- 1Department of Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management, Business School, Central University of Finance and Economics, Beijing, China
- 2Division of Leadership, Management, and Organizations, Nanyang Business School, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
- 3Department of Business Administration, School of Management, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
- 4Department of Business Statistics and Econometrics, Guanghua School of Management, Peking University, Beijing, China
- 5Policy Research Office, China National Petroleum Corporation, Beijing, China
The relationship between narcissism and creativity has inspired interesting debates for decades. Drawing on a new perspective, the current study tried to explain how narcissism influences others’ creativity evaluation in the organizational context. Based on the theory of impression management, we suggested that narcissism and creativity may have a more complex relationship rather than a simple linear link. To test this relationship, we conducted a survey of 596 subordinates and 60 leaders in three high-technology companies. The result showed that there was an inverted U-shaped relationship between narcissism and creativity evaluation. Moreover, personal reputation mediated this curvilinear relationship and this relationship was significant only when narcissists were low in political skill. Theoretical and practical implications, limitations and future directions have also been discussed.
Introduction
“We don’t settle for anything less than excellence.” -------Steve Jobs
Da Vinci, Nietzsche, Oscar Wilde, and Salvador Dali; such genii have the best minds in human history. They have left us with a fortune of thought, their famous stories illustrating the fine line between genius and insanity providing a source of great delight to explore. Their pomposity amazes the public, who laugh it off: How can we blame a person for simply being gifted? Similarly, in the organizational context, business leaders—such as Mark Zuckerberg, Elon Musk, and Donald Trump—are often self-aggrandizing and self-involved, eager to push their vision and products on the masses. It seems that there exists a common stereotype of highly creative or capable individuals as being narcissistic (Macdonald and Wilson, 2005; Smith and Webster, 2018). Anecdotes, stories, and biographies share the common view of the link between narcissists and creative talents.
The relationship between narcissism and creativity has likewise inspired interesting debates (Lebuda et al., 2021). Several scholars suggest no such relationship, in that narcissism may be an inevitable by-product of creative talent (Goncalo et al., 2010). Generally speaking, creative people spend a considerable deal of time alone, are often absorbed in their work to the point of obsession, and refuse to conform to social conventions; such behavior leads to the perception of narcissism to others. By contrast, given that narcissists are motivated to generate novel ideas to “stand out” and draw others’ attention, the opposite view is that narcissism may directly contribute to creativity (Raskin, 1980) and entrepreneurial behavior (Li et al., 2021). With personality traits involving grandiose self-views, sense of entitlements, and desire for uniqueness and superiority (Morf and Rhodewalt, 2001; Campbell et al., 2011), narcissists are compelled to engage in praiseworthy behaviors to affirm their favorable self-views (Resick et al., 2009). This view that narcissism is positively linked to creativity is gaining evidence in psychological fields, but has barely been tested in actual organizational contexts. Moreover, in literature, self-reported measures of creativity are often used (Raskin, 1980; Goncalo et al., 2010; Furnham et al., 2013), which may cause bias given that narcissists have a strong self-enhancement tendency and may view themselves more positively than others do (Grijalva and Harms, 2014). The difficulty to determine whether narcissists simply believe they are creative or actually demonstrate creative behaviors warrants investigation (Mao et al., 2021), therefore presenting considerable significance to examine the question: Is narcissism related to perceived creativity in real organizational contexts, and through which mechanism can narcissists achieve such evaluation?
The current study suggests that in actual organizations, rather than a simple linear link, narcissism and creativity may have a more complex relationship, such as a reversed U-shape mediated by personal reputation. On the basis of impression management theory, this study posits that narcissists are both motivated to manage their impression on others to achieve high creativity evaluation and unconsciously act to be perceived as creative individuals. Narcissists may intentionally gain admiration (motivation) while also (unconsciously) inspiring others to consider them to have leadership, confidence, and charm that contribute to their good reputation (Glad, 2002; Back et al., 2010), leading to high creativity evaluation. However, consistent with the Chinese philosophy of “Modest is the best,” when narcissism exceeds a moderate level, its dark side becomes so salient that others can view their antagonistic self-protection intentions as vulnerability, aggressiveness, arrogance, and grandiosity (Amabile, 1996; Back et al., 2010). These perceived impressions can be detrimental to the narcissist’s reputation, which in turn, influence others’ creativity evaluation. Thus, not all narcissists can win others’ good impressions; and only those with moderate narcissism level can achieve this feat.
In impression management field, a central topic that is gaining considerable attention is political skill, which is suggested to help various personalities achieve their social reputation. Hogan and Shelton (1998) argued that social skills serve as a moderator that can help or impede the interpretation of people’ motivations into observers’ evaluation. Accordingly, the present study posits that the relationship between narcissism and reputation is moderated by political skill, which refers to an interpersonal effectiveness construct that combines social understanding with the flexibility to adjust behaviors to fit the demands of the situation in ways that appear sincere, inspire trust and support, and effectively influence others (Ferris et al., 2005). Thus, individuals with high political skill can better manipulate others’ perceptions to underestimate their dark sides, thereby restricting the influence of narcissism. We propose that the inverted U-shape correlation of narcissism with both personal reputation and creativity may be significant only when narcissists have low political skill.
In sum, we examine the relationship between narcissism and creativity by building a moderated mediation model (Figure 1), which contributes to existing research in several ways. First, examining creativity as an outcome of narcissism enriches the present understanding by highlighting the potential “bright side” of a “dark trait,” given that narcissism is typically regarded as undesirable with severe implications for negative employee behavior (O’Boyle et al., 2012; Grijalva and Harms, 2014), such as counterproductive work behavior (Li et al., 2020). Second, this study contributes to narcissism literature by providing evidence demonstrating that moderate narcissism level has an optimal effect, complementing previous studies that simply suggest a positive relationship between narcissism and creativity (Furnham et al., 2013). Third, diverging from self-reports of creativity, we use other-rater (supervisor) reports of creativity in field study (Zhou et al., 2019), which can better explore the relationship between narcissism and true creativity. Finally, by exploring the mediating effect of personal reputation and moderating effect of political skill, we contribute to creativity research by examining and demonstrating the effect of personality on impression.
Argument and hypothesis
Narcissism and creativity: a new perspective of impression management
Greek Mythology tells the story of Narcissus, a proud young man who fell in love with his own reflection. Taken from his name, narcissism can be defined as a pervasive pattern of grandiosity, self-importance, and self-regard (Resick et al., 2009). Narcissists are often described as arrogant, confident, charming, always a leader, have a strong desire to be unique, need attention and admiration, and are likely to take risks (Bogart et al., 2004; Chatterjee and Hambrick, 2011). This trait involves both the bright side of self-enhancement and the dark side of self-protection, characteristics that lead to contrary outcomes in the organization (Küfner et al., 2013); thus, the effect of narcissism on processes and consequences have always been paradoxical. In terms of the bright side, narcissists are highly charming, self-assured, and more successful in short-term romantic relationships (Rhodewalt and Eddings, 2002; Holtzman and Strube, 2010) as well as humorous (Back et al., 2010). In terms of the dark side, narcissists may impede cooperation with others (Blair et al., 2008), have high correlations with aggressive behaviors (e.g., Bushman et al., 2003) and counterproductive work behaviors (Penney and Spector, 2002).
Most importantly, the relationship between narcissism and creativity currently attracts much academic attention (Elsbach and Kramer, 2003; Brunell et al., 2008; Goncalo et al., 2010; Mao et al., 2021). The possible reason may be the anecdotes, stories, and biographies that provide the impression that the most talented people are self-aggrandizing and narcissistic. Whether or not this logic is true has become the center of debate (John and Robins, 1994; Farwell and Wohlwend-Lloyd, 1998; Dugosh and Paulus, 2005). By using a sample of 1,375 young adults, Martinsen et al. (2019) suggested that narcissism can contribute to creativity. Yet, laboratory experiments such as by Farwell and Wohlwend-Lloyd (1998) revealed that narcissists are not necessarily more creative than their peers. In addition, narcissism is found to only cause a devious evaluation of creativity (Goncalo et al., 2010), which in turn is actually influenced by the traits of storytellers (Elsbach and Kramer, 2003).
Although the relationship between narcissism and creativity has undergone considerable exploration, several limitations still need further verification. First, existing research mainly used experimental method and may ignore complex real organizational contexts. Second, previous research mainly focuses on the motivations of narcissists regarding creativity, demonstrating the efforts they exert to gain others’ admiration or attention (Dugosh and Paulus, 2005). However, the social side of narcissists’ creativity—that is, how others’ perceptions or evaluations can influence such process—has been largely overlooked. Therefore, the present study focuses on others’ evaluation of narcissists’ creativity in the organizational context and proposes an inverted U-shape correlation. Narcissists’ tendencies toward impression management bring them better creativity evaluations. In other words, to explore the relation between narcissism and individual creativity, we proposed a new perspective that the evaluation of narcissists’ creativity can be a result of managed personal reputation.
Narcissism and personal reputation
Basically, one’s personal reputation is a summary of the evaluation and perception of outside cues, including efforts and creativity (Hogan and Shelton, 1998). In other words, “personal reputation is the link between the actor’s efforts to achieve acceptance and status and how those efforts are evaluated by observers” (Hogan and Holland, 2003, p.100). Highhouse et al. (2009) stated that personal reputation is a shared impression; the amalgamation of individual impressions. This study posits that, to obtain good personal reputation, narcissists can use impression management, a ubiquitous act of both consciously and unconsciously controlling “the images that are projected in … social interactions” by different patterns of behaviors. With the use of impression management, individuals can achieve greater career success, obtain job offers (Ellis et al., 2002) and become highly evaluated to have good organizational citizenship (Bolino, 1999; Hui et al., 2000; Rioux and Penner, 2001).
We believe that narcissists are highly motivated to carry out impression management to gain a personal reputation. The reasons may be quite straightforward. First, as suggested by Wink (1991), narcissists have high vulnerability–sensitivity, which indicates that they are defensive, anxious, emotional, and discontented. On this basis, Atlas and Them (2008) suggested that narcissists are highly sensitive to negative feedback on their performance or appearance while Morf and Rhodewalt (2001) suggested that they can be deeply hurt by criticism. In addition, narcissists are believed to have grandiosity–exhibitionism, which implies their power orientation, manipulativeness, self-dramatization, and broad interests (Wink, 1991). As such, narcissists seriously consider projecting an image of competence (Elliot and Thrash, 2001), having a very strong need for others’ approval, attention, and recognition of their valuable contributions (John and Robins, 1994). Therefore, narcissists have considerable motivation to avoid negative feedback and seek positive evaluations, and are thus driven to manage their personal reputations. This argument has considerable evidence. For instance, Gardner and Pierce (2011) found that narcissists can be sufficiently skilled at consciously managing their impression on others. John and Robins (1994) also suggested that narcissists repetitively stress on their achievements and express their vision enthusiastically to achieve a good reputation.
Thus, narcissists—by definition, those who think highly of themselves—attempt to use every means and endeavors to maintain good personal reputations. More importantly, the desired admiration drives narcissists to actively display unique positive characteristics to maintain their personal reputation. However, at the same time, too much narcissism can also damage personal reputation. When the level of narcissism increases to a certain point, its relationship with reputation changes. Atlas and Them (2008) suggested that overly narcissistic individuals may evolve and become uncaring about others’ approval or aversion. When narcissists’ need for others’ approval, attention, and recognition decline, narcissists’ conscious impression management recedes accordingly. In addition, excessive narcissistic behaviors—such as being selfish, excessively dominant, aggressive, and arrogant (e.g., Colvin et al., 1995; Morf and Rhodewalt, 2001)—can be perceived by others as incompetent or annoying (Tsang et al., 2010). Therefore, personal reputation may deteriorate due to excessive narcissism.
Moreover, compared with their peers, narcissistic individuals have a significantly higher view of themselves. Park et al. (2013) found that narcissists’ self-ratings are excessively positive and much higher than those of their friends. This discrepancy and overconfidence may cause trouble. As overconfidence increases, their accuracy of problem-solving declines, which can lead to petty mistakes and defective work (Vallone et al., 1990). As narcissists become increasingly self-aggrandizing, their work quality may deteriorate, damaging their personal reputation. As such, we believe the relationship between narcissism and personal reputation is decided by the extent of the former. We propose the following hypothesis.
Hypothesis 1: Narcissism and personal reputation have an inverted U-shaped relationship.
Narcissism, personal reputation, and creativity
The collection of perceptions of individuals from others represent personal reputation, which can be influenced by impression management and bring positive outcomes to individuals such as performance evaluations, promotions, and compensation (Ferris et al., 2003; Zinko et al., 2007; De Cremer and Sedikides, 2008). Personal reputation can also influence others’ evaluation of creativity of the focal individual. The reasons are three-fold. First, as Hogan and Shelton (1998) stated, built on individuals’ past behaviors as the best reference, personal reputation can serve as a valid assessment. Individuals with higher personal reputations are often perceived as more competent (Gioia and Sims, 1983), higher in status (Hochwarter et al., 2007), and more trustworthy (Ostrom, 2003) than their peers. To a certain extent, personal reputation is a signal of all these positive characteristics, serving as a reference when one needs to evaluate others’ creativity and especially when clear and exact information is difficult to acquire. In others’ eyes, a person with good personal reputation has a greater probability to be trustworthy and do better work (Ferris et al., 2003; Hochwarter et al., 2007; Zinko et al., 2007). Thus, one’s subjective creativity evaluation can be highly influenced by personal reputation.
Second, individuals with good personal reputations have greater autonomy than their peers, which indicates greater freedom to be creative (Humphrey et al., 2007) because good personal reputation can reduce ambiguity and the need for others’ monitoring (Zinko et al., 2007). Agency theory and current research provide evidence that individuals with high personal reputation are discretionary, because the qualities of their actions are predictable and thus, they do not need supervision (Hayward et al., 2004). Such autonomy supports individuals with more freedom and fewer constrictions, which facilitates emerging novel and creative actions.
Finally, personal reputation is also believed to bring about the halo effects that promote others’ evaluation toward the focal person in almost every aspect (Nisbett and Wilson, 1977). For one thing, good personal reputation positively relates to power (Gioia and Sims, 1983; Pfeffer, 1992; Zinko et al., 2007) and can carry out formal or informal authority that others may wish to identify. For another, personal reputation is a social factor associated with others’ evaluation such as perceived performance (Herbig and Milewicz, 1993; Ferris et al., 2003). Therefore, others have greater expectations and set them higher goals. Personal reputation has a positive relationship with perceived—regardless of actual—performance (Dossett and Greenberg, 1981; Herbig and Milewicz, 1993; Kierein and Gold, 2000), and thereby contributes to a positive effect on perceived usefulness and novelty when evaluating the individual’s creativity. Therefore, people tend to believe that those with fine personal reputations are capable of generating useful and novel ideas. Thus, we present the following hypothesis.
Hypothesis 2: Personal reputation is positively related to creativity evaluation.
On the basis of the above arguments, we propose that narcissists have inner motivations to consciously manage their impression on others and form their personal reputations, further influencing their creativity evaluation. However, narcissism that surpasses a certain level may give rise to its dark side—“too much confidence” and “care less about others’ opinion.” Revelation of this dark side of narcissism can damage the personal reputation, which also destruct the creativity evaluation. Therefore, on the whole, we propose that:
Hypothesis 3: Personal reputation mediates the inverted U-shaped relationship between narcissism and creativity.
Political skill as an important boundary condition
Defined as “the ability to effectively understand others at work, and to use such knowledge to influence others, to act in ways that enhance one’s personal and/or organizational objectives” (Ahearn et al., 2004, p. 311), political skill is found to convey honest and believable messages (Riggio et al., 1987) and has a positive relationship with trust, job satisfaction, perceived organizational support, job tension, and workplace outcomes (Treadway et al., 2004; Ferris et al., 2005). Based on social influence theory (Harris et al., 2007), individual characteristics as political skill have an effective influence on impression management strategies and the ability to understand and manage dynamic relationships with targets is critical for success. Harris et al. (2007) demonstrated that apart from the main effect, political skill also serves as a moderator between impression management strategies and work outcomes.
Based on the previous study, we propose that political skill may also moderate the efficacy of narcissists’ impression management. Specifically, high level of political skill would reduce the effect of narcissism on personal reputation and creativity evaluation for the following reasons. Political skill enables individuals to use social cues to perceive and understand others’ feelings in different situations and accordingly adjust their behaviors to gain an advantageous influence (Treadway et al., 2007). Furthermore, individuals high in political skill can become aware and mask both their conscious and unconscious behaviors regardless of their positive or negative effects on others’ evaluations. As stated by Ferris et al. (2005, p. 128), “people high in political skill not only know precisely what to do in different social situations at work, but how to do it in a manner that disguises any ulterior, self-serving motives, and appears to be sincere.” Thus, for those with high political skill, instead of the personality, their interpersonal ability determine the outcome of impression management. By contrast, individuals with low political skill can hardly restrict the narcissism effect on others’ impressions and retain the previously discussed inverted U-shape relationship between narcissism, personal reputation, and creativity. Consequently, we propose the following hypothesis.
Hypothesis 4: Political skill moderates the relationship between narcissism and personal reputation: the relationship is inverted U-shaped only when individuals’ political skill is low.
Hypothesis 5: Political skill moderates the indirect effect of narcissism on creativity evaluation via personal reputation: the inverted U-shaped relationship is significant only when individuals’ political skill is low.
Research context
Sample and procedure
We conducted a two-wave, three-source survey study to test our hypotheses. Data are collected in one high-tech industrial development zone in Southern Mainland China for the survey. Three high-technology companies are approached to distribute questionnaires to group leaders and their team members on-site, and nearly 95 R & D teams carrying out creative tasks are selected as respondents. At time 1, questionnaires were distributed to 816 employees, who are asked to rate their own narcissism, political skill, and demographics. Respondents are also randomly asked to rate one of their colleagues’ personal reputations in the same team, such that each individual gained one peer rating. Subsequently, we retrieved 633 questionnaires. After 1 week, we ask the employees’ 60 leaders to rate their creativity evaluation, finding 596 that successfully match the followers’ questionnaires.
Among the employees, 332 (55.7%) were male, mean age was 31.71 years old (SD = 8.86). Employees’ average education was 14.79 years (SD = 5.48), average job tenure was 6.25 years (SD = 6.37), and average organization tenure was 4.64 years (SD = 5.36).
Measures
Narcissism (Time 1)
Employee narcissism was measured by NPI-16 (Ames et al., 2006). The NPI-16 is a 16-item forced-choice questionnaire. Sample items are “I like to be the center of attention (1 = yes, 0 = no)” and “I think I am a special person (1 = yes, 0 = no).” The reliability coefficient for this scale was 0.70.
Political skill (Time 1)
Employee’s political skill was measured by 18 items from the Political Skill Inventory developed by Ferris et al. (2005). This inventory includes four dimensions, such as networking ability, apparent sincerity, social astuteness and interpersonal influence. Sample item includes “I am good at using my connections and networks to make things happen at work (1 = totally disagree, 5 = totally agree).” The reliability coefficient for this scale was 0.90.
Personal reputation (Time 1)
Employees’ personal reputation is reported by their peers. In each group, we randomly select one peer to rate the focal person’s personal reputation. We used the 12 items developed by Hochwarter et al. (2007). We adjust their reference from “I” to “He or she,” sample items include “He or she is regarded highly by others” and “If people want things done right, they ask him or her to do it (1 = totally disagree, 5 = totally agree).” In addition, we add one overall judgment item into our study, this item refers to “Generally speaking, he or she has a good personal reputation.” The reliability coefficient for 13 items was 0.91.
Creativity evaluation (Time 2)
Employees’ creative evaluation was rated by their leaders using the six-item scale from Madjar et al. (2011). Sample items include “This follower is a good source of highly creative ideas” and “Uses previously existing ideas or work in an appropriate new way” (1 = totally disagree, 5 = totally agree). The reliability coefficient for this scale was 0.83.
Control variables
We controlled for several variables, including employees’ gender (1 = male, 0 = female), age, education years, organization tenure, and job tenure, because these demographics have been found to influence supervisor-rated employee creativity (Ng and Feldman, 2013; Liu et al., 2016).
Analytical strategies
To segment the variance from different organizational levels (team members and team levels) in hypothesis testing, we conducted hierarchical linear models (HLM) with Stata 12.1 (Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal, 2008) to test our hypotheses. Even though our hypotheses did not contain level 2 variables, it has been suggested that we should control the multilevel effects because employees are nested in groups in our sample (Edwards and Lambert, 2007). We follow the suggestion by Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal (2008) to control these effects. To have a computational advantage by reducing nonessential multicollinearity between the linear terms and their quadratic counterparts (Aiken et al., 1991), we treat the independence variable and moderate variable in models with a grand-mean-centered process (Hofmann and Gavin, 1998). The following HLM equations were established in our analysis:
Y=+X+Z+++e (1)
Y=+X+Z++XZ +Z + e (2)
M=+X+Z++XZ +Z + e (3)
Y=+X+Z ++XZ +Z + M + e (4)
where Y was creative evaluation, C was the vector of control variables, X was narcissism, M was reputation and Z was political skill. We entered the variables into the regression equation in five steps. First, control variables (C) were entered along in the equation in step 1. Second, the main effect of narcissism (X) and political skill (Z) were entered in step 2. Third, to test the quadratic relationship in Hypothesis 1 we added the squared term (X2) in step3. Next, the interaction term of both linear interaction term (XZ) and quadratic interaction term (X2Z) were added to demonstrate the moderate effect in Hypothesis 4. In addition, to test the moderated mediation model (involving Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4), we followed the analysis approach suggested by Edwards and Lambert (2007). They suggested a general analytical framework that integrates moderated regression analysis and path analysis, which clarifies how moderator variables influence the paths that constitute the direct, indirect, and total effects of mediated models. In addition, to better analyze the conditional indirect effect size and confidence interval, we followed the Monte Carlo method suggested by Bauer et al. (2006) to test the multilevel conditional indirect confidence interval.
Results
Confirmatory factor analysis
To verify the variables measured in our research captured separate constructs, we conducted confirmatory factor analyses. Following recommendations by Little et al. (2002), we built parcels for scales with more than five items using the items-to-construct balance technique. Results showed that the four-factor model (e.g., narcissism, political skill, personal reputation, and creativity evaluation) not only fit the data fairly (χ2 [21] = 123.29, p < 0.01; CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.92, SRMR = 0.03, RMSEA = 0.09), but was also better than a few alternative models, including the three-factor model with narcissism and political skill combined (χ2 [24] = 309.14, p < 0.01; CFI = 0.87, TLI = 0.81, SRMR = 0.08, RMSEA = 0.14; Δχ2 [3] =185.85, p < 0.01), the three-factor model with narcissism and personal reputation combined (χ2 [24] = 325.58, p < 0.01; CFI = 0.86, TLI = 0.79, SRMR = 0.08, RMSEA = 0.15; Δχ2 [3] = 202.29, p < 0.01), and the three-factor model with political skill and personal reputation combined (χ2 [24] = 570.15, p < 0.01; CFI = 0.75, TLI = 0.63, SRMR = 0.08, RMSEA = 0.20; Δχ2 [3] = 446.86, p < 0.01). thereby providing support for the construct validity.
Hypothesis testing
Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlations among the studied variables of current study. Employee narcissism was significantly correlated with creativity evaluation (r = 0.21, p < 0.01). In addition, both political skill (r = 0.26, p < 0.01) and reputation (r = 0.45, p < 0.01) were significantly correlated with creativity.
Hypothesis 1 states that there is an inverted U-shaped relationship between narcissism and creativity evaluation. As shown in Table 2, after entering the squared term of narcissism, the narcissism squared reached significance on creativity evaluation (b3 = −0.84, p < 0.01, model 5) which means there is a curvilinear relationship between narcissism and creativity evaluation. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported. Finger 2 depicts this relationship between employee narcissism and creativity evaluation of his/her leader. As we can see, after the middle level of narcissism, the employee’s creativity evaluation begins to decrease. Before the culmination, the relationship between narcissism and one’s creativity evaluation was positive (Figures 2).
Hypothesis 2 suggests that the employee’s reputation is positively related to creativity evaluation. As shown in Table 1, reputation (r = 0.45, p < 0.01) was significantly correlated with creativity evaluation. In addition, Model 6 in Table 2 also demonstrates that reputation is significantly related to one’s creativity evaluation (b6 = 0.55, p < 0.01). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was supported. Hypothesis 3 involves the mediating effects of reputation on the quadratic relationship between narcissism and creativity evaluation. To demonstrate such hypothesis, two steps were conducted. First, as shown in Table 2, after entering the squared term of narcissism in Model 2 the narcissism squared reached significance on reputation (b3 = −0.96, p < 0.01). Then, we want to see whether the quadratic effects of narcissism on creativity evaluation still exist after controlling reputation. Model 5 suggests that without controlling reputation, the coefficient of narcissism quadratic term is significant (b3 = −0.84, p < 0.01). However, Model 6 demonstrates that the quadratic term of narcissism is no longer significant when reputation entered the equation (b3 = −0.32, ns). Then Hypothesis 3 was demonstrated.
Hypotheses 4 and 5 involve a moderated mediated model. To test such model, we conduct several steps to demonstrate it suggested by Edwards and Lambert (2007). First, we demonstrate that political skill moderates the effect of narcissism on both reputation and creativity evaluation. Results in Table 2 indicated that although the interaction of narcissism and political skill was not significant for reputation (b4 = −0.10, ns, model 3) and creativity evaluation (b4 = −0.26, ns, model 7), the interaction of squared narcissism and political skill reached significance for reputation (b5 = 2.91, p < 0.01, model 3) and creativity evaluation (b5 = 2.52, p < 0.01, model 7). Further, we take a simple slope analysis (Aiken et al., 1991) to probe the moderating effect of political skill. When the political skill is high (1 SD above the mean), neither narcissism nor squared narcissism was significant for reputation (narcissism: b = 0.16, ns; squared narcissism: b = −0.09, ns) and creativity (narcissism: b = 0.13, ns; squared narcissism: b = 0.04, ns) which means that narcissism does not have a significant effect on dependence variables. In contrast, when the political skill is low (1 SD below the mean), only squared narcissism has a significant effect on reputation (narcissism: b = 0.25, ns; squared narcissism: b = −2.80, p < 0.01) and creativity (narcissism: b = 0.46, p < 0.01; squared narcissism: b = −2.30, p < 0.01) indicates that the relationship between narcissism and dependent variables showed a positive trend at lower levels of narcissism and a negative trend at higher levels of narcissism. In addition, at any point of narcissism, the reputation or creativity at high-level political skill is higher than low-level political skill. Hence, the moderating effects of political skill on the relationship between quadratic terms of narcissism on both reputation and creativity evaluation have been demonstrated. Figures 3 and 4 describe the relationship between narcissism and both reputation and creativity at two levels of political skill which are plus and minus one standard deviation, respectively.

Figure 3. The moderating effect of political skill on the relationship between narcissism and personal reputation.

Figure 4. The moderating effect of political skill on the relationship between narcissism and creativity evaluation.
In the second step, we want to demonstrate that political skill can moderate the indirect effects of the quadratic term of narcissism on creativity evaluation through reputation. We conducted the moderated mediation hypotheses tests using the analysis approach suggested by Edwards and Lambert (2007). In addition, we use the Monte Carlo method (Bauer et al., 2006) to estimate the conditional indirect effect confidence interval. The method suggests that when the 95% bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) of the indirect effect (a*b) excludes zero, this denotes a statistically significant indirect effect. When political skill is low (1 SD below the mean), the indirect effect of quadratic narcissism term on creativity evaluation through reputation is significant (a*b = −0.45, CI = [−0.83, −0.07]). In contrast, the indirect effect through reputation is not significant (a*b = 0.02, CI = [−0.40, 0.44]) when political skill is high (1 SD above the mean). Thus, Hypotheses 4 and 5 are supported.
Discussion
The present study explains how narcissism influences others’ evaluations about their creativity from an impression management perspective in the organizational context. First, individuals with a moderate narcissism level are found to have a higher creativity evaluation than those with higher or lower narcissism levels. Second, others’ reported reputations mediate the relationship between narcissism and creativity. Third, narcissism influences creativity only when individuals have low political skill.
Theoretical implications
The findings present several important theoretical implications. First, this study contributes to work on the relationship between narcissism and creativity by conceptualizing and examining whether this theory can complement real organizational contexts. The effects of narcissism on creativity have been established via substantial experiments (Goncalo et al., 2010), but do not adequately reflect the complex real-world contexts. Thus, conclusions about the relationship between narcissism and creative performance may be potentially inaccurate. Chinese philosophy “ZhongYong” also suggests that excessive positives may become negatives, and only moderate “positives” may achieve the best. The current study identifies a similar boundary condition of the proposed relationship, unveiling the mystery of the complicated effects of narcissism to a greater extent and showing its inverted and curvilinear U-shape.
Second, this study also contributes to creativity literature by explicitly extending the relationship between narcissistic personality, personal reputation, and creativity. Compared with the plethora of research on personality and creativity (for a review, see Feist, 1998), this study examines the potential mechanism that a particular personality can lead to a better personal reputation and eventually influence creativity evaluation from others. Thus, we enrich our understanding of how personality can be translated into creativity in organizational settings based on an impression management perspective. We also advance our understanding of the social process of how personality can influence creativity in organizations and reveal the importance of personal reputation (Liu et al., 2016).
Third, this study contributes to narcissism literature. In psychology, narcissism is one of the most enigmatic constructs because of its contradictory processes and consequence: the confidence and charisma of narcissists can bring tremendous energy to fascinate others, whereas their aggressiveness and lack of empathy can cause conflicts (Back et al., 2010). The effect of narcissism on performance and behavior also shows mixed results (Judge et al., 2006; O’Boyle et al., 2012). Our empirical findings promote narcissism research by providing evidence that its bright side is a dominant factor to improve personal reputation, but after exceeding a threshold, the dark side of narcissism becomes dominant and reduces personal reputation. Thus, narcissism has an optimum point that shows the best effect.
Finally, this study contributes to the theory and examination of impression management. Based on fundamental assumptions that such perspective mainly focuses on performance and career success, we hypothesize and test its applicability in creativity research by illustrating that narcissists use both conscious and unconscious methods to manage their impression on others, which in turn influences their reputation and creativity. This extension can shed light on future research on how creativity unfolds socially in organizations. Specifically, as an important impression management skill, political skill is proven to have a moderating role in the relationship between narcissism and creativity.
Practical implications
Apart from theoretical contributions, this study also presents significant practical meaning to organizations. Narcissistic employees must be cautious about their double-edged personality while decision-makers must carefully consider their subjective evaluations. In the contemporary changing world, organizations need creative employees and leaders to survive, compete, and develop. In addition to the causes of creativity, its methods of evaluation also need research attention. However, creativity is often evaluated subjectively rather than by objective criteria (Sutton and Hargadon, 1996; Goncalo et al., 2010). This study reveals that, interestingly, personality traits such as narcissism can influence others’ evaluations about people. Thus, managers that need to select and adopt creative ideas without an ideal object criterion must remain vigilant on whether their decisions are contaminated by the targets’ reputations and personalities. Therefore, an objective evaluation system must be established. In addition, narcissists must also realize that such personality trait is a double-edged sword, and therefore need to control its damage to themselves and others. One possible way is to improve their political skill to better adjust their personality effect and obtain a more stable and higher reputation.
Future directions and limitations
Although this study presents the above strengths, several limitations must be noted when interpreting the findings. First, as a cross-sectional study, the causal inference is limited. Nevertheless, adopting multiple, diverse methodologies can provide certain advantages and disadvantages (Wiesenfeld et al., 2007). Future studies can adopt a longitudinal design to verify our findings.
Second, creativity is a complex construct with many potential ways of measurement; thus, this study only demonstrates a definitive link between narcissism and one way of measured creativity. Therefore, future studies can use different measuring of creativity, objective and subjective, and see whether narcissism has consistent effects.
Third, narcissism is not the only personality that influences creativity and impression management. Future research can examine other traits that can increase people’s skills in impression management, such as positive self-evaluation, self-acceptance confidence, self-esteem, or self-efficacy (Tierney and Farmer, 2004). For example, individuals with high self-monitoring may convince others to believe in their creativity by detecting different evaluation cues of creativity in different contexts (Snyder, 1974).
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Author contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.
Funding
Haiyang Liu’s work on this research was supported by the Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, under its Start-Up-Grant (SUG #022274-00001). Shengming Liu’s work on this research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (72002038).
Conflict of interest
ZL is employed by China National Petroleum Corporation.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1091770/full#supplementary-material
References
Ahearn, K. K., Ferris, G. R., Hochwarter, W. A., Douglas, C., and Ammeter, A. P. (2004). Leader political skill and team performance. J. Manag. 30, 309–327. doi: 10.1016/j.jm.2003.01
Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., and Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity and Innovation in Organizations. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School.
Ames, D. R., Rose, P., and Anderson, C. P. (2006). The NPI-16 as a short measure of narcissism. J. Res. Pers. 40, 440–450. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.03.002
Atlas, G. D., and Them, M. A. (2008). Narcissism and sensitivity to criticism: a preliminary investigation. Curr. Psychol. 27, 62–76. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9023-0
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., and Egloff, B. (2010). Why are narcissists so charming at first sight? Decoding the narcissism–popularity link at zero acquaintance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 132–145. doi: 10.1037/a0016338
Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., and Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 11, 142–163. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.142
Blair, C. A., Hoffman, B. J., and Helland, K. R. (2008). Narcissism in organizations: a multisource appraisal reflects different perspectives. Hum. Perform. 21, 254–276. doi: 10.1080/08959280802137705
Bogart, L. M., Benotsch, E. G., and Pavlovic, J. D. P. (2004). Feeling superior but threatened: the relation of narcissism to social comparison. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 26, 35–44. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp2601_4
Bolino, M. C. (1999). Citizenship and impression management: good soldiers or good actors? Acad. Manag. Rev. 24, 82–98. doi: 10.5465/amr.1999.1580442
Brunell, A. B., Gentry, W. A., Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Kuhnert, K. W., and DeMarree, K. G. (2008). Leader emergence: the case of the narcissistic leader. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1663–1676. doi: 10.1177/0146167208324101
Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Van Dijk, M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Narcissism, sexual refusal, and aggression: testing a narcissistic reactance model of sexual coercion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1027–1040. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1027
Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Campbell, S. M., and Marchisio, G. (2011). Narcissism in organizational contexts. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 21, 268–284. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2010.10.007
Chatterjee, A., and Hambrick, D. C. (2011). Executive personality, capability cues, and risk taking: how narcissistic CEOs react to their successes and stumbles. Adm. Sci. Q. 56, 202–237. doi: 10.1177/0001839211427534
Colvin, C. R., Block, J., and Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: negative implications for mental health. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 1152–1162. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.6.1152
De Cremer, D., and Sedikides, C. (2008). Reputational implications of procedural fairness for personal and relational self-esteem. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 66–75. doi: 10.1080/01973530701866557
Dossett, D. L., and Greenberg, C. I. (1981). Goal setting and performance evaluation: an attributional analysis. Acad. Manag. J. 24, 767–779. doi: 10.5465/256175
Dugosh, K. L., and Paulus, P. B. (2005). Cognitive and social comparison processes in brainstorming. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 313–320. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.05.009
Edwards, J. R., and Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychol. Methods 12, 1–22. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.1
Elliot, A. J., and Thrash, T. M. (2001). Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 13, 139–156. doi: 10.1023/A:1009057102306
Ellis, A. P., West, B. J., Ryan, A. M., and DeShon, R. P. (2002). The use of impression management tactics in structured interviews: a function of question type? J. Appl. Psychol. 87, 1200–1208. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.6.1200
Elsbach, K. D., and Kramer, R. M. (2003). Assessing creativity in Hollywood pitch meetings: evidence for a dual-process model of creativity judgments. Acad. Manag. J. 46, 283–301. doi: 10.5465/30040623
Farwell, L., and Wohlwend-Lloyd, R. (1998). Narcissistic processes: optimistic expectations, favorable self-evaluations, and self-enhancing attributions. J. Pers. 66, 65–83. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00003
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2, 290–309. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5
Ferris, G. R., Blass, F. R., Douglas, C., Kolodinsky, R. W., and Treadway, D. C. (2003). “Personal reputation in organizations,” in Organizational behavior: The state of the science. ed. J. Greenberg (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 211–246.
Ferris, G. R., Treadway, D. C., Kolodinsky, R. W., Hochwarter, W. A., Kacmar, C. J., Douglas, C., et al. (2005). Development and validation of the political skill inventory. J. Manag. 31, 126–152. doi: 10.1177/0149206304271386
Furnham, A., Hughes, D. J., and Marshall, E. (2013). Creativity, OCD, narcissism and the big five. Think. Skills Creat. 10, 91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2013.05.003
Gardner, D. G., and Pierce, J. L. (2011). A question of false self-esteem: organization-based self-esteem and narcissism in organizational contexts. J. Manag. Psychol. 26, 682–699. doi: 10.1108/02683941111181770
Gioia, D. A., and Sims, H. P. Jr. (1983). Perceptions of managerial power as a consequence of managerial behavior and reputation. J. Manag. 9, 7–24. doi: 10.1177/014920638300900103
Glad, B. (2002). Why tyrants go too far: malignant narcissism and absolute power. Polit. Psychol. 23, 1–2. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00268
Goncalo, J. A., Flynn, F. J., and Kim, S. H. (2010). Are two narcissists better than one? The link between narcissism, perceived creativity, and creative performance. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 1484–1495. doi: 10.1177/0146167210385109
Grijalva, E., and Harms, P. D. (2014). Narcissism: an integrative synthesis and dominance complementarity model. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 28, 108–127. doi: 10.5465/amp.2012.0048
Harris, K. J., Kacmar, K. M., Zivnuska, S., and Shaw, J. D. (2007). The impact of political skill on impression management effectiveness. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 278–285. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.278
Hayward, M. L., Rindova, V. P., and Pollock, T. G. (2004). Believing one’s own press: the causes and consequences of CEO celebrity. Strateg. Manag. J. 25, 637–653. doi: 10.1002/smj.405
Herbig, P., and Milewicz, J. (1993). The relationship of reputation and credibility to brand success. J. Consum. Mark. 10, 18–24. doi: 10.1108/EUM0000000002601
Highhouse, S., Brooks, M. E., and Gregarus, G. (2009). An organizational impression management perspective on the formation of corporate reputations. J. Manag. 35, 1481–1493. doi: 10.1177/0149206309348788
Hochwarter, W. A., Ferris, G. R., Zinko, R., Arnell, B., and James, M. (2007). Reputation as a moderator of political behavior-work outcomes relationships: a two-study investigation with convergent results. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 567–576. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.567
Hofmann, D. A., and Gavin, M. B. (1998). Centering decisions in hierarchical linear models: implications for research in organizations. J. Manag. 24, 623–641. doi: 10.1177/014920639802400504
Hogan, J., and Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality and job-performance relations: a socioanalytic perspective. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 100–112. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.100
Hogan, R., and Shelton, D. (1998). A socioanalytic perspective on job performance. Hum. Perform. 11, 129–144. doi: 10.1080/08959285.1998.9668028
Holtzman, N. S., and Strube, M. J. (2010). Narcissism and attractiveness. J. Res. Pers. 44, 133–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.10.004
Hui, C., Lam, S. S., and Law, K. K. (2000). Instrumental values of organizational citizenship behavior for promotion: a field quasi-experiment. J. Appl. Psychol. 85, 822–828. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.822
Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., and Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Integrating motivational, social, and contextual work design features: a meta-analytic summary and theoretical extension of the work design literature. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 1332–1356. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1332
John, O. P., and Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 206–219. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.206
Judge, T. A., LePine, J. A., and Rich, B. L. (2006). Loving yourself abundantly: relationship of the narcissistic personality to self-and other perceptions of workplace deviance, leadership, and task and contextual performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 91, 762–776. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.762
Kierein, N. M., and Gold, M. A. (2000). Pygmalion in work organizations: a meta-analysis. J. Organ. Behav. 21, 913–928. doi: 10.1002/1099-1379(200012)21:8<913::AID-JOB62>3.0.CO;2-%23
Küfner, A. C., Nestler, S., and Back, M. D. (2013). The two pathways to being an (un-) popular narcissist. J. Pers. 81, 184–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00795.x
Lebuda, I., Figura, B., and Karwowski, M. (2021). Creativity and the dark triad: a meta-analysis. J. Res. Pers. 92:104088. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104088
Li, C., Murad, M., Ashraf, S. F., and Naz, S. (2021). Impact of dark tetrad personality traits on nascent entrepreneurial behavior: the mediating role of entrepreneurial intention. Front. Bus. Res. China 15, 1–19. doi: 10.1186/s11782-021-00103-y
Li, C., Murad, M., Shahzad, F., Khan, M. A. S., and Ashraf, S. F. (2020). Dark tetrad personality traits and counterproductive work behavior among doctors in Pakistan. Int. J. Health Plann. Manag. 35, 1173–1192. doi: 10.1002/hpm.3025
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., and Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: exploring the question, weighing the merits. Struct. Equ. Model. 9, 151–173. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1
Liu, D., Jiang, K., Shalley, C. E., Keem, S., and Zhou, J. (2016). Motivational mechanisms of employee creativity: a meta-analytic examination and theoretical extension of the creativity literature. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 137, 236–263. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.08.001
Macdonald, R., and Wilson, G. (2005). Musical identities of professional jazz musicians: a focus group investigation. Psychol. Music 33, 395–417. doi: 10.1177/0305735605056151
Madjar, N., Greenberg, E., and Chen, Z. (2011). Factors for radical creativity, incremental creativity, and routine, noncreative performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 96, 730–743. doi: 10.1037/a0022416
Mao, J. Y., Quan, J., Li, Y., and Xiao, J. (2021). The differential implications of employee narcissism for radical versus incremental creativity: a self-affirmation perspective. J. Organ. Behav. 42, 933–949. doi: 10.1002/job.2540
Martinsen, Ø. L., Arnulf, J. K., Furnham, A., and Lang-Ree, O. C. (2019). Narcissism and creativity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 142, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.032
Morf, C. C., and Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: a dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol. Inq. 12, 177–196. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1
Ng, T. W., and Feldman, D. C. (2013). A meta-analysis of the relationships of age and tenure with innovation-related behaviour. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 86, 585–616. doi: 10.1111/joop.12031
Nisbett, R. E., and Wilson, T. D. (1977). The halo effect: evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 250–256. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250
O’Boyle, E. H. Jr., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., and McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: a social exchange perspective. J. Appl. Psychol. 97, 557–579. doi: 10.1037/a0025679
Ostrom, E. (2003). “Toward a behavioral theory linking trust, reciprocity, and reputation,” in Trust and Reciprocity: Interdisciplinary Lessons from Experimental Research. eds. E. Ostrom and J. Walker (New York: Sage), 9–79.
Park, S. W., Ferrero, J., Colvin, C. R., and Carney, D. R. (2013). Narcissism and negotiation: economic gain and interpersonal loss. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 35, 569–574. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2013.840633
Penney, L. M., and Spector, P. E. (2002). Narcissism and counterproductive work behavior: do bigger egos mean bigger problems? Int. J. Sel. Assess. 10, 126–134. doi: 10.1111/1468-2389.00199
Pfeffer, J. (1992). Managing with power: Politics and influence in organizations. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Rabe-Hesketh, S., and Skrondal, A. (2008). Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Stata. College Station, TX: STATA Press.
Raskin, R. N. (1980). Narcissism and creativity: are they related? Psychol. Rep. 46, 55–60. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1980.46.1.55
Resick, C. J., Whitman, D. S., Weingarden, S. M., and Hiller, N. J. (2009). The bright-side and the dark-side of CEO personality: examining core self-evaluations, narcissism, transformational leadership, and strategic influence. J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 1365–1381. doi: 10.1037/a0016238
Rhodewalt, F., and Eddings, S. K. (2002). Narcissus reflects: memory distortion in response to ego-relevant feedback among high-and low-narcissistic men. J. Res. Pers. 36, 97–116. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.2002.2342
Riggio, R. E., Tucker, J., and Throckmorton, B. (1987). Social skills and deception ability. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 13, 568–577. doi: 10.1177/0146167287134013
Rioux, S. M., and Penner, L. A. (2001). The causes of organizational citizenship behavior: a motivational analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 1306–1314. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.6.1306
Smith, M. B., and Webster, B. D. (2018). Narcissus the innovator? The relationship between grandiose narcissism, innovation, and adaptability. Personal. Individ. Differ. 121, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.018
Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 30, 526–537. doi: 10.1037/h0037039
Sutton, R. I., and Hargadon, A. (1996). Brainstorming groups in context: effectiveness in a product design firm. Adm. Sci. Q. 41:685. doi: 10.2307/2393872
Tierney, P., and Farmer, S. M. (2004). The Pygmalion process and employee creativity. J. Manag. 30, 413–432. doi: 10.1016/j.jm.2002.12.001
Treadway, D. C., Ferris, G. R., Duke, A. B., Adams, G. L., and Thatcher, J. B. (2007). The moderating role of subordinate political skill on supervisors’ impressions of subordinate ingratiation and ratings of subordinate interpersonal facilitation. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 848–855. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.848
Treadway, D. C., Hochwarter, W. A., Ferris, G. R., Kacmar, C. J., Douglas, C., Ammeter, A. P., et al. (2004). Leader political skill and employee reactions. Leadersh. Q. 15, 493–513. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.05.004
Tsang, H. W., Fong, M. W., Fung, K. M., and Corrigan, P. W. (2010). “Reducing employers’ stigma by supported employment,” in Vocational Rehabilitation and Mental Health. ed. C. Lloyd (Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd), 51–64.
Vallone, R. P., Griffin, D. W., Lin, S., and Ross, L. (1990). Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by self and others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 582–592. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.582
Wiesenfeld, B. M., Swann, W. B., Brockner, J., and Bartel, C. A. (2007). Is more fairness always preferred? Self-esteem moderates reactions to procedural justice. Acad. Manag. J. 50, 1235–1253. doi: 10.5465/amj.2007.20159922
Wink, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 590–597. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.590
Zhou, J., Wang, X. M., Bavato, D., Tasselli, S., and Wu, J. (2019). Understanding the receiving side of creativity: a multidisciplinary review and implications for management research. J. Manag. 45, 2570–2595. doi: 10.1177/0149206319827088
Keywords: narcissism, creativity evaluation, personal reputation, political skill, impression managment
Citation: Ji Y, Liu H, Liu S, Xu M and Lin Z (2023) Are narcissists more creative? Only if we believe it: How narcissism can relate to creativity. Front. Psychol. 13:1091770. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1091770
Edited by:
Majid Murad, Jiangsu University, ChinaReviewed by:
Chunhui Qi, Henan Normal University, ChinaXuan Yu, Southwest Petroleum University, China
Copyright © 2023 Ji, Liu, Liu, Xu and Lin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Haiyang Liu, ✉ aGFpeWFuZy5saXVAbnR1LmVkdS5zZw==