevuori, A., Keski-Rahkonen, A., and Hoek, H. W. (2014). A review of eating disorders in males. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 27, 426–430. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reifman, A., and Keyton, K. (2010). Winsorize. in Encyclopedia of Research Design. ed. N. J. Salkind Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1636–1638.

Google Scholar

Riva, G., and Dakanalis, A. (2018). Altered processing and integration of multisensory bodily representations and signals in eating disorders: a possible path toward the understanding of their underlying causes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:49. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva, G., and Gaudio, S. (2018). Locked to a wrong body: eating disorders as the outcome of a primary disturbance in multisensory body integration. Conscious. Cogn. 59, 57–59. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B., Harris, M. G., and Yates, T. A. (2005). The roles of inducer size and distance in the Ebbinghaus illusion (Titchener circles). Perception 34, 847–856. doi: 10.1068/p5273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E., and Kirkham, T. C. (2014). Is he a healthy weight? Exposure to obesity changes perception of the weight status of others. Int. J. Obes. 38, 663–667. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohde, P., Stice, E., Shaw, H., Gau, J. M., and Ohls, O. C. (2017). Age effects in eating disorder baseline risk factors and prevention intervention effects. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 50, 1273–1280. doi: 10.1002/eat.22775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ropar, D., and Mitchell, P. (1999). Are individuals with autism and Asperger's syndrome susceptible to visual illusions? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 40, 1283–1293. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ropar, D., and Mitchell, P. (2001). Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visuospatial tasks in individuals with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 42, 539–549. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouse, H., Donnelly, N., Hadwin, J. A., and Brown, T. (2004). Do children with autism perceive second-order relational features? The case of the Thatcher illusion. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1246–1257. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00317.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, M., and Meilleur, D. (2010). Body image distortion change during inpatient treatment of adolescent girls with restrictive anorexia nervosa. Eat. Weight Disord. 15, e108–e115. doi: 10.1007/BF03325289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., et al. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: a systematic review of the autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Mol. Autism. 6, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-6-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, M., Xu, H., Zhang, H., Shui, R., Zhang, M., and Zhou, J. (2015). The working memory Ponzo illusion: involuntary integration of visuospatial information stored in visual working memory. Cognition 141, 26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shisslak, C. M., Crago, M., and Estes, L. S. (1995). The spectrum of eating disturbances. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 18, 209–219. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(199511)18:3<209::AID-EAT2260180303>3.0.CO;2-E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. E., Mason, T. B., Johnson, J. S., Lavender, J. M., and Wonderlich, S. A. (2018). A systematic review of reviews of neurocognitive functioning in eating disorders: the state-of-the-literature and future directions. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 798–821. doi: 10.1002/eat.22929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solmi, F., Sharpe, H., Gage, S. H., Maddock, J., Lewis, G., and Patalay, P. (2020). Changes in the prevalence and correlates of weight-control behaviors and weight perception in adolescents in the UK, 1986-2015. JAMA Pediatr. 175, 267–275. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.4746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinhausen, H.-C. (2009). Outcome of eating disorders. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 18, 225–242. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stereo Optical (2017). Titmus Stereo Fly Stereotest; Graded Circles [Stereoacuity Test]. Retrieved from https://www.stereooptical.com/products/stereotests-color-tests/original-stereo-fly/

