Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 June 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Not All Daydreaming Is Equal: A Longitudinal Investigation of Social and General Daydreaming and Marital Relationship Quality

\r\nShogo Kajimura*&#x;Shogo Kajimura1*†Yuki Nozaki&#x;Yuki Nozaki2†Takayuki GotoTakayuki Goto3Jonathan SmallwoodJonathan Smallwood4
  • 1Department of Information and Human Sciences, Kyoto Institute of Technology, Kyoto, Japan
  • 2Faculty of Letters, Konan University, Kobe, Japan
  • 3School of Human Cultures, University of Shiga Prefecture, Shiga, Japan
  • 4Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada

Preliminary evidence suggests that daydreaming about other people has adaptive value in daily social lives. To address this possibility, we examined whether daydreaming plays a role in maintaining close, stable relationships using a 1-year prospective longitudinal study. We found that individuals’ propensity to daydream about their marital partner is separate to general daydreaming. In contrast to general daydreaming, which was associated with lower subsequent relationship investment size (i.e., magnitude and importance of resources attached to a relationship) in the marital partner, partner-related social daydreaming led to a greater subsequent investment size. Additionally, attachment styles moderated these effects. The effect of daydreaming regarding investment size was found only in securely attached individuals. This research advances the emerging field of social daydreaming and highlights self-generated thought as a critical tool that can help people navigate the complex social world.

Introduction

In daily life, cognition is not always related to events currently occurring. It can arise independently of concurrent perceptual input and any external task being performed. Evidence shows that people spend up to half of their waking time engaged in thoughts that are only loosely tied to their current activity (Kane et al., 2007; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Song and Wang, 2012). Such self-generated mental activity, often called daydreaming (Smallwood, 2013), can help individuals absorbed in it feel happy and calm, and may be used as a coping mechanism to handle the frustrations of daily living (Bigelsen and Schupak, 2011).

Despite these benefits, existing literature suggests that engagement in daydreaming may be detrimental to well-being. For example, daydreaming has been associated with higher levels of anxiety and depression (Stawarczyk et al., 2012), daily unhappiness (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010), and poor sleep quality (Carciofo et al., 2014). Furthermore, excessive daydreaming is considered maladaptive daydreaming, which is associated with shame and dissociation (Ferrante et al., 2022), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, and obsessive-compulsive and related disorders (Somer et al., 2017).

However, daydreaming is not experienced homogenously; consequences of daydreaming may depend on the specific features of imagination patterns (Ruby et al., 2013; Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). One important and major content of daydreaming involves other people, which has recently been conceptualized as social daydreaming (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). This study aims to offer novel insights into the emerging field of social daydreaming.

Social Daydreaming and General Daydreaming

When daydreaming, individuals spend a significant amount of time thinking about others (Mar et al., 2012; Song and Wang, 2012; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2013). One fundamental human motive is the desire to form and maintain social connections (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). Because daydreaming typically reflects engagement with personal goals (Baird et al., 2011), it is associated with the pursuit of social connections (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). Therefore, it plays an important role regarding social well-being. We define general daydreaming as daydreaming that includes all kinds of content and social daydreaming as daydreaming that only includes social content.

Preliminary evidence suggests that unlike general daydreaming, social daydreaming may have an adaptive value for daily social life. A cross-sectional study revealed that a higher propensity for daydreaming about significant others was related to higher life satisfaction (Mar et al., 2012). A one-day experience sampling study showed that everyday social daydreaming, but not non-social daydreaming, was associated with increased happiness and feelings of connection (Poerio et al., 2015). Additionally, a 4-week longitudinal study demonstrated that positive-valence social daydreaming predicted a reduction in loneliness during the transition to university (Poerio et al., 2016). Given the different functionalities of social daydreaming, these findings highlight the importance of capturing it as a construct separate from general daydreaming.

However, it remains unclear whether social daydreaming has adaptive value within a specific context. Theoretically, daydreaming about another person may facilitate the pursuit and attainment of meaningful social goals, e.g., maintaining a positive relationship with that person (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). Therefore, it is important to identify who participants daydream about and whether this propensity is connected to the relationship quality with that person. Nevertheless, previous studies did not specify a target of social daydreaming during assessment (Poerio et al., 2016) or capture the relationship quality with a specific other as a correlate of social daydreaming (Mar et al., 2012). Moreover, given that these studies captured the change in adjustments in a maximum of 4 weeks, further long-term research is necessary.

To fill this gap, we focus on a marital partner as a target of social daydreaming and examine how it is associated with marital relationship quality through a 1-year longitudinal study. Marital partners play an important role in an individual’s well-being (Proulx et al., 2007). Therefore, focusing on the propensity for daydreaming of the marital partner as a separate construct from general daydreaming will offer important empirical evidence to deepen the understanding of its interpersonal functionality.

