Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 March 2022
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Moderating Role of Information Asymmetry Between Cognitive Biases and Investment Decisions: A Mediating Effect of Risk Perception

  • 1School of Business, Macau University of Science and Technology, Macau, Macao SAR, China
  • 2Institute of Administrative Sciences, University of the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Central Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan
  • 4Faculty of Management Sciences, Riphah International University, Faisalabad, Pakistan
  • 5Department of Management Sciences, Comsats University Islamabad, Lahore, Pakistan

Behavioral Finance is an evolving field that studies how psychological factors affect decision making under uncertainty. This study seeks to find the influence of certain identified behavioral financial biases on the decision-making process of investors in developing countries. This research examines the moderating effect of Information asymmetry on the two most important and commonly used cognitive biases, namely Anchoring bias and Optimism bias and decision making and investigates whether Risk perception mediates the relationship between them or not. Quantitative research has been conducted using a structured questionnaire for data collection. After completing the pilot study, a questionnaire was designed and sent to investors via online channels. Data has been collected from 317 real estate investors. Mediation analysis has been performed using model 4 and moderation analysis by applying model 15 of Process Macros (Hayes, 2017) for the interaction effect. The study investigated that both cognitive biases have a significant positive effect on investors’ decisions and Risk perception also significantly mediates the relationship between them. Consistency with other studies suggests that Information asymmetry has a significant moderating effect. The proposed conceptual model provides insight into how investors’ decisions are influenced by behavioral biases in the real estate sector and enhances the understanding of cognitive biases in the real estate sector. This study is recommended for real estate investors and policymakers of emerging and developed countries. The current study is the first of its kind, focusing on cognitive biases on investment decisions with mediating role of Risk perception and the moderating effect of Information asymmetry.

Introduction

Standard finance is also termed traditional finance based on diverse theories and principles. It has built upon the Portfolio theory (Rubinstein, 2002), Arbitrage Principles (Modigliani and Miller, 1958) and the Efficient Market Hypothesis (EMH) (Malkiel and Fama, 1970). These theories have common assumptions that the market is efficient, investors are rational, and their decisions are not affected by cognitive errors.

The rationality was first challenged by Simon (1956), who investigated that rationality is limited and affected by external and internal factors. After the energy crisis of the 1970s, behavioral finance transpires as a new concept with a combination of psychological aspects and financial decisions (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), which established a milestone of individual behavior in finance. They argued that people prefer gains and losses differently as investors’ decisions are based on perceived gains instead of losses. Expected utility theory by Fishburn (1988) stated that people construct decisions through rational resources since they possess unlimited rationality and an approach to useful and perfect information that is why they can make rational decisions (Caplin and Leahy, 2001).

The paradigm of behavioral finance is shifted from traditional finance. The origin of behavioral finance took place by the idea of Tversky and Kahneman (1979), prospect theory, led by the expected utility theory. If investors know about the market information and behavior, their irrational behavior still exists because investors fear the future in terms of loss. According to the classical financial paradigm, researchers have focused on behavioral finance reactions regarding investors’ profit fluctuations in explaining the investor’s decisions. Behavioral finance focuses on different beliefs that encourage people to act unreasonably. Many people have different mental biases that may change into the biggest hurdle to grow their money or wealth.

The cognitive biases and real estate investment has scarcely been studied. Besides, none of the prior studies known to the researcher has been conducted in the context of developing countries. The real estate sector is integral component of the economy in all countries. Several technological developments have changed the economy and promoted growth throughout the world. The real estate sector and its business models face transformations caused by fundamental changes in technology, economy, and society (Pfnür and Wagner, 2020). Therefore, the complexity and diversity of real estate markets and their interdependence with the economy require further investigation (Pandey and Jessica, 2018). In addition, real estate investments are considered a complex human cognitive process in which decisions are made about potentially uncertain future returns. The real estate sector plays a vital role in ensuring a sustainable economy. Current political and economic matters, such as high inflation rates, cause real estate prices to rise sharply (Zain-ul-Abdin et al., 2019).

Anchoring is one of the most robust cognitive biases with implications in all decision-making processes (Furnham and Boo, 2011). When people rely on anchor value or explore anchor information, bias occurs (Jung and Young, 2019). In Anchoring bias, investors depend on a piece of initial information (Shin and Park, 2018). An investor starts with the initial approximation and then makes judgments based on additional information (Sharot, 2011).

Optimism bias enhances the positivity in investors’ minds, which is why they perceive the event more favorably (Anderson and Galinsky, 2006; Englmaier, 2010). In literature, Optimism bias is directly linked with the overreaction of information (Trevelyan, 2008). This overreaction enhances the investor’s confidence and becomes optimistic (Barros and Di Miceli da Silveira, 2007; Gudmundsson and Lechner, 2013).

Risk perception is the essential component of decision-making (Singh and Bhowal, 2010). Risk is a complex and essential factor in understanding and investigating. Speculators with an abnormal state of budgetary proficiency favor values while financial specialists with low money-related education lean toward bank stores. Risk perceptions are beliefs about potential harm/profit or the possibility of a loss. It is a subjective judgment that people make about the characteristics and severity. The degree of risk associated with a given behavior is generally considered to represent the likelihood and consequences of harmful effects that result from that behavior. Risk perception is a highly personal process of decision making, based on an individual’s frame of reference developed over a lifetime, among many other factors (Robinson and Marino, 2015).

The main objective of this study is to check the effects of cognitive biases on decision making and via the mediating role of Risk perception. It also strives to explore the moderating role of Information asymmetry for investors’ awareness about the hazards of Information asymmetry when making investment decisions.

Theoretical Background

Modern finance has been dismissing the concept of the EMH introduced by Fama (1970), which prompts the advancement of behavioral finance because psychology is becoming deciphered as a key component (Chaffai and Medhioub, 2014). Rubinstein (2002) stated that people construct decisions through rational resources. Firstly, rationality was challenged by Simon (1956), who investigated that rationality is limited and also affected by some other external and internal factors.

Prospect Theory

Within prospect theory, Kahneman and Tversky (1979) described that when the outcome of a decision is uncertain, an investor will focus on making a profit instead of reducing losses. This theory also states that when two choices are available in the securities market (in the shape of profit and losses), the investor’s preference is based on the perceived returns instead of losses. This theory is related to investors’ judgments called cognitive biases and their affect on investment decisions.

Hypotheses Development

Optimism Bias and Investment Decision Making

A huge literature underlined the numerous biases of investors’ behavior such as Optimism bias, Anchoring bias, availability bias, and overconfidence bias to explain economic decisions (Zacharakis and Shepherd, 2001). Investors believe in their mental conditions and shortcuts when making decisions (Maguire and Albright, 2005). Scheier and Carver (1985) have revealed optimism as derived positive beliefs and expectations about upcoming future incidents. If a person has a high level of optimism, he can expect higher subjective well-being in his good or bad times. The optimism comes face-to-face when he underestimates the negative happenings and will consist in anticipation of great results (Limongi et al., 2019). Another study also revealed that people with higher optimistic feelings are interested in more risky investments because they are not afraid of failing (Anderson and Galinsky, 2006).

From the above discussion, a hypothesis is generated:

H1: Optimism bias has a significant positive effect on investment decisions.

Anchoring Bias and Investment Decision Making

People move to depend on their judgments when they just utilize the available information (Costa et al., 2017). Tversky and Kahneman primarily initiated this Anchoring bias in 1974. Anchoring bias is a cognitive bias that occurs when prices are fixed (anchored) by recent and contemporary observations. Investors mostly use the previous price as a reference for making the regulations (Kahneman and Riepe, 1998; Costa et al., 2017; Lieder et al., 2018). Meanwhile, Anchoring bias is a situational bias that varies as per the situation at the time of decision making (Kartini and Nahda, 2021). Anchoring bias happens when individuals depend on previous data they find when deciding (de Wilde et al., 2018; Lieder et al., 2018). The prior studies have explored that the Anchoring effect positively influences investors’ decision-making process (Chaudhary, 2013). An empirical study investigated that Anchoring bias significantly affects financial decision-making (Costa et al., 2017) and most commonly used bias in managerial and investment matters (Gaudet et al., 1998; Shin and Park, 2018). In real estate, anchoring was firstly shown by Northcraft and Neale (1987). The main factor influencing investors in Kenya is Anchoring bias (Waweru et al., 2014). In consequence of the discussion, a hypothesis is generated as:

H2: Anchoring bias has a significant positive effect on investment decisions.

Risk Perception and Decision Making

Risk can be minimized by understanding the investor’s perception (Odean, 1998). However, traditionally, investors’ decisions are based on irrational behavior owing to different biases. For example, individual investors often focus more on potential negative outcomes than positive ones. Behavioral finance uses perceptions from other science and business areas to analyze investors’ choices. Some human behavior studies consider how investors make financing decisions, while neurologists have researched how investors’ mindsets can affect their financial decisions (Akhtar et al., 2020).

Moreover, behavior examined how investors make and act upon their decisions. Risk perception appears to be reflected in subjective behaviors influenced by other factors (Weber et al., 2004). Thus, Risk perception plays a fundamental role in investors’ behavior. When an individual makes a judgment about a financial instrument, the judgment process involves behavioral risk indicators and financial risk measures. In addition, Risk perception affects behavioral finance.

Moreover, Forlani and Mullins (2000) examined perceived risk and found significant links with behavioral biases. In previous researches, Risk perception is used as an intervening variable. Houghton et al. (2000) also investigated that Risk perception mediated the relationship between cognitive biases and the decision to start a venture. They also proposed that other biases need to be considered for future research. Nguyen and Rozsa (2019) investigated that Risk perception and risk tolerance significantly affect investment decision-making.

Investment involves the allocation of money with the hope of gaining returns and benefits in the future. These returns are based on whether the investor’s behavior is rational or irrational and are associated with risk. Investors face difficulties regarding how much they can invest in the stock market. Traditional finance clearly explains that investors should not make decisions based on emotion and behaviors. Traditional finance accepts that an investor is a rational person who can fairly process all details and investors are expected to be more rational and risk aversion.

H3: Risk perception mediates the effect of Optimism bias on investment decisions.

H4: Risk perception mediates the effect of Anchoring bias on investment decisions.

Moderating Role of Information Asymmetry

In most manifestations in real estate market, asymmetric information is the norm (Lützkendorf and Speer, 2005). For more than two decades, the investigation of asymmetric information has been the salient economic theory. Asymmetric information is a very common feature of market interaction (Bukhari et al., 2021). When one party inside the market approaches more information than the other party, Information asymmetry occurs (Lützkendorf and Speer, 2005). In the context of asymmetric information, one party convinces the other party upon the quality and price of any product. The previous literature investigated asymmetric information has been conducted between principal and agent as principal is investor and manager is an agent. Literature has also established that Information asymmetry affects real investment in various ways and it gives satisfaction to investors (Rao et al., 2021).

Harris and Raviv (2010) also examined why managers (one party) had taken the informational advantage over shareholders (opposite part). To take the exclusive benefits, they retain the information that helps them pursue favorable investment decisions (1991). Property related data is not accessible for forthcoming the buyer’s/tenant’s informational asymmetries. Hence information asymmetries arise in real estate markets, as in other industries. The party offering a service or product (the agent) holds or can process information better than the party who requires it (Stiglitz, 2000). The decision-makers need a certain amount of financial decision-making information (Chewning and Harrell, 1990).

An investigation has been made in prior studies related to providing quality information and accurate data for investment decision-making. Ling et al. (2016) stated that the lack of supply of quality data on the commercial level derives the extremity of real estate unpredictability.

H5a: Information asymmetry moderates the effect of Optimism bias on investment decisions.

H5b: Information asymmetry moderates the effect of Anchoring bias on investment decisions.

An empirical study investigated that information plays a crucial role in financial institutions’ planning and decision-making. The information goes beyond subject domain boundaries and shows awareness of psycho-social barriers in acquisition and handling. Another study also investigated the importance of information in financial and strategic decision making and concluded that complete information leads to better decision making (Citroen, 2011). Information symmetry leads to the inauspicious selection of alternatives and market failure (Ahmad et al., 2021). These both are major effects of Information asymmetry. Adverse selection is a term utilized in financial aspects that alludes to a cycle wherein undesired outcomes happen when purchasers and dealers approach unique/insufficient data (Cohen and Siegelman, 2010; Boone, 2020). Market failure is when an individual will face challenges because the cost that could acquire will not be felt by the gathering facing the challenge (Jackson and Jabbie, 2019). The other consequence of asymmetric information is the agency problem between principal and agent. This phenomenon is also called the agency dilemma (Ross, 1973; Chod and Lyandres, 2020). The agency problem is created when there is a conflict between an agent and principal (Liao et al., 2009). One of the reasons for this conflict is asymmetric information. Empirical research investigated that the agency problem disturbs the self-interest of both parties (Chod and Lyandres, 2020).

H6: Information asymmetry moderates the effect of Risk perception on investment decision making.

In the above Figure 1, optimism and Anchoring bias are the predictors, which are effecting to investment decisions via the Risk perception (mediator). Information asymmetry works as a moderating variable between Risk perception and decision making.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual framework.

Methods

The methodological part of this exploration is to measure the effect of cognitive biases on investment decision-making and explore the mediating role of Risk perception. Moreover, the methodology part also examines the moderating effect of Information asymmetry.

Target Population and Sample Size

One of the major and central aims of the research was to put attention on real investors. For this purpose, the target population of this study was the investors of the real estate sector. The study comprises only primary data, which will be assembled by utilizing the survey through the questionnaire. In the present research, the questionnaire included 34 items for measuring the variables. According to Hair et al. (2017) recommendations, the admissible sample size is the ratio between 5 and 10 observations per estimable criterion. According to Hair et al. (2017), it has been explained that the more admissible sample size should have the 10:1 ratio. Here, the lower limit is selected for creating the number of respondents.

Data Collection

A pilot study has been conducted for translation of the questionnaire. The pilot study aims to evaluate the feasibility of the questionnaire on a trial basis (Thabane et al., 2010). During a pilot study, the instrument is given to 30 literate financial investors. These 30 respondents have confirmed the instrument’s content validity and face validity. In the prescribed description of the given sample (Table 1), data collection is done using a structured questionnaire sent to 340 respondents, 13 questionnaire are discarded due to incomplete information, 10 questionnaires are not received so the final 317 are accurate and complete. Therefore, the sample size is 317 for the analysis. The net response rate is 93.2%. A convenient sampling method is used to select respondents to provide the highest degree of responses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Break down of sample size.

Measurement

There are two cognitive variables consists of Optimism bias and Anchoring bias. Optimism bias has nine items adopted from Mishra et al. (2008) (“In uncertain times, I usually expect the best” is a sample item). Anchoring bias has two items adopted from Waweru et al. (2008). The sample item is “My investment is affected by my recent investment experiences.” The Risk perception scale was measured using the four items proposed by Weber et al. (2004) and a sample item is “I invest 10% of my annual income in a moderate growth mutual fund.” The Information asymmetry has eight items adopted from Mahaney and Lederer (2011), “My investment agent describes all the issues to me openly” is a sample item. The dependent variable was investment decision making consisting of ten items adopted from Mayfield et al. (2008) “I decide to invest every year” is a sample item of decision making.

Reliability Analysis

Cronbach’s alpha is a measurable action of internal equilibrium and consistency known as scale reliability. It is also very useful to determine whether the scale we are using is fit for purpose or not (Taber, 2018). The minimum permissible value for Cronbach’s alpha is 0.70 (Taber, 2018), and if the outcome value approaches below the accepted value, internal consistency is low. The results indicate that Cronbach’s alpha value of all the factors is greater than 0.70 (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reliability statistics.

Demographic Variables

Table 3, the frequency of age is given below as there are 4.2% respondents with the age of less than 20 years, 5.7% respondents having age of 21–30 years, 27.4% respondents lies between 31 and 40 years, 42.9% respondents exist between 41 and 50 years, and 19.8% respondents having age of more than 50 years. It analyzes 41–50 years of investors in the real estate market.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics.

The frequency of gender is given below as there are 14.2% female respondents and 85.8% male respondents, which analyzes that males are more engaged in real estate investment the females. This analysis is consistent with another research in Sweden (Pauli et al., 2014) that males are more engaged in the real estate business than females.

The frequency of education is given below as there are 17.0% respondents having matriculation, 24.1% respondents are intermediate, 31.1% respondents with the education of bachelors, 19.3% respondents with the education of masters, and 8.5% respondents with the education of post-graduates. It analyzes that most investors in developing countries are bachelor degree holders and very few postgraduates.

The frequency of frequency is given below as there are 14.2% respondents with financial experience of less than 1 year, 21.7% respondents with financial experience of 1–5 years, 51.9% respondents with financial experience 6–10 years, and 12.3% respondents with the financial experience of more than 10 years.

Statistical Technique

PROCESS is known as a calculating tool available for SPSS and SAS. Its major purpose is to clarify the execution of mediation, moderation, and conditional process accompanied by perceived variables (Hayes, 2017).

Table 4, the effect of Anchoring bias has been analyzed on the mediator who is Risk perception. The table indicates that the effect of Anchoring bias on Risk perception is positively significant. Consequently, Anchoring bias is a significant predictor of Risk perception, β = 0.687, SE = 0.0410, 95% CI, (0.7335–0.9571), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05 and Risk perception is the significant predictor of investment decision making, β = 0.7835, SE = 0.0791, 95% CI, (0.2737–0.5316), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05. These results support the mediation analysis. It represents the effect of independent variable Anchoring bias on investment decision-making without considering mediator Risk perception.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effect of cognitive biases (Anchoring bias and Optimism bias) on investment decision making (DM) via mediating role of Risk perception.

Anchoring bias is a significant predictor of investment decision making after controlling the mediator Risk perception, β = 0.4621, SE = 0.0565, 95% CI, (0.4498–0.7522), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05. The effect of Optimism bias has been analyzed on the mediator who is Risk perception. The table indicates that the effect of Optimism bias on Risk perception is positively significant and Optimism bias is a significant predictor of Risk perception, β = 0.8453, SE = 0.0567, 95% CI, (0.7335–0.9571), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05 and Risk perception is the significant predictor of investment decision making, β = 0.4026, SE = 0.0654, 95% CI, (0.2737–0.5316), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05. These results support the mediation analysis. The above table represents the effect of independent variable Optimism bias on investment decision making without the mediator Risk perception. Optimism bias is a significant predictor of investment decision making via Risk perception, β = 0.9414, SE = 0.0577, 95% CI, (0.8276–1.0551), p = 0.0000 which is p < 0.05. Moreover, results indicated that there is a partial mediation exist against the effect of cognitive biases on decision making via mediating role of Risk perception as all paths are significant.

Regression Analysis

Specifically, regression analysis described how dependent value Y fluctuates when any independent variable decreases or increases by holding the independent variable constant. The highest value of R is desirable (Cohen et al., 2013).

The below (Table 5) indicates a 54.2% direct association between Anchoring bias and decision making. Optimism bias and decision making are 75.2% associated. Anchoring bias is positively associated with the mediator called Risk perception, which is 75.9%. The mediator Risk perception is positively 72.3% associated with investment decision making. The relationship of Anchoring bias through Risk perception with investment decision making is 72.3% and the association of Optimism bias through Risk perception with investment decision making is 79.6%. The value of R2 indicates the proportion of variance in the dependent variable predicted from the independent variable. The sig values of the coefficients indicate whether these relationships are statistically significant. The significant value, denoted as sig in the above table, indicates the acceptance of the entire hypothesis. A value less than 0.05 (95% CI) indicates a significant relationship between concerned variables in the selected model. The above table indicates that the entire hypothesis shows 0.000 values, which means the relationship is statistically significant.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Model summary.

Mediated Moderation Effect of Information Asymmetry

In this research, it has been proposed that Information asymmetry moderates the effect of Anchoring biases on investment decision-making and moderates the effect of mediator, Risk perception on investment decision making. The moderation effect has been tested with the (Model 15) of PROCESS macro (Preacher and Hayes, 2008; Hayes, 2017). In the given Table 6, it has been indicated that the moderator Information asymmetry significantly moderates the direct effect of Anchoring bias on investment decision making. The effect of Anchoring bias on investment decision making is significantly moderated by Information asymmetry (coeff = 0.1718, BootSE = 0.1112, 95% CI, 0.3919–0.0474). The effect of Anchoring bias on Risk perception has a significant effect. The effect of Risk perception on investment decision making is also moderated by Information asymmetry (coeff = 0.1265, BootSE = 0.1169, 95% CI, 0.1039–0.3570).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Mediated moderation effect of Information asymmetry.

The second cognitive bias in this research is Optimism bias. The effect of Optimism bias on investment decision making is also significantly moderated by Information asymmetry (coeff = 0.0576, BootSE = 0.1149, 95% CI, 0.1054–0.2847). The effect Risk perception on investment decision making is significantly moderated by the moderator Information asymmetry (coeff = 0.0896, BootSE = 0.0989, 95% CI, 0.2842–0.1690). Hayes and Scharkow (2013) also reported a method for evaluating mediation moderation by an index value, BootLLCI, and BootULCI. The zero value did not fall within the upper or lower limit of the class interval and the index value is also less than 5%. This indicates that the model is perfectly mediation moderation (Table 7).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Index of mediation moderation.

Two-Way Interaction Effect

The management research is fully crowded with the theories that describe that the effect of a causal variable on outcome variable depends on one another variable called moderator (Dawson, 2014).

The above Figure 2 demonstrates that Anchoring bias and investment decision making is always positive. Still, it is far more for low Information asymmetry and far low when Information asymmetry is high. If there is high Information asymmetry, then the slope is less steep, which depicts that the moderator changes the association of Anchoring bias and decision making. Consequently, Information asymmetry reduces the effect of Anchoring bias on investment decision making.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Interaction effect between Anchoring and Decision Making.

The above Figure 3 demonstrates that Risk perception and investment decision-making are always positive. Still, it is far more for when there is low Information asymmetry and far low when Information asymmetry is high. If there is high Information asymmetry, then the slope is less steep, indicating that the moderator changes the association of Risk perception and decision-making. Consequently, Information asymmetry reduces the effect of Risk perception on investment decision making.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction effect between Risk Perception and Decision Making.

The above Figure 4 demonstrates Optimism bias and investment decision making is always positive but far more for when there is low Information asymmetry and far low when Information asymmetry is high. If there is high Information asymmetry, then the slope is less steep, which depicts that the moderator changes the association of Optimism bias and decision making.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction effect between Optimism Bias and Decision Making.

Discussion

This study investigates the decision-making perception of real estate investors. In behavioral finance, investment decisions are complicated activities for the investors. Generally, investors face many complex financial situations of uncertainty when making decisions. This uncertainty affects investors’ perception and leads to the worst decision making. Still, in this highly stimulating property market industry, it is necessary to take advantage of all opportunities and employ the resources of information. In this complex situation, cognitive biases affect investors’ decisions and revamp their performance of investors.

The Anchoring bias has a significant positive effect on investment (Figure 2). This finding is similar to Simmons’s (1999) and Evans (2002) findings. They investigated that investors utilize current prices before making investment decisions and presume that they are accurate. Investors use previous prices to make investment decisions (Waweru et al., 2014). Optimism bias has a positive effect on investment decisions (Figure 3). This finding is consistent with the study examining that investor’s decisions are significantly influenced by Optimism bias (Riaz and Iqbal, 2015; Jehiel, 2018; Limongi Concetto and Ravazzolo, 2019).

Risk perception mediates the effect of cognitive biases on investment decisions. The finding is similar to Greene (1998) and Cassar and Friedman (2009), who found that Risk perception affects investment decisions. Information asymmetry is a problem of prejudiced information distribution between agent and principal to take discriminatory advantage. This inequitable distribution of information enhances uncertainty (Newell and MacFarlane, 2006), ultimately leading to imprecise investment decisions. Information asymmetry significantly moderates the effect of cognitive biases on investment decisions (Chewning and Harrell, 1990). The study’s findings confirm that asymmetrical information negatively influences investment decision making (Gallimore et al., 2000) because investors need some sort of relatable information before investing (Chewning and Harrell, 1990) (Figure 4).

The results of the study suggested that the investment strategy relying on fast and frugal rules that would give better returns to investors. Based on our findings, researchers would like to suggest that investors should not rely on market information as Fama (1970) reported, but conduct a proper analysis on the behavior of the investors, develop quantitative investment measures and establish investment objectives and constraints.

Conclusion and Implications

This study aimed to investigate the effects of cognitive biases on decision-making and explore the mediating effect of Risk perception and the moderating role of Information asymmetry on the effect of cognitive biases on investment decisions.

The results explored that cognitive biases positively affect investment decision making. In this study, two major cognitive biases are taken, i.e., Anchoring bias and Optimism bias positively influence decision making. This study indicates that Risk perception mediates cognitive bias on investment decision-making in the investment industry. It means that if an investor has strong beliefs in his ability to cope with situations when making investment decisions, cognitive biases positively affect decision-making.

In this study, we combine the theoretical fields of cognitive psychology and perception of risk along with the moderating effect of Information asymmetry on investment decision. Thus, the study makes an academic and practical implications by providing further insights into the cognitive biases and decision-making via the mediating role of Risk perception and the moderating effect of Information asymmetry. This study would be academically contribute in the curriculum of business studies especially the new horizons for the philosophy degree and also the behaviors of the investors. This study provides several practical implications for the government and real estate investors. For instance, the government should conduct seminars and workshops on the financial securities knowledge and behavior for real estate investors to reduce biases in their investment decisions, thereby helping the real estate market prosper. In the real estate context, awareness should be made for the brokerage houses and investors regarding the cognitive biases and as well as Risk perception.

This study contributes to the existing literature on cognitive biases and investment decision making. It is also helpful in the educational sector for business management studies, i.e., MBA students. This research will also provide insights into how situational (Anchoring) and emotional bias (Optimism) work together. This study enhances the skill set of financial advisors and investors themselves by a better understanding of investor’s goals. Furthermore, investors’ decisions are very important to set the market trends and enhance the economy. The real estate market is risky, but it is an investment-intensive sector that affects the economy and enhances other auxiliary activities such as mortgage enterprises. Therefore it is necessary to make rational and efficient decisions at investment. This study’s major role is to inform the investors about the hazards of Information asymmetry when making decisions. Generally, investors are unconscious of biases and thorough financial theories.

Hence investors behave irrationally by taking into account all prior information called Anchoring bias and optimism to maximize their profits or returns. All investment decisions are based on accurate information and past data that is perfect for decision making. In behavioral finance, biases are an individual’s judgments about specific things that they like and dislike, with such thoughts varying from person to person. Some factors that influence investors’ choices and behavior exist in investors’ thoughts, beliefs, and perceptions as biases. Consequently, this study helps the investors enforce the biases accurately and enhance the business sector. Moreover, this study helps to reduce the problem of asymmetrical information and enhances the investor’s efficiency. This exploration can be an important expansion to the literature in behavioral finance.

Limitations and Future Recommendations

This study explores the impact of major cognitive biases on investment decisions in the specific context of developing countries which manifests that the sample size is limited. Future research should be at a huge geographical level for more generalized results. In this study, only investors are taken as population, but stock exchange investors should be taken as a unit of analysis in future research. In addition, the study can be done on the investors of the commodity market. The findings of this study is limited to the moderator, but in future, research can be done by investigating the moderating role of financial literacy (Nauman Sadiq and Asad Azad Khan, 2019; Sabir et al., 2019), Gender and Personality Traits (Gul, 1984) and also personality types. The study can also be done to the emotional intelligence of investors as an independent variable. This study can explore if other cognitive biases (i.e., overconfidence bias, herding bias, availability bias, and representative bias) are taken into the research. This study will explore more if it conducts on long-term and short-term investment intentions. Furthermore, this study used the convenience sampling method, but other sampling techniques can be used in future.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Riphah International University, Management Sciences Department. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MZ identified the problem area. MN did work on the contribution and conceptual framework. RF did work on the data collection. MI has done work on the contribution and limitation of the study. All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahmad, W., Kim, W. G., Choi, H. M., and Haq, J. U. (2021). Modeling behavioral intention to use travel reservation apps: a cross-cultural examination between US and China. J. Retailing Consum. Serv. 63:102689.

Google Scholar

Akhtar, N., Akhtar, M. N., Usman, M., Ali, M., and Siddiqi, U. I. (2020). COVID-19 restrictions and consumers’ psychological reactance toward offline shopping freedom restoration. Serv. Indust. J. 40, 891–913.

Google Scholar

Anderson, C., and Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 36, 511–536. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de C Barros, L. A. B., and Di Miceli da Silveira, A. (2007). Overconfidence, Managerial Optimism and the Determinants of Capital Structure (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 953273). Available online at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=953273 (accessed February 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Boone, J. (2020). Pricing Above Value: Selling to an Adverse Selection Market (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 3696360). Available online at: https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3696360 (accessed February 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Bukhari, S. A. H., Ul Haq, J., Ishfaq, M., and Ali, A. (2021). Females are more rational! how consumer expertise moderates attitude toward advertisements. J. Promot. Manage. 1–20. doi: 10.1080/10496491.2021.1989538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplin, A., and Leahy, J. (2001). Psychological expected utility theory and anticipatory feelings. Q. J. Econ. 116, 55–79. doi: 10.1162/003355301556347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassar, G., and Friedman, H. (2009). Does self-efficacy affect entrepreneurial investment? Strateg. Entrep. J. 3, 241–260. doi: 10.1002/sej.73

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaffai, M., and Medhioub, I. (2014). Behavioral finance: an empirical study of the tunisian stock market. Int. J. Econ. Financ. Issues 4, 527–538.

Google Scholar

Chaudhary, A. K. (2013). Impact of behavioral finance in investment decisions and strategies–a fresh approach. Int. J. Manage. Res. Bus. Strategy 2, 85–92.

Google Scholar

Chewning, E. G., and Harrell, A. M. (1990). The effect of information load on decision makers’ cue utilization levels and decision quality in a financial distress decision task. Account. Organ. Soc. 15, 527–542. doi: 10.1016/0361-3682(90)90033-Q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chod, J., and Lyandres, E. (2020). A Theory of ICOs: Diversification, Agency, and Information Asymmetry (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 3159528). Available online at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3159528 (accessed February 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Citroen, C. L. (2011). The role of information in strategic decision-making. Int. J. Inform. Manage. 31, 493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2011.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A., and Siegelman, P. (2010). Testing for adverse selection in insurance markets. J. Risk Insur. 77, 39–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6975.2009.01337.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Turner, R. B., Marsland, A. L., Casselbrant, M. L., Li-Korotky, H. S., et al. (2013). Childhood socioeconomic status, telomere length, and susceptibility to upper respiratory infection. Brain Behav. Immunity 34, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, D. F., de Melo Carvalho, F., de Melo Moreira, B. C., and do Prado, J. W. (2017). Bibliometric analysis on the association between behavioral finance and decision making with cognitive biases such as overconfidence, anchoring effect and confirmation bias. Scientometrics 111, 1775–1799. doi: 10.1007/s11192-017-2371-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, J. (2014). Moderation in management research: what, why, when, and how. J. Bus. Psychol. 29, 1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10869-013-9308-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wilde, T. R. W., Ten Velden, F. S., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2018). The anchoring-bias in groups. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 76, 116–126. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Englmaier, F. (2010). Managerial optimism and investment choice. Manage. Decis. Econ. 31, 303–310. doi: 10.1002/mde.1498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. (2002). Taking control of their lives? Agency in young adult transitions in England and the New Germany. J. Youth Stud. 5, 245–269. doi: 10.1080/1367626022000005965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: a review of theory and empirical work. J. Finance 25, 383–417. doi: 10.2307/2325486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishburn, P. C. (1988). Expected utility: an anniversary and a new era. J. Risk Uncertain. 1, 267–283.

Google Scholar

Forlani, D., and Mullins, J. W. (2000). Perceived risks and choices in entrepreneurs’ new venture decisions. J. Bus. Ventur. 15, 305–322.

Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of the anchoring effect. J. Soc. Econ. 40, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallimore, P., Hansz, J. A., and Gray, A. (2000). Decision making in small property companies. J. Prop. Invest. Finance 18, 602–612. doi: 10.1108/14635780010357569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudet, G., Lasserre, P., and Van Long, N. (1998). Real investment decisions under adjustment costs and asymmetric information. J. Econ. Dyn. Control 23, 71–95. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1889(97)00107-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, W. H. (1998). Gender economics courses in liberal arts colleges: further results. J. Econ. Educ. 29, 291–300. doi: 10.2307/1182919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gudmundsson, S. V., and Lechner, C. (2013). Cognitive biases, organization, and entrepreneurial firm survival. Eur. Manage. J. 31, 278–294. doi: 10.1016/j.emj.2013.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gul, F. A. (1984). The joint and moderating role of personality and cognitive style on decision making. Account. Rev. 59, 264–277.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Matthews, L. M., Matthews, R. L., and Sarstedt, M. (2017). PLS-SEM or CB-SEM: updated guidelines on which method to use. Int. J. Multivar. Data Anal. 1, 107–123.

Google Scholar

Harris, M., and Raviv, A. (2010). Control of corporate decisions: shareholders vs. management. Rev. Financ. Stud. 23, 4115–4147.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, Second Edition: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., and Scharkow, M. (2013). The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: does method really matter? Psychol. Sci. 24, 1918–1927. doi: 10.1177/0956797613480187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houghton, S. M., Simon, M., Aquino, K., and Goldberg, C. B. (2000). No safety in numbers. Group Organ. Manage. 25, 325–353. doi: 10.1177/1059601100254002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, E. A., and Jabbie, M. (2019). Understanding Market Failure in the Developing Country Context. Berlin: Springer Nature, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Jehiel, P. (2018). Investment strategy and selection bias: an equilibrium perspective on overoptimism. Am. Econ. Rev. 108, 1582–1597. doi: 10.1257/aer.20161696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, H., and Young, M. J. (2019). Motivated to confront: how experiencing anger affects anchoring bias. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 32, 493–503. doi: 10.1002/bdm.2126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Riepe, M. W. (1998). Aspects of investor psychology. J. Portf. Manage. 24, 52–65. doi: 10.3905/jpm.1998.409643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). On the interpretation of intuitive probability: a reply to Jonathan Cohen. Cognition 7, 409–411. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(79)90024-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kartini, K., and Nahda, K. (2021). Behavioral biases on investment decision: a case study in Indonesia. J. Asian Finance Econ. Bus. 8, 1231–1240. doi: 10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no3.1231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, H.-H., Chen, T.-K., and Lu, C.-W. (2009). Bank credit risk and structural credit models: agency and information asymmetry perspectives. J. Bank. Finance 33, 1520–1530. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2009.02.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieder, F., Griffiths, T. L., Huys, Q. J. M., and Goodman, N. D. (2018). The anchoring bias reflects rational use of cognitive resources. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 322–349. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1286-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Limongi Concetto, C., and Ravazzolo, F. (2019). Optimism in financial markets: stock market returns and investor sentiments. J. Risk Financ. Manage. 12:85. doi: 10.3390/jrfm12020085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Limongi, T., Canta, M., Racca, L., Ancona, A., Tritta, S., Vighetto, V., et al. (2019). Improving dispersal of therapeutic nanoparticles in the human body. Nanomedicine 14, 797–801. doi: 10.2217/nnm-2019-0070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ling, L., Zhou, X., Liang, Q., Song, P., and Zeng, H. (2016). Political connections, overinvestments and firm performance: evidence from Chinese listed real estate firms. Finance Res. Lett. 18, 328–333.

Google Scholar

Lützkendorf, T., and Speer, T. M. (2005). Alleviating asymmetric information in property markets: building performance and product quality as signals for consumers. Build. Res. Inform. 33, 182–195. doi: 10.1080/0961321042000323815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, L. A., and Albright, E. A. (2005). Can behavioral decision theory explain risk-averse fire management decisions? For. Ecol. Manage. 211, 47–58. doi: 10.1016/j.foreco.2005.01.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahaney, R. C., and Lederer, A. L. (2011). An agency theory explanation of project success. J. Comput. Inform. Syst. 51, 102–113. doi: 10.1080/08874417.2011.11645506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malkiel, B. G., and Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: a review of theory and empirical work*. J. Finance 25, 383–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6261.1970.tb00518.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayfield, C., Perdue, G., and Wooten, K. (2008). Investment management and personality type. Financ. Serv. Rev. 17, 219–236.

Google Scholar

Mishra, H., Shiv, B., and Nayakankuppam, D. (2008). The blissful ignorance effect: pre- versus post-action effects on outcome expectancies arising from precise and vague information. J. Consum. Res. 35, 573–585. doi: 10.1086/591104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modigliani, F., and Miller, M. H. (1958). The cost of capital, corporation finance and the theory of investment. Am. Econ. Rev. 48, 261–297.

Google Scholar

Nauman Sadiq, M., and Asad Azad Khan, R. (2019). Impact of personality traits on investment intention: the mediating role of risk behaviour and the moderating role of financial literacy. J. Finance Econ. Res. 4, 1–18. doi: 10.20547/jfer1904101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newell, G., and MacFarlane, J. (2006). The accuracy of commercial property forecasting in Australia. Pac. Rim Prop. Res. J. 12, 311–325.

Google Scholar

Nguyen, T. A. N., and Rozsa, Z. (2019). Financial literacy and financial advice seeking for retirement investment choice. J. Competitiveness 11, 70–83. doi: 10.7441/joc.2019.01.05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northcraft, G. B., and Neale, M. A. (1987). Experts, amateurs, and real estate: an anchoring-and-adjustment perspective on property pricing decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 39, 84–97.

Google Scholar

Odean, T. (1998). Volume, volatility, price, and profit when all traders are above average. J. Finance 53, 1887–1934.

Google Scholar

Pandey, R., and Jessica, V. M. (2018). Measuring behavioural biases affecting real estate investment decisions in India: using IRT. Int. J. Hous. Mark. Anal. 11, 648–668. doi: 10.1108/IJHMA-12-2017-0103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauli, G. F., Chen, S. N., Simmler, C., Lankin, D. C., Gödecke, T., Jaki, B. U., et al. (2014). Importance of purity evaluation and the potential of quantitative 1H NMR as a purity assay: miniperspective. J. Med. Chem. 57, 9220–9231. doi: 10.1021/jm500734a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfnür, A., and Wagner, B. (2020). Transformation of the real estate and construction industry: empirical findings from Germany. J. Bus. Econ. 90, 975–1019.

Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, Y., Saleem, A., Saeed, W., and Haq, J. U. (2021). Online consumer satisfaction during COVID-19: perspective of a developing country. Front. Psychol. 12:751854. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.751854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riaz, T., and Iqbal, H. (2015). Impact of overconfidence, illusion of control, self control and optimism bias on investors decision making; evidence from developing markets. Res. J. Finance Account. 6, 110–116.

Google Scholar

Robinson, A. T., and Marino, L. D. (2015). Overconfidence and risk perceptions: do they really matter for venture creation decisions? Int. Entrep. Manage. J. 11, 149–168.

Google Scholar

Ross, S. A. (1973). The economic theory of agency: the principal’s problem. Am. Econ. Rev. 63, 134–139.

Google Scholar

Rubinstein, M. (2002). Markowitz’s” portfolio selection”: a fifty-year retrospective. J. Finance 57, 1041–1045.

Google Scholar

Sabir, S., Mohammad, H., and Shahar, H. (2019). The role of overconfidence and past investment experience in herding behaviour with a moderating effect of financial literacy: evidence from pakistan stock exchange. Asian Econ. Financ. Rev. 9, 480–490.

Google Scholar

Scheier, M. F., and Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 4:219.

Google Scholar

Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Curr. Biol. 21, R941–R945. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, H., and Park, S. (2018). Do foreign investors mitigate anchoring bias in stock market? Evidence based on post-earnings announcement drift. Pac. Basin Finance J. 48, 224–240. doi: 10.1016/j.pacfin.2018.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychol. Rev. 63, 129–138.

Google Scholar

Simmons, L. (1999). Effects of an Anchoring Configuration on the Static Response of Geotextile and Geogrid Fabrics. Mississippi State, MS: Mississippi State University.

Google Scholar

Singh, R., and Bhowal, A. (2010). Risk perception of employees with respect to equity shares. J. Behav. Finance 11, 177–183.

Google Scholar

Stiglitz, J. E. (2000). The contributions of the economics of information to twentieth century economics. Q. J. Econ. 115, 1441–1478. doi: 10.1162/003355300555015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 1273–1296. doi: 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thabane, L., Ma, J., Chu, R., Cheng, J., Ismaila, A., Rios, L. P., et al. (2010). A tutorial on pilot studies: the what, why and how. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 10:1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-10-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevelyan, R. (2008). Optimism, overconfidence and entrepreneurial activity. Manage. Decis. 46, 986–1001. doi: 10.1108/00251740810890177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1979). Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 5, 297–323.

Google Scholar

Waweru, N. M., Munyoki, E., and Uliana, E. (2008). The effects of behavioural factors in investment decision-making: a survey of institutional investors operating at the nairobi stock exchange. Int. J. Bus. Emerg. Mark. 1, 24–41. doi: 10.1504/IJBEM.2008.019243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waweru, N. M., Mwangi, G. G., and Parkinson, J. M. (2014). Behavioural factors influencing investment decisions in the Kenyan property market. Afro Asian J. Finance Account. 4:26. doi: 10.1504/AAJFA.2014.059500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, E. U., Shafir, S., and Blais, A. R. (2004). Predicting risk sensitivity in humans and lower animals: risk as variance or coefficient of variation. Psychol. Rev. 111, 430–445.

Google Scholar

Zacharakis, A. L., and Shepherd, D. A. (2001). The nature of information and overconfidence on venture capitalists’ decision making. J. Bus. Ventur. 16, 311–332. doi: 10.1016/S0883-9026(99)00052-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zain-ul-Abdin, S., Shahzad, K., Kaleemullah, M., and Ahmad, M. (2019). Real-estate investor’s psychology: heuristics and prospect factors. Paradigms 13, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Keywords: optimism bias, anchoring bias, risk perception, information asymmetry, decision making

Citation: Zhang M, Nazir MS, Farooqi R and Ishfaq M (2022) Moderating Role of Information Asymmetry Between Cognitive Biases and Investment Decisions: A Mediating Effect of Risk Perception. Front. Psychol. 13:828956. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.828956

Received: 04 December 2021; Accepted: 31 January 2022;
Published: 22 March 2022.

Edited by:

Tahir Islam, Mohammad Ali Jinnah University, Pakistan

Reviewed by:

Sanaullah Tariq, University of Aberdeen, United Kingdom
Muhammad Usman, Government College University Faisalabad, Pakistan

Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Nazir, Farooqi and Ishfaq. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Muhammad Ishfaq, bS5pc2hmYXFAcmlwaGFoZnNkLmVkdS5waw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.