Google Scholar

Stice, E., Gau, J. M., Rohde, P., and Shaw, H. (2017). Risk factors that predict future onset of each DSM–5 eating disorder: predictive specificity in high-risk adolescent females. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126, 38–51. doi: 10.1037/abn0000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Marti, C. N., and Rohde, P. (2013). Prevalence, incidence, impairment, and course of the proposed DSM-5 eating disorder diagnoses in an 8-year prospective community study of young women. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 445–457. doi: 10.1037/a0030679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., and Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. J. Psychosom. Res. 53, 985–993. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00488-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturman, D., Stephen, I. D., Mond, J., Stevenson, R. J., and Brooks, K. R. (2017). Independent Aftereffects of Fat and Muscle: Implications for neural encoding, body space representation, and body image disturbance. Scientific reports 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1038/srep40392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, A., Geuss, M. N., Mölbert, S. C., Giel, K. E., Streuber, S., Romero, J., et al. (2018). Body size estimation of self and others in females varying in BMI. PLoS One 13:e0192152. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Benson, P. J., Emery, J. L., Mason, S. M., and Cohen-Tovée, E. M. (2003). Measurement of body size and shape perception in eating-disordered and control observers using body-shape software. Br. J. Psychol. 94, 501–516. doi: 10.1348/000712603322503060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Edmonds, L., and Vuong, Q. C. (2012). Categorical perception of human female physical attractiveness and health. Evolution and Human Behavior 33, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Emery, J. L., and Cohen-Tovée, E. M. (2000). The estimation of body mass index and physical attractiveness is dependent on the observer's own body mass index. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 267, 1987–1997. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treasure, J. (2013). Coherence and other autistic spectrum traits and eating disorders: building from mechanism to treatment. The Birgit Olsson lecture. Nord. J. Psychiatry 67, 38–42. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2012.674554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turnbull, G., Alexi, J., Mann, G., Li, Y. R., Engel, M., Bayliss, D., et al. (2022). The influence of three-dimensional cues on body size judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 75, 2318–2331. doi: 10.1177/17470218221076850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L. (2011). Refinement of the tripartite influence model for men: Dual body image pathways to body change behaviors. Body image 8, 199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unity Technologies (2019). Unity Software (Version 2.21) [Computer Software]. Available at: https://unity.com

Google Scholar

Urgesi, C., Fornasari, L., Canalaz, F., Perini, L., Cremaschi, S., Faleschini, L., et al. (2014). Impaired configural body processing in anorexia nervosa: evidence from the body inversion effect. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 486–508. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urgesi, C., Fornasari, L., Perini, L., Canalaz, F., Cremaschi, S., Faleschini, L., et al. (2012). Visual body perception in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 501–511. doi: 10.1002/eat.20982

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Autreve, S., De Baene, W., Baeken, C., Van Heeringen, C., and Vervaet, M. (2013). Do restrictive and bingeing/purging subtypes of anorexia nervosa differ on central coherence and set shifting? Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 21, 308–314. doi: 10.1002/erv.2233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardle, J., Haase, A. M., and Steptoe, A. (2006). Body image and weight control in young adults: international comparisons in university students from 22 countries. Int. J. Obes. 30, 644–651. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehouse, A. M., Freeman, C. P., and Annandale, A. (1988). Body size estimation in anorexia nervosa. Br. J. Psychiatry 153, 23–26. doi: 10.1192/S0007125000298942

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, D. A., Cubic, B. A., and Gleaves, D. H. (1993). Equivalence of body image disturbances in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 102, 177–180. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, C., and Rhodes, G. (2005). Perceptual adaptation affects attractiveness of female bodies. Br. J. Psychol. 96, 141–154. doi: 10.1348/000712605X36343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, M., and Hughes, E. (2011). Eating disorders: early identification in general practice. Aust. Fam. Physician 40, 108–111. doi: 10.3316/informit.807032232283518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: body size judgements, perceptual body image disturbance, integration, virtual reality, perception

Citation: Turnbull G, Lego S, Kennedy BL, Alexi J, Li YR, Engel MM, Mann G, Bayliss DM, Farrell S and Bell J (2023) Sizing up the crowd: Assessing spatial integration difficulties in body size judgements across eating disorder symptomatology. Front. Psychol. 13:1003250. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1003250

Received: 26 July 2022; Accepted: 30 November 2022;
Published: 06 January 2023.

Edited by:

Alix Timko, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Reviewed by:

Piers Louis Cornelissen, Northumbria University, United Kingdom
Ke Ma, Southwest University, China

Copyright © 2023 Turnbull, Lego, Kennedy, Alexi, Li, Engel, Mann, Bayliss, Farrell and Bell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Georgia Turnbull, yes georgia.turnbull@research.uwa.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.