Attachment Style as a Potential Moderator of the Relationships Between Social Daydreaming and Marital Relationship Quality

Although previous research has generally found positive aspects of social daydreaming, Poerio and Smallwood (2016) stressed that social daydreaming per se was not inherently adaptive or maladaptive. Rather, according to the context regulation hypothesis of daydreaming (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013), adaptiveness depends on the content of thought (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). Therefore, a third factor, which could influence the characteristics of daydreams, would moderate the relationship between daydreaming about marital partners and marital relationship quality.

Attachment style is known to influence how people daydream about their marital partners in their daily lives (e.g., Birnbaum et al., 2011). This construct reflects how individuals build mental representations of themselves and significant others based on their interpersonal experiences (Bowlby, 1973; Mikulincer et al., 1993). Although the original attachment theory addresses early relationships between children and caregivers (Bowlby, 1969), it could serve as the basis for the theory of romantic relationships (Hazan and Shaver, 1987).

Among the different attachment styles, individuals with secure attachment to their partner describe love experiences as friendly, happy, and trusting (Hazan and Shaver, 1987). Contrastingly, there are two types of insecure attachment to partners: anxious and avoidant (Hazan and Shaver, 1987; Feeney and Noller, 1990)1. Although their detailed features are different,2 both types of attachments are characterized by a negatively valanced internal working model (Collins, 1996). Positively and negatively valanced repetitive thoughts are associated with good and poor adjustment, respectively (Segerstrom et al., 2003; Watkins, 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2013).

Recent studies have reported differences in daydreaming experience across attachment styles. Individuals who experienced frequent maladaptive daydreaming tended to show high anxiety, while individuals who experienced normal daydreaming tended to have secure attachment styles (Mariani et al., 2021; Sándor et al., 2021). The relationship between attachment style and problematic social media use was significantly mediated by maladaptive daydreaming in high anxiety-related styles, but not in the secure style (Costanzo et al., 2021). These results indicate that individuals with an insecure attachment style show a greater negative association between daydreaming about their marital partner and subsequent marital relationship quality, whereas individuals with a secure attachment style show a greater positive association between them.

The Present Study

We examined how the propensity for daydreaming of a marital partner was associated with marital relationship quality over 1 year, via a three-wave longitudinal study. Following an emerging body of research on social daydreaming, we conceptualized the propensity for daydreaming about the marital partner as a separate construct from the propensity for general daydreaming. We modified an existing daydreaming measure by emphasizing that the target was an individual’s marital partner. We expected to find two separate but related factors when we jointly analyzed the existing measure of general daydreaming and modified measures of social daydreaming (Hypothesis 1).

Moreover, given that social daydreaming generally has adaptive value for daily social lives, we hypothesized that the daydreaming of marital partners would be positively associated with subsequent marital relationship quality (Hypothesis 2). We also hypothesized that attachment style would moderate this association; individuals with a secure attachment style would show a greater positive association between daydreaming about their marital partner and subsequent marital relationship quality, whereas individuals with an insecure attachment style would show a greater negative association between them. (Hypothesis 3). Because gender and marital duration are related to marital relationship quality (Proulx et al., 2007), we used them as control variables.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

All participants were Japanese and married, and were recruited by an online survey company (Rakuten Insight, Japan).3 The survey was conducted in Japanese. The data supporting the findings of this study are available upon request from the corresponding author. The surveys were conducted in January 2016 (Time 1), July 2016 (Time 2), and January 2017 (Time 3). To increase the quality of the data, the online survey company screened participants who failed to correctly answer at least one of the two screening questions (“Please select the leftmost option” and “Please select the rightmost option”) at Time 1. Thus, we obtained data only from participants who answered these questions correctly.

At Time 1, 327 participants (156 men and 171 women; mean age = 42.6, SD = 12.80, age range = 22–69 years) completed the study. At Times 2 and 3, 251 participants (119 men and 132 women) and 215 participants (102 men and 113 women), respectively, completed all questionnaires. Participants provided personal information (marital duration, family composition, occupation, salary, and educational background) at Time 1. At each time point, the participants completed a set of questionnaires, as detailed below. They also completed several individual difference measures unrelated to the current research question (see Supplementary Material 1 for a full description of these measures). The study procedure was approved by the ethics committee of a university in Japan.

The sample size was determined a priori by considering similar prior studies and budget constraints. The required sample size to detect misfit was satisfied, corresponding to root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.08, involving df > 15, with a power of 95% on α = 0.05. This power analysis was followed by Moshagen and Erdfelder (2015) procedure and conducted using the semPower 1.1.0 package (Moshagen, 2020).

Measures

Propensity for General and Partner-Related Daydreaming (Time 1–3)

General daydreaming was assessed using the Daydream Frequency Scale (DDFS; Giambra, 1993), which composes the Imaginal Process Inventory (Singer and Antrobus, 1970). It consists of 12 items and measures the frequency of general daydreams in daily life. Previous research has validated the Japanese version of the DDFS (Kajimura and Nomura, 2016). In addition, we slightly modified the DDFS to measure the propensity for partner-related daydreaming (DDFS-partner) by simply adding the phrase “about your partner” after the word “daydreaming” in the instruction and items of the DDFS. Both scales were answered on a 5-point scale.

Marital Relationship Quality (Time 1–3)

The subjective quality of marital relationships was measured using the Investment Model Scale (IMS; Rusbult et al., 1998). It measures commitment (intent to persist in a relationship; via seven items), satisfaction (positive versus negative affect experienced in a relationship; five items), investment size (magnitude and importance of resources attached to a relationship; five items), and quality of alternative relationships (perceived desirability of the best available alternative to a relationship; five items), which enabled us to explore the effect of daydreaming on various factors associated with marital relationship quality. The items were answered on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). The Japanese version of the IMS was validated in a previous study (Komura and Nakamine, 2013).

Attachment Style (Time 1)

Attachment style was measured using the Experiences in Close Relationship Scale-Short Form (ECR-S; Wei et al., 2007), which consists of 12 items answered on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). It measures two attachment dimensions: anxiety (fear of interpersonal rejection or abandonment, excessive need for approval from others, and distress when one’s partner is unavailable or unresponsive), and avoidance (fear of dependence and interpersonal intimacy, excessive need for self-reliance, and reluctance to self-disclose). The original items were translated into Japanese by the authors, and consistency was checked via back-translation (NAI, Inc., Japan).4

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted with R 4.0.3 (R Core Team, 2020) and Mplus 8.3 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2017) via the MplusAutomation 0.8 package (Hallquist and Wiley, 2018). As in most longitudinal studies, data resulting from participant attrition were missing. Thus, we compared participants who remained in the study and those who dropped out using Little’s Missing Completely at Random Test (Little, 1988) with the BaylorEdPsych 0.5 package (Beaujean, 2012). We obtained a non-significant χ2 value for this test (χ2(42) = 47.79, p = 0.249), suggesting that missing values in the dataset were completely missing at random and could be reliably estimated. Therefore, we applied the full information maximum likelihood method to structural equation modeling to address missing data (Enders, 2010).

Factor Analyses of Daydreaming Scales

To investigate whether partner-related and general daydreaming can be regarded as separate (but related) factors, we first conducted cross-sectional factor analyses on the data from each time point separately. Following a recent recommendation for factor analyses (Schmitt et al., 2018), we first conducted a parallel analysis using minimum rank factor analysis (Timmerman and Lorenzo-Seva, 2011) with the EFA.MRFA 1.0.9 package (Navarro-Gonzalez and Lorenzo-Seva, 2020) to initially assess the number of factors. We then conducted exploratory factor analyses (EFA) using promax rotation with the psych 2.0.9 package (Revelle, 2020).

Longitudinal Measurement Invariance

We examined the longitudinal measurement invariance for daydreaming and investment scales. We tested a series of four models with increasing invariance for each subscale (Widaman et al., 2010): (1) configural invariance (same pattern of fixed and free factor loadings across time), (2) weak factorial invariance (invariant factor loadings across time), (3) strong factorial invariance (invariant factor loadings and intercepts across time), and (4) strict factorial invariance (invariant factor loadings, intercepts, and unique variances across time). To identify the same latent construct longitudinally, a strong or strict factorial invariance must hold across measurement times (Widaman et al., 2010, p.13).

To evaluate the invariance between successive models, we investigated the changes in the comparative fit index (ΔCFI) because the chi-square difference test is sensitive to large samples (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002). We followed the rule of thumb of Cheung and Rensvold (2002) that a CFI decrease ≤ 0.01 indicates an invariant model fit. All models were estimated using robust full information maximum likelihood to accommodate non-normality. Residual correlations between identical items over time were freely estimated to account for the non-independence of uniqueness over time.

Descriptive Statistics

We report the means and standard deviations of each variable. Moreover, we evaluated internal consistency using McDonald’s (1999) total omega coefficient (ωt) with the psych 2.0.9 package (Revelle, 2020). This coefficient is superior to Cronbach’s alpha in evaluating a scale’s internal consistency (Revelle and Zinbarg, 2009; McNeish, 2018).

Cross-Lagged Panel Models

Our main interest was between-person effects (i.e., when individuals have a high propensity for partner-related daydreaming relative to others, they experience a subsequent rank-order increase in marital relationship quality versus individuals with a low propensity for partner-related daydreaming) rather than within-person effects (i.e., when individuals have a higher propensity for partner-related daydreaming than usual, they experience a subsequent increase in marital relationship quality). Therefore, we analyzed the longitudinal relationships between daydreaming and marital relationship quality using cross-lagged panel models (Figure 1), following the recommendations of Orth et al. (2021).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cross-lagged model. Control variables (gender and marital duration) are not displayed for clarity of presentation. Marital relationship quality is indicated by any subscale of the Investment Model Scale.

We tested bidirectional relationships among observed partner-related daydreaming, general daydreaming, and marital relationship quality at three time points. We also included first-order autoregressive paths (i.e., stability paths from Time 1 to Time 2 and from Time 2 to Time 3 for all variables), second-order autoregressive paths (i.e., stability paths from Time 1 to Time 3 for all variables), and within-time correlations among all the variables. The corresponding path coefficients were constrained to be equal across intervals, and gender and marital duration were set as the control variables. To evaluate the overall model fit, we used the following indices (Browne and Cudeck, 1992; Hu and Bentler, 1999): CFI ≥ 0.90, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) ≤ 0.08, and RMSEA ≤ 0.08.

Specifying Different Type of Attachment Style

Following previous research (Kawamoto, 2016), we attempted to specify different types of attachment styles based on anxiety and avoidance scores. Thus, we conducted a two-stage cluster analysis, following the procedure recommended by Punj and Stewart (1983). This procedure has frequently been used in psychological research (Luyckx et al., 2005; Stellar et al., 2020). All scores were standardized prior to analysis. First, we carried out a hierarchical cluster analysis using Ward’s method on squared Euclidean distances to identify the number of clusters. We examined the dendrogram and clustering validity indices from the NbClust package (version 3.0; Charrad et al., 2014). We then conducted k-means non-hierarchical cluster analysis using the number of clusters and centroids identified using Ward’s method to form the final clusters.

Multigroup Cross-Lagged Panel Models

To test whether attachment style moderated the cross-lagged effects, we conducted a multigroup cross-lagged analysis. We tested whether model fit was significantly worsened by equality constraints across the groups for cross-lagged effects of social daydreaming and general daydreaming on subsequent marital relationship quality. If the chi-square difference test revealed that model fit was significantly worsened by the equality constraint, the cross-lagged effects were moderated by attachment style.

Results

Factor Analyses for Daydreaming Scales

Parallel analysis using minimum rank factor analysis recommended two common factors for general daydreaming and partner-related items at all the time points. Thus, we conducted an EFA by setting two factors in the data from each time point separately. Table 1 reports the factor loadings and correlations obtained from the EFA. At all time points, we found two positively correlated factors that clearly corresponded to partner-related and general daydreaming. All items loaded highly on the intended factor, with coefficients exceeding 0.40. Moreover, none of the items exhibited cross-factor loadings exceeding an absolute value of 0.27. These results support Hypothesis 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Factor loadings and correlations based on the exploratory factor analyses of the general and partner-related daydreaming scale items.

Longitudinal Measurement Invariance

We examined the longitudinal measurement invariance of partner-related daydreaming, general daydreaming, and each subscale of marital relationship quality. Table 2 reports the results of the model fit indices and the nested model comparisons. Strict factorial invariances were achieved for all variables (CFI decrease ≤ 0.01),5 except for the alternatives. Therefore, longitudinal relationships associated with alternatives should be interpreted with caution.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model-fit indices and nested model comparisons from the analyses of longitudinal measurement invariance.

Descriptive Statistics

Table 3 summarizes the means, standard deviations, and McDonald’s (1999) total omega coefficients (ωt) for each variable. Although the internal consistency was slightly low for anxiety, it was satisfactory for the remaining variables.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean, standard deviation, and total omega coefficient of each variable.

Cross-Lagged Panel Models

We analyzed the bidirectional longitudinal relationships between partner-related daydreaming, general daydreaming, and each subscale of marital relationship quality using cross-lagged panel models (Figure 1). The model fits were adequate for all models (CFIs > 0.981, SRMRs < 0.049, RMSEAs < 0.063). The results showed that all cross-lagged effects were non-significant, except for investment (see Supplementary Material 2 for full parameter estimates).

Table 4 summarizes the cross-lagged effects of the relationships between social daydreaming, general daydreaming, and investment. Specifically, the cross-lagged effect of partner-related daydreaming on investment is positive and statistically significant (β = 0.18, p = 0.001). Contrastingly, the cross-lagged effect of general daydreaming on investment is negative and statistically significant (β = −0.13, p = 0.008). The remaining cross-lagged effects were not statistically significant. These results suggest that investment supports Hypothesis 2.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Unstandardized and standardized coefficients of the cross-lagged effects.

Moderated Effects of the Attachment Style

Cluster analysis revealed three attachment styles that corresponded to previous studies (Hazan and Shaver, 1987; Kawamoto, 2016): secure (low anxiety and low avoidance; n = 118), anxious (high anxiety and middle avoidance; n = 76), and avoidant (middle anxiety and high avoidance; n = 133). We report the details of the cluster analysis and differences in descriptive values across attachment styles in Supplementary Material 3.

Next, we conducted multigroup analyses to investigate whether attachment style moderated the cross-lagged effects. The chi-square difference tests revealed that this equality constraint significantly worsened the model fit for investment (Δχ2(4) = 14.76, p = 0.005) but not for commitment (Δχ2(4) = 1.36, p = 0.850), satisfaction (Δχ2(4) = 6.59, p = 0.159), and alternatives (Δχ2(4) = 5.43, p = 0.246). These results suggest that attachment style moderates the cross-lagged effects of social daydreaming and general daydreaming on investment.6

Table 5 reports cross-lagged effects from social daydreaming and general daydreaming to investment per attachment style from the model without equality constraint. The model fit was adequate (χ2(45) = 57.23, p = 0.105, CFI = 0.988, SRMR = 0.052, RMSEA = 0.050 90% CI [0.000, 0.085]). Specifically, in the secure group, the cross-lagged effect of partner-related daydreaming on investment was positive and statistically significant (β = 0.43, p < 0.001), whereas the cross-lagged effect of general daydreaming on investment was negative and statistically significant (β = −0.34, p < 0.001). These cross-lagged effects were negligible and non-significant in the anxious and avoidant groups. These results partially supported Hypothesis 3.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Cross-lagged effects from social daydreaming and general daydreaming to investment per attachment style.

Discussion

Summary of Results

Our study provides important evidence that the social features of daydreams can play an important role in marital relationship quality, especially for the investments people make in their marital relationships. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, we found that individuals’ propensity to daydream about their marital partners was a separate factor from general daydreaming. Moreover, we found that partner-related daydreaming and general daydreaming had opposing effects on investment in marital partners. In contrast to general daydreaming, which had a negative impact on subsequent investment size in the marital partner, thinking about the marital partner at idle moments led to larger subsequent investments. These results are consistent with Hypothesis 2. Additionally, attachment styles moderated these cross-lagged effects. We found a positive effect of partner-related daydreaming and a negative effect of general daydreaming with investment only in the secure group, partially supporting Hypothesis 3.

Theoretical Implications

Our study contributes to the theoretical advancement of social daydreaming (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016). A growing body of evidence has demonstrated that understanding the consequences of daydreaming in daily life requires an understanding of the specific features of the pattern of thought (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). Our research extends these findings by showing that in the context of close personal relationships, the daydreaming of a marital partner is functionally distinct from general daydreaming. Hence, our results are theoretically important because they provide empirical evidence showing the uniquely positive consequences of social daydreaming in the context of marital relationships.

We find these effects only for investment size in marital partners, possible since only the investment size has a property—accumulation, which is directly affected by the propensity of partner-related daydreaming. Daydreaming about a partner can be considered a way in which people put time and effort into their relationship, and prior studies suggest that the more often an individual spends time thinking about the partner, the more resources are accumulated in the relationship (Rusbult, 1983; Rusbult et al., 1998; Le and Agnew, 2003). This directed relationship is reflected in the results of the cross-lagged models; the direction from partner-related daydreaming to investment size, but not from investment size to partner-related daydreaming, was significant. Contrastingly, other subscales of relationship quality are unlikely to be directly affected by partner-related daydreaming. Satisfaction level is influenced by the extent to which the partner fulfills an individual’s most important needs, indicating that the partner’s propensity to daydream about the individual is more important for the individual’s satisfaction level. The quality of the alternatives is based on the situation “outside” the current relationship, which is independent of the propensity to daydream about the partner. Commitment emerges because of the investment size, satisfaction level, and quality of the alternatives; thus, the effect of partner-related daydreaming on commitment would be blurred.

Our study also provides further insight into the context regulation hypothesis of daydreaming (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013; Smallwood and Schooler, 2015). Consistent with this hypothesis, the results clearly highlight the fact that the benefits of social daydreaming are not universal. Specifically, individuals with anxious and avoidant attachment styles did not show a significant association between partner-related daydreaming and subsequent investment in their partner. This might be because daydreaming about marital partners was only a way of compensation for individuals with insecure attachment styles. Individuals who display frequent maladaptive daydreaming, which is associated with insecure attachment styles (Costanzo et al., 2021; Mariani et al., 2021; Sándor et al., 2021), tend to address unmet emotional needs by engaging in compensatory fantasies specific to personality traits (Brenner et al., 2022). If partner-related daydreaming served the sole purpose of compensation in individuals with insecure attachment styles, it would be reasonable that the time spent daydreaming did not reflect in the relationship. The hypothetical compensatory function of daydreaming in individuals with insecure attachment styles should be tested in future research.

Contrastingly, individuals with a secure attachment style showed a positive association between partner-related daydreaming and subsequent investment in their marital partners. This result is reasonable, as secure individuals have a positively valenced model of the social world (Collins, 1996), and positively valenced repetitive thought is associated with positive adjustment (Segerstrom et al., 2003; Watkins, 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2013). Thus, positively valenced daydreaming about their partners, which is daydreaming not for compensation or other purposes but for the sake of daydreaming itself, could reflect positively in relationships.

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite the strengths of this study, it has certain limitations. First, it would have been helpful to use online experience sampling in daily life to examine the situations in which individuals use self-generated thoughts to facilitate smooth social relationships. While our study reveals the long-term effect of socially focused daydreaming on social relationships, momentary experience sampling allows for the assessment of short-term effects of ongoing thoughts on subsequent behaviors. This fills the gap between thinking about another person on the one hand, and the pursuit and attainment of meaningful social goals on the other (Poerio and Smallwood, 2016).

Second, investigating the mutual effect of thinking about a partner in marital relationships will further expand this study’s findings. Although this study reveals that individuals’ partner-related daydreaming affects their subsequent subjective investment size, it is possible that this association would have maximum benefits for relationships in which both partners engage in these patterns of cognition.

Third, we identified three attachment styles from the empirical data, in spite of the presence of existing studies using four attachment styles (e.g., Brennan et al., 1998). Specifically, the collected data were best fitted to the model omitting the cluster of fearful attachment style, which represents both high anxiety and avoidance (Brennan et al., 1998). This is consistent with some previous studies, including a study with Japanese participants, which identified the same three clusters from empirical data (e.g., Ceglian and Gardner, 2001; Kawamoto, 2016). Moreover, as this study mainly focused on the difference between secure and insecure styles, the difference in the number of clusters did not remarkably affect hypotheses testing. Nevertheless, future research should examine whether the fearful attachment style can be identified with larger samples.

Fourth, the sample we studied was only from Japan. It is possible that important cultural boundaries in our results will determine for whom and when socially focused daydreaming is most likely to have adaptive functions. However, we would like to emphasize that our results are consistent with the existing theory, which is largely based on Western population studies (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013; Poerio and Smallwood, 2016).

Conclusion

Our study showed that social daydreaming plays an important role in marital relationships. Moreover, daydreaming about a marital partner has beneficial associations with investment in the partner over a relatively long period of time. Given the important role of social relationships, our study highlights that periods of self-generated thinking are not always idle fantasies but can play a crucial role as one of the most defining features of the human condition.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committees of the Kyoto University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SK: conceptualization, data collection, and writing the manuscript. YN: conceptualization, statistical analysis, and writing the manuscript. TG: conceptualization and statistical analysis. JS: supervision and editing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (14J05285).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904025/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ Although existing literature using four attachment styles is prevalent (e.g., Brennan et al., 1998), some previous studies have identified three clusters consisting of secure, anxious, and avoidant styles from empirical data (e.g., Ceglian and Gardner, 2001; Kawamoto, 2016). Considering this inconsistency, we explored the number of attachment styles based on the empirical data, following the statistical procedure used in a study by Brennan et al. (1998) detailed in the Method section. Three styles were identified from the current data and used in this study, as indicated by the results (Supplementary Figures 1, 2). This point will be discussed in the section “Discussion.”
  2. ^ Anxious attachment is linked to a fear of interpersonal rejection, excessive need for approval from others, and distress when one’s partner is unavailable or unresponsive, whereas avoidant attachment is linked to a fear of dependence and interpersonal intimacy, excessive need for self-reliance, and reluctance to self-disclose (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Hazan and Shaver, 1987).
  3. ^ https://research.rakuten.co.jp/en/aboutus.html
  4. ^ https://www.nai.co.jp/
  5. ^ The absolute values of CFI were not satisfactory for partner-related daydreaming and general daydreaming. However, Moshagen and Auerswald (2018) strongly recommend not using the absolute value of CFI in evaluating the fit of a factor analysis model, despite being useful for model comparison. This is because CFI reflects not only amount of discrepancy but also loading magnitude (i.e., lack of indicator reliability). For this reason, “…a superior approach is to consider indices that are pure indicators of model fit or pure indicators of construct reliability (Moshagen et al., 2020, p.187).” Thus, we did not consider the absolute value of CFI for evaluating each factor model.
  6. ^ Although we did not have a specific hypothesis, we tested whether model fit was significantly worsened by equality constraint for cross-lagged effects from marital relationship quality to social daydreaming and general daydreaming across attachment styles. The chi-square difference tests revealed that this equality constraint significantly worsen model fit for alternative (Δχ2(4) = 19.90, p < 0.001), whereas not for commitment (Δχ2(4) = 1.47, p = 0.832), satisfaction (Δχ2(4) = 1.86, p = 0.762), and investment (Δχ2(4) = 2.49, p = 0.647). They suggest that attachment style moderated cross-lagged effects from alternative to social daydreaming and general daydreaming. Specifically, the cross-lagged effect of alternative on partner-related daydreaming was negative and statistically significant in the secure group (β = 0.18, p = 0.001). However, it was negligible and non-significant in the anxious (β = 0.07, p = 0.369) and avoidant groups (β = −0.03, p = 0.557). The cross-lagged effect of alternative on general daydreaming was non-significant in all groups. The model fit was adequate (χ2(45) = 54.94, p = 0.147, CFI = 0.990, SRMR = 0.049, RMSEA = 0.045 90% CI [0.000, 0.082]).

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Kaiser, R. H., Turner, A. E., Reineberg, A. E., Godinez, D., Dimidjian, S., et al. (2013). A penny for your thoughts: dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 4:900. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baird, B., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1604–1611. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaujean, A. A. (2012). BaylorEdPsych: R package for Baylor University educational psychology quantitative courses. R package version 0.5. Available Online at: https://cran.r-project.org/src/contrib/Archive/BaylorEdPsych/ (accessed August 7, 2020).

Google Scholar

Bigelsen, J., and Schupak, C. (2011). Compulsive fantasy: proposed evidence of an under-reported syndrome through a systematic study of 90 self-identified non-normative fantasizers. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1634–1648. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birnbaum, G. E., Mikulincer, M., and Gillath, O. (2011). In and out of a daydream: attachment orientations, daily couple interactions, and sexual fantasies. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1398–1410. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Separation, Anxiety and Anger. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., and Shaver, P. R. (1998). “Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview,” in Attachment theory and close relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York: The Guilford Press), 46–76.

Google Scholar

Brenner, R., Somer, E., and Abu-Rayya, H. M. (2022). Personality traits and maladaptive daydreaming: fantasy functions and themes in a multi-country sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 184:111194. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol. Methods Res. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carciofo, R., Du, F., Song, N., and Zhang, K. (2014). Mind wandering, sleep quality, affect and chronotype: an exploratory study. PLoS One 9:e91285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceglian, C. P., and Gardner, S. (2001). Attachment style and the “wicked stepmother” spiral. J. Divorce Remarriage 34, 111–129. doi: 10.1300/J087v34n01_07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charrad, M., Ghazzali, N., Boiteau, V., and Niknafs, A. (2014). NbClust: an R package for determining the relevant number of clusters in a data set. J. Stat. Softw. 61, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v061.i06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007sem0902_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 810–832. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.4.810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costanzo, A., Santoro, G., Russo, S., Cassarà, M. S., Midolo, L. R., Billieux, J., et al. (2021). Attached to virtual dreams. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 209, 656–664. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000001356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Feeney, J. A., and Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 281–291. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrante, E., Marino, A., Guglielmucci, F., and Schimmenti, A. (2022). The mediating role of dissociation and shame in the relationship between emotional trauma and maladaptive daydreaming. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 9, 27–39. doi: 10.1037/cns0000253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Exp. Gerontol. 28, 485–492. doi: 10.1016/0531-5565(93)90073-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallquist, M. N., and Wiley, J. F. (2018). MplusAutomation: an R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in Mplus. Struct. Equ. Model. 25, 621–638. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1402334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kajimura, S., and Nomura, M. (2016). Development of Japanese versions of the Daydream Frequency Scale and the Mind Wandering Questionnaire. Jpn. J. Psychol. 87, 79–88. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.14223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., and Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 18, 614–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, T. (2016). Personality change from life experiences: moderation effect of attachment security. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 58, 218–231. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killingsworth, M. A., and Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komura, K., and Nakamine, S. (2013). Development of a Japanese version of the Investment Model Scale and an examination of its reliability and validity. Tsukuba Psychol. Res. 46, 39–47.

Google Scholar

Le, B., and Agnew, C. R. (2003). Commitment and its theorized determinants: a meta-analysis of the Investment Model. Pers. Relationsh. 10, 37–57. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 83, 1198–1202. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Soenens, B., Beyers, W., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Identity statuses based on 4 rather Than 2 identity dimensions: extending and refining Marcia’s paradigm. J. Youth Adolesc. 34, 605–618. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-8949-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, R. A., Mason, M. F., and Litvack, A. (2012). How daydreaming relates to life satisfaction, loneliness, and social support: the importance of gender and daydream content. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 401–407. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariani, R., Musetti, A., Di Monte, C., Danskin, K., Franceschini, C., and Christian, C. (2021). Maladaptive daydreaming in relation to linguistic features and Attachment Style. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:386. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19010386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

McNeish, D. (2018). Thanks coefficient alpha, We’ll take it from here. Psychol. Methods 23, 412–433. doi: 10.1037/met0000144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., and Weller, A. (1993). Attachment styles, coping strategies, and posttraumatic psychological distress: the impact of the Gulf War in Israel. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 817–826. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.5.817

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moshagen, M. (2020). semPower: Power analyses for SEM. R package version 1.1.0. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semPower (accessed August 7, 2020).

Google Scholar

Moshagen, M., and Auerswald, M. (2018). On congruence and incongruence of measures of fit in structural equation modeling. Psychol. Methods 23, 318–336. doi: 10.1037/met0000122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moshagen, M., and Erdfelder, E. (2015). A new strategy for testing structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. 23, 54–60. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.950896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moshagen, M., Zettler, I., and Hilbig, B. E. (2020). Measuring the dark core of personality. Psychol. Assess. 32, 182–196. doi: 10.1037/pas0000778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Navarro-Gonzalez, D., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2020). EFA.MRFA: Dimensionality assessment using minimum rank factor analysis. R package version 1.0.9. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=EFA.MRFA (accessed August 7, 2020).

Google Scholar

Orth, U., Clark, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., and Robins, R. W. (2021). Testing prospective effects in longitudinal research: comparing seven competing cross-lagged models. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 120, 1013–1034. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poerio, G. L., and Smallwood, J. (2016). Daydreaming to navigate the social world: what we know, what we don’t know, and why it matters. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 10, 605–618. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., Emerson, L. M., and Miles, E. (2015). Love is the triumph of the imagination: daydreams about significant others are associated with increased happiness, love and connection. Conscious. Cogn. 33, 135–144. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., Emerson, L. M., and Miles, E. (2016). Social daydreaming and adjustment: an experience-sampling study of socio-emotional adaptation during a life transition. Front. Psychol. 7:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., and Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 576–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00393.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Punj, G., and Stewart, D. W. (1983). Cluster analysis in marketing research: review and suggestions for application. J. Mark. Res. 20, 134–148. doi: 10.2307/3151680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R 4.0.3: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Revelle, W. (2020). psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA. R package version 2.0.9. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed August 7, 2020).

Google Scholar

Revelle, W., and Zinbarg, R. E. (2009). Coefficients alpha, beta, omega, and the glb: comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika 74, 145–154. doi: 10.1007/s11336-008-9102-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, F. J., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., and Singer, T. (2013). How self-generated thought shapes mood: the relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PLoS One 8:e77554. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: the development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 101–117. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., and Agnew, C. R. (1998). The Investment Model Scale: measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size. Pers. Relationsh. 5, 357–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1998.tb00177.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sándor, A., Bugán, A., Nagy, A., Bogdán, L. S., and Molnár, J. (2021). Attachment characteristics and emotion regulation difficulties among maladaptive and normal daydreamers. Curr. Psychol. 1, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01546-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, T. A., Sass, D. A., Chappelle, W., and Thompson, W. (2018). Selecting the “Best” factor structure and moving measurement validation forward: an illustration. J. Pers. Assess. 100, 345–362. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2018.1449116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segerstrom, S. C., Stanton, A. L., Alden, L. E., and Shortridge, B. E. (2003). A multidimensional structure for repetitive thought: what’s on your mind, and how, and how much? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 909–921. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, J. L., and Antrobus, J. S. (1970). Manual for the Imaginal Processes Inventory. Princeton NJ: Education Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychol. Bull. 139, 519–535. doi: 10.1037/a0030010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., and Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Front. Psychol. 4:441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 487–518. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., and Ross, C. A. (2017). The comorbidity of Daydreaming Disorder (maladaptive daydreaming). J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 205, 525–530. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, X., and Wang, X. (2012). Mind wandering in Chinese daily lives: an experience sampling study. PLoS One 7:e44423. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Using the Daydreaming Frequency Scale to investigate the relationships between mind-wandering, psychological well-being, and present-moment awareness. Front. Psychol. 3:363. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stellar, J. E., Anderson, C. L., and Gatchpazian, A. (2020). Profiles in empathy: different empathic responses to emotional and physical suffering. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 1398–1416. doi: 10.1037/xge0000718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmerman, M. E., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychol. Methods 16, 209–220. doi: 10.1037/a0023353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychol. Bull. 134, 163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Russell, D. W., Mallinckrodt, B., and Vogel, D. L. (2007). The Experiences in Close Relationship Scale (ECR)-short form: reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess. 88, 187–204. doi: 10.1080/00223890701268041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widaman, K. F., Ferrer, E., and Conger, R. D. (2010). Factorial invariance within longitudinal structural equation models: measuring the same construct across time. Child Dev. Perspect. 4, 10–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00110.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: daydreaming, social daydreaming, marital relationship, cross-lagged panel model, attachment style in close relationship

Citation: Kajimura S, Nozaki Y, Goto T and Smallwood J (2022) Not All Daydreaming Is Equal: A Longitudinal Investigation of Social and General Daydreaming and Marital Relationship Quality. Front. Psychol. 13:904025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904025

Received: 25 March 2022; Accepted: 23 May 2022;
Published: 17 June 2022.

Edited by:

Ralph Erich Schmidt, Université de Genève, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Erika Ferrante, Kore University of Enna, Italy
Eli Somer, University of Haifa, Israel

Copyright © 2022 Kajimura, Nozaki, Goto and Smallwood. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shogo Kajimura, kajimura.shogo.1204@gmